Abstract
Memory consolidation involves the synchronous reactivation of hippocampal cells active during recent experience in sleep sharp-wave ripples (SWRs). How this increase in firing rates and synchrony after learning is counterbalanced to preserve network stability is not understood. We discovered a network event generated by an intrahippocampal circuit formed by a subset of CA2 pyramidal cells to cholecystokinin-expressing (CCK+) basket cells, which fire a barrage of action potentials (“BARR”) during non–rapid eye movement sleep. CA1 neurons and assemblies that increased their activity during learning were reactivated during SWRs but inhibited during BARRs. The initial increase in reactivation during SWRs returned to baseline through sleep. This trend was abolished by silencing CCK+ basket cells during BARRs, resulting in higher synchrony of CA1 assemblies and impaired memory consolidation.
New memories are initially consolidated in the hippocampus during sleep after experience (1–3). As part of this process, neurons encoding a recent experience increase their firing rate during subsequent sleep sharp-wave ripples (SWRs) (4–8). Hippocampal ensembles that represent behaviorally relevant aspects of experience are reactivated together during SWRs (4–8). Disruption or enhancement of SWRs in sleep after learning results in memory impairment (9–11) or improvement (11), respectively. These elevated neuronal firing rates and ensemble reactivations rapidly return to baseline levels after experience (5, 8). However, it is not yet known how the brain achieves this rebalance after the selective potentiation of experience-related activity patterns. We hypothesized that the hippocampus leverages distinct circuit mechanisms to balance the increase in firing rates during memory consolidation. We thus conducted hippocampal CA1, CA2, and CA3 recordings in mice during various memory tasks and sleep (Fig. 1A and fig. S1A).
A distinct network event anticorrelated with SWRs and modulated by learning
SWRs are prominent during non–rapid eye movement (NREM) sleep, tend to occur simultaneously across hippocampal subregions (Fig. 1B), and recruit the firing of most pyramidal cells and interneurons in the hippocampus (12, 13). In addition to SWRs, we observed a different type of network event wherein a subset of CA2 pyramidal cells fired long barrages of action potentials (BARRs), coincident with an increased firing in a subset of CA1 and CA2 interneurons (Fig. 1B and figs. S1 to S3). BARRs had a longer duration than SWRs (300- versus 50-ms median duration; fig. S1J). Both SWRs and BARRs occurred mostly during NREM sleep (fig. S1D) and were anticorrelated in time (Fig. 1C and fig. S1C). Pyramidal cells in CA1, CA2, and CA3 were highly entrained during SWRs (Fig. 1D, left). A subpopulation of CA2 cells ramped (CA2ramp) their firing rate earlier and then became silenced (12, 14) (Fig. 1D, left), which was followed by the strong phasic firing of the rest of the CA2 (“CA2phas”), CA3, and CA1 cells (Fig. 1D, left). This dynamic was the opposite during BARRs; CA2ramp cells were the most active, whereas CA1 and CA3 cells did not increase their firing rates on average (Fig. 1D, right, and fig. S2, A to D). Principal components analysis of population bursts detected during SWRs, BARRs, and theta oscillations showed clearly segregated clusters (fig. S4, H and I). Overall, CA2 pyramidal cells were more bursty than CA1 (fig. S2E), with BARR-contributing CA2 cells being the most bursty of all (fig. S2F). CA2ramp cells fired long bursts of action potentials, featuring up to ~20 spikes within individual BARR events (fig. S2, H and I). Additionally, there was an anatomical segregation of CA2 pyramidal cells: Deep cells fired more in BARRs and less in SWRs, and superficial cells displayed the opposite modulation (Fig. 1E and fig. S2J). We also identified a similar modulation of interneurons. Putative parvalbumin-expressing (PV+) interneurons strongly increased their firing during SWRs but not during BARRs, whereas a different subset of interneurons was strongly recruited during BARRs but not SWRs (Fig. 1F and fig. S3).
The characteristic local field potential (LFP) oscillations at ~150 Hz during SWRs across the CA1 and CA2 pyramidal layer were absent during BARRs (Fig. 1G and fig. S2K). The firing of both pyramidal cells and interneurons was synchronized at the population level at ~150 Hz during SWRs (fig. S4, A and B) but not during BARRs (fig. S4, C and D). During BARRs, both CA2 pyramidal cells (fig. S4E) and the subset of active CA1 and CA2 interneurons (fig. S4, F and G) coordinated their firing at ~15 Hz. Despite having such distinctive spiking and LFP properties, BARR occurrence did not correlate with global brain state changes (fig. S1, K and L).
