Skip to main content
Scientific Reports logoLink to Scientific Reports
. 2024 Sep 27;14:22079. doi: 10.1038/s41598-024-73438-0

Movement synchrony among dance performers predicts brain synchrony among dance spectators

Guido Orgs 1,, Staci Vicary 2, Matthias Sperling 3, Daniel C Richardson 4, Adrian L Williams 5
PMCID: PMC11436841  PMID: 39333777

Abstract

Performing dance is an intrinsically social art form where at least one person moves while another person watches. Dancing in groups promotes social bonding, but how does group dance affect the people watching? A group of dancers and dance novices watched a 30 min dance video individually in an fMRI scanner. In a follow-up behavioural study, the same people watched the video again and provided continuous enjoyment ratings. Firstly, we computed cross-recurrence of continuous enjoyment ratings and inter-subject correlations (ISCs) in fMRI separately for both groups, and with the choreographer of the dance work. At both behavioural and neural levels, dancers responded more similarly to each other than novices. ISCs among dancers extended beyond brain areas involved in audio-visual integration and sensory areas of human movement perception into motor areas, suggesting greater sensorimotor familiarity with the observed dance movements in the expert group. Secondly, we show that dancers’ brain activations and continuous ratings are more similar to the choreographer’s ratings in keeping with sharing an aesthetic and artistic perspective when viewing the dance. Thirdly, we show that movement synchrony among performers is the best predictor of brain synchrony among both expert and novice spectators. This is consistent with the idea that changes in emergent movement synchrony are a key aesthetic feature of performing dance. Finally, ISCs across perceptual and motor brain areas were primarily driven by movement acceleration and synchrony, whereas ISCs in orbital and pre-frontal brain areas were overall weaker and better explained by the continuous enjoyment ratings of each group. Our findings provide strong evidence that the aesthetic appreciation of dance involves a common experience between dance spectators and the choreographer. Moreover, the similarity of brain activations and of enjoyment increases with shared knowledge of - and practice in - the artform that is being experienced, in this case contemporary performing dance.

Keywords: Dance, Inter-subject correlations, Synchrony, Joint action, Neuroaesthetics, fMRI, Action observation

Subject terms: Cognitive neuroscience, Human behaviour, Perception

Introduction

Performing dance and music are fundamentally social art forms, often performed and experienced in groups14. Whether in theatres, concert halls, cinemas or galleries, experiencing art together rather than alone can greatly enhance our engagement with an artwork5.

If collective aesthetic experiences involves a common experience between at least two people, cognitive neuroscience suggests that they involve a form of mutual entrainment between spectators or listeners6. In live performance contexts, it has been shown that spectators synchronise their heartrate and breathing rate when watching dance or a fire ritual7,8 and synchronise their head movements when watching a rock concert9. In fMRI, brain synchrony between people is often measured by computing inter-subject correlations (ISCs) while people watch naturalistic stimuli such as film. Brain activity between multiple movie watchers predicts better memory for movie content10,11 and affective responses to TV series and ads12,13. Brain synchrony in these studies reflects a combination of both exogenous stimulus features and endogenous, more idiosyncratic processing of these features12,14,15. In particular, sensory areas in posterior regions of the brain are more closely tied to stimulus features, whereas brain synchrony in the orbital and medial frontal cortices is more closely tied to subjective experience, including aesthetic judgement15,16.

Brain synchrony has been argued to reflect shared emotions among people watching movies, dance or listening to stories. In one study, when listening to emotional stories, brain synchrony was correlated with heart rate measures as well as valence and arousal ratings after scanning. Negative shifts in valence were associated with greater phase synchrony in ACC and S1, and midline brain regions typically associated with self-referential processing. Positive shifts in valence were associated with greater phase synchrony in medial prefrontal cortices. In contrast increased arousal was related to greater brain synchrony in medial prefrontal and auditory areas; a decrease in arousal was associated with greater brain synchrony in visual cortices, the middle cingulum, the precuneus and supplementary motor areas6. More recent work has also shown that brain synchrony correlates with the rhetorical quality of political speeches17. Interestingly, ISCs during movie viewing are greater if spectators are instructed to take the same psychological perspective and the pattern of eye movements does not predict differences in ISCs18.

Much research using ISCs is conducted with narrative stimuli (audiobook, TV episodes and film) in which peak brain synchronisation typically occurs at salient turning points in the plot or during highly emotionally laden moments. Yet, research in joint action perception suggests that mere observation of other people moving together is rewarding in itself, in the absence of any storyline19. In dance, the synchrony of group dancing is linked to group affiliation20 and predicts how much audiences enjoy a live dance performance21. In the latter study, synchrony of movement acceleration was measured continuously among a group of ten dancers while they were performing a 30-min choreography. Additionally, continuous heart rate and enjoyment ratings were collected from spectators. Synchrony predicted continuous enjoyment ratings and heart rate, but only if spectators formed a consistently positive or negative aesthetic evaluation of the dance performance.

Only very few neuroimaging studies have applied ISCs to study brain synchrony while watching dance. One study compared brain synchrony among dance naïve spectators during the observation of edited and unedited videos of a single ballet dancer22. Viewing the dance videos—which contained both music and movement—was associated with shared activations in brain areas related to audio-visual integration, bilaterally in the posterior superior temporal gyri (STG) and with activations in the human action observation network, such as motor and premotor cortices and the superior parietal lobe. Brain synchrony in STG was stronger for unedited as compared to edited videos, perhaps indicating better alignment between auditory visual information in the unedited condition, see also23. One study combined a qualitative assessment of the spectators’ live experience of watching dance with a follow-up fMRI experiment in which a video of the same performance was watched by a different group of spectators with different musical soundtracks24. Spectators in the scanner watched two versions of the choreography: in one condition, the choreography was performed to classical music, in the second condition it was performed without music so that only the breathing and footfalls were audible. Brain synchrony among spectators was most pronounced in superior temporal and middle occipital cortices, but also included frontal motor areas as well the superior parietal lobule. Interestingly, brain synchrony in the breathing only condition was stronger in the superior temporal gyrus and involved the right postcentral gyrus. The authors argue that greater synchrony in the STG reflects the perfect simultaneity of auditory and visual input in the breathing only condition. The authors argue that these activations in somatosensory cortex might reflect ‘kinaesthetic empathy’ i.e. engagement of somatosensory areas in participants, that was triggered by the breathing sounds and footfalls of the performers. Together these findings suggest that watching dance does not only synchronize brain areas involved in audio-visual integration of movement and sound, but also involves the human action observation network25. However, none of these studies on dance perception showed activation in prefrontal brain areas related to reward processing, although these areas have been reported more consistently for the aesthetic appreciation of images, paintings16,26,27 and music28,29. Similarly, it is not known whether greater brain synchrony can be linked to specific features of the movement, for example movement complexity30 or synchrony among performers21,31.

