Skip to main content

This is a preprint.

It has not yet been peer reviewed by a journal.

The National Library of Medicine is running a pilot to include preprints that result from research funded by NIH in PMC and PubMed.

medRxiv logoLink to medRxiv
[Preprint]. 2024 Sep 18:2024.09.17.24313838. [Version 1] doi: 10.1101/2024.09.17.24313838

Executive Function and Adherence in Children and Adolescents Living with HIV: Evidence from the HIV-associated Neurocognitive Disorders in Zambia (HANDZ) Study

Sylvia Mwanza-Kabaghe 1, Kristen Sportiello 3, Mina Shah 3, Heather R Adams 2, Esau G Mbewe 1, Pelekelo P Kabundula 1, Colleen Schneider 3, Milimo Mweemba 4, Gretchen L Birbeck 6,7, David R Bearden 1,2
PMCID: PMC11451720  PMID: 39371114

Abstract

Introduction:

Executive function (EF) may be impaired in people with human immunodeficiency virus (HIV) infection, and poor EF may affect medication adherence. However, there is little data on EF in children with HIV in sub-Saharan Africa.

Methods:

208 children/adolescents with perinatally acquired HIV and 208 HIV-exposed uninfected controls were recruited in Zambia for this prospective cohort study. EF was measured using performance-based, self-report, and parental report measures. Adherence over one year of follow-up was assessed through questionnaires and viral load measurement.

Results:

Children with HIV performed significantly worse on all three measures of EF. Lower parental rating of EF was associated with poorer antiretroviral therapy adherence (OR: 1.5, 95% CI = 1.02 – 2.2, p = 0.04).

Conclusion:

Children with HIV have EF impairments which may lead to consequences like poor medication adherence and treatment failure. Interventions to improve EF or compensate for impaired EF may be necessary in this population.

Keywords: HIV, children, Africa, executive function, cognitive function, adherence

Introduction

Executive function (EF) includes a suite of cognitive skills including attention, planning, and behaviour control which are critical for educational and professional success, as well as facilitating activities such as medication adherence.14 A substantial body of research has demonstrated impairment in executive function in people living with HIV,532 although the precise mechanism for this impairment and the extent to which it affects function is unknown. With the widespread deployment of antiretroviral therapy (ART), children and adolescents with perinatally acquired HIV are now surviving into adulthood.3335 Because of this, the focus of care for children with HIV has shifted to treatment adherence, quality of life, and disease-associated morbidities such as cognitive impairment and psychiatric disorders.3336

Multiple prior studies have demonstrated that cognitive impairment is common in children with HIV infection.3338 With widespread use of ART, the major burden of cognitive impairment has shifted away from progressive encephalopathy toward milder forms of cognitive impairment.35, 36, 39 Of the numerous cognitive domains affected by HIV, executive function (EF) has been found to be one of the most commonly affected domains in previous studies.6, 4043 However, there is little data on executive function in children living with HIV in sub-Saharan Africa (where the majority of children living with HIV reside), and few published studies have evaluated whether executive function deficits in children living with HIV are clinically significant.19, 35

Prior studies in adults have demonstrated a relationship between poor executive function and poor ART adherence.2, 4450 However, this relationship has been evaluated in only one pediatric study,51 which had inconclusive results. Taking ART as prescribed is a crucial skill for children and adolescents living with HIV to develop. 27, 52, 53 ART adherence is especially important for people living in sub-Saharan Africa because of limited availability of third-line and salvage ART regimens.34, 35, 52, 5456 Adherence requires the recruitment of executive function because consistently taking medications involves developing and implementing a plan to adhere, remembering to adhere, trading immediate benefits for future gains, and developing strategies for adherence in the face of potential barriers.2, 46, 47 In younger children, parents or other relatives may supply some of these functions, but as children age into adolescence and adulthood, they are required to bear greater responsibility for adherence on their own.

The aim of the current study was to examine executive function among children and adolescents living with HIV compared to a control group of HIV-exposed uninfected (HEU) children and adolescents in Zambia. We hypothesized that both neuropsychological performance-based measures of executive function and parental ratings of executive function would be worse in participants living with HIV than in HEU controls. We further hypothesized that poor executive function measures at baseline would predict poor adherence over the subsequent one-year period.