To verify whether our results extend to other species, we analyzed a dataset of hippocampal recordings in rats performing spatial memory tasks (12, 15). BARRs in rats occurred during NREM sleep and recruited the strong firing of a subset of CA2 pyramidal cells, similar to those in mice (fig. S5).
To identify the mesoscopic signature of BARRs, we decomposed CA2 LFPs into their underlying sources using independent components analysis (ICA). ICA extracted two different sources in CA2 for SWR and BARR activity, characterized by dominant power at ~150 and ~250 Hz, respectively (fig. S6).
Next, we sought to understand whether BARRs, in addition to SWRs, were modulated by different hippocampus-dependent memory tasks. We trained mice in a delayed alternation spatial memory task (16), an object displacement memory task (17), and a social memory task (11). The incidence of both SWRs and BARRs increased during postlearning sleep for all memory tasks (Fig. 1H).
Experience-related ensemble activity is inhibited during BARRs
We aimed to understand the functional role of BARRs during memory consolidation. We focused on the CA1 region because it is the main output of the hippocampus. We first classified CA1 cells on the basis of their firing rate modulation during learning tasks and quantified their activity in SWRs and BARRs after the task, normalized by their activity in the same events during pretask sleep (“FR gain”). Neurons that selectively increased their firing rates during learning were more reactivated in posttask sleep SWRs compared with others (Fig. 2A, left). These “task-active” cells decreased their firing rates during posttask BARRs more than other cells (Fig. 2A, right). Similarly, CA1 place cells (fig. S7, A to C) increased their firing rates more during postsleep SWRs compared with nonplace cells (Fig. 2B, left) and decreased their firing rate during postsleep BARRs more than nonplace cells (Fig. 2B, right). Both CA2 BARR+ and CA2 BARR− pyramidal cells displayed spatial modulation (fig. S7, D and E) and formed assemblies during behavior that were differentially modulated by SWRs and BARRs (fig. S7, F and G).
The firing rate gain of individual CA1 neurons in SWRs and BARRs was anticorrelated (Fig. 2C). Cells that increased their firing rates more in SWRs from pre- to postlearning sleep were the most silenced during BARRs (Fig. 2C and fig. S8A). However, thesameanalysis applied to REM epochs (fig. S8B) or random periods of NREM sleep (fig. S8C) showed a positive correlation. This firing rate modulation also had a Characteristic temporal dynamic. Firing rates of task-active cells were more elevated in early SWRs and gradually decreased (Fig. 2D) with the amount of NREM sleep (fig. S8H). On the contrary, cells were more strongly suppressed in BARRs at the beginning of sleep and increased their firing rate over a similar timescale (Fig. 2D). Next, we detected CA1 assemblies during learning (11, 18–20) and found that these assembly patterns were significantly reactivated during postsleep SWRs (Fig. 2E, left, and fig. S8, D to F). By contrast, the same assembly patterns were significantly suppressed during postsleep BARRs (Fig. 2E, right, and fig. S8, D to F). We also calculated the explained variance (EV) (5, 11, 19, 20) and found a robust increase in CA1 pyramidal cells’ EV during SWRs but a decrease during BARRs (Fig. 2F).
To mimic a learning-induced increase in SWR incidence, we artificially generated SWRs (11, 21, 22). We delivered 100-ms blue light pulses to the CA1 region of CamKII::ChR2 mice (Fig. 2, G and H) (11, 21) at 0.2 Hz for 2 hours while mice rested in their home cage, which resulted in an increase in SWR incidence during this period (fig. S8G). The stimulation session was preceded and followed by rest periods of 2 hours (Fig. 2G). BARR rate increased in the poststimulation period compared with baseline, whereas SWRs maintained a similar rate (Fig. 2I). Furthermore, CA1 pyramidal cells that increased their firing rate during artificially generated SWRs decreased their firing rate during poststimulation BARRs (Fig. 2J).