Dance observation depends on dance expertise which modulates brain activations in visual, parietal and motor areas of the action observation network3134. Learning to perform a movement increases the aesthetic appeal of these movements, and correlates with greater activation in the left superior temporal sulcus for these movements35. It is however an open question as to how dance training impacts on brain synchrony when watching dance. One behavioural study on the aesthetics of mathematical equations suggests that expertise produces greater agreement, based on shared formal training and knowledge in maths36, but is such greater agreement measurable at the neural level? Arguably, contemporary dancers might not only share more knowledge with each other about contemporary dance, but also with a maker /choreographer of a contemporary dance work as they are more likely to understand the choreographic principles, dance practices and cultural context within which an artwork was made37,38. In this way, greater brain synchrony between experienced dancers and choreographers watching a specific dance work might reflect a shared artistic and aesthetic perspective18.

In the present study, we compare ISCs between dancers and dance novices while watching the recording of a live dance performance and use kinematic measures collected during the live performance to predict ISCs among both groups. We propose that the size of ISCs will covary with both dance experience and salient aesthetic features of the observed performance, in this case synchronous movement among a group of dance performers20,21. We will test three hypotheses at behavioural and neural levels: Firstly, we predict greater overall ISCs and behavioural agreement in the dance experienced group relative to the dance novice group (H1). Secondly, we predict that both continuous ratings of enjoyment and brain activations will be more similar between the choreographer and the dancers, than between the choreographer and the novices (H2). Thirdly, we predict that ISCs in dancer and novice spectators are best explained by movement synchrony among performers and that stimulus features should predict ISCs across posterior sensorimotor areas of the brain, whereas enjoyment ratings should better explain ISCs across prefrontal areas related to reward and self-referential processing (H3).

Methods

Participants

Existing studies that use ISCs in the context of dance viewing22,23 tested between 12 and 16 participants, albeit using stimuli with a much shorter video duration than in our study (< 7 min vs. 30 min), we therefore aimed for a total sample size of 30 participants39. Twenty-eight people took part in the study, including the choreographer of the dance performance (MS, age = 40, 30 years of dance experience, left-handed). Two fMRI data sets were lost, one novice participant due to technical error, and one dancer based on adverse experience in the scanner, resulting in 11 novice 14 dancer fMRI datasets. Dance novices (Mean age = 31 years, SD = 7 years), had no formal dance training and less than 5 years of casual involvement with dance (for example taking community classes or dance for fitness). Professional dancers (M age = 28 years, SD = 7 years), had between 9 and 33 years of professional dance experience (mean experience = 17 years). All participants were right handed, except for the choreographer and one dancer who reported left handedness as assessed by the Flinders handedness survey40. Two participants were unavailable for the follow up session and five follow up sessions resulted in incomplete continuous enjoyment data (three sessions due to technical error, two due to user error). The final sample for continuous enjoyment data is N = 19, with 6 sessions from novices and 13 sessions recorded from dancers. All participants provided informed consent, and all data collection and data analyses were carried out in accordance with relevant guidelines and regulations. The research was approved by the ethics committee at Brunel University London.

Materials

Dance video

Participants viewed a recorded version of the choreography Group Study by Matthias Sperling. This work was developed in the context of research on the perception of movement synchrony in dance. In Group Study, 10 professional dancers perform a task-based choreography that consist of mainly pedestrian movements such as walking, running and arm swinging. Over the course of the performance, movement synchrony systematically varies among performers, with moments of near perfect synchrony (i.e. all dancers stop moving at the same time) and moments where dancers move completely independently. In Group Study, synchrony and asynchrony are emergent properties of the live interactions between performers. In contrast with conventional ideas of synchrony in dance, this approach relies neither on the repetition of pre-planned movement sequences, nor external signals (such as music) to establish group co-ordination (Fig. 1). Accordingly, Group Study involves vocalizations and the sound of the performers moving, but no music. The video was filmed frontally in a single shot without editing and lasted 33 min and 54 s, the video used in this study is available to view at https://research.gold.ac.uk/id/eprint/34238/. For detailed information on the performance and the choreographic score, see21.

Fig. 1.

Fig. 1

Snapshot of Group Study by Matthias Sperling (2015). Movement acceleration from all ten performers was recorded using Empatica E4 wrist sensors. Image by Matthias Sperling.

Quantification of the performers’ movements

During the dance performance, we recorded the movements of all 10 performers using wrist accelerometers (Empatica E4 wrist sensors41) and computed measures of overall acceleration and synchrony using cross-recurrence analysis. In addition, motion and sound energy were also extracted from the dance video.

We calculated an overall measure of how much performers moved by taking the square root of the sum of the squared x, y and z -axis values of the acceleration data, leaving a single time series vector for each of the 10 performers. Acceleration was then averaged across performers within 2 s time windows. To compute movement synchrony, we applied cross-recurrence quantification analysis (CRQA) to the non-windowed acceleration vectors to obtain a continuous measure of synchrony among performers across time using the CRQA package implemented in R. Cross recurrence was calculated with an embedding dimension of 1, a radius of 10 and a delay of 142. The recurrence rate was calculated for every possible pair of performers (N = 90 pairs), within a + /− 2 s lag window, for more detail, see21.

Finally, we extracted motion and sound energy from the video. We calculated motion energy as the pixel-wise change in successive frames of the gray scale version of the dance video, using a custom made matlab script43. The spectral power of the video soundtrack was computed using the miraudio toolbox for MATLAB44.

Summative audience engagement

After completion of the scanning and the follow-up viewing sessions, all participants completed a short quantitative performance engagement scale, see supplementary materials. Eight questions, addressed the participant’s level of engagement, i.e. “I was absorbed by what was happening in the performance”, “I found the performance interesting” or “I didn’t enjoy the performance”. A further four questions probed the participants felt understanding, i.e. “I felt like I understood what the performance was about” or “I couldn’t figure out why the performers were moving in particular ways” of the performance. One additional question asked participants to what extent they were distracted by scanner noise. All of 13 questions were answered on a 5-point Likert Scale ranging from Strongly Agree to Strongly Disagree. 7 questions were reverse coded.

Continuous audience engagement

To quantify each participant’s enjoyment of the performance outside of the scanner, ASUS touchscreen devices running the android version of OpenSesame experiment software were used45. A custom finger tracking experiment designed for investigation of live audience response continuously tracked the location of the participant’s index finger on the touch screen (24 Hz sampling rate). Shown on the touch screen was a single vertical axis (white text on black background), ranging from “Enjoy Very Much” at the top of the screen to “Enjoy Very Little” at the bottom of the screen. Participants were instructed to hold their right index finger on the device and move, continuously throughout the performance, in the direction most consistent with their current evaluation. For example, if the participant thought the performance was enjoyable, they should move their finger upwards. A practice session with the tablet was given before the performance began to allow participants to familiarize themselves with the tablet.

