Skip to main content
PLOS ONE logoLink to PLOS ONE
. 2024 Sep 25;19(9):e0306817. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0306817

Environmental effects on explosive detection threshold of domestic dogs

Lauren S Fernandez 1, Sarah A Kane 1, Mallory T DeChant 1, Paola A Prada-Tiedemann 2, Nathaniel J Hall 1,*
Editor: Sankarganesh Devaraj3
PMCID: PMC11423966  PMID: 39321177

Abstract

Detection canines are deployed to detect explosives in a wide range of environmental conditions. These environmental conditions may have negative impacts on canine capabilities as a sensor. This study leveraged an air dilution olfactometer to present controlled odor concentrations of four different energetic materials (double base smokeless powder, Composition C4, ammonium nitrate, and flake Trinitrotoluene) to dogs working in a range of high temperature, standard, and low temperature conditions with high and low humidity conditions. The air dilution olfactometer controlled concentrations independent of environmental condition. Dogs’ detection threshold limits were measured using a descending staircase procedure. We measured dogs’ threshold twice for each energetic under each environmental condition. Results indicated heterogeneity in effects based on energetic, but all odors were detected at their lowest concentrations under standard conditions. Smokeless powder detection was reduced under all environmental conditions compared to standard and was least detectable under high temperature and humidity conditions. AN detection was poorest under high temperature high and low humidity conditions. C4 in contrast, was least detectable at low temperatures with high humidity. TNT detection was difficult under all conditions, so decrements due to environmental conditions were not statistically detectable. Additional measures were also found to be associated with detection limits. Under high temperature conditions, correlations were observed between canine mean subcutaneous temperature and detection limits, such that dogs experiencing greater temperature increases showed poorer detection limits. In addition, dog’s latency to sample the odor port from the onset of a trial was longest in the high temperature conditions. Longer latencies were also predictive of poorer detection performance. Overall, dogs showed deficits in detection sensitivity limits under all environmental conditions for at least one energetic material when the concentration of that energetic material was not directly impacted by the environmental conditions. These results suggest that behavioral factors related to environmental exposure can have important impacts on canine detection sensitivity and should be considered in operational environments.

Introduction

Dogs have a highly developed olfactory system giving them keen odor detection capabilities. Humans have used this ability in the dog for scent detection tasks, including detection of diseases [1, 2], pathogens [3], human scent [4], cancer [57], narcotics [8], and explosives [911]. For this reason, military, law enforcement, and other government agencies heavily rely on the detection capabilities of working dogs.

For decades, explosives detection dogs (EDD) have been one of the main tools for the detection of energetic materials and explosives [12]. The remarkable capabilities of these dogs, including their mobility and sensitivity, make it possible to detect various munitions used in acts of terrorism or war in a timely and efficient manner. Previous studies have successfully shown that dogs can be trained to detect a wide variety of energetic materials with excellent sensitivity and specificity [13].

It is important, however, to acknowledge the limitations of dogs’ capabilities to detect specific concentrations of relevant target odorants. Studies have been conducted to quantify threshold detection limits of working dogs to various substances. For example, accelerant detection canines can detect low concentrations of gasoline, as low as 0.1μL [14, 15]. Dogs’ detection threshold for methyl benzoate, an odor associated with cocaine, has also been measured to be approximately 16ppb [16]. Two dogs’ detection threshold for amyl acetate was shown to be 1.14 ppt and 1.90 ppt [17], several folds lower than that found for methyl benzoate, highlighting that detection limits vary substantially by odorant.

Quantifying canine detection limits is an important research task, however, there has been few studies on canine thresholds with energetic materials. This can be important, especially for energetics, because many have very low vapor pressure, indicating limited availability of the energetic molecule as an odor source [18].

In addition to the vapor pressure of the odor, environmental conditions can also physically affect the dog and odor source [19, 20]. Working dogs may need to be deployed in environments that reach temperatures higher than 40°C and lower than 0°C [21, 22]. Environmental conditions can have important implication for canine odor detection [19] and dogs’ general working ability [22]. Extreme conditions such as these pose a threat of heat exhaustion or hypothermia, potentially impeding the dog’s ability to perform odor detection work. The standard range of canine temperature core body temperature is between 37.2°C and 39.2°C [22]; previous studies have demonstrated that while working, dogs can exceed 40.6°C [23]. Temperatures of this severity have been shown to increase fatigue and cause clinical signs of heat exhaustion [22, 24]. Gazit and Terkel (2003) showed that increases in panting decreased detection accuracy [25]. This suggests that hot conditions could lead to poorer olfactory sensitivity in the dog.

The humidity of the environment can also influence heat stress risk [26, 27]. During strenuous work or activity in extreme conditions, heat from the core of the dog is transferred to the skin via passive conduction through the tissue, and cooling is achieved by blood flow to the skin through active convection [27]. During this time the dog will begin to pant in attempt to thermoregulate. This process is drastically impeded as the moisture content in the air increases [27]. Previous studies have found that dogs working in extreme hot and humid environmental conditions display greater heat stress at a quicker rate [28].

Due to heat stress, there is a potential that detection dogs may experience a decrease in detection accuracy while working in hot and humid environments. It has been found that landmine detection dogs had a significant decrease in detection accuracy after the working environment experienced heavy rain, ultimately increasing the humidity [28]. However, it should also be considered that the heavy rain potentially disrupted the soil containing the odor by washing the odorant away. Further data in this study showed that dogs had a decrease in detection accuracy early in the morning when the humidity was the highest and had an increase in detection success as humidity decreased throughout the day [28]. However, a literature review by Jenkins et al., 2018 noted that a moist environment is crucial for olfactory perception, indicating that humidity may aid in olfaction [29].

Cold stress and hypothermia are additional potential environmental concerns for working dogs. Diverio et al., (2016) found that search and rescue dogs working in -8.5°C to -10.4°C showed acclimatization to the extreme environment conditions and completed the search task without showing signs of stress or fatigue. This search task, however, only lasted 10 minutes, which is not representative of the duration of time a dog may be tasked with working in the cold [30]. It is necessary to establish changes in detection accuracy for dogs working in cold environments for longer durations. A survey conducted in 2012 found that out of 53 handlers working with avalanche dogs 39% reported a decrease in dog performance while working in -23°C, -12°C [31] Previous work investigating changes in detection performance of methyl benzoate in various environmental conditions, found dogs had the lowest threshold while working in cold and standard conditions compared to high temperature environments [20]. Cold temperature effects have not been investigated in explosive detection work.

Given the limited data evaluating canine olfactory detection performance to energetic materials in a variety of operationally relevant environmental conditions, the goal of this project was to create a systematic evaluation of temperature and humidity effects on canine detection for a series of energetic materials. Few prior studies accounted for how odor availability may impact canine performance. For this reason, we kept the energetic materials at a controlled environmental condition and manipulated temperature (40°C vs 0°C) and humidity conditions (90/70% vs 40/50%) for dogs. We also measured canine physiological parameters during the detection task to better understand how these measures relate to detection performance.

Methods and materials

Participants

Participants included eight spayed/ neutered adult working dogs of two breeds (Refer to Table 1). All dogs were previously eliminated from working dog programs and had previous training and/or odor work experience. Detailed descriptions of each participant can be found in Table 1. Dogs varied in their reasons for removal from their prior working program including preference for food vs. toy rewards, distractibility or environmental sensitivities. Dogs received a minimum of two walks a day and obedience training to maintain enrichment and exercise during the duration of the study. The primary reinforcement for testing and training were dog treats and dog food. Therefore, one-quarter of the total food ration was fed to the dogs in the morning, and the remaining 75% was given as a second meal at the end of the day. Dogs received additional food throughout the day during walks and training. Lastly, all dogs had free access to water at all times.

Table 1. Participant description.

Dog Approximate Age (Years) Breed Approximate weight (kg) Reproductive Status
Zulu 4 German Shorthaired pointer 22.68 Altered male
Luna 2 German Shorthaired pointer 27.67 Altered female
Moni 3 German Shorthaired pointer 24.49 Altered female
Dasty 3 Labrador retriever 27.21 Altered male
Jack 2 Labrador retriever 29.48 Altered male
Bello 3 German Shorthaired pointer 25.63 Altered male
Dalton 2 Labrador retriever 34.02 Altered male
Rocket 2 Labrador retriever 27.21 Altered male

Animal welfare considerations

All procedures utilized in the following experiments were approved by the Texas Tech University Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee (Protocol # 21051–07) and approved by the US Army Medical Research and Development Command Animal Care and Use Office (#78018-ST-H.e001). During testing in environmental conditions, participants were monitored to avoid hypothermia and hyperthermia. All dogs were first trained to detect odors at 85% accuracy or higher, while in standard environmental conditions. Thus, if the participant showed signs of distress or refusal to search for five consecutive trials, then the dog was immediately removed from the chamber and the session was terminated. Furthermore, dogs were tested for a maximum of 40 minutes, after which dogs were removed from the chamber, and sessions were terminated.

Experimental set-up

An environmental chamber measuring 3m x 3m was the primary testing and training environment during this study (see Fig 1). The chamber had built in Heating, Ventilation, Air Conditioning (HVAC) and supplemental heater units to allow the room to reach temperatures below 0°C and above 43°C. Supplemental equipment, such as dehumidifiers and humidifiers, were placed in the room to control relative humidity conditions.