Patients and Methods

Methods for the HANDZ study, which involves various analyses including those in this paper, have previously been described. 5761 Briefly, children and adolescents living with HIV and a control group of HIV-exposed uninfected (HEU) children and adolescents were recruited in a prospective cohort study. Participants living with HIV were recruited from the Pediatric HIV/AIDS Center of Excellence (PCOE, a referral center for children living with HIV located in Lusaka, Zambia) during routine medication refill visits. Controls were recruited from the community by a trained community health worker. All participants were recruited from 2017-2018 and were 8-17 years old at time of enrollment. Participants were evaluated every three months after enrollment. We report results here from the baseline visit and the first year of follow-up.

Inclusion/Exclusion Criteria

Children and adolescents living with HIV were excluded if they had not been receiving ART for at least one year prior to enrollment. Children and adolescents not living with HIV were included only if their mother had been infected with HIV during or prior to their pregnancy. Any potential participant with a history of central nervous system opportunistic infection or a chronic medical or psychiatric condition that would preclude participation in the study was excluded.

Data Collected

Demographic and medical history information was collected using participant and parent interviews and chart review of electronic and paper medical records. Socioeconomic status (SES) was evaluated using an adapted version of the Multiple Indicator Cluster Survey-5 (MICS5, http://mics.unicef.org/tools). Measures of socioeconomic status were combined into a socioeconomic status index (SESI; see Mbewe et al., 2022 for details 62). Nutritional Status was assessed by transcribing participants’ growth curves from their medical records onto a standard WHO growth curve and then prospectively plotting growth at subsequent visits. Severe malnutrition, wasting, and stunting were defined according to standard WHO definitions.63

HIV History:

For participants living with HIV, HIV history was obtained via participant and parent interview and through chart review. Measures of current and historical HIV disease severity (current and lowest recorded CD4 counts, current and worst recorded WHO stage, and number of previous hospitalizations) were combined into a disease severity index.

Adverse Life Events:

A ten question survey developed in Zambia regarding adverse life events over the prior 12 months was administered to each participant. The survey includes questions related to illness and hospitalization of family members, change of residence, violence and abuse, and other negative life events. The survey generates an adverse life events score of 0-10, with higher scores indicating a greater number of adverse life events.

Executive Function:

Full details of neuropsychological testing in the HANDZ study have previously been reported. 57 Executive function was assessed through a combination of performance-based neuropsychological tests and participant and parent interviews. Parent interviews were used to complete the Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function, Second Edition, Parent Report (BRIEF2-PR). 64 The BRIEF is a standardized age-normed inventory for the assessment of executive function which has been widely utilized in studies in Zambia and elsewhere in sub-Saharan Africa. 7, 65 Parent reports were utilized regardless of participant age. Age-adjusted T-scores for BRIEF subscales including the Emotional Regulation Index, Behavior Regulation Index, and Cognitive Regulation Index were computed, with higher scores indicating worse performance. A Z-score transformation of the BRIEF executive composite was used in logistic regression models for ease of interpretation. In addition, an executive function self-report measure developed by the authors for use in Africa, the Brief Executive Function Inventory (BEFI), was administered to participants.57 BEFI scores have a range of 0-10, with higher scores indicating worse performance. Performance-based neuropsychological testing is summarized in Table 1. Testing was conducted using a combination of standard measures and computerized testing using a custom NIH Toolbox Cognition Battery. 6670 Domain scores from all performance-based tests were combined into a global measure of executive function and transformed into a Z-score referred to as the NP Global Composite Score. Neuropsychological testing was conducted by PhD psychology students for standard measures and by a trained study nurse or research assistant for NIH Toolbox measures.

Table 1:

Performance-based measures of executive function

Domain Test
Set-shifting and cognitive flexibility NIH Toolbox Directional Change Card Sort Test66, 69
Wisconsin Card Sort Test 64 Item Version
DKEFS Trail Making Test Condition 4
Attention/working memory WISC5 Digit Span Total Score
California Verbal Learning Test Trial 1
NIH Toolbox List Sorting Working Memory Test66, 69
Response inhibition Pencil Tapping Test7173
NIH Toolbox Flanker Test66, 69

Adherence and Medication Supervision:

Antiretroviral adherence was assessed at baseline and every 3 months using an established Pediatric HIV/AIDS Cohort Study (PHACS) instrument in addition to pill counts.74 Participants and their parents were asked to rate the participant’s adherence over the last 7 days and last 30 days. For the primary outcome variable, adherence was treated as a dichotomous variable (100% reported adherence vs. less than 100% reported adherence at any point over one year of follow-up). Viral load was assessed at baseline and after one year of follow-up. Participants with a viral load >50 copies/ml had repeat viral load testing scheduled for confirmation. Participants who were virally suppressed at baseline and had viral load >1000 copies/ml at one year were considered to have virologic treatment failure. All participants had standard adherence counseling, with escalation of adherence counseling in those participants with noted adherence issues or detectable viral load. Level of supervision was determined by asking participants to rate their level of supervision when taking ART on a three-point scale (from no supervision to complete supervision of all doses).