Procedure

Scanning took place at the Combined Universities Brain Imaging Centre (CUBIC; www.cubic.rhul.ac.uk). On arrival, participants were given an overview of the imaging procedure, completed a short demographic questionnaire including dance expertise and were taken through a standard imaging safety report. Once participants were cleared to take part in MRI scanning, participants were asked to view a contemporary dance performance without music and were told that they would be asked to provide feedback on the performance after the scanning session.

Participants returned between two and three weeks after their fMRI scan, to complete a follow-up session where we collected the continuous enjoyment ratings. We chose to not collect continuous ratings during the fMRI session to better approximate the live viewing situation. Moreover, we requested a minimum of two weeks between fMRI and follow-up sessions in order to reduce the potential influence of familiarity with the video on continuous ratings. During the follow-up session, participants wore noise cancelling headphones and sat in front of a laptop to observe the same dance video as observed during the scanning session whilst making continuous enjoyment ratings on the tablet. At the completion of this follow-up session, participants were debriefed as to the purpose of the study.

Behavioural data analyses

Summative audience engagement

To assess whether the experience of watching the performance was comparable between the first and the second viewing of the performance, we computed mean scores for engagement and understanding for each participant and submitted these to two 2 × 2 mixed ANOVAs with the factors session (FMRI vs. follow-up) and group (novices vs. dancers). We used a between subject t-test to see if there were any group differences in how much participants were distracted by scanner noise.

cross-recurrence analysis of continuous enjoyment ratings

Cross recurrence analysis is well suited to the dynamics of human behaviour46. Similarly to our analysis of movement synchrony among performers, we applied cross recurrence analysis to compute agreement of continuous enjoyment ratings among all spectators using the CRQA package in R47. We calculated the recurrence rate between pairs of continuous ratings, which quantifies their degree of temporal coupling. To assess how this temporal coupling changed over time, we used a windowed recurrence analysis, calculating pairwise recurrence rate every 3 s. These pairwise metrics can then be averaged over different groups. We computed two agreement measures. To test whether agreement differed between groups (H1) we computed pairwise cross recurrence between all expert dancers’ enjoyment ratings (dancers with dancers, hereafter DD) and all dance novices (novices with novices, hereafter NN) separately. Finally, to assess similarity between dancers, novices and the choreographer at the behavioural level, we computed Pearson correlations between the choreographer’s continuous enjoyment ratings and the average ratings of all dancers and all novices separately.

fMRI data analyses

Data collection

MRI data were recorded with a Siemens 3 T MRI scanner (Magnetom Trio, Siemens, Erlangen, Germany) using a standard Siemens eight-channel array headcoil. Whole-brain functional images were acquired with a T2*-weighted gradient echo, echoplanar sequence sensitive to BOLD contrast (TR 3000 ms, TE 31 ms, voxel size 3 × 3x3mm) comprising 41 axial slices and a 192 mm FOV. During the experimental run, 680 volumes were acquired followed by a standard T1-weighted high-resolution (1 × 1 × 1 mm) anatomical scan of the head.

Pre-processing

All functional data was pre-processed using the FSL software library (Smith et al., 2004) in preparation for analysis with the ISC toolbox (see below). The 680-volume sequence was first motion corrected using MCFLIRT48 and the resulting series skull-stripped using BET49. A high-pass temporal filter was then applied with a cut-off of 60 s. All data were subsequently aligned to the MNI-152 2 mm template space in a multi-stage process using FLIRT48: first, the timeseries mean volume was aligned with the skull-stripped anatomical volume using a six-parameter rigid-body transformation; second, the anatomical volume was aligned with the MNI-152 template using a twelve-parameter affine transformation; third, the combined transforms were applied to the 4D timeseries data. Finally, the normalised functional data were smoothed with a Gaussian kernel with FWHM of 5 mm.

ROIs analysis

In addition to whole brain analyses, we identified three regions of interest to disentangle three levels of processing of the dance video. These regions were selected based on group differences between dancers and novices and the extant literature. The first ROI covers two brain areas involved in audio-visual processing of dance videos, the superior temporal gyrus, BA41 and the medial occipital gyrus, BA 1823. The second area covers visual, motor and parietal brain areas involved in action observation50. These include the middle temporal gyrus (BA37), middle frontal gyrus (BA6), the inferior and superior parietal lobe (BA40/BA7). The third ROI includes brain areas of the default mode network which has previously been shown to also be involved in aesthetic judgement15,16. These areas include the prefrontal and orbitofrontal cortex (BA10/BA 47) as well as anterior and posterior cingulate cortices (BA32/BA31). ROIs were generated on the basis of Brodmann areas as defined in WFU Pickatlas51.

Inter-subject correlations (ISCs)

The data were analysed using the ISC toolbox version 3.052. The voxel-wise ISC value represents the mean correlation coefficient calculated across all possible subject pairs53. A non-parametric bootstrapping method is used to test the significance of each ISC value, with each time series shifted by a random amount and the statistic recalculated 1 million times. Critical significance thresholds are then calculated by correcting p-values of the true realizations for each voxel using the false discovery rate (FDR) based multiple comparisons correction54. In parallel to the analysis of behavioural data we computed summative and continuous measures of ISCs.

Comparing ISCs between dancer and novices (H1)

To measure summative ISCs across the entire performance, we calculated pairwise ISCs for the whole time series (860 volumes), separately for dancers and novices.

Comparing ISCs between dancers, novices and the choreographer (H2)

To assess brain synchrony between the two groups of spectators and the choreographer, we computed pairwise ISCs between the choreographer and each dancer, and pairwise ISCs between the choreographer and each novice. A Fisher z-transform was applied to each correlation map followed by a 3 mm spatial smoothing filter. These were then used to test whether ISCs were significant between pairs by conducting voxel-wise non-parametric permutation tests (FSL Randomise55) utilising threshold-free cluster enhancement (TFCE) to correct for multiple comparisons and a fully exhaustive set of permutations of the data. Specifically, we conducted nonparametric 1-sample t-tests to establish areas of significant ISC between the choreographer and dancers, and between the choreographer and novices. We also conducted a nonparametric 2-sample unpaired t-test to establish regions of significant differences between choreographer/dancers and choreographer/novices ISC maps.