Fig 1. Schematic diagram of environmental chamber.

Fig 1

Testing environment set up, containing a built-in heater and AC unit. An environmental sensor was attached to the back wall to monitor the environmental conditions while testing to maintain proper conditions. A go/ no go olfactometer testing apparatus was placed in the front of the room to preform training and testing.

Odorants

Four energetic materials were used in this experiment for testing and training. Double-base smokeless powder (SP), prill ammonium nitrate (AN), flaked trinitrotoluene (TNT), and Composition C4 (C4) were used in this experiment (see Table 2). Materials were supplied by an energetics supplier (see Table 2) at the start of the experiment. Each material, in a glass vial, was placed into a heated water bath during training and testing to standardize odor environmental conditions. Because odor availability is dependent on temperature, the temperature of the water bath was based on the odor identity and was increased for less volatile odors to improve detection. All odors underwent systematic air dilutions during testing for baseline and experimental environmental conditions. Due to expected variation in canine detection capability, the flow rate used for dilution was dependent on odor (Table 2). Based on the olfactometer design, the overall flow rate determined the highest range of concentrations that could be presented. During initial training, if dogs struggled to detect the 0.01 dilution of any odor at >85% accuracy, the total flow was set to 3 SLPM to allow for a higher range of concentrations to be presented.

Table 2. Description of odorants.

Energetic material Mass (g) Total flow rate (L/min) Temperature of water bath (°C) Brand of energetic material Vial size Diameter (cm)
Double-base smokeless powder 10g 10 L/min 30°C Hodgdon H335 2.54 cm
Ammonium nitrate 20g 3 L/min 40°C Omni explosives 5.08 cm
Trinitrotoluene 10.5g 3 L/min 40°C Omni explosives 5.08 cm
C4 8g 3 L/min 40°C Omni explosives 2.54 cm

Samples of AN and TNT were placed in wider glass vials to increase the surface area of the material. Lastly, smokeless powder underwent air dilution factors of 0.01, 0.00316, 0.001, 0.000316, and 0.0001. The remaining energetics underwent dilution factors of 1.0, 0.316, 0.1, 0.0316, and 0.01, due to dogs failing to reach qualification criterion (>85%accuracy) at a dilution factor of 0.01. Note that final air flow of non-odor trials was the same as the final air flow of diluted odor trials.

Data collection equipment

Participants were trained and tested using an automated Go/NoGo air dilution olfactometer system (Fig 2). The system utilized Alicat (Tucson, AZ, USA) gas mass airflow controllers to create a serial air dilution of the odorant being tested. A glass vial (containing the explosive) was placed in a water bath set at the desired temperature (30°C– 40°C) for each specific odor being tested. The glass vial was then pierced with two Teflon (PTFE) tubes to allow for clean air introduction and displacement of sample headspace into the system to begin a serial dilution. The odorant/ air mixture then travels through the mass airflow controllers and static mixers to create the desired air dilution. Lastly, the diluted air traveled to the port to allow participants to sniff the odor. The total airflow delivered to the dog was dependent on the odor material and ranged from 3 L/min to 10L/min but was always consistent for all dilutions for the same odor. Refer to Fig 2 for a detailed diagram of the function and air flow of the apparatus.

Fig 2. Diagram of mass air flow dilution olfactometer.

Fig 2

The path of air flow is shown from left to right. Clean air travels through a warm water bath heated to replicate the same temperature of the odorant water bath. The clean air then travels to the manifold and is dispersed to the MFC A, MFC B and MFC E. Air from MFC A is pushed into the vial containing an odorant located in a separate water bath to collect the headspace from the vial and then pushed out of the vial to a three-way junction connected to MFC B. The air from the vial is mixed with clean air from the manifold the travels to the remaining mass air flow controllers. The odorant is systematically diluted by the mass air flow controllers and then pushed down the odor line to be delivered to the port.

The olfactometer was designed for Go/NoGo testing and therefore delivered either clean air, or air containing volatiles of the target odors to a stainless-steel odor port. The probability for a correct response was 50%. The odor port was fitted with an exhaust fan, which exhausted odor from the odor port during an inter-trial intervals. The odor port also contained an infrared beam pair which detected canine nose entries into the port. Canine responses were measured by the infrared beam pair, in which dogs were trained to hold their nose in the port for 4 continuous seconds as a Go response. A NoGo response was scored when the dog sampled the odor port (inserting their nose) followed by the removal of their nose for 4 consecutive seconds. Tones were used by the computer to indicate if a response was correct or incorrect to a handler that was blind to the presence/absence of the odor.

The correct tone was played when a dog responded with a four second nose hold in the port when the port contained the diluted target (hit), or if the dog responded by inserting their nose into the port and disengaging from the port when the port contained clear air, a non-target trial (correct rejection). Furthermore, an incorrect tone was played when participants incorrectly held their nose for four seconds in the port during a non-target trial (false alert) or disengaged from the port during a target present trial (miss). A “timeout” was recorded if the dog did not sample the odor port within 45s of a trial initiation tone and being prompted to search by the handler. Inter-trial intervals were 30 seconds. For the last 10 seconds of the inter-trial interval, the olfactometer presented the odor of the next trial to ensure a stable concentration was presented prior to the dog sampling.

Physiological measures

Heart rate

Participants wore a Polar H10 heart rate monitor during each testing session. However, due to substantial missing data due to movement artifacts, heart rate was excluded from further statistical analysis.

Subcutaneous temperature

Subcutaneous temperature of each participant was taken before entering the chamber and every five minutes during testing. Temperature data was collected by scanning a HomeAgain TempScan 134.2 kHz ISO microchip [32] located subcutaneously in the shoulder of the dog. The temperature was recorded by the handler. Microchips were implanted in the dogs while working in previous training facilities before entering the canine olfaction lab.

Respiration effort (RE)

Videos were coded using BORIS [33] to determine the changes of the dogs’ respiratory effort throughout the duration of the session and differences between conditions. Each video was coded at five-minute intervals (0, 5, 10, 15, 20, etc.) for the first five seconds of each interval. The first five second clip started at the start of the first trial. However, for the video to meet criteria for coding the dog needed to be in sight of the camera with 75% of the body showing, to determine if the abdomen and chest were actively involved in the respiratory process. Further, the dog’s nose needed to be outside of the odor port and the mouth needed to be visible. If a video clip did not meet criteria, the video was scanned for the nearest five second clip that met this criteria from the specific interval. A RE scale rating system described by DeChant et al. (submitted) was used to determine the respiration effort of each participant during the five second clip for each five-minute interval. Scoring for this scale ranges from 0 to 10 (Refer to S1 Table). Zero being defined as resting and breathing is an involuntary process, and ten being very heavy breathing and the chest and abdomen are expanding and collapsing violently. For inter-observer agreement, two coders involved in the data collection scored the same 20% of the videos for RE. Inter-class correlations were used to assess agreement.

Environmental conditions

Canine’s detection sensitivity was tested twice for each energetic (4 odors) in four different extreme environmental conditions and at a standard condition. Details of the environmental conditions can be found in Table 3. Conditions were selected to match possible extremes encountered in an operational setting. A sensor (SensorPush HT.w wireless thermometer/hygrometer sensor; rated accuracy: ± 2% RH and ± 0.2°C) was placed in the room that monitored temperature and humidity conditions throughout the testing session to ensure the environmental conditions remained within limits of variation.

Table 3. Environmental conditions of threshold assessment.

Condition Temperature °C (± 5 degrees) Relative humidity (±5%)
Baseline 21°C 50%
High temperature/ high humidity 40°C 70%
High temperature/ low humidity 40°C 40%
Low temperature/ high humidity 0°C 90%
Low temperature/ low humidity 0°C 50%

Canine odor detection training and testing

Preliminary training

Dogs were previously trained to three explosive compounds (double-base smokeless powder, AN, and C4) using an automated 3-alternative line-up olfactometer while at another training facility. Previous research provides a detailed description of the automated odor delivery system used for initial training [34]. Once dogs arrived at the Canine Olfaction Lab they were then initially trained to TNT using a Go/NoGo air dilution system.

Following initial training, dogs were transferred to the Go/NoGo air dilution system, consisting of one port presenting either clean air (non-target trials) or the odor dilution of the specific energetic being tested (50% probability of odor presence). Each dog received two sessions of 20 trials with each odor of the four odors or the necessary number of sessions needed to achieve 85% accuracy or higher for two consecutive double blinded sessions.

Canine odor threshold testing

Once participants reached initial training criteria, dogs were tested on the Go/NoGo air dilution system with a descending staircase, three-down one-up procedure. Dogs were blindly presented with both odor and non-odor trials during testing (equal probability, such that at least one of each trial type occurred every three trials). Following the descending staircase method, if the dog made three correct consecutive responses, the concentration of the odor was automatically lowered by a half-log step (see Fig 3). The concentration decreased until the dog made an incorrect response. If incorrect, the concentration was raised to the previous dilution step. Concentration was lowered or raised over the session until dogs showed 8 reversals in the direction of change of concentration. The session continued until dogs either reached 8 reversals, 40 min of training, or met a welfare criterion for discontinuing (described in Animal Welfare Conditions).