Similarly in rats, posttask firing rate gain in SWRs and BARRs was anticorrelated, and place cells were more inhibited in BARRs than nonplace cells (fig. S8, I and J).
Distinct interneuron subtypes modulate SWRs and BARRs
To elucidate the mechanism that supports the selective inhibition of CA1 firing during BARRs, we compared the electrophysiological properties of the different interneuron subpopulations that were most active during SWRs (putative PV+ cells) (21, 23, 24) and BARRs (BARR+ interneurons) (Fig. 1F). Putative PV+ interneurons had narrow waveforms and short refractory periods (Fig. 3A) and were highly active during SWRs (21, 23, 24) but not during BARRs (Fig. 1F). By contrast, BARR+ interneurons had wider waveforms and longer refractory periods (Fig. 3A) and were suppressed or only weakly active during SWRs (Fig. 1F). Both cell types showed different state-dependent firing. Putative PV+ interneurons fired more during periods of running than immobility and more during theta oscillations compared with nontheta waking periods, whereas BARR+ interneurons displayed the opposite modulation (Fig. 3B and fig. S3J). These properties were consistent with features of a subset of hippocampal basket cells that express cholecystokinin (CCK+) (25, 26), targeted by the selective expression of the SNCG promoter (Sncg+ cells). Therefore, we hypothesized that BARR+ interneurons largely overlap with this genetically defined interneuron subtype. We injected Flp-dependent channelrhodopsin (ChR2) in the CA1 region of Sncg-Flp mice (25) to specifically express ChR2 in Sncg+ cells (fig. S3K). Applying long-duration (~500 ms) blue light pulses resulted in a decreased firing rate of CA1 pyramidal cells (fig. S9A). Next, we identified Sncg+ cells (putative CCK+ basket cells) from their spiking response to blue or green light pulses [in Sncg-Flp mice expressing Flp-dependent ChR2 or archaerhodopsin (Arch), respectively] (fig. S3, K and L). Optogenetically identified Sncg+ cells were highly active during BARRs but not during SWRs, and they preferred firing during awake immobility periods compared with awake theta periods (Fig. 3, C and D). The waveforms and autocorrelograms of identified Sncg+ cells were similar to those of the previously identified BARR+ interneurons (fig. S9, B to E). In addition, a t-distributed stochastic neighbor embedding (t-SNE) analysis of physiological properties comparing all interneuron groups revealed a large overlap of Sncg+ cells and BARR+ interneurons subgroups (Fig. 3E), which supports our hypothesis that many BARR+ interneurons were CCK+ basket cells. Of note, although the main group of BARR+ interneurons overlapped with optogenetically identified Sncg+, other BARR+ cells were scattered outside this main group. On the basis of their low or negative modulation by SWRs and locomotion speed, these cells could correspond to axo-axonic (26) or vasoactive intestinal peptide–expressing (VIP+) (27) interneurons.
CA2 and CCK+ basket cells form an anatomically defined functional circuit
Next, we assessed whether CA2 pyramidal cells and CA1 Sncg+ interneurons form a functionally connected anatomical circuit, given their entrainment during BARRs. We used a set of viral-genetic strategies based on rabies-virus tracing of presynaptic partners of specific cellular subtypes (28–30) (Fig. 4A). We validated the infection of Sncg+ cells (Fig. 4B, red arrows) and identified starter cells (Fig. 4, B and C, yellow arrows) as well as their direct inputs (eGFP+/Sncg−; Fig. 4, B and C, green arrows), which clustered in the CA2 region of the hippocampus. Quantification of input cells to CA1 Sncg+ interneurons across animals revealed that a higher proportion of the inputs were from CA2 pyramidal cells (Fig. 4D and fig. S10). To validate this anatomical circuit in vivo, we identified putative monosynaptic connections by short-lag (2 to 3 ms) peaks in pyramidal cell–interneuron spike train cross-correlograms (31) (Fig. 4, E and F). CA2 pyramidal cells had a higher proportion of monosynaptic connections with CA1 BARR+ than with PV+ interneurons (Fig. 4E), whereas CA1 pyramidal cells had a higher proportion of connections with CA1 PV+ interneurons than with BARR+ interneurons (Fig. 4F). In addition, silencing CA2 pyramidal cells in Amigo2-Cre animals injected with Cre-dependent Arch3.0 also decreased firing rate in putative Sncg+ interneurons in CA1 (fig. S10, F and G).