Predicting ISCs from performed movement synchrony (H3)

We used the wrist sensor and video data collected during the initial live performance experiment21 to link dynamic changes in spectators’ ISCs to the performers’ movements. We computed dynamic ISC maps for consecutive 30 s windows (10 volumes per window), across the length of the performance video, separated by 3 s (1 TR). This resulted in 671 ISC maps for the entire performance. The group-averaged mean time-series of the ten performers’ movements (synchrony and acceleration) and the video (motion energy and sound) were resampled to represent the same 30 s windows as the ISC and used as explanatory variables to predict temporal variation of ISCs within each voxel using a GLM approach.

All fMRI data analyses were carried out using FEAT (fMRI Expert Analysis Tool) Version 6.00, part of FSL (fMRIB’s Software Library, www.fmrib.ox.ac.uk/fsl). The only pre-statistics processing applied to the dynamic ISC data was spatial smoothing using a Gaussian kernel of FWHM 5 mm; high-pass temporal filtering was applied at the standard setting of 100 s. All explanatory variables were scaled to the interval [0,1] and convolved with a Gaussian filter (sigma 2.8 s, lag 5 s) and orthogonalized with respect to each other. The resulting z-statistic images were thresholded using clusters determined by z > 3.1 and a corrected cluster significance threshold of p < 0.05. These maps thus reflect the degree to which dynamic ISCs depend on the performers movements as measured directly during the live performance (synchrony and acceleration) or extracted from the video (motion and sound energy). Our fMRI dataset is available at https://openneuro.org/datasets/ds004783.

Results

The results section is structured according to our three hypotheses. Firstly, we report summative and continuous enjoyment ratings and compare average ISCs between dancers and novices (H1). Secondly, we report summative ISCs between both groups and the choreographer as an implicit measure of artistic perspective sharing (H2). Thirdly, we assess to what extent dynamic ISCs in both groups are explained by exogenous features of the performance and endogenous aesthetic experience as measured by the similarity of continuous ratings (H3).

Summative engagement and understanding

Ten novices and thirteen dancers completed both questionnaires immediately after the scanning session and following the second viewing of the dance video in the follow-up session, 2–3 weeks after rating their overall engagement with the performance video as whole.

Overall engagement with the dance video tended to be higher in dancers than in novices (F(1,21) = 3.3, p = 0.08, η2 = 0.14, mean novices = 3.16 (SE = 0.1), mean dancers = 3.48(SE = 0.11)). Engagement did not differ between fMRI and follow-up session and F(1,21) = 0.006, p = 0.93) and there was no interaction between session and group F(1,21) = 1.2, p = 0.28).

Dancers reported greater overall understanding of the dance video (F(1,21) = 9.4, p = 0.006, η2 = 0.31, mean novices = 2.46 (SE = 0.2), mean dancers = 3.29 (SE = 0.18)). Felt understanding did not differ between fMRI and follow-up sessions F(1,21) = 0.01, p = 0.85) and there was no interaction between session and group F(1,21) = 0.002, p = 0.97). Perceived distraction from noise did not differ between groups or sessions (all p = n. s.)

Continuous enjoyment ratings (H1)

Continuous enjoyment ratings were more similar between dancers than they were between dance novices. Figure 2A shows the ratings for the choreographer and the means of individual participants who are novices and dancers unfolding over time. Whereas the dancers’ mean ratings were correlated positively with the choreographer (r = 0.57, p < 0.0001), novices’ ratings were negatively correlated (r = − 0.35, p < 0.005). There was also higher consistency within the dancers’ ratings than amongst the novices. The higher temporal coupling of dancers’ was shown by a windowed CRQA analysis. Figure 2B shows the distribution of mean recurrence rates, averaged across all time windows, for pairwise comparisons between dancers and other dancers, and novices and other novices. A t-test on these recurrence values showed a significant difference between the groups (T(30.2) = 6.61, p < 0.00001).

Figure 2.

Figure 2

(A) Enjoyment ratings of the choreographer, and means of novices and dancers, with SEs shaded areas. (B) Temporal coupling time series are quantified in density plots of the recurrence rate between all dancer-dancer and novice-novice pairs.

Average ISCs dancers vs. novices (H1)

We computed summative ISCs separately for Dancers and Novices. In both groups, we observe a large cluster encompassing bilateral areas of temporal, frontal, parietal and occipital cortices with peak activations in the superior and middle temporal gyri (see Fig. 3, Table 1). This cluster is nearly twice as large in dancers as in novices and includes subpeaks that in novices are either absent or separate from the main cluster. Notably, only dancers’ brains exhibit bilateral activations along the superior and inferior parts of the precentral gyrus including premotor and motor areas. Dancers also show stronger and more widespread similarity of activations in the middle occipital and lingual gyri (MOG) and the precuneus. Finally, dancers reveal significantly greater similarity of brain activations in the anterior (ACC) and posterior cingulate cortices (PCC), the angular gyrus, and the ventromedial prefrontal (vmPFC). A full table of all local maxima is included in the supplementary materials (S1). A direct comparison of the strength of ISCs between Dancers and Novices yielded significant differences in the superior temporal gyrus, bilaterally, the right middle temporal gyrus, and across inferior, middle and superior frontal gyri. However, only the bilateral activations in the STG survived FDR correction (see supplementary materials, S2).

Fig. 3.

Fig. 3

Group level ISC maps for dancers (top) and novices (bottom). These show statistically significant ISCs for each group (p < 0.001, FDR corrected) during viewing of the dance video.

Table 1.

Cluster locations of the summative ISCs for Dancers and Novices.

Cluster region Cluster extent (# voxels) MNI coordinates (mm) Peak statistic (r)
x y z
Dancers
Superior temporal gyrus 69012a 60  − 28 8 0.3198
Anterior cingulate 349a  − 12 40  − 8 0.0290
Superior frontal gyrus 161  − 26 56 6 0.0273
Superior frontal gyrus 99  − 18 32 50 0.0260
Frontal pole 84a 24 68 0 0.0231
Anterior cingulate 66 12 50  − 8 0.0221
Inferior frontal gyrus 43 32 34  − 14 0.0225
Caudate 29 18 12 18 0.0203
Inferior frontal gyrus 28  − 26 28  − 12 0.0215
Medial frontal gyrus 22  − 2 56 20 0.0183
Superior frontal gyrus 18 28 50 12 0.0188
Middle frontal gyrus 16  − 34 16 54 0.0203
Postcentral gyrus 15 68  − 18 34 0.0233
Insula 14 36 8 8 0.0216
Cingulate gyrus 12  − 8 26 24 0.0196
Novices
Superior temporal gyrus 28328a 50  − 18 4 0.2035
Precentral gyrus 237 48 4 30 0.0489
Middle frontal gyrus 230 22  − 4 46 0.0489
Superior parietal lobe 215  − 18  − 60 64 0.0444
Thalamus 108 16  − 28  − 6 0.0432
Cingulate gyrus 27  − 12  − 24 40 0.0330
Superior frontal gyrus 19  − 14 64 4 0.0295

Clusters were defined using the FSL cluster tool. ISC maps were thresholded at p < 0.001 (FDR corrected) and a cluster extent threshold of 96 mm3 applied.

aLocal maxima were found for these clusters using a 20 mm separation threshold (see supplementary Table S1).