Fig 3. The dog Jack following the 3-down 1-up adaptive threshold procedure for C-4 in standard conditions.

Fig 3

After three correct responses, the concentration was decreased by a half-log. Once Jack responded incorrectly when presented with 0.01 dilution of C4, the concentration was increased by a half-log. Jack correctly alerted 0.01 dilution of C4 but was not able to detect lower concentrations. Eight reversals were reached during testing and threshold was calculated as the geometric mean of the last six reversals.

Experimental design

Dogs completed all testing for one odorant before proceeding to the next in the following order (SP, C4, AN, TNT). For each odor, dogs completed initial Go/NoGo qualification training. After meeting qualification, dogs completed two threshold assessments at standard conditions. Dogs were then pseudo-randomly assigned to one of two conditions such that half of the dogs started in the hot and humid conditions working to hot/dry, cold/dry, and ended with cold/humid. The other half started in the cold/humid conditions, then moved to cold/dry, hot/dry, and ended with hot/humid. Dogs completed two threshold sessions per environmental condition before advancing to the next environmental condition. Dogs alternated between starting in hot or cold conditions when advancing from one target odorant to the next.

Statistical analysis

Threshold was calculated as the geometric mean of the last 6 (of 8) reversal points. If a dog did not complete 8 reversals due to early termination (due to welfare criterion, exceeding the session time, or failing to achieve three consecutive responses to decrease in concentration) the starting concentration (i.e., highest concentration) was imputed as the missed reversal points to reflect poorer performance.

To evaluate the impact of environmental condition a linear mixed effect model was fit in which log transformed threshold was predicted by the odor, environmental condition, and their interaction. A random intercept was fit for each dog. Fixed effects were evaluated with Anova from the car package [35]. False discovery rate adjusted post hoc tests were conducted in which each environmental condition was compared to standard conditions.

The R code is available in Supplemental information.

Results

Threshold

Average thresholds across all eight dogs for each odor in each condition are shown in Fig 4, which highlights large differences in sensitivity limits for the different energetics. Dogs showed highest sensitivity for SP followed by C4, AN and TNT. A linear mixed effect model showed a main effect of odor type on threshold detection limits (X2 = 2,512, df = 3, p<0.001). Post hoc tests indicate there was a significant difference in sensitivity between all energetics except for between AN and TNT (t = 0.83, p = 0.84).

Fig 4. Average threshold of each odor.

Fig 4

Displaying average dilution of vapor saturation threshold across all dogs for both testing sessions in standard conditions of each odor. SP, C4, AN, and TNT. Error bars show 95% bootstrap estimated confidence intervals.

Table 4 shows the detection limits (log transformation of proportion of vapor saturation) for each target odor under environmental conditions and standard conditions. A mixed effect model indicated that log threshold was unrelated to breed (X2 = 0.11, df = 1, p<0.74). However, there was a significant odor by condition interaction (X2 = 38.45, df = 12, p<0.001) as well as main effects of odor (X2 = 2,922, df = 3, p<0.001) and condition (X2 = 28.02, df = 4, p<0.001) on the log threshold scale.

Table 4. Detection limit (log of vapor saturation) for each odor in each condition and 95% confidence intervals.

Odor Standard High Temp High Humid High Temp Low Humid Low Temp High Humid Low Temp Low Humid
AN Mean -0.4511 -0.2321 -0.0907 -0.2686 -0.4636
95% CI -0.612, -0.2897 -0.393, -0.0707 -0.252, 0.0707 -0.430, -0.1072 -0.625, -0.3023
C-4 Mean -1.1848 -1.2181 -0.9844 -0.8170 -0.9716
95% CI -1.346, -1.0235 -1.380, -1.0568 -1.146, -0.8230 -0.978, -0.6556 -1.133, -0.8102
SP Mean -2.5939 -2.1563 -2.3230 -2.2918 -2.3751
95% CI -2.755, -2.4325 -2.318, -1.9950 -2.484, -2.1617 -2.453, -2.1304 -2.536, -2.2137
TNT Mean -0.2515 -0.2126 -0.2114 -0.2786 -0.3524
95% CI -0.413, -0.0902 -0.374, -0.0512 -0.373, -0.0501 -0.440, -0.1172 -0.514, -0.1911

To evaluate the odor by condition interaction, post hoc tests were conducted between conditions for each energetic. All environmental conditions were compared to the standard environmental condition (room temperature) as the control (see Table 4). Each environmental condition led to a decrement in detection limits for at least one energetic material. Importantly, performance was not better in an environmental condition in comparison to the standard condition. High Temp & High Humidity, High Temp & Low Humidity, and Low Temp & High Humidity all led to decrements for two target odors. Low Temp & Low humidity only impacted smokeless powder, and overall had the smallest magnitude impact on performance (See Fig 5 and Table 5). Further, examination of individual canine performance in all conditions detecting SP showed that individual patterns followed the mean trends (Refer to Fig 6). Six out of eight dogs showed the largest decrements in high temperature and high humidity and the least decrement in low temperature and low humidity.

Fig 5. Threshold (log proportion of vapor saturation) for each environmental condition and odor.

Fig 5

Error bars show bootstrap estimated 95% confidence intervals.

Table 5. Threshold difference for each environment condition compared to standard condition.

Positive t-ratio indicates poorer sensitivity compared to standard.

Conditions High Temp High Humid vs. Standard High Temp Low Humid vs. Standard Low Temp High Humid vs. Standard Low Temp Low Humid vs. Standard
Odor: t-ratio p t-ratio p t-ratio p t-ratio p
AN 2.21 0.05 3.64 <0.001 1.84 0.09 -0.12 0.90
C-4 -0.03 0.73 2.02 0.06 3.71 <0.01 2.15 0.06
SP 4.42 <0.01 2.73 0.02 3.05 <0.01 2.21 0.03
TNT 0.39 0.78 0.40 0.78 -0.27 0.78 -1.02 0.78

Fig 6. Individual threshold difference for each environment condition compared to standard condition for dogs detecting SP.

Fig 6

Average threshold between both testing sessions for each condition.

Table 5 shows the post hoc results for each energetic material comparing detection threshold under each extreme condition to the standard condition. Positive t-ratio values indicate higher (poorer) detection threshold in the extreme environment compared to standard. Detection threshold for AN was poorer (compared to standard) at high temperature high humidity and high temperature low humidity. Comparisons for low temperature conditions did not reach statistical significance. C4 had poorer detection limits in low temperature high humidity and a trend (p = 0.06) for poorer detection at high temperature low humidity and low temperature low humidity. SP had poorer detection in all conditions, with the poorest detection limit in high temperature high humidity. TNT did not reach statistical significance for any comparison.

Subcutaneous temperature

Fig 7 shows the subcutaneous temperature across the first 20 minutes of the session. A cutoff time of 20 minutes was selected because nearly all sessions lasted this long. In the hot and standard conditions, subcutaneous temperature increased as dogs completed the threshold test. During the cold conditions, temperature decreased across sessions to below biologically plausible values, indicating the cutaneous sensor was susceptible to the room temperature conditions. Therefore, analyses were limited to standard and high temperature conditions.

Fig 7. Implanted skin temperature during testing.

Fig 7

Error bars show the 95% confidence interval.

First, a linear mixed effect model analysis of differences in mean subcutaneous temperatures between the three conditions indicated there was a significant difference in mean temperature between conditions (X2 = 242, df = 2, p<0.001). Post hoc tests indicate that subcutaneous temperature was highest in High Temperature High Humidity in comparison to High Temperature Low Humidity (p<0.001) and Standard conditions (p<0.001), indicating an important impact of humidity between the two high temperature conditions at the same temperature. Further, High Temperature Low Humidity also led to higher subcutaneous temperatures compared to standard (p<0.001).

To evaluate the impact of mean subcutaneous temperature on threshold, a linear mixed effect model was fit in which threshold was predicted by odor (energetic), mean subcutaneous temperature and their interaction. There was a significant interaction between energetic and mean temperature (X2 = 8.64, df = 3, p = 0.03). To evaluate this interaction, a regression model was fit for each energetic. There was a significant positive relationship between mean subcutaneous temperature and threshold from SP (X2 = 58.12, df = 1, p<0.001) and AN (X2 = 7.16,df = 1, p = 0.007), but not for TNT or C4 (p>0.05; see Fig 8). This indicates that higher mean subcutaneous temperature was associated with lower sensitivity (poorer threshold) for SP and AN.

Fig 8. Threshold relation to mean subcutaneous temperature.

Fig 8

Line shows the best fit regression.

Respiratory effort

Dogs showed increased RE score across the session during standard and hot conditions (high and low humidity) but stable scores in the low temperature conditions (see Fig 9). Linear mixed effect model shows that RE differed between the environmental conditions (X2 = 948, df = 4, p<0.001). Post hoc tests indicate that there was a significant difference in RE score between all conditions except for the two low temperature conditions (t = 0.38, p = 0.99).

Fig 9. Respiratory effort ratings during the threshold assessments.

Fig 9

Error bars show the boot strap estimated 95% confidence intervals.