Sample Size and Power

A sample size of 200 participants per group was planned based on simulation studies for each aim, with the goal of ensuring model stability and avoiding overfitting for regression models. Participants were overenrolled by 4% to account for potential loss to follow-up.

Statistical Methods

Statistical analyses were performed using Stata 16 (Stata Corp LP, College Station, TX). Comparisons between groups were performed using chi-squared tests for categorical variables, T-tests for normally distributed continuous variables, and log rank tests for non-normally distributed continuous variables. Bivariable and multivariable linear regression models were constructed to evaluate determinants of executive function. Separate models were fit for participants with and without HIV. Logistic regression models were used to evaluate the relationship between executive function and adherence in participants with HIV. Causal models were pre-specified using Directed Acyclic Graphs (DAGs, see Figure 1). Multivariable models included variables prespecified for likely causal importance and indicated by the DAG as appropriate for adjustment. These variables included age, sex, and socioeconomic status index. Receiver operating characteristic (ROC) curves were constructed to investigate the performance characteristics of EF measures in predicting adherence problems (treated as a dichotomous variable) and virologic treatment failure over one year of follow-up for each level of parental supervision. Missing data were treated with pairwise deletion.

Figure 1:

Figure 1:

Directed Acyclic Graph (DAG) diagramming the causal model utilized in the study to estimate the effect of the exposure (HIV status) on the outcome of executive function. According to the DAG, the minimal sufficient adjustment set contains age, sex, parental HIV status, and socioeconomic status.

Ethics Statement

This study was approved by the institutional review boards of the University of Rochester (protocol #00068985), the University of Zambia (reference #004-08-17), and the National Health Research Association of Zambia. Verbal and written informed consent was obtained from a parent for all participants for participation in the study, and verbal and written assent was obtained from all participants aged 12 or older.

Results

Demographics

A total of 416 participants were enrolled in the study. 29 participants were excluded from these analyses due to failure to complete neuropsychological testing, leaving 387 analyzable participants, including 205 participants living with HIV and 182 HEU controls. Participants living with HIV and HEU participants were demographically similar (see Table 2). However, participants living with HIV had slightly higher average socioeconomic status than controls, were more likely to attend school, and were less likely to report difficulty with food security. Additionally, participants with HIV were more likely to have a history of severe malnutrition. A detailed description of demographics and HIV-specific characteristics of this cohort and an explanation for the differences between groups has previously been published.62 All participants with HIV were treated with ART (median time on treatment: 7 years). Most participants with HIV had relatively high CD4 counts (median: 745, IQR: 537-975), and 83% had undetectable viral loads at baseline.

Table 2:

Demographic/clinical characteristics of children living with HIV and controls. HEU: HIV-exposed Uninfected, TB: tuberculosis, IQR: interquartile range, WHO: World Health Organization. All data are presented as mean (SD) or n (%) unless otherwise specified. All p-values calculated using unpaired t-test or chi-squared test.

Participants Living with HIV HEU Participants P-value
Demographics:
   • Age at enrollment 12 (2. 2) 12 (2.7) N/A
   • Male sex (% male) 114 (55%) 87 (47%) 0.15
   • Median socioeconomic status index (IQR) 6 (5-8) 5 (4-7) <.001
   • Years of maternal education 7.6 (3.4) 6.4 (3.5) <.001
Nutritional Status:
   • Low weight for age 74 (36%) 35 (20%) <.001
   • Growth stunting 66 (32%) 32 (18%) 0.001
   • History of severe malnutrition 44 (21%) 5 (2%) 0.001
Child Physical Health:
   • Self-reported poor health 21 (10%) 11 (6%) 0.135
   • History of hospitalization 172 (83%) 55 (27%) <0.001
   • History of TB 70 (33%) 0 (0%) <0.001
Education:
   • In school 193 (92%) 176 (85%) .03
   • In private school 100 (52%) 57 (37%) 0.01
HIV-specific Factors:
   • Current CD4 count 772 (348) N/A N/A
   • Nadir CD4 551 (347) N/A N/A
   • History of CD4 count below 200 17 (8%) N/A N/A
   • History of WHO stage 4 90 (43%) N/A N/A
   • History of opportunistic infection other than TB 16 (8%) N/A N/A
   • Undetectable viral load at enrollment 173 (83%) N/A N/A
   • Age at ART initiation <1 year: 23 (11%)
1-2 years: 33 (16%)
2-5 years: 70 (34%)
5-10 years: 56 (27%)
> 10 years: 26 (13%)
N/A N/A