In sum, ISCs among dancers were overall more widespread, stronger and bilaterally symmetrical than those for the novices. In particular, ISCs along the precentral and superior, middle and inferior frontal gyri were only present for dancers but not for novices.

Average ISCs with choreographer (H2)

We computed ISCs between both groups of spectators and the choreographer as an indirect measure of sharing the choreographer’s perspective when viewing the dance video (Fig. 4, Table 2)3,18,37.

Fig. 4.

Fig. 4

Regions of significant ISCs between choreographer and dancers (top) and choreographer and novices (bottom). Clusters derived using TFCE and a threshold of p < 0.05, FWE corrected. The blue clusters represent areas where ISCs display significant differences between the groups (dancers > novices, p < 0.01 uncorrected).

Table 2.

Cluster locations of the summative ISCs for Dancers and Novices with the Choreographer.

Cluster region Cluster extent (# voxels) MNI coordinates (mm) p-value
x y z
Dancers and choreographer
Lateral occipital cortex 6612a  − 42  − 72 0  < 0.001
Superior temporal gyrus 5483a 62  − 10  − 2  < 0.001
Superior temporal gyrus 2442a  − 50  − 40 10  < 0.001
Inferior parietal lobule 836a 38  − 40 54  < 0.001
Inferior frontal gyrus 273a 38 12 20 0.006
Inferior parietal lobule 142  − 34  − 42 54 0.004
Precentral gyrus 76  − 34  − 8 50 0.005
Middle frontal gyrus 68 28  − 4 46 0.003
Superior occipital gyrus 35 36  − 72 34 0.015
Precuneus 20 22  − 66 26 0.028
Posterior cingulate gyrus 13  − 10  − 22 38 0.006
Novices and choreographer
Occipital fusiform gyrus 2890a  − 18  − 74  − 16  < 0.001
Superior temporal gyrus 2691a 46  − 30 6  < 0.001
Superior temporal gyrus 939a  − 42  − 22  − 2  < 0.001
Lateral occipital cortex 133  − 48  − 68  − 2 0.003
Precuneus 86 4  − 60 54 0.011
Middle frontal gyrus 29 24  − 6 46 0.01
Lateral occipital cortex 22  − 48  − 72  − 16 0.01
Dancers and choreographer > novices and choreographer (p < 0.001 uncorrected)
Superior temporal gyrus 204a  − 66  − 22 6  < 0.001
Superior temporal gyrus 194a 44  − 36  − 2  < 0.001
Inferior frontal gyrus 56 68  − 48 2  < 0.001
Precuneus 41 50 14 8  < 0.001
Cerebellum 19 48  − 72 40  < 0.001
Inferior temporal gyrus 16  − 34  − 80  − 50  < 0.001
Fusiform 12 60  − 18  − 28  < 0.001

Clusters were defined using the FSL cluster tool. ISC maps were thresholded at p < 0.05 (FWE corrected) and a cluster extent threshold of 96 mm3 applied.

aLocal maxima were found for these clusters using a 20 mm separation threshold (see supplementary results, S3).

The pattern of shared brain activations for both dancers and novices with the choreographer resembles that of the basic ISC analyses for dancers and novices. For both groups, ISCs with the choreographer are strongest across right the superior temporal and middle temporal gyri. Smaller clusters of activation are located bilaterally in the lingual gyri, the right cuneus, and the right superior parietal sulcus. The pattern of activations is similar between dancers and novices, but ISCs between dancers and the choreographer are consistently larger than between novices and the choreographer. In case of the frontal activations, dancers show bilateral activations of motor cortices, whereas in novices these activations are typically limited to the right hemisphere. A direct comparison between dancers and novices, reveals significantly greater similarity (p < 0.001, uncorrected) bilaterally in STG and MTG, the right inferior frontal gyrus, the right fusiform gyrus and the precuneus, but these activations do not survive FDR correction. A full table of subpeaks is available in the supplementary material, S3).

Dynamic ISCs in relation to performance features and continuous ratings (H3)

We computed a regression analysis of dynamic ISCs and the average time series of both the performers movements, (movement acceleration and synchrony) and features of the videos (motion and sound energy), see Fig. 5. For both groups, synchrony emerged as the best predictor of ISCs, explaining approximately 10% of changes in ISCs. Accordingly, brain synchrony among both dancers and novices was most pronounced if dance performers moved in synchrony. Whereas ISCs among dancers were primarily related to synchrony only, ISCs among novices were better explained by a combination of synchrony and movement acceleration. This is particularly true for the brain areas encompassing the audiovisual ROI including the bilateral superior temporal gyri and the lingual and middle occipital gyri. Among novices, movement acceleration was a stronger predictor of ISCs in the evaluation ROI, including the ventromedial prefrontal cortex and the posterior and anterior cingulate cortices. In dancers, synchrony was a better predictor of ISCs overall, with stronger relationships across all three ROIs including in pre-motor and motor areas as well anterior and posterior cingulate cortices and dorsomedial prefrontal and orbitofrontal cortices (see supplementary materials for PEs for specific BAs). Both motion and sound energy were only weakly positive or negatively correlated with ISCs in both groups.

Fig. 5.

Fig. 5

Relationship between dynamic ISCs and performance features. Left: cortical regions where ISCs are significantly associated with different performance features (Acc, performer acceleration; Vis, video motion; Syn, performer synchrony; Sou, video sound). Z-statistic images were thresholded using clusters determined by z > 3.1 and a corrected cluster significance threshold of p < 0.05. Right: Mean parameter estimates derived from three ROIs for the four performance features (red bars, dancers; blue bars, novices). See supplementary material for individual BAs.

Finally, we used windowed CRQA to compute the change over time of the similarity between groups’ continuous enjoyment ratings. We regressed this measure onto the similarity of brain activations within each group (see Fig. 6). For both audiovisual integration and action observation ROIs, similarity among dancers’ enjoyment was a better predictor of ISCs than similarity among the ratings of novices, particularly along the superior temporal gyrus and the middle temporal gyrus with two prominent subpeaks in these areas in both groups. In contrast, the ROI for stimulus evaluation revealed the anticipated pattern of group specificity, albeit with relatively small PEs: similarity of continuous enjoyment among dancers was a better predictor of similarity of brain active activations among dancers, whereas similarity of continuous enjoyment among novices was a better predictor of ISCs among novices. Group specificity was particularly pronounced in the anterior and posterior cingulate cortices (BA31, BA 32 and BA34) and the orbitofrontal cortex (BA10 and BA 47), see supplementary material for PEs for specific BAs.