To evaluate whether RE was related to detection threshold, the log transformed threshold was predicted by odor (energetic), RE score and their interaction. There was a significant interaction (X2 = 17.45, df = 3, p<0.001) between RE and odor/energetic on threshold. Conducting separate analysis for each energetic indicated that increased RE led to higher threshold for AN (X2 = 4.71, df = 1, p<0.03), but for lower threshold for C4 (X2 = 7.28, df = 1, p<0.01; see Fig 10). No significant effect of RE was found for SP (X2 = 1.86, df = 1, p = 0.17) and TNT (X2 = 2.67, df = 1, p = 0.1).

Fig 10. Relationship between mean RE and log threshold.

Fig 10

Latency

Latency was scored as the time between the olfactometer initializing the trial with the start tone and the first nose poke into the odor port. Latency to initialize a trial varied by environmental condition (X2 = 30.1, df = 4, p<0.001). Fig 11 and post-hoc tests shows the latency was highest in the two high temperature conditions but was similar in the low temperature and standard conditions.

Fig 11. Latency to sample odor port by environmental condition.

Fig 11

Error bars show the 95% bootstrap estimated confidence intervals.

A mixed effect model was fit in which log threshold was predicted by the mean session latency, energetic and their interaction. There was no significant interaction (X2 = 1.85, df = 3, p = 0.60), but there was a main effect of energetic (X2 = 2,762, df = 3, p<0.001) and latency (X2 = 14.26, df = 1, p<0.001). Fig 12 shows there was a positive relationship such that increases in latency (or delay in initiating a trial) was associated with higher (or poorer) thresholds. Thus, when dogs quickly approached the odor port at the trial start, detection sensitivity was higher.

Fig 12. Relationship between mean latency to initiate a trial in a session with overall threshold.

Fig 12

Discussion

In this study we established differences in odor sensitivity to varying energetic materials and changes in sensitivity due to extreme environmental conditions. Detection sensitivity limits were lowest (optimal) for canines for each odor at standard temperature and humidity conditions (22°C and 50% RH). Canines did not perform statistically better in any extreme environmental condition in comparison to standard conditions. Decrements in detection limits, were observed in each target odor for at least one of the extreme environmental conditions. High temperature and high humidity caused decrements in performance for SP and AN. High temperature low humidity caused decrements for AN, SP and a trend for C4. Low temperature high humidity led to decrements for C4 and SP. Low temperature low humidity only caused a decrement for SP.

No statistically significant effects between conditions were observed for TNT. This may reflect a restriction of range effect, dogs’ threshold for TNT was near the highest odor concentration that could be presented by the air dilution olfactometer, even under standard conditions. Further, it was seen that the mean threshold of TNT across all 8 dogs was between the highest concentration and the second concentration presented to the dogs. Therefore there was a restriction in the decrement that could be observed because dogs’ sensitivity limits were already at or near the highest concentrations that could be presented by the air dilution olfactometer.

Another unexpected finding is that dogs performed remarkably well in the high temperature and high humidity condition for C4. This appears to be driven largely by two dogs that started in the cold conditions and worked up to the hot conditions, whereas dogs that started in the hot conditions showed more of the expected decrement. C4 testing happened to coincide with ambient low humidity conditions in the region that caused more environmental variation when opening and closing the chamber to enter and begin testing. Retrospective analysis of the room conditions showed that although sessions started at the correct initial temperature and humidity conditions (high temperature and humidity), the temperature and relative humidity levels fell below expected levels after opening the chamber and starting testing in 7 of 16 testing sessions for C4, which did not occur for the other energetics (AN, SP, TNT). This may in part have led to the better than expected performance in this condition. In a follow-up study, Kane et al., (2024) demonstrated that dogs do show a performance decrement in the high temperature and high humidity condition for C4, highlighting that the observed drop in temperature may have been the reason for the lack of effect observed here [36].

There were interesting relationships in the measured physiological data and detection sensitivity. Subcutaneous temperature measurements were highest in the hot and humid conditions in comparison to all other conditions; this indicates heat stress is at greatest risk in these environmental conditions. Dogs’ mean subcutaneous temperature predicted detection performance for SP and AN, indicating that detection performance declined with greater increases in temperature. For example, for SP, a 1°C increase in subcutaneous temperature was associated with a 0.19 change in log threshold, reflecting a potentially important impact of canine subcutaneous temperature on performance. In the future it would be worth exploring how subcutaneous temperature and rectal, or core body temperature were related in this type of detection task study.

Respiratory effort (RE) also increased most in high temperature and high humidity conditions, followed by high temperature low humidity and standard conditions. RE was little impacted in the cold conditions. There is also some conflicting evidence of the effect of RE on threshold, such that overall, increases did lead to poorer detection limits for AN, which is to be expected based on prior work showing that respiratory effort can lead to poorer detection [25]. Notably, however, there was an opposite effect for C4. As RE scores increased from 1 to 6, it appears that there was a 1 log reduction in threshold. To better understand this discrepancy, further data is necessary.

Latency to initialize a trial also increased in both high temperature conditions compared to standard and cold conditions. Furthermore, longer latency to initiate a trial was associated with poorer detection thresholds. This suggests that lags in the dogs working behavior could be an important early cue that detection sensitivity may be poorer, or at least non-optimal. Latency to begin a search task, however, is not a common research topic and the field would benefit substantially from examining this in an operational context. A useful follow up experiment would evaluate to what degree the latency effect leads to more global decrements in canine performance (such as with obedience vs. a detection task). This would help elucidate whether the latency effect reflects a more generalized behavioral decrement or an olfactory specific decrement.

There are important considerations for the present study. First, odor concentration was maintained and controlled across environmental conditions with an air dilution olfactometer.

In operational searches, the odor source itself will be exposed to the extreme conditions and vary odor availability. This may change the effect observed such that although canine detection limits may change in high temperature and high humidity conditions, changes in odor availability may compensate minimizing overall performance decrements. Similarly, in cold conditions, even if canine detection capability remains similar, lower odor availability may change performance. Thus, additional evaluation in which odor source is allowed to change with environmental conditions is an important next step and this work is underway. As a follow up study to the present work the relationship between odor availability, environmental condition and canine threshold was examined Kane et al., (2024). Additionally this study examined how effects of environment can be mitigated with acclimatization training in adverse environmental conditions [36].

Additionally, it should be noted that dogs were removed from the testing area based on our criterion for welfare considerations, or testing reached 40 minutes. Overall, across all dogs in all conditions and odors, dogs were removed early from 57 sessions out of 320 total sessions. However, performance prior to removal was poor, with a mean accuracy of 47%, indicating performance prior to early termination was below that expected by chance alone (50%). Importantly, only one dog was removed from one session during standard conditions, while the remaining 56 early termination sessions occurred during testing in extreme environmental conditions. Thus, early removal from testing sessions highlights the inability of the dogs to perform the working task better than chance, due to influences from the environmental conditions.

Conclusion

Overall, this research demonstrates important considerations for working dogs. Environmental conditions had a negative impact on odor sensitivity for each odor tested in one or more of the conditions. This indicates that working dogs may face decrements in performance when working in varying environmental conditions. Lastly, physiological changes and changes in latency are indicators of possible decrements in odor sensitivity. Results do suggest that subcutaneous temperature, latency, and environmental conditions can be important predictors of changes in canine detection threshold (sensitivity) limits.

Supporting information

S1 Table. A RE scale rating system described by DeChant et al. (submitted) was used to determine the respiration effort of each participant during the five second clip for each five-minute interval.

Scoring for this scale ranges from 0 to 10. Zero being defined as resting and breathing is an involuntary process, and ten being very heavy breathing and the chest and abdomen are expanding and collapsing violently.

(DOCX)

pone.0306817.s001.docx (22.5KB, docx)
S1 Data. Raw data across all sessions compiled into one file.

(CSV)

pone.0306817.s002.csv (2.1MB, csv)
S2 Data. Raw temperature data from each session.

(CSV)

pone.0306817.s003.csv (27.6KB, csv)
S3 Data

(XLSX)

pone.0306817.s004.xlsx (100.2KB, xlsx)
S1 Code. Code in R used for all analyses and graphs presented.

(CSV)

pone.0306817.s005.qmd (9.6KB, qmd)

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank Dr. Edgar O. Aviles-Rosa and Avery Bramlett for their help with data collection and helping with the care of the dogs. Further, we would like to thank the undergraduate students at the canine olfaction lab for providing care for the dogs involved in this project. Lastly, we would like to acknowledge Dr. Dillon Huff for providing helpful background information and samples used in this project.

Data Availability

All data is fully available without restriction. This is provided in supplemental information. We have provided all temperature data, olfactometer data, and the R code used for analysis. We have also provided the behavioral coding results for PRE.

Funding Statement

This research was made possible through funding provided by the DoD Army Research Office under Contract No. W911NF2120124. https://www.arl.army.mil/who-we-are/aro/. SAK’s work was supported by the National Science Foundation Graduate Research Fellowship Program (DGE 2140745).https://www.nsfgrfp.org/. This funder played no role in the study design, data collection, analysis, preparation or decision to publish this manuscript.