Silencing Sncg+ cells during consolidation impairs memory
We assessed whether BARRs, in addition to SWRs (4–12), are also necessary for memory consolidation. We disrupted the specific BARR-associated inhibition of CA1 by silencing Sncg+ cells (fig. S10E). To optimize the silencing to specific BARR periods, we calculated the timing from each BARR to the nearest preceding SWR, which displayed an average peak distance of ~2 s (Fig. 5, A and B). We recorded Sncg-Flp mice injected with Flp-dependent Arch in the CA1 region during an object displacement memory task (fig. S1A). During postsleep periods, we detected SWRs and, 2 s after, delivered 2-s green light pulses to silence Sncg+ firing (fig. S10E) and disrupt BARR-associated inhibition of CA1 pyramidal cells (“BARR disruption”). SWR properties did not change during light activation of Sncg+ cells (fig. S9, G to J), nor did the overall SWR rate increase from pre- to postsleep (Fig. 5C). Furthermore, the reactivation of task-related assemblies in SWRs was also preserved (Fig. 5D). Yet this manipulation impaired memory performance (Fig. 5E). By contrast, closed-loop silencing of Sncg+ cells during SWRs did not affect memory performance (Fig. 5E). Additionally, in control sessions, the firing rate gain during SWRs increased in the first ~30 min of postlearning sleep and rapidly went back to baseline. On the contrary, in BARR disruption sessions, the firing rate gain during SWRs remained elevated during postlearning sleep (Fig. 5F). Population synchrony was also elevated in BARR disruption compared with control sessions (Fig. 5, G and H).
Discussion
The present results expand our understanding of the hippocampal mechanisms of memory consolidation by identifying a network event (BARR) wherein specific neurons that encoded a recent experience became inhibited. BARR events consisted of periods of persistent firing of a subset of deep-sublayer CA2 pyramidal cells along with an increased firing of Sncg+ cells (putative CCK+ basket cells). They occurred during NREM sleep and increased in rate after learning.
BARRs selectively inhibited task-related CA1 pyramidal cells in an activity-dependent manner. The more reactivated a given cell was during the learning and, subsequently, during postsleep SWRs, the more it was inhibited during BARRs. Similarly, assembly coactivity patterns that formed during learning were reactivated in subsequent sleep SWRs but inhibited during BARRs. Optogenetic disruption of BARRs after learning impaired memory performance. Although this manipulation did not affect the incidence of SWRs, it resulted in higher firing rates and synchrony for CA1 assemblies during SWRs, in line with recent results (32). This suggests that an optimal and dynamic range of assembly reactivation is required for memory consolidation, with a lack of reactivation (such as during SWRs disruption) or hypersynchronous reactivation (such as during pathological conditions) resulting in memory deficits. Furthermore, our findings show that BARRs play a homeostatic role in balancing SWR-associated neuronal reactivation. Our results were replicated for several types of memory tasks and different species, indicating that BARRs are a general hippocampal mechanism that contributes to memory consolidation. This mechanism could prevent firing rates and synchrony from continuously increasing over time because of the enhanced cell recruitment during SWRs and overall postsleep firing rate increase (4–12), which could otherwise reach a pathological regime (33, 34).
Various physiological mechanisms have been identified in support of the homeostatic role of sleep (35) at the cellular (36), synaptic (37), and neuronal population levels (38–40). However, it is not yet understood how the selective reactivation of coactive task-relevant assemblies during sleep SWRs is related to the synaptic homeostasis theory of sleep (35). It has been suggested that the reactivation of specific cell assemblies can only occur alongside a decrease in the firing rate of other assemblies, so the overall network firing rates are balanced (41). An alternative hypothesis is that complementary mechanisms (SWRs and BARRs) reactivate and inhibit the same task-related assemblies. Our data support the latter, as the same assemblies are up- and down-modulated during SWRs and BARRs, respectively. In favor of this idea, it was recently shown that SWRs are necessary for the hippocampus to express long-term depression during sleep (42). Our results provide experimental evidence that can help reconcile memory consolidation models (41) and the synaptic homeostasis hypothesis of sleep (35).