Fig. 6.

Fig. 6

ISCs and CRQA of continuous enjoyment ratings. All performance measures were orthogonalized and convolved with a Gaussian function, spatial smoothing: 5 mm, T-contrasts, cluster threshold, z = 2.3, p < 0.05, FDR corrected. D-D refers to the similarity of enjoyment ratings among dancers, N–N refers to the similarity of enjoyment ratings among novices. See supplementary material for individual BAs.

Discussion

Evolutionary biology argues that dance and music have evolved as social signalling systems, facilitating nonverbal communication between groups2,21,56. To test this hypothesis, we conducted an fMRI experiment to study whether behavioural and neural synchrony among people watching dance can be linked to movement synchrony among people performing dance, and to what extent behavioural and brain synchrony engagement and felt understanding depend on expertise in dance.

At the behavioural level we show that the dancers’ continuous enjoyment ratings are more similar to each other than the novices’ enjoyment ratings. Dancers’ ratings are also more similar to those of the choreographer. The continuous enjoyment ratings therefore provide clear evidence for a common aesthetic experience among dancers and among dancers and the choreographer38. Greater similarity of continuous ratings among dancers is accompanied but higher ratings of felt understanding of the dance video. Arguably, a common experience among dancers relates to shared knowledge36, in this case the choreographic principles and movement practices of contemporary performing dance. This shared knowledge also helps dancers to better understand the artistic perspective of the choreographer18 even if the specific dance work has not been experienced before. Somewhat paradoxically, our results suggest that dance expertise reduces the subjective variability of the aesthetic experience of dance. More generally, our findings suggest that expertise with an art form can reduce otherwise large individual differences of aesthetic judgement14,57 and of brain activations related to aesthetic judgement15,16.

Our fMRI findings revealed similar expertise effects at the neural level and show that enjoyment ratings are mediated by distinct dynamic features of the dance video. Both dancers and novices show highest ISCs in brain areas associated with audio-visual integration and action observation along the STS and the extra-striate body area, consistent with prior research on dance observation using fMRI and ISCs2325. However, two important group differences emerge from our analyses. Firstly, only dancers show consistent bilateral activations of premotor and motor cortices, suggesting greater sensorimotor familiarity with the observed movements24,31,32,35,58. Secondly, although synchrony is the strongest predictor of ISCs in both groups, ISCs among novices are more strongly driven by the overall amount of movement of the group of performers59,60. Arguably, dancers more consistently focus on the relationships between performers rather than how much they are dancing21. Importantly, visual change and sound extracted from the video were only weakly or negatively correlated with ISCs among dancers and novices, in line with findings using narrative stimuli in which ISCs are better explained by higher level narrative rather than basic sensory features of the video11. Unsurprisingly, kinematics of the dancers’ movements are better captured by the accelerometers (3D) worn during the performance than the offline motion analysis of the video (2D), and our findings support previous research showing that changes in movement speed and synchrony are a key component of dance aesthetics30,6164.

Finally, combining behavioural and neural measures our study provides insights into the question whether brain synchrony is primarily driven by stimulus features or whether it reflects a common experience65,66. Behavioural synchrony among dancers was the best predictor of brain synchrony in brain areas for audio-visual integration and for action observation across both groups of participants. One way to explain this seemingly paradoxical finding is that the dancers’ ratings tracked the video more reliably than the novices’ ratings which were less consistent over time. Therefore, ISCs in posterior brain areas primarily processing the movements and sounds of the performers were best explained by the dancers’ ratings because their ratings are more reliable and more accurately track the movement features in the video. Only frontal brain areas typically associated with stimulus evaluation (pre-, orbitofrontal and cingulate cortices) exhibited a group-specific pattern, in keeping with greater sensitivity of orbitofrontal and medial frontal cortices to subjective experience and self-referential processing in the default mode network12,15,16,25,67.

There are several limitations to our study. Firstly, the sample size of individual groups is relatively small. However, the close correspondence between behavioural and neural measures and reliable peak activations across both groups strongly indicate that group differences in brain activations reflect meaningful differences in subjective experience. Secondly, we identified ROIs broadly according Brodmann areas rather than using a localiser approach, due to the already long duration of the experiment. Overall, analysing our fMRI data in this conservative way is likely to reduce any potential group differences rather than amplify them. Inter-subject correlations in the medial frontal cortex in particular are highly variable across individuals12, yet our between-subject design is able to identify meaningful relationships between aesthetic judgement and ISCs in these areas.

In sum, our findings provide strong evidence for a common experience between performers and spectators of a dance performance and—additionally—the choreographer of the dance performance in keeping with the importance of nonverbal communication for the aesthetic appreciation of dance3,37. Expertise with dance facilitates this common experience36,38 which—in the case of dance—involves not only visual but also motor areas of the brain. More generally, our findings support the idea that ISCs measured among individual viewers in the scanner indeed reflect a common experience rather than merely watching the same stimulus, as the strength of the ISCs depends on prior experience with the art form and—for the dancers—the ability to better share the choreographers’ artistic perspective when viewing the work.

Supplementary Information

Supplementary Information. (164.9KB, docx)

Acknowledgements

The authors would like to thank all performers of Group Study for making this study possible: Ben Ash, Andrea Buckley, A de la Fe, Kip Johnson, Michael Kitchin, Evangelina Kolyra, Annie Lok, Robert Vesty, Rosalie Wahlfrid and Sara Wookey. This research was funded by an ESCR transformative research grant on synchronous movement cooperation and the performing arts (ES/M000680/2) awarded to GO and DCR. GO, MS and DCR received additional funding from the European Research Council (ERC) under the European Union’s Horizon 2020 research and innovation programme (grant agreement No. 864420—Neurolive).

Author contributions

GO, ALW and DCR wrote the manuscript. GO, SV, ALW designed the experiment. MS choreographed, produced and staged Group Study. SV and ALW collected the data. ALW, DCR, SV and GO analysed the data. All authors reviewed and edited the manuscript.

Data availability

The data supporting this study are openly available at https://openneuro.org/datasets/ds004783. The dance video shown in the scanner is available at https://research.gold.ac.uk/id/eprint/34238/.

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing interests.

Footnotes

Publisher’s note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary Information

The online version contains supplementary material available at 10.1038/s41598-024-73438-0.