References

  • 1.Guest C. Canine Olfactory Detection of Human Disease. In: Grassberger M, Sherman RA, Gileva OS, Kim CMH, Mumcuoglu KY, editors. Biotherapy—History, Principles and Practice: A Practical Guide to the Diagnosis and Treatment of Disease using Living Organisms, Dordrecht: Springer Netherlands; 2013, p. 285–302. [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Jendrny P, Schulz C, Twele F, Meller S, von Köckritz-Blickwede M, Osterhaus ADME, et al. Scent dog identification of samples from COVID-19 patients—a pilot study. BMC Infectious Diseases 2020;20:536. doi: 10.1186/s12879-020-05281-3 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Gottwald T, Poole G, McCollum T, Hall D, Hartung J, Bai J, et al. Canine olfactory detection of a vectored phytobacterial pathogen, Liberibacter asiaticus, and integration with disease control. PNAS 2020;117:3492–501. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1914296117 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Greatbatch I, Gosling RJ, Allen S. Quantifying Search Dog Effectiveness in a Terrestrial Search and Rescue Environment. Wilderness & Environmental Medicine 2015;26:327–34. doi: 10.1016/j.wem.2015.02.009 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Gordon RT, Schatz CB, Myers LJ, Kosty M, Gonczy C, Kroener J, et al. The Use of Canines in the Detection of Human Cancers. The Journal of Alternative and Complementary Medicine 2008;14:61–7. doi: 10.1089/acm.2006.6408 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.McCulloch M, Jezierski T, Broffman M, Hubbard A, Turner K, Janecki T. Diagnostic Accuracy of Canine Scent Detection in Early- and Late-Stage Lung and Breast Cancers. Integr Cancer Ther 2006;5:30–9. doi: 10.1177/1534735405285096 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Moser E, McCulloch M. Canine scent detection of human cancers: A review of methods and accuracy. Journal of Veterinary Behavior 2010;5:145–52. doi: 10.1016/j.jveb.2010.01.002 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Shellman Francis V, Holness HK, Furton KG. The Ability of Narcotic Detection Canines to Detect Illegal Synthetic Cathinones (Bath Salts). Front Vet Sci 2019;6. doi: 10.3389/fvets.2019.00098 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Furton KG, Myers LJ. The scientific foundation and efficacy of the use of canines as chemical detectors for explosives1Invited paper for the special issue of Talanta ‘Methods for Explosive Analysis and Detection’.1. Talanta 2001;54:487–500. doi: 10.1016/S0039-9140(00)00546-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Gazit I, Lavner Y, Bloch G, Azulai O, Goldblatt A, Terkel J. A simple system for the remote detection and analysis of sniffing in explosives detection dogs. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers 2003;35:82–9. doi: 10.3758/bf03195499 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Lazarowski L, Dorman DC. Explosives detection by military working dogs: Olfactory generalization from components to mixtures. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 2014;151:84–93. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2013.11.010 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Harper RJ, Furton KG. Chapter 13—Biological Detection of Explosives. In: Yinon J, editor. Counterterrorist Detection Techniques of Explosives, Amsterdam: Elsevier Science B.V.; 2007, p. 395–431. [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Waggoner P, Lazarowski L, Hutchings B, Angle C, Porritt F. Effects of learning an increasing number of odors on olfactory learning, memory and generalization in detection dogs. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 2022;247:105568. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2022.105568 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Kurz ME, Billard M, Rettig M, Augustiniak J, Lange J, Larsen M, et al. Evaluation of Canines for Accelerant Detection at Fire Scenes. J Forensic Sci 1994;39:1528–36. doi: 10.1520/JFS13740J [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Tindall R, Lothridge K. An Evaluation of 42 Accelerant Detection Canine Teams. J Forensic Sci 1995;40:561–4. doi: 10.1520/JFS13825J [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Waggoner LP, Johnston JM, Williams M, Jackson J, Jones MH, Boussom T, et al. Canine olfactory sensitivity to cocaine hydrochloride and methyl benzoate. Chemistry- and Biology-Based Technologies for Contraband Detection, vol. 2937, International Society for Optics and Photonics; 1997, p. 216–26. doi: 10.1117/12.266775 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Walker DB, Walker JC, Cavnar PJ, Taylor JL, Pickel DH, Hall SB, et al. Naturalistic quantification of canine olfactory sensitivity. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 2006;97:241–54. [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Ewing RG, Waltman MJ, Atkinson DA, Grate JW, Hotchkiss PJ. The vapor pressures of explosives. TrAC Trends in Analytical Chemistry 2013;42:35–48. doi: 10.1016/j.trac.2012.09.010 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Cablk ME, Heaton JS. Accuracy and Reliability of Dogs in Surveying for Desert Tortoise (gopherus Agassizii). Ecological Applications 2006;16:1926–35. doi: 10.1890/1051-0761(2006)016[1926:aarodi]2.0.co;2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Brustkern M, Thompson R, Lawhon S, Good K, Bunker P, Prada-Tiedemann PA, et al. Effect of Rapid Changes in Environmental Conditions on Canine Detection of Methyl Benzoate—ScienceDirect. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 2023. [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Diverio S, Barbato O, Cavallina R, Guelfi G, Iaboni M, Zasso R, et al. A simulated avalanche search and rescue mission induces temporary physiological and behavioural changes in military dogs. Physiology & Behavior 2016;163:193–202. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2016.05.006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Robbins PJ, Ramos MT, Zanghi BM, Otto CM. Environmental and Physiological Factors Associated With Stamina in Dogs Exercising in High Ambient Temperatures. Frontiers in Veterinary Science 2017;4. doi: 10.3389/fvets.2017.00144 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Hall EJ, Carter AJ, Bradbury J, Barfield D, O’Neill DG. Proposing the VetCompass clinical grading tool for heat-related illness in dogs. Sci Rep 2021;11:6828. doi: 10.1038/s41598-021-86235-w [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Lopedote M, Valentini S, Musella V, Vilar JM, Spinella G. Changes in Pulse Rate, Respiratory Rate and Rectal Temperature in Working Dogs before and after Three Different Field Trials. Animals 2020;10:733. doi: 10.3390/ani10040733 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Gazit I, Terkel J. Explosives detection by sniffer dogs following strenuous physical activity. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 2003;81:149–61. doi: 10.1016/S0168-1591(02)00274-5 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.O’Brien C, Berglund LG. Predicting recovery from exertional heat strain in military working dogs. Journal of Thermal Biology 2018;76:45–51. doi: 10.1016/j.jtherbio.2018.07.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Geor RJ, McCutcheon LJ. Thermoregulatory Adaptations Associated with Training and Heat Acclimation. Veterinary Clinics of North America: Equine Practice 1998;14:97–120. doi: 10.1016/s0749-0739(17)30214-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.McLean I, Sargisson R, Mansfield I. Detection of Landmines by Dogs: Environmental and Behavioural Determinants. Global CWD Repository 2005.
  • 29.Jenkins EK, DeChant MT, Perry EB. When the Nose Doesn’t Know: Canine Olfactory Function Associated With Health, Management, and Potential Links to Microbiota. Front Vet Sci 2018;5. doi: 10.3389/fvets.2018.00056 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Menchetti L, Iaboni M, Santoro MM, Guelfi G, Diverio S. How Do Avalanche Dogs (and Their Handlers) Cope with Physical Exercise? Heart Rate Changes during Endurance in a Snowy Environment. Animals 2022;12:168. doi: 10.3390/ani12020168 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Gould RR, Latosuo E. Handler perceptions of optimal scenting conditions for avalanche rescue dogs, Anchorage, Alaska: 2012.
  • 32.Hoskinson C, McCain S, Allender MC. Comparison of body temperature readings between an implantable microchip and a cloacal probe in lorikeets (Trichoglossus haematodus sp.). Zoo Biology 2014;33:452–4. doi: 10.1002/zoo.21174 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Friard O, Gamba M. BORIS: a free, versatile open-source event-logging software for video/audio coding and live observations. Methods in Ecology and Evolution 2016;7:1325–30. doi: 10.1111/2041-210X.12584 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Aviles-Rosa EO, Gallegos S, Tiedeman P, Hall NJ. An Automated Canine Line-Up for Detection Dog Research. Frontiers in Veterinary Science 2021:1477. doi: 10.3389/fvets.2021.775381 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Fox J, Weisberg S. An R Companion to Applied Regression. SAGE Publications; 2018. [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Kane SA, Fernandez LS, Huff DE, Prada-Tiedemann PA, Hall NJ. Canine detection of explosives under adverse environmental conditions with and without acclimation training. PLOS ONE 2024;19:e0297538. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0297538 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Decision Letter 0

Sankarganesh Devaraj

28 Nov 2023

PONE-D-23-31665Environmental effects on explosive detection threshold of domestic dogsPLOS ONE

Dear Dr. Kane,

Thank you for submitting your manuscript to PLOS ONE. After careful consideration, we feel that it has merit but does not fully meet PLOS ONE’s publication criteria as it currently stands. Therefore, we invite you to submit a revised version of the manuscript that addresses the points raised during the review process.

==============================

As suggested by the reviewers, I am inviting you to revise the manuscript and submit the revised version for consideration.

Please ensure that your decision is justified on PLOS ONE’s publication criteria and not, for example, on novelty or perceived impact.

For Lab, Study and Registered Report Protocols: These article types are not expected to include results but may include pilot data. 