Executive Function in Participants Living with HIV Compared to HEU Controls and Comparison of Measures

Participants living with HIV performed significantly worse than HEU participants on all measures of executive function (see Table 3 and Figures 2a and 2b). These differences persisted after adjusting for age, sex, and socioeconomic status index (adjusted beta coefficient for BRIEF global executive composite: 2.6, 95% CI: 0.91 - 4.3, p = 0.003; adjusted beta coefficient for NP global composite: −0.52, 95% CI: −0.72 - −0.31, p < 0.001). Participants living with HIV also rated themselves as having poorer executive function on the BEFI (mean score: 2.3 vs. 1.7, p=0.004, adjusted beta coefficient: 0.66, 95% CI: 0.3 - 1.1, p = 0.002). Results from the BRIEF were mildly correlated with results from performance-based measures (Pearson correlation between BRIEF global executive composite and NP global composite (r) = −0.28, p<0.001).

Table 3:

Executive function measures by HIV status

Measure Participants Living with HIV HIV-exposed Uninfected Participants P-value
BRIEF measures (higher scores indicate worse performance)
Behavior regulation 49.5 (48.2-50.8) 46.7 (45.4-47.9) 0.002
Emotional regulation 53.7 (52.4-54.9) 49.8 (49.0-51.0) <0.001
Cognitive regulation 48.9 (47.9-49.8) 46.9 (45.8-48.1) 0.01
Global executive composite 50.8 (49.5-51.6) 47.8 (46.6-49.0) <0.001
Performance-based measures (higher scores indicate better performance)
Attention/working memory index −0.17 (−0.30, 0.03) 0.18 (0.04-0.32) <0.001
Set shifting −0.17 (−0.30, −0.04) 0.19 (0.04-0.34) <0.001
Response inhibition −0.12 (−0.26-0.02) 0.13 (0-0.27) 0.01
NP global composite −0.20 (−0.34, −0.6) 0.22 (0.08, 0.35) <0.001

Figure 2:

Figure 2:

2a) BRIEF subscale scores by HIV status, with participants living with HIV demonstrating worse performance (higher scores) on all subscales. 2b) Performance-based test scores for multiple domains of executive function, with children living with HIV demonstrating worse performance (lower scores) than HIV-exposed uninfected controls.

Determinants of Executive Function in Participants Living with HIV

Separate bivariable and multivariable regression models were fit for the BRIEF global executive composite and the performance-based NP global composite scores. In the bivariable analyses for participants living with HIV, executive function was significantly associated with measures of nutritional status, socioeconomic status, and measures of HIV disease severity for both BRIEF and NP measures (see Table 4). Negative life events and younger age were associated with the BRIEF score but not with the NP global composite score. Sex was not associated with executive function on either measure.

Table 4:

Determinants of executive function in participants living with HIV determined using bivariable linear regression

Variable β Coefficient (95% CI) P-value
BRIEF Global Executive Composite
Age −0.38 (−0.69, −0.069) 0.01*
Sex 0.7 (−0.91,−2.30) 0.34
Socioeconomic Status Index −0.38 (−0.71, −0.05) 0.03
Hx of Severe Malnutrition 4.1 (1.7, 6.6) 0.001*
Growth Stunting 2.8 (1.0, 4.7) 0.002*
History of WHO Stage 4 2.0 (−0.23, 4.3) 0.08
History of CD4 Count <200 4.4 (0.30, 8.5) 0.04*
HIV Illness Severity Index 1.1 (0.11, 2.1) 0.03*
Negative Life Event Index 0.55 (0.03, 1.1) 0.04*
NP Global Composite
Age 0.02 (−0.02, 0.06) 0.32
Sex 0.07 (−0.13,0.28) 0.47
Socioeconomic Status Index 0.08 (0.04, 0.12) <0.001*
Hx of Severe Malnutrition −0.38 (−0.69, −0.08) 0.02*
Growth Stunting −0.59 (−0.81, −0.36) <0.001*
History of WHO Stage 4 −0.33 (−0.62,−0.04) 0.03*
History of CD4 Count <200 −0.72 (−1.2, −0.21) 0.006 *
HIV Illness Severity Index −0.18 (−0.31,−0.05) 0.006*
Negative Life Event Index −.005 (−0.07,−0.06) 0.88