References

  • 1.Fink, B., Bläsing, B., Ravignani, A. & Shackelford, T. K. Evolution and functions of human dance. Evol. Hum. Behav.42, 351–360 (2021). [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Grau, A. Why people dance—evolution, sociality and dance. Dance Mov. Spiritual.2, 233–254 (2015). [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Hanna, J. L. The Performer-Audience Connection: Emotion to Metaphor in Dance and Society (University of Texas Press, 1983). [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Savage, P. E., Brown, S., Sakai, E. & Currie, T. E. Statistical universals reveal the structures and functions of human music. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci.112, 8987–8992 (2015). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Hanich, J. Audience effect: On the Collective Cinema Experience (Edinburgh University Press, 2019). [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Nummenmaa, L. et al. Emotions promote social interaction by synchronizing brain activity across individuals. PNAS109, 9599–9604 (2012). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Bachrach, A., Fontbonne, Y., Joufflineau, C. & Ulloa, J. L. Audience entrainment during live contemporary dance performance: Physiological and cognitive measures. Front. Hum. Neurosci.9, (2015). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 8.Konvalinka, I. et al. Synchronized arousal between performers and related spectators in a fire-walking ritual. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci.108, 8514–8519 (2011). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Swarbrick, D. et al. How Live Music Moves Us: Head Movement Differences in Audiences to Live Versus Recorded Music. Front. Psychol.9, (2019). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 10.Hasson, U., Furman, O., Clark, D., Dudai, Y. & Davachi, L. Enhanced intersubject correlations during movie viewing correlate with successful episodic encoding. Neuron57, 452–462 (2008). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Song, H., Finn, E. S. & Rosenberg, M. D. Neural signatures of attentional engagement during narratives and its consequences for event memory. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci.118, e2021905118 (2021). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Chang, L. J. et al. Endogenous variation in ventromedial prefrontal cortex state dynamics during naturalistic viewing reflects affective experience. Sci. Adv.7, eabf7129 (2021). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Dmochowski, J. P. et al. Audience preferences are predicted by temporal reliability of neural processing. Nat. Commun.5, 4567 (2014). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Isik, A. I. & Vessel, E. A. Continuous ratings of movie watching reveal idiosyncratic dynamics of aesthetic enjoyment. PLOS ONE14, e0223896 (2019). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Vessel, E. A., Isik, A. I., Belfi, A. M., Stahl, J. L. & Starr, G. G. The default-mode network represents aesthetic appeal that generalizes across visual domains. PNAS116, 19155–19164 (2019). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Chikazoe, J., Lee, D. H., Kriegeskorte, N. & Anderson, A. K. Population coding of affect across stimuli, modalities and individuals. Nat. Neurosci.17, 1114–1122 (2014). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Schmälzle, R., Häcker, F. E. K., Honey, C. J. & Hasson, U. Engaged listeners: Shared neural processing of powerful political speeches. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci.10, 1137–1143 (2015). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Lahnakoski, J. M. et al. Synchronous brain activity across individuals underlies shared psychological perspectives. Neuroimage100, 316–324 (2014). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Eskenazi, T., Rueschemeyer, S.-A., de Lange, F. P., Knoblich, G. & Sebanz, N. Neural correlates of observing joint actions with shared intentions. Cortex70, 90–100 (2015). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.von Zimmermann, J., Vicary, S., Sperling, M., Orgs, G. & Richardson, D. C. The choreography of group affiliation. Top. Cognit. Sci.10, 80–94 (2018). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Vicary, S., Sperling, M., von Zimmermann, J., Richardson, D. C. & Orgs, G. Joint action aesthetics. PLOS ONE12, e0180101 (2017). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Herbec, A., Kauppi, J.-P., Jola, C., Tohka, J. & Pollick, F. E. Differences in fMRI intersubject correlation while viewing unedited and edited videos of dance performance. Cortex71, 341–348 (2015). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Jola, C. et al. Uni- and multisensory brain areas are synchronised across spectators when watching unedited dance recordings. Iperception4, 265–284 (2013). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Reason, M. et al. Spectators’ aesthetic experience of sound and movement in dance performance: A transdisciplinary investigation. Psychol. Aesthet. Creat. Arts10, 42–55 (2016). [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Pollick, F. E. et al. Chapter 17 - Exploring collective experience in watching dance through intersubject correlation and functional connectivity of fMRI brain activity. in Progress in Brain Research (eds. Christensen, J. F. & Gomila, A.) vol. 237 373–397 (Elsevier, 2018). [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 26.Kawabata, H. & Zeki, S. Neural correlates of beauty. J. Neurophysiol.91, 1699–1705 (2004). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Vessel, E. A., Starr, G. G. & Rubin, N. The brain on art: intense aesthetic experience activates the default mode network. Front. Human Neurosci.6, (2012). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 28.Kim, S.-G., Mueller, K., Lepsien, J., Mildner, T. & Fritz, T. H. Brain networks underlying aesthetic appreciation as modulated by interaction of the spectral and temporal organisations of music. Sci. Rep.9, 19446 (2019). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Salimpoor, V. N., Zald, D. H., Zatorre, R. J., Dagher, A. & McIntosh, A. R. Predictions and the brain: How musical sounds become rewarding. Trends Cognit. Sci.19, 86–91 (2015). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Orlandi, A., Cross, E. S. & Orgs, G. Timing is everything: Dance aesthetics depend on the complexity of movement kinematics. Cognition205, 104446 (2020). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Calvo-Merino, B., Grèzes, J., Glaser, D. E., Passingham, R. E. & Haggard, P. Seeing or doing? Influence of visual and motor familiarity in action observation. Curr. Biol.16, 1905–1910 (2006). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Cross, E. S., Kraemer, D. J. M., de Hamilton, A. F. C., Kelley, W. M. & Grafton, S. T. Sensitivity of the action observation network to physical and observational learning. Cereb. Cortex19, 315–326 (2009). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Orgs, G., Dombrowski, J.-H., Heil, M. & Jansen-Osmann, P. Expertise in dance modulates alphabeta event-related desynchronization during action observation. Eur. J. Neurosci.27, 3380–3384 (2008). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Orlandi, A., Zani, A. & Proverbio, A. M. Dance expertise modulates visual sensitivity to complex biological movements. Neuropsychologia104, 168–181 (2017). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Kirsch, L. P., Dawson, K. & Cross, E. S. Dance experience sculpts aesthetic perception and related brain circuits. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci.1337, 130–139 (2015). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Hayn-Leichsenring, G. U., Vartanian, O. & Chatterjee, A. The role of expertise in the aesthetic evaluation of mathematical equations. Psychol. Res.10.1007/s00426-021-01592-5 (2021). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Orgs, G. You move , I watch , it matters: Aesthetic communication in dance. in Shared Representations: Sensorimotor foundations of Social Life (eds. Sukhvinder, S. O. & Cross, E. S.) (Cambridge University Press, 2016). 10.13140/RG.2.1.1136.0088.