==============================

Please submit your revised manuscript by Jan 12 2024 11:59PM. If you will need more time than this to complete your revisions, please reply to this message or contact the journal office at plosone@plos.org. When you're ready to submit your revision, log on to https://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/ and select the 'Submissions Needing Revision' folder to locate your manuscript file.

Please include the following items when submitting your revised manuscript:

  • A rebuttal letter that responds to each point raised by the academic editor and reviewer(s). You should upload this letter as a separate file labeled 'Response to Reviewers'.

  • A marked-up copy of your manuscript that highlights changes made to the original version. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Revised Manuscript with Track Changes'.

  • An unmarked version of your revised paper without tracked changes. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Manuscript'.

If you would like to make changes to your financial disclosure, please include your updated statement in your cover letter. Guidelines for resubmitting your figure files are available below the reviewer comments at the end of this letter.

If applicable, we recommend that you deposit your laboratory protocols in protocols.io to enhance the reproducibility of your results. Protocols.io assigns your protocol its own identifier (DOI) so that it can be cited independently in the future. For instructions see: https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/submission-guidelines#loc-laboratory-protocols. Additionally, PLOS ONE offers an option for publishing peer-reviewed Lab Protocol articles, which describe protocols hosted on protocols.io. Read more information on sharing protocols at https://plos.org/protocols?utm_medium=editorial-email&utm_source=authorletters&utm_campaign=protocols.

We look forward to receiving your revised manuscript.

Kind regards,

Sankarganesh Devaraj

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Journal requirements:

1. When submitting your revision, we need you to address these additional requirements.

Please ensure that your manuscript meets PLOS ONE's style requirements, including those for file naming. The PLOS ONE style templates can be found at 

https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=wjVg/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_main_body.pdf and 

https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=ba62/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_title_authors_affiliations.pdf.

2. Please include captions for your Supporting Information files at the end of your manuscript, and update any in-text citations to match accordingly. Please see our Supporting Information guidelines for more information: http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/supporting-information. 

3. We are unable to open your Supporting Information file [Analysis_Plos1_ptI .qmd]. Please kindly revise as necessary and re-upload.

4. PLOS ONE now requires that authors provide the original uncropped and unadjusted images underlying all blot or gel results reported in a submission’s figures or Supporting Information files. This policy and the journal’s other requirements for blot/gel reporting and figure preparation are described in detail at https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/figures#loc-blot-and-gel-reporting-requirements and https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/figures#loc-preparing-figures-from-image-files. When you submit your revised manuscript, please ensure that your figures adhere fully to these guidelines and provide the original underlying images for all blot or gel data reported in your submission. See the following link for instructions on providing the original image data: https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/figures#loc-original-images-for-blots-and-gels. 

In your cover letter, please note whether your blot/gel image data are in Supporting Information or posted at a public data repository, provide the repository URL if relevant, and provide specific details as to which raw blot/gel images, if any, are not available. Email us at plosone@plos.org if you have any questions.

5. Please review your reference list to ensure that it is complete and correct. If you have cited papers that have been retracted, please include the rationale for doing so in the manuscript text, or remove these references and replace them with relevant current references. Any changes to the reference list should be mentioned in the rebuttal letter that accompanies your revised manuscript. If you need to cite a retracted article, indicate the article’s retracted status in the References list and also include a citation and full reference for the retraction notice.

[Note: HTML markup is below. Please do not edit.]

Reviewers' comments:

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Author

1. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

********** 

2. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

********** 

3. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

********** 

4. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

********** 

5. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #1: I find this paper as an interesting publication well written and presenting well-performed experiments. The methods are well described and are adequate to the subject. The number of animals used could be bigger, however, but in that kind of study usually a similar or even smaller amount of animals used to be used. The experimental group consisting of two breeds also seems to be even, which may be important in the context of the type of fur and sensitivity to environmental factors tested in this experiment. Results are clearly presented and come from the well-planned and performed experiment. The discussion is well led and the authors present interesting and important presenting also considerations on the impact of changing environmental conditions on behaviour, the "availability" of odors and not only the ability of dogs as detectors.

Below I attaching some doubts, questions and suggestions to the text and I will be grateful to the authors for their responses to the following comments.

Line 116 118 Interestingly, it should be noted that previous work has seen that dogs are able to detect target odors with no decline in performance while working in a variety of extreme 1conditions [30–32]

From other side there are papers presenting opposite opinion. Kokocińska et al reported numerous of papers indicating that humidity has been found to be an important factor influencing olfactory skills in dogs. In this case increased humidity can even improve canine olfactory skills, as if mentioned in a few papers cited in mentioned review of Kokocińka

Line 140-148 Due to greater heat stress, there is a potential that detection dogs may experience a decrease in detection accuracy while working in hot and humid environments. It has been found that landmine detection dogs had a significant decrease in detection accuracy after the working environment experienced heavy rain, ultimately increasing the humidity [48]. However, it should also be considered that the heavy rain potentially disrupted the soil containing the odor by washing the odorant away. But further data in this study showed that dogs had a decrease in detection accuracy early in the morning when the humidity was the highest and had an increase in detection success as humidity decreased throughout the day [48]. This supports the idea that an increase in humidity hinders an EDD’s ability to perform detection tasks

In my opinion, it would be worth to mention another opinion in this matter. In the mentioned review authors reported that: With regard to the effect of environmental conditions, humidity has been found to be an important factor in improving olfactory skills in dogs, probably due to improved nasal humidity and odorant trapping [Jenkins, E.K.; DeChant, M.T.; Perry, E.B. When the Nose Doesn’t Know: Canine Olfactory Function Associated with Health, Management, and Potential Links to Microbiota. Front. Veter. Sci. 2018, 5, 56.]

]. Moreover, according to Gutzwiller [Gutzwiller, K.J. Minimizing Dog-Induced Biases in Game Bird Research. Wildl. Soc. Bull. (1973–2006) 1990, 18, 351–356. [Google Scholar]

], increased humidity could be responsible for increased odor intensity, positively influencing the tracking efficiency of dogs. This phenomenon is also thought to be true in the context of semiochemical substance detection [Majumder, S.S.; Bhadra, A. When Love Is in the Air: Understanding Why Dogs Tend to Mate when It Rains. PLoS ONE 2015, 10, e0143501. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed][Green Version]

].

If authors do not agree with this thesis it would be worth discussing it anyway.

Line 182 All dogs were previously eliminated from working dog program.

What was the reason for elimination of those dogs from the working dog program?

Line 209 HVAC

please expand the abbreviation. I understand that it is Heating, Ventilation, Air Conditioning, but all abbreviations in the text supposed to be explained.

Line 232 Due to the variation of volatility of each material, flow rate used for dilution was dependent on odor

Can you explain if it was calculated somehow to propose that kind of dilution or maybe it was just established by the authors.

Line 279 a photo presenting dogs working is maybe not necessary but could be a useful addition that allows the reader to better understand the course of the experiment, not so much to imagine it

I have also a question about the latency. If I understand well, dogs in suboptimal conditions start working with lags and have a poorer results. Could it be connected with the fact that dogs sometimes don’t want to work ( have a worse day for work). Bad environmental conditions could influence the general involvement of the dogs and that could influence the detection results. I mean all other requested tasks could also be realized poorer ( obedience aspects for example). What do you think about this- could it be the reason for the worse final result of work?

Reviewer #2: Line 89: Suggest replace “sensitivity” with “detectability”. Vapor pressure does not affect olfactory sensitivity it affects detection. Also line 109, 118-119, 175 and 225 as well as additional occasions. For contrast, the term “sensitivity” used appropriately in line 324.

Line 95: Does “brand” really change vapor pressure of energetic material? Brand may change the chemicals present, but not the vapor pressure of the chemical.

Lines 96-100: This discussion needs some further background provided for the reader that is not well versed about energetic materials. First, TNT and DNT should be spelled out upon this or prior use of acronym as well as other acronyms, although in case of RDX, suggest may put chemical name in parentheses vs. typical way acronym is introduced. I think it should be noted that DNT is a constituent of the vapor from TNT. I don’t follow well the phrase “This fluctuation is also seen with RDX (a component of C4)” what fluctuation – between RDX and other energetic materials – way written could be construed as fluctuation in vapor pressure of RDX.

Line 104: Explosives contain other chemicals often due to purposeful addition of other materials to render them more user friendly in addition to contaminants and degradation products. Such purposeful additions are more important to detection by dogs because they are consistent.

Line 107: Table title references vapor pressure of explosive materials; while this is okay by itself, the term “energetic materials” has been used earlier in manuscript and I think that keeping the distinction between energetic chemicals and an explosive material produced using this chemical would be helpful throughout discussion to this point in manuscript. Indeed, of course, TNT is TNT and AN is not energetic in and of itself. Perhaps use of some explanation like “explosive materials are composed of energetic chemicals or some material in combination are energetic compounds. Also, note in abstract (line 30-31, AN is listed as an “energetic” material. I think some clearer description is warranted here for readers that do not have a background regarding energetic chemicals and explosive materials. Also line 397 and 400.

Line 131: Term “manipulate” here seems awkward.