All P-values calculated using bivariable linear regression. P-values ≤ 0.05 are marked with *

For the multivariable models, all variables included in Table 4 were included. Age and sex were not significantly associated with outcomes in any of the final models. In the multivariable model for the BRIEF, SESI and adverse life event index had the strongest associations with EF (for SESI, adjusted β: −0.88, 95% CI: −1.1 - 0.05, p = 0.03; for adverse life event index, adjusted β: 1.2, 95% CI: 0.18 - 2.1, p= 0.02). In the multivariable model for the NP global composite score, growth stunting had the strongest association with EF (adjusted β: −0.38, 95% CI: −0.71 - 0.06, p = 0.02).

Determinants of Executive Function in HEU Participants

For HEU participants, only history of severe malnutrition was significantly associated with the BRIEF global executive composite in the bivariable analysis (β = 10.1, 95% CI: 3.1 - 17.1, p = 0.005). This association remained significant in the multivariable model. For the NP global composite, growth stunting (β = −0.68, p < 0.001) and socioeconomic status (β = 0.11, p = 0.02) were the only variables with significant associations, and both remained significant in the multivariable model.

Executive Function and Adherence

182 participants living with HIV had complete follow-up data at one year. 100% adherence was noted in 143 (79%) participants, while less than 100% adherence was noted in 39 out of the 182 participants (21%). There was no association between level of supervision and adherence. In a multivariable model controlling for SESI, age, and sex, the BRIEF executive composite at baseline predicted adherence problems over one year of follow-up (OR: 1.5, 95% CI: 1.02 - 2.2, p = 0.04). Performance-based and self-report measures of EF were not significantly associated with adherence problems.

In addition, worse executive function was associated with higher levels of parental supervision of adherence (see Figure 3). Participants reporting that parents observed all medication doses (compared to participants reporting at least some independence in taking medications) had higher scores on the BRIEF (mean BRIEF global executive composite: 53.0 vs. 50.0, p = 0.04) and BEFI (BEFI mean score: 2.8 vs. 2.2, p = 0.04) and lower scores on performance-based measures of EF (NP global composite: −0.62 vs. 0.08, p < 0.001). This relationship remained significant for the BRIEF cognitive regulation index and behavior regulation index but not the emotion regulation index and was significant for each individual performance-based measure of EF. ROC curve analysis suggested that all subscales of the BRIEF had moderate ability to predict poor adherence over the subsequent year of follow-up in participants with the lowest level of supervision (AUC 0.68 - 0.72, see Figure 4a). However, this was not the case in patients with moderate or greater levels of supervision (see Figure 4b). Executive was not significantly associated with virologic treatment failure for performance-based, parent-report, or self-report measures.

Figure 3:

Figure 3:

Performance-based measures of executive function by level of parental supervision of medication, demonstrating increasing levels of parental supervision in participants with poorer executive function.

Figure 4:

Figure 4:

Receiver operator characteristic curves demonstrating the performance characteristics of BRIEF subscale scores in predicting adherence problems in participants with the lowest level of parental supervision (4a) and in those with complete parental supervision (4b). All subscales of the BRIEF had moderate ability (AUC: 0.68 to 0.71) to predict adherence problems in participants with minimal supervision but not in more highly supervised participants.

Discussion

We examined the executive function of children living with HIV and not living with HIV as well as the effect of executive function on medication adherence among children with HIV in Lusaka, Zambia. Key findings included poorer performance on all measures of executive function in children living with HIV compared to HIV-exposed uninfected controls. The current study was not designed to investigate mechanisms of executive function impairment, but this EF impairment may be part of the spectrum of HIV-associated neurocognitive impairment. Interestingly, although measures of HIV disease severity were associated with EF, some of the strongest predictors of executive function were nutritional indicators, suggesting that some elements of EF impairment may be preventable with nutritional interventions and prevention of HIV-associated stunting and wasting. We identified a complex relationship between executive function, adherence, and level of parental supervision. Participants with worse executive function were more likely to have increased levels of parental supervision, suggesting that at least in a significant number of participants, poor executive function was compensated for by parents providing additional help with adherence. However, especially in participants with the lowest level of supervision, poor executive function assessed by parental report using the BRIEF predicted poor adherence over the following year. Future studies are necessary to determine whether problems with adherence become more of an issue as adolescents mature into adulthood and have to take additional responsibility for their own adherence, and we plan to address this as the HANDZ study continues.