We previously showed that CA2 deep-sublayer pyramidal cells became rapidly silenced during SWRs (11, 12). In addition, a subset of CA2 pyramidal cells has been shown to become highly active during immobility and later on during sleep, likely corresponding to the same CA2 ramping population (12, 14) and probably to BARR+ CA2 cells. A similar response was observed in CA1 CCK+ basket cells (25, 26). CA1 pyramidal cells from deep and superficial sublayers send distinct information to different cortical areas (19), emerge at different developmental stages (43, 44), and have different physiological and functional correlates (19, 43–47). Our results suggest that a deep-superficial functional organization also exists in CA2, with superficial CA2 cells being highly active during SWRs and deep CA2 firing more during BARRs. Therefore, CA2 could act as a hub to set the network state (SWRs or BARRs) within the hippocampus. The selectivity of which cells are to be recruited during SWRs and BARRs could be supported by genetically distinct interneuron subtypes locally in CA1. PV+ interneurons contribute to the timing and coordination of cell assemblies during SWRs (21, 48). However, we showed that CCK+ basket cells were highly active during BARRs but not during SWRs. CCK+ basket cells express receptors for cannabinoids (CB1) (49, 50), retrograde messengers that can be activated by burst-like firing of CA1 pyramidal cells (51). Cannabinoid release after CA1 pyramidal cell firing during SWRs, possibly also aided by PV disinhibition (25, 50), could recruit CCK+ firing, which would then inhibit cells that are more active after learning.
Experimental and theoretical work has suggested that, in neocortical structures, network homeostasis is maintained by the balanced firing rates of the overall population (38, 40). In the hippocampus, we showed that generating artificial SWRs resulted in more BARRs. Population activity recruited during SWRs propagates through the network via lateral inhibition (52) or through entorhinal cortex (15), which could provide accumulated feedforward excitation into CA2, resulting in the generation of more BARRs.
Supplementary Material
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
We thank members of the Oliva and Fernandez-Ruiz laboratories and F. Leroy for useful comments on the manuscript and M. R. Warden for providing access to the confocal microscope.
Funding:
National Institutes of Health (grants R00MH122582 and R01MH130367 to A.O. and grants 4R00MH120343 and DP2MH136496 to A.F.-R.); New Frontiers grant (to A.O.); Sloan Fellowship (to A.F.-R.); Whitehall Research Grant (to A.F.-R.); Klingenstein-Simons Fellowship (to A.F.-R.); National Science Foundation Graduate Research Fellowship (grant DGE-2139899 to L.A.K.); National Institutes of Health, Ruth L. Kirschstein National Research Service Award Individual Postdoctoral Fellowship (F32MH134673 to H.L.R.).
Footnotes
Competing interests: Authors declare that they have no competing interests.
Data and materials availability:
All data are available in the main text or the supplementary materials. Custom scripts used in this study can be downloaded from https://github.com/ayalab1/neurocode and Zenodo (53).
REFERENCES AND NOTES
- 1.Scoville WB, Milner B, J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 20, 11–21 (1957). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Andersen P, Morris R, Amaral D, Bliss T, O’Keefe J, Eds., The Hippocampus Book (Oxford Univ. Press, 2007). [Google Scholar]
- 3.Diekelmann S, Born J, Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 11, 114–126 (2010). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Wilson MA, McNaughton BL, Science 265, 676–679 (1994). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Kudrimoti HS, Barnes CA, McNaughton BL, Neurosci J. 19, 4090–4101 (1999). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Lee AK, Wilson MA, Neuron 36, 1183–1194 (2002). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Grosmark AD, Buzsáki G, Science 351, 1440–1443 (2016). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Giri B, Miyawaki H, Mizuseki K, Cheng S, Diba K, Neurosci J. 39, 866–875 (2019). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Girardeau G, Benchenane K, Wiener SI, Buzsáki G, Zugaro MB, Nat. Neurosci. 12, 1222–1223 (2009). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Ego-Stengel V, Wilson MA, Hippocampus 20, 1–10 (2010). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Oliva A, Fernández-Ruiz A, Leroy F, Siegelbaum SA, Nature 587, 264–269 (2020). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Oliva A, Fernández-Ruiz A, Buzsáki G, Berényi A, Neuron 91, 1342–1355 (2016). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Csicsvari J, Hirase H, Czurkó A, Mamiya A, Buzsáki G, Neurosci J. 19, 274–287 (1999). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Kay K et al. , Nature 531, 185–190 (2016). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Fernández-Ruiz A et al. , Science 372, eabf3119 (2021).33795429 [Google Scholar]