Line 153: Grammar change – “…in which working dogs are tasked to search”

Line 153: Throughout manuscript there is a style issue of using a lot of transitional “further”, furthermore. For example in this line “Further, it has also been shown…” is wordy and not efficient. This could be written as “Divero et al. reported search and rescue dogs acclimated to working in -8.5C to -10.4C completing search tasks without signs of stress of fatigue [50].”

Line 157: “maybe” should be “may be”.

Line 158: suggest change “establish” to “investigate”.

Line 161: The phrase “It should be noted” and similar is overused throughout manuscript. If it is a reference to relevant previous studies, then the authors including it supposes it should be noted. In this line, perhaps consider “However, previous work found that dogs working in cold and standard conditions had the lowest threshold for methyl benzoate detection.” Also, this sentence does not contain a reference – was this previous unpublished work of the authors?

Line 252-254, perhaps noted elsewhere and can be ascertained by the description of olfactometer function , but suggest add that on non-odor trials the final air flow was the same as the total air flow of the diluted explosive (odor) trials.

Line 311. I do not believe that acronym PRE (assume “perceived respiratory effort” was defined earlier

Lines 348 – 357: Not clear about non-odor, blank, trials or were there NO non-odor (diluent only) trials during testing?

Lines 423~427 and table 7: Recommend that t-ratio comparison be described and explained in text.

Lines 447 – 467: It would have been useful to have a comparison of the sensor body temperature and that from another measurement (e.g., rectal) as a way to evaluate the sensor performance.

Line 545: Term “search performance” used for, what I believe is the first time. Suggest not using this term, particularly for first time in manuscript here. Environmental impact on “search performance” suggests searching and task was a fixed-sampling position activity that may not be considered a “search performance”.

Lines 610-616: This is an interesting finding, but it would be made more useful to reader if the numbers of standard sessions from which dogs were removed was noted. Perhaps reader is to assume these were all in the more extreme environmental conditions.

Lines 620 – 629: Suggest some discussion by authors about acclimation of dogs to environmental conditions and what impact it may have on changes seen in sensitivity due to environment in present experiment.

********** 

6. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #1: No

Reviewer #2: Yes: Paul Waggoner

**********

[NOTE: If reviewer comments were submitted as an attachment file, they will be attached to this email and accessible via the submission site. Please log into your account, locate the manuscript record, and check for the action link "View Attachments". If this link does not appear, there are no attachment files.]

While revising your submission, please upload your figure files to the Preflight Analysis and Conversion Engine (PACE) digital diagnostic tool, https://pacev2.apexcovantage.com/. PACE helps ensure that figures meet PLOS requirements. To use PACE, you must first register as a user. Registration is free. Then, login and navigate to the UPLOAD tab, where you will find detailed instructions on how to use the tool. If you encounter any issues or have any questions when using PACE, please email PLOS at figures@plos.org. Please note that Supporting Information files do not need this step.

PLoS One. 2024 Sep 25;19(9):e0306817. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0306817.r002

Author response to Decision Letter 0


18 Apr 2024

Dear Reviewers,

Thank you for your comments on our manuscript “Environmental effects on explosive detection threshold of domestic dogs.” Please see our responses below.

Reviewer #1:

I find this paper as an interesting publication well written and presenting well-performed experiments. The methods are well described and are adequate to the subject. The number of animals used could be bigger, however, but in that kind of study usually a similar or even smaller amount of animals used to be used. The experimental group consisting of two breeds also seems to be even, which may be important in the context of the type of fur and sensitivity to environmental factors tested in this experiment. Results are clearly presented and come from the well-planned and performed experiment. The discussion is well led and the authors present interesting and important presenting also considerations on the impact of changing environmental conditions on behaviour, the "availability" of odors and not only the ability of dogs as detectors.

Thank you for your thoughtful comments.

Below I attaching some doubts, questions and suggestions to the text and I will be grateful to the authors for their responses to the following comments.

Line 116 118 Interestingly, it should be noted that previous work has seen that dogs are able to detect target odors with no decline in performance while working in a variety of extreme 1conditions [30–32]

From other side there are papers presenting opposite opinion. Kokocińska et al reported numerous of papers indicating that humidity has been found to be an important factor influencing olfactory skills in dogs. In this case increased humidity can even improve canine olfactory skills, as if mentioned in a few papers cited in mentioned review of Kokocińka

Thank you for your comment, we have addressed this oversight in line 153.

Line 140-148 Due to greater heat stress, there is a potential that detection dogs may experience a decrease in detection accuracy while working in hot and humid environments. It has been found that landmine detection dogs had a significant decrease in detection accuracy after the working environment experienced heavy rain, ultimately increasing the humidity [48]. However, it should also be considered that the heavy rain potentially disrupted the soil containing the odor by washing the odorant away. But further data in this study showed that dogs had a decrease in detection accuracy early in the morning when the humidity was the highest and had an increase in detection success as humidity decreased throughout the day [48]. This supports the idea that an increase in humidity hinders an EDD’s ability to perform detection tasks

In my opinion, it would be worth to mention another opinion in this matter. In the mentioned review authors reported that: With regard to the effect of environmental conditions, humidity has been found to be an important factor in improving olfactory skills in dogs, probably due to improved nasal humidity and odorant trapping [Jenkins, E.K.; DeChant, M.T.; Perry, E.B. When the Nose Doesn’t Know: Canine Olfactory Function Associated with Health, Management, and Potential Links to Microbiota. Front. Veter. Sci. 2018, 5, 56.]

]. Moreover, according to Gutzwiller [Gutzwiller, K.J. Minimizing Dog-Induced Biases in Game Bird Research. Wildl. Soc. Bull. (1973–2006) 1990, 18, 351–356. [Google Scholar]

], increased humidity could be responsible for increased odor intensity, positively influencing the tracking efficiency of dogs. This phenomenon is also thought to be true in the context of semiochemical substance detection [Majumder, S.S.; Bhadra, A. When Love Is in the Air: Understanding Why Dogs Tend to Mate when It Rains. PLoS ONE 2015, 10, e0143501. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed][Green Version]

].

If authors do not agree with this thesis it would be worth discussing it anyway.

Thank you for providing additional citations to add nuance to our arguments. Addressed in lines 128-131, and noted below:

This supports the idea that an increases in humidity hinder an EDD’s ability to perform detection tasks. However, a literature review by Jenkins et al., 2018 noted that a moist environment is crucial for olfactory perception, indicating the humidity may aid in olfaction [38]. Previous work with hunting canines has supports that increased humidity impacts tracking time [39].

Line 182 All dogs were previously eliminated from working dog program.

What was the reason for elimination of those dogs from the working dog program?

Description added :

“Dogs varied in their reasons for removal from their prior working program including preference for food vs. toy rewards, distractibility or unknown reasons.”

Line 209 HVAC please expand the abbreviation. I understand that it is Heating, Ventilation, Air Conditioning, but all abbreviations in the text supposed to be explained.

Good catch, acronym explained 267-268.

Line 232 Due to the variation of volatility of each material, flow rate used for dilution was dependent on odor. Can you explain if it was calculated somehow to propose that kind of dilution or maybe it was just established by the authors.

Details on flow rate determination have been added to the manuscript:

“The flow rate determined the highest range of concentration presented. During initial training, if dogs struggled to detect the 0.01 dilutions >85% accuracy, the total flow was set to 3 SLPM to allow for a higher range of concentrations.”

Line 279 a photo presenting dogs working is maybe not necessary but could be a useful addition that allows the reader to better understand the course of the experiment, not so much to imagine it

Photo included in supplemental materials. Text included in line 709:

A photo of Rocket holding a freeze-nose hold alert in the olfactometer is included in supplemental materials. He is wearing all sensors mentioned above.

I have also a question about the latency. If I understand well, dogs in suboptimal conditions start working with lags and have a poorer results. Could it be connected with the fact that dogs sometimes don’t want to work ( have a worse day for work). Bad environmental conditions could influence the general involvement of the dogs and that could influence the detection results. I mean all other requested tasks could also be realized poorer ( obedience aspects for example). What do you think about this- could it be the reason for the worse final result of work?

Yes, we agree completely. We have edited the discussion to hopefully clarify that this latency effect may suggest a generalized behavioral decrement and suggest future work looking at other behaviors. We have added the following:

“A useful follow up experiment would evaluate to what degree the latency effect leads to more global decrements in canine performance (such as with obedience vs. a detection task). This would help elucidate whether the latency effect reflects a more generalized behavioral decrement or an olfactory specific decrement.”

Reviewer #2:

Line 89: Suggest replace “sensitivity” with “detectability”. Vapor pressure does not affect olfactory sensitivity it affects detection. Also line 109, 118-119, 175 and 225 as well as additional occasions. For contrast, the term “sensitivity” used appropriately in line 324.

Thank you, this was fixed.

Line 95: Does “brand” really change vapor pressure of energetic material? Brand may change the chemicals present, but not the vapor pressure of the chemical.

Fixed in line 101.

Lines 96-100: This discussion needs some further background provided for the reader that is not well versed about energetic materials. First, TNT and DNT should be spelled out upon this or prior use of acronym as well as other acronyms, although in case of RDX, suggest may put chemical name in parentheses vs. typical way acronym is introduced. I think it should be noted that DNT is a constituent of the vapor from TNT. I don’t follow well the phrase “This fluctuation is also seen with RDX (a component of C4)” what fluctuation – between RDX and other energetic materials – way written could be construed as fluctuation in vapor pressure of RDX.