In general, these findings are in line with most but not all prior studies of executive function in people living with HIV.1219 Notably, a large study conducted in six African countries as part of the IMPAACT 1104 study67 noted deficits in performance-based measures of executive function, but not on parental ratings using the BRIEF. However, there was significant variability in results by site, with some sites reporting significant differences and others reporting no differences. Differences in our study compared to the 1104 study may be related to differences in the control group utilized or to the relative health of the participants living with HIV. While our cohort was relatively healthy, a substantially greater number of participants in our study started antiretroviral therapy relatively late and had a history of WHO stage 4 disease.

Results regarding determinants of EF in our cohort are generally comparable to results from prior studies, although we noted stronger relationships between EF and measures of HIV-specific disease severity than many prior studies did. The finding that socioeconomic status is strongly associated with EF is consistent with prior studies performed in Zambia61,62 and the United States.3 A relatively novel finding of our study is the strong relationship between malnutrition, stunting, and EF.

Our study contributes to findings from the PHACS study,51 which evaluated the relationship between HIV, executive function, and adherence in a U.S. cohort. Like the PHACS investigators, we noted that executive function deficits may be partly compensated for by increased levels of parental supervision.

Limitations, Bias, and Generalizability

One notable limitation is the reliance on self-report and parental report measures of adherence, which have consistently been shown to underestimate rates of poor adherence. In the HANDZ cohort, there is a strong relationship between self-reported poor adherence and detectable viral load, suggesting that self-reported adherence is at least partially accurate, but objective measures of adherence would likely improve the accuracy of these assessments. In addition, given the relatively good rates of adherence noted in our study, we had inadequate power to implement more complex statistical techniques that could better illuminate the relationship between adherence and parental supervision. We plan to address these limitations in future longitudinal analyses after more non-adherence events occur. Additionally, our study was conducted at a single urban center in Zambia, and these findings may not generalize to other areas. In addition, we excluded participants with no available biological parents, and it is likely that the relationship between executive function and adherence is different in participants who are living with non-parent caregivers or in orphanages.

Conclusion

Children and adolescents living with HIV had significantly poorer executive function compared to HIV-exposed uninfected controls. This is likely to be driven by a combination of factors, including social factors, health-related effects of HIV, and effects of HIV on the developing brain. The relationship between executive function and adherence is at least partially modified by parental supervision, as children with poor executive functioning are more likely to be highly supervised. However, especially in participants with the greatest level of independence, parental ratings of executive function were able to predict poor adherence over the subsequent year of follow-up. This finding raises concerns about the future as children age into adulthood and are required to take greater responsibility for medication adherence. Future longitudinal follow-up studies are planned as part of the HANDZ study to further investigate these issues.

Funding Source:

The authors disclosed receipt of the following financial support for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article: this work was supported by the University of Rochester Center for AIDS Research (CFAR), an NIH funded program [grant number: P30 AI 045008]; the McGowan Foundation; and the National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke of the National Institutes of Health [grant numbers: R01NS094037, K23NS117310]. The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the National Institutes of Health.

David Bearden and Milimo Mweemba were supported by an MRC strategic award to establish an International Centre for Genomic Medicine in Neuromuscular Diseases (ICGNMD) MR/S005021/1’.

Footnotes

Competing interests: The authors declare that there is no conflict of interest.