- 16.Valero M et al. , Nat. Neurosci. 24, 401–411 (2021). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Genzel L et al. , PLOS Biol. 17, e3000322 (2019). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Dupret D, O’Neill J, Pleydell-Bouverie B, Csicsvari J, Nat. Neurosci. 13, 995–1002 (2010). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Harvey RE, Robinson HL, Liu C, Oliva A, Fernandez-Ruiz A, Neuron 111, 2076–2090.e9 (2023). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Liu C, Todorova R, Tang W, Oliva A, Fernandez-Ruiz A, Science 382, eadi8237 (2023). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Stark E et al. , Neuron 83, 467–480 (2014). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Fernández-Ruiz A et al. , Science 364, 1082–1086 (2019). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Gan J, Weng SM, Pernía-Andrade AJ, Csicsvari J, Jonas P, Neuron 93, 308–314 (2017). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.English DF et al. , J. Neurosci. 34, 16509–16517 (2014). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Dudok B et al. , Neuron 109, 997–1012.e9 (2021). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Vancura B, Geiller T, Grosmark A, Zhao V, Losonczy A, Nat. Neurosci. 26, 788–797 (2023). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Turi GF et al. , Neuron 101, 1150–1165.e8 (2019). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Schwarz LA et al. , Nature 524, 88–92 (2015). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Miyamichi K et al. , Neuron 80, 1232–1245 (2013). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Wickersham IR et al. , Neuron 53, 639–647 (2007). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.English DF et al. , Neuron 96, 505–520.e7 (2017). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Rangel Guerrero DK et al. , Neuron 112, 2045–2061.e10 (2024). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Valero M et al. , Neuron 94, 1234–1247.e7 (2017). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Gelinas JN, Khodagholy D, Thesen T, Devinsky O, Buzsáki G, Nat. Med. 22, 641–648 (2016). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Tononi G, Cirelli C, Brain Res. Bull. 62, 143–150 (2003). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Turrigiano GG, Leslie KR, Desai NS, Rutherford LC, Nelson SB, Nature 391, 892–896 (1998). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Vyazovskiy VV, Cirelli C, Pfister-Genskow M, Faraguna U, Tononi G, Nat. Neurosci. 11, 200–208 (2008). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Hengen KB, Torrado Pacheco A, McGregor JN, Van Hooser SD, Turrigiano GG, Cell 165, 180–191 (2016). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Levenstein D et al. , bioRxiv 2021.09.20.461152 [Preprint] (2022); 10.1101/2021.09.20.461152. [DOI]
- 40.Watson BO, Levenstein D, Greene JP, Gelinas JN, Buzsáki G, Neuron 90, 839–852 (2016). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Buzsáki G, Hippocampus 25, 1073–1188 (2015). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Norimoto H et al. , Science 359, 1524–1527 (2018). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Cavalieri D et al. , eLife 10, e69270 (2021). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Huszár R, Zhang Y, Blockus H, Buzsáki G, Nat. Neurosci. 25, 1201–1212 (2022). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Mizuseki K, Diba K, Pastalkova E, Buzsáki G, Nat. Neurosci. 14, 1174–1181 (2011). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Valero M et al. , Nat. Neurosci. 18, 1281–1290 (2015). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Danielson NB et al. , Neuron 91, 652–665 (2016). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Schlingloff D, Káli S, Freund TF, Hájos N, Gulyás AI, Neurosci J. 34, 11385–11398 (2014). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Bartos M, Elgueta C, Physiol J. 590, 669–681 (2012). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Dudok B et al. , Science 383, 967–970 (2024). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Chevaleyre V, Castillo PE, Neuron 38, 461–472 (2003). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.McKenzie S et al. , Neuron 109, 1040–1054.e7 (2021). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Harvey R et al. , Zenodo (2023); 10.5281/zenodo.7819979. [DOI]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
All data are available in the main text or the supplementary materials. Custom scripts used in this study can be downloaded from https://github.com/ayalab1/neurocode and Zenodo (53).