Thank you for these suggestions. We have done substantial re-writing of the introduction to address these unclarities and noted stylistic issues below.

Line 104: Explosives contain other chemicals often due to purposeful addition of other materials to render them more user friendly in addition to contaminants and degradation products. Such purposeful additions are more important to detection by dogs because they are consistent.

Thank you for this note, we have added a comment on this in lines 127-129.

Line 107: Table title references vapor pressure of explosive materials; while this is okay by itself, the term “energetic materials” has been used earlier in manuscript and I think that keeping the distinction between energetic chemicals and an explosive material produced using this chemical would be helpful throughout discussion to this point in manuscript. Indeed, of course, TNT is TNT and AN is not energetic in and of itself. Perhaps use of some explanation like “explosive materials are composed of energetic chemicals or some material in combination are energetic compounds. Also, note in abstract (line 30-31, AN is listed as an “energetic” material. I think some clearer description is warranted here for readers that do not have a background regarding energetic chemicals and explosive materials. Also line 397 and 400.

Thank you for this clarification. We have revised the manuscript throughout for clarity in terminology and use energetic materials throughout.

Line 131: Term “manipulate” here seems awkward.

Addressed line 166

Line 153: Grammar change – “…in which working dogs are tasked to search”

Thank you, this change was made in line 163.

Line 153: Throughout manuscript there is a style issue of using a lot of transitional “further”, furthermore. For example in this line “Further, it has also been shown…” is wordy and not efficient. This could be written as “Divero et al. reported search and rescue dogs acclimated to working in -8.5C to -10.4C completing search tasks without signs of stress of fatigue [50].”

We have substantially edited the introduction and the writing throughout to improve quality.

Line 157: “maybe” should be “may be”.

Addressed line 214.

Line 158: suggest change “establish” to “investigate”.

Thank you, addressed in line 215.

Line 161: The phrase “It should be noted” and similar is overused throughout manuscript. If it is a reference to relevant previous studies, then the authors including it supposes it should be noted. In this line, perhaps consider “However, previous work found that dogs working in cold and standard conditions had the lowest threshold for methyl benzoate detection.” Also, this sentence does not contain a reference – was this previous unpublished work of the authors?

Citation oversight fixed. We have re-worked the writing style.

Line 252-254, perhaps noted elsewhere and can be ascertained by the description of olfactometer function , but suggest add that on non-odor trials the final air flow was the same as the total air flow of the diluted explosive (odor) trials.

A very important clarification point, added in lines 367-368.

Line 311. I do not believe that acronym PRE (assume “perceived respiratory effort” was defined earlier

Acronym corrected 389-390.

Lines 348 – 357: Not clear about non-odor, blank, trials or were there NO non-odor (diluent only) trials during testing?

We have clarified that there were no-odor (clean air) trials presented with equal probability to odor present trials.

Lines 423~427 and table 7: Recommend that t-ratio comparison be described and explained in text.

Thank you for this note, issue has been addressed in lines 662-667.

Lines 447 – 467: It would have been useful to have a comparison of the sensor body temperature and that from another measurement (e.g., rectal) as a way to evaluate the sensor performance.

Yes, this would have been an interesting area to explore, a note on this added in lines 691-692 in the discussion.

Line 545: Term “search performance” used for, what I believe is the first time. Suggest not using this term, particularly for first time in manuscript here. Environmental impact on “search performance” suggests searching and task was a fixed-sampling position activity that may not be considered a “search performance”.

Thank you, changed to “detection performance”

Lines 610-616: This is an interesting finding, but it would be made more useful to reader if the numbers of standard sessions from which dogs were removed was noted. Perhaps reader is to assume these were all in the more extreme environmental conditions.

Addressed 731-732, we agree this is an important clarification for the reader. We have added: “Importantly, only one dog was removed from one session during all testing sessions in standard conditions, while the remaining 56 early termination sessions occurred during testing in extreme environmental conditions.”

Lines 620 – 629: Suggest some discussion by authors about acclimation of dogs to environmental conditions and what impact it may have on changes seen in sensitivity due to environment in present experiment.

We did a continuation of this study which examined the relationship between acclimatization and detection performance. Results from that study, and citation for it added at the end of the discussion; lines 748-750.

Attachment

Submitted filename: Rebuttal letter2.docx

pone.0306817.s006.docx (31.9KB, docx)

Decision Letter 1

Sankarganesh Devaraj

25 Jun 2024

Environmental effects on explosive detection threshold of domestic dogs

PONE-D-23-31665R1

Dear Dr. Kane,

We’re pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been judged scientifically suitable for publication and will be formally accepted for publication once it meets all outstanding technical requirements.

Within one week, you’ll receive an e-mail detailing the required amendments. When these have been addressed, you’ll receive a formal acceptance letter and your manuscript will be scheduled for publication.

An invoice will be generated when your article is formally accepted. Please note, if your institution has a publishing partnership with PLOS and your article meets the relevant criteria, all or part of your publication costs will be covered. Please make sure your user information is up-to-date by logging into Editorial Manager at Editorial Manager® and clicking the ‘Update My Information' link at the top of the page. If you have any questions relating to publication charges, please contact our Author Billing department directly at authorbilling@plos.org.

If your institution or institutions have a press office, please notify them about your upcoming paper to help maximize its impact. If they’ll be preparing press materials, please inform our press team as soon as possible -- no later than 48 hours after receiving the formal acceptance. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information, please contact onepress@plos.org.

Kind regards,

Sankarganesh Devaraj

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Additional Editor Comments (optional):

The comments of the reviewers have been addressed appropriately. Therefore, I recommend the manuscript for publication in its current form.

Reviewers' comments:

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Author

1. If the authors have adequately addressed your comments raised in a previous round of review and you feel that this manuscript is now acceptable for publication, you may indicate that here to bypass the “Comments to the Author” section, enter your conflict of interest statement in the “Confidential to Editor” section, and submit your "Accept" recommendation.

Reviewer #1: All comments have been addressed

**********

2. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.

Reviewer #1: Yes

**********

3. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

Reviewer #1: Yes

**********

4. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #1: Yes

**********

5. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

Reviewer #1: Yes

**********

6. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #1: In my opinion, the MS improved after changes made according the reviewers suggestioons and in this form it can be accepted, although I think I still notice a small error in the description of the tables: Line:

134 ( 230 in corrected version) Detailed descriptions of each participant can be found in Table 2.

As I can see, description of the participants are available in the table 1 not 2 which presents “Description of odorants”

**********

7. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #1: No

**********

Acceptance letter

Sankarganesh Devaraj

2 Sep 2024

PONE-D-23-31665R1

PLOS ONE

Dear Dr. Kane,

I'm pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been deemed suitable for publication in PLOS ONE. Congratulations! Your manuscript is now being handed over to our production team.

At this stage, our production department will prepare your paper for publication. This includes ensuring the following:

* All references, tables, and figures are properly cited

* All relevant supporting information is included in the manuscript submission,

* There are no issues that prevent the paper from being properly typeset

If revisions are needed, the production department will contact you directly to resolve them. If no revisions are needed, you will receive an email when the publication date has been set. At this time, we do not offer pre-publication proofs to authors during production of the accepted work. Please keep in mind that we are working through a large volume of accepted articles, so please give us a few weeks to review your paper and let you know the next and final steps.

Lastly, if your institution or institutions have a press office, please let them know about your upcoming paper now to help maximize its impact. If they'll be preparing press materials, please inform our press team within the next 48 hours. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information, please contact onepress@plos.org.

If we can help with anything else, please email us at customercare@plos.org.

Thank you for submitting your work to PLOS ONE and supporting open access.

Kind regards,

PLOS ONE Editorial Office Staff

on behalf of

Dr. Sankarganesh Devaraj

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Associated Data

    This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

    Supplementary Materials

    S1 Table. A RE scale rating system described by DeChant et al. (submitted) was used to determine the respiration effort of each participant during the five second clip for each five-minute interval.

    Scoring for this scale ranges from 0 to 10. Zero being defined as resting and breathing is an involuntary process, and ten being very heavy breathing and the chest and abdomen are expanding and collapsing violently.

    (DOCX)

    pone.0306817.s001.docx (22.5KB, docx)
    S1 Data. Raw data across all sessions compiled into one file.

    (CSV)

    pone.0306817.s002.csv (2.1MB, csv)
    S2 Data. Raw temperature data from each session.

    (CSV)

    pone.0306817.s003.csv (27.6KB, csv)
    S3 Data

    (XLSX)

    pone.0306817.s004.xlsx (100.2KB, xlsx)
    S1 Code. Code in R used for all analyses and graphs presented.

    (CSV)

    pone.0306817.s005.qmd (9.6KB, qmd)
    Attachment

    Submitted filename: Rebuttal letter2.docx

    pone.0306817.s006.docx (31.9KB, docx)

    Data Availability Statement

    All data is fully available without restriction. This is provided in supplemental information. We have provided all temperature data, olfactometer data, and the R code used for analysis. We have also provided the behavioral coding results for PRE.


    Articles from PLOS ONE are provided here courtesy of PLOS

    RESOURCES