Data Accessibility:

Data is available upon reasonable request from David R. Bearden at david_bearden@urmc.rochester.edu

References

  • 1.Goldberg E. Executive Functions in Health and Disease. Academic Press; 2017. [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Gorman AA, Foley JM, Ettenhofer ML, Hinkin CH, van Gorp WG. Functional consequences of HIV-associated neuropsychological impairment. Neuropsychol Rev. 2009; 19: 186–203. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Last BS, Lawson GM, Breiner K, Steinberg L, Farah MJ. Childhood socioeconomic status and executive function in childhood and beyond. PLoS One. 2018; 13: e0202964. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Malloy-Diniz LF, Miranda DMD, Grassi-Oliveira R. Executive Functions in Psychiatric Disorders. Frontiers Media SA; 2017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Antinori A, Arendt G, Becker JT et al. Updated research nosology for HIV-associated neurocognitive disorders. Neurology. 2007; 69: 1789–1799. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Boivin MJ, Barlow-Mosha L, Chernoff MC et al. Neuropsychological performance in African children with HIV enrolled in a multisite antiretroviral clinical trial. AIDS. 2018; 32: 189–204. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Boivin MJ, Chernoff M, Fairlie L et al. African Multi-Site 2-Year Neuropsychological Study of School-Age Children Perinatally Infected, Exposed, and Unexposed to Human Immunodeficiency Virus. Clin Infect Dis. 2020; 71: e105–e114. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Coulehan K, Byrd D, Arentoft A et al. The role of decision-making ability in HIV/AIDS: impact on prospective memory. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 2014; 36: 730–741. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Ezeamama AE, Kizza FN, Zalwango SK et al. Perinatal HIV Status and Executive Function During School-Age and Adolescence: A Comparative Study of Long-Term Cognitive Capacity Among Children From a High HIV Prevalence Setting. Medicine (Baltimore). 2016; 95: e3438. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Fama R, Sullivan EV, Sassoon SA, Pfefferbaum A, Zahr NM. Impairments in Component Processes of Executive Function and Episodic Memory in Alcoholism, HIV Infection, and HIV Infection with Alcoholism Comorbidity. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2016; 40: 2656–2666. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.González-Andrade A, García-Torres A, Perez-Garcia M, Vergara-Moragues E. Assessment of executive functions as a measure of impairments in everyday functioning in persons with HIV. Psychol Health Med. 2021; 1–7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Haase VG, Nicolau NC, Viana VN, Barreto GV, Pinto JA. Executive function and processing speed in Brazilian HIV-infected children and adolescents. Dement Neuropsychol. 2014; 8: 32–39. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Kanmogne GD, Fonsah JY, Tang B et al. Effects of HIV on executive function and verbal fluency in Cameroon. Sci Rep. 2018; 8: 17794. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Kerr SJ, Puthanakit T, Malee KM et al. Increased Risk of Executive Function and Emotional Behavioral Problems Among Virologically Well-Controlled Perinatally HIV-Infected Adolescents in Thailand and Cambodia. J Acquir Immune Defic Syndr. 2019; 82: 297–304. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Laughton B, Cornell M, Boivin M, Van Rie A. Neurodevelopment in perinatally HIV-infected children: a concern for adolescence. J Int AIDS Soc. 2013; 16: 18603. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Lentoor AG. Clinico-Immunological Status and Neurocognitive Function of Perinatally Acquired HIV-Positive Children on cART: A Cross-Sectional Correlational Study in South Africa. Front Neurol. 2020; 11: 243. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Lew BJ, McDermott TJ, Wiesman AI et al. Neural dynamics of selective attention deficits in HIV-associated neurocognitive disorder. Neurology. 2018; 91: e1860–e1869. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Llorente AM, Brouwers P, Leighty R et al. An analysis of select emerging executive skills in perinatally HIV-1-infected children. Appl Neuropsychol Child. 2014; 3: 10–25. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Malee KM, Chernoff MC, Sirois PA et al. Impact of Perinatally Acquired HIV Disease Upon Longitudinal Changes in Memory and Executive Functioning. J Acquir Immune Defic Syndr. 2017; 75: 455–464. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.McHenry MS, McAteer CI, Oyungu E et al. Neurodevelopment in Young Children Born to HIV-Infected Mothers: A Meta-analysis. Pediatrics. 2018; 141: e20172888. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Milligan R, Cockcroft K. Working Memory Profiles in HIV-Exposed, Uninfected and HIV-Infected Children: A Comparison with Neurotypical Controls. Front Hum Neurosci. 2017; 11: 348. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Neurological Mintz M. and developmental problems in pediatric HIV infection. J Nu.tr. 1996;126: 2663S-2673S. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Moran LM, Booze RM, Mactutus CF. Modeling deficits in attention, inhibition, and flexibility in HAND. J Neuroimmune Pharmacol. 