  • 38.Pelowski, M., Specker, E., Gerger, G., Leder, H. & Weingarden, L. S. Do you feel like I do? A study of spontaneous and deliberate emotion sharing and understanding between artists and perceivers of installation art. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts No Pagination Specified-No Pagination Specified, 10.1037/aca0000201, (2018).
  • 39.Pajula, J. & Tohka, J. How many is enough? Effect of sample size in inter-subject correlation analysis of fMRI. Comput. Intell. Neurosci.2016, 2094601 (2016). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Nicholls, M. E. R., Thomas, N. A., Loetscher, T. & Grimshaw, G. M. The Flinders Handedness survey (FLANDERS): A brief measure of skilled hand preference. Cortex49, 2914–2926 (2013). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Garbarino, M., Lai, M., Bender, D., Picard, R. W. & Tognetti, S. Empatica E3—A wearable wireless multi-sensor device for real-time computerized biofeedback and data acquisition. in 39–42 (IEEE, 2014).
  • 42.Dale, R. & Spivey, M. J. Unraveling the dyad: Using recurrence analysis to explore patterns of syntactic coordination between children and caregivers in conversation. Lang. Learn.56, 391–430 (2006). [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Kirsch, L. P., Drommelschmidt, K. A. & Cross, E. S. The impact of sensorimotor experience on affective evaluation of dance. Front. Human Neurosci.7, 521 (2013). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Lartillot, O., Toiviainen, P. & Eerola, T. A Matlab toolbox for music information retrieval. in Data Analysis, Machine Learning and Applications (eds. Preisach, C., Burkhardt, H., Schmidt-Thieme, L. & Decker, R.) 261–268 (Springer, Berlin, Heidelberg, 2008). 10.1007/978-3-540-78246-9_31.
  • 45.Mathôt, S., Schreij, D. & Theeuwes, J. OpenSesame: An open-source, graphical experiment builder for the social sciences. Behav. Res. Methods44, 314–324 (2012). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Fusaroli, R., Konvalinka, I. & Wallot, S. Analyzing Social Interactions: The promises and challenges of using cross recurrence quantification analysis. in Translational Recurrences (eds. Marwan, N., Riley, M., Giuliani, A. & Webber, Jr., Charles L.) 137–155 (Springer International Publishing, Cham, 2014). 10.1007/978-3-319-09531-8_9.
  • 47.Coco, M. I. & Dale, R. Cross-recurrence quantification analysis of categorical and continuous time series: An R package. Front. Psychol.5, (2014). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 48.Jenkinson, M., Bannister, P., Brady, M. & Smith, S. Improved optimization for the robust and accurate linear registration and motion correction of brain images. NeuroImage17, 825–841 (2002). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Smith, S. M. Fast robust automated brain extraction. Hum. Brain Mapp.17, 143–155 (2002). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Grosbras, M.-H., Beaton, S. & Eickhoff, S. B. Brain regions involved in human movement perception: A quantitative voxel-based meta-analysis. Human Brain Mapp.33, 431–454 (2012). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Maldjian, J. A., Laurienti, P. J., Kraft, R. A. & Burdette, J. H. An automated method for neuroanatomic and cytoarchitectonic atlas-based interrogation of fMRI data sets. Neuroimage19, 1233–1239 (2003). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Kauppi, J.-P., Pajula, J. & Tohka, J. A versatile software package for inter-subject correlation based analyses of fMRI. Front. Neuroinform.8, (2014). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 53.Kauppi, J.-P., Jääskeläinen, I. P., Sams, M. & Tohka, J. Inter-subject correlation of brain hemodynamic responses during watching a movie: Localization in space and frequency. Front. Neuroinformatics4, (2010). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 54.Benjamini, Y. & Hochberg, Y. Controlling the false discovery rate: A practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. J. Royal Stat. Soc.: Ser. B57, 289–300 (1995). [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Winkler, A. M., Ridgway, G. R., Webster, M. A., Smith, S. M. & Nichols, T. E. Permutation inference for the general linear model. NeuroImage92, 381–397 (2014). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Hagen, E. H. & Bryant, G. A. Music and dance as a coalition signaling system. Human Nat.14, 21–51 (2003). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Vessel, E. A., Maurer, N., Denker, A. H. & Starr, G. G. Stronger shared taste for natural aesthetic domains than for artifacts of human culture. Cognition179, 121–131 (2018). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Calvo-Merino, B., Glaser, D. E., Grèzes, J., Passingham, R. E. & Haggard, P. Action observation and acquired motor skills: An fMRI study with expert dancers. Cereb. Cortex15, 1243–1249 (2005). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Calvo-Merino, B., Jola, C., Glaser, D. E. & Haggard, P. Towards a sensorimotor aesthetics of performing art. Conscious. Cognit.17, 911–922 (2008). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Orlandi, A., Cross, E. S. & Orgs, G. Timing is everything: Aesthetic perception of movement kinematics in dance. 10.31219/osf.io/gu2nh, (2019).
  • 61.Chen, Y.-C., Pollick, F. & Lu, H. Aesthetic preferences for causality in biological movements arise from visual processes. Psychon. Bull. Rev.10.3758/s13423-022-02106-z (2022). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Christensen, J. F., Pollick, F. E., Lambrechts, A. & Gomila, A. Affective responses to dance. Acta Psychologica168, 91–105 (2016). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Cracco, E. et al. EEG frequency tagging reveals the integration of form and motion cues into the perception of group movement. Cerebral Cortex10.1093/cercor/bhab385 (2021). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Pollick, F. E., Paterson, H. M., Bruderlin, A. & Sanford, A. J. Perceiving affect from arm movement. Cognition82, B51-61 (2001). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.de Hamilton, A. F. C. Hyperscanning: Beyond the Hype. Neuron109, 404–407 (2021). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Novembre, G. & Iannetti, G. D. Hyperscanning alone cannot prove causality. Multibrain stimulation can. Trends Cognit. Sci.25, 96–99 (2021). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Jang, S. H. & Pollick, F. E. Experience influences brain mechanisms of watching dance. Dance Res.29, 352–377 (2011). [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Supplementary Information. (164.9KB, docx)

Data Availability Statement

The data supporting this study are openly available at https://openneuro.org/datasets/ds004783. The dance video shown in the scanner is available at https://research.gold.ac.uk/id/eprint/34238/.


Articles from Scientific Reports are provided here courtesy of Nature Publishing Group

RESOURCES