2014; 9: 508–521. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Nichols S, Mahoney EM, Sirois PA et al. HIV-associated changes in adaptive, emotional, and behavioral functioning in children and adolescents with hemophilia: results from the Hemophilia Growth and Development Study. J Pediatr Psychol. 2000; 25: 545–556. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Nichols SL, Brummel SS, Smith RA et al. Executive Functioning in Children and Adolescents With Perinatal HIV Infection. Pediatr Infect Dis J. 2015; 34: 969–975. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Nichols SL, Chernoff MC, Malee KM et al. Executive Functioning in Children and Adolescents With Perinatal HIV Infection and Perinatal HIV Exposure. J Pediatric Infect Dis Soc. 2016; 5: S15–S23. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Okawa S, Mwanza Kabaghe S, Mwiya M et al. Psychological well-being and adherence to antiretroviral therapy among adolescents living with HIV in Zambia. AIDS Care. 2018; 30: 634–642. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Patel PB, Belden A, Handoko R et al. Behavioral impairment and cognition in Thai adolescents affected by HIV. Glob Ment Health (Camb). 2021; 8: e3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Puthanakit T, Aurpibul L, Louthrenoo O et al. Poor cognitive functioning of school-aged children in thailand with perinatally acquired HIV infection taking antiretroviral therapy. AIDS Patient Care STDS. 2010; 24: 141–146. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Reger M, Welsh R, Razani J, Martin DJ, Boone KB. A meta-analysis of the neuropsychological sequelae of HIV infection. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2002; 8: 410–424. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Rowe K, Buivydaite R, Heinsohn T et al. Executive function in HIV-affected children and adolescents: a systematic review and meta-analyses. AIDS Care. 2021; 1–25. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Walker KA, Brown GG. HIV-associated executive dysfunction in the era of modern antiretroviral therapy: A systematic review and meta-analysis. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 2018; 40: 357–376. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Smith R, Wilkins M. Perinatally acquired HIV infection: long-term neuropsychological consequences and challenges ahead. Child Neuropsychol. 2015; 21: 234–268. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Thakur KT, Boubour A, Saylor D, Das M, Bearden DR, Birbeck GL. Global HIV neurology: a comprehensive review. AIDS. 2019; 33: 163–184. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Wilmshurst JM, Hammond CK, Donald K, Hoare J, Cohen K, Eley B. NeuroAIDS in children. Handb Clin Neurol. 2018; 152: 99–116. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Bearden DR, Ciccone O, Patel AA. Global Health: Pediatric Neurology. Semin Neurol. 2018; 38: 200–207. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Phillips N, Amos T, Kuo C et al. HIV-Associated Cognitive Impairment in Perinatally Infected Children: A Meta-analysis. Pediatrics. 2016; 138: e20160893. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Smith R, Chernoff M, Williams PL et al. Impact of HIV severity on cognitive and adaptive functioning during childhood and adolescence. Pediatr Infect Dis J. 2012; 31: 592–598. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Rubin LH, Maki PM. HIV, Depression, and Cognitive Impairment in the Era of Effective Antiretroviral Therapy. Curr HIV/AIDS Rep. 2019; 16: 82–95. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Cohen RA, Siegel S, Gullett JM et al. Neural response to working memory demand predicts neurocognitive deficits in HIV. J Neurovirol. 2018; 24: 291–304. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Cysique LA, Maruff P, Brew BJ. The neuropsychological profile of symptomatic AIDS and ADC patients in the pre-HAART era: a meta-analysis. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2006; 12: 368–382. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Foley J, Ettenhofer M, Wright M, Hinkin CH. Emerging issues in the neuropsychology of HIV infection. Curr HIV/AIDS Rep. 2008; 5: 204–211. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Heaton RK, Franklin DR, Ellis RJ et al. HIV-associated neurocognitive disorders before and during the era of combination antiretroviral therapy: differences in rates, nature, and predictors. J Neurovirol. 2011; 17: 3–16. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Contardo C, Black AC, Beauvais J, Dieckhaus K, Rosen MI. Relationship of prospective memory to neuropsychological function and antiretroviral adherence. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 2009; 24: 547–554. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Hinkin CH, Castellon SA, Durvasula RS et al. Medication adherence among HIV+ adults: effects of cognitive dysfunction and regimen complexity. Neurology. 2002; 59: 1944–1950. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Hinkin CH, Hardy DJ, Mason KI et al. Medication adherence in HIV-infected adults: effect of patient age, cognitive status, and substance abuse. AIDS. 2004; 18 Suppl 1: S19–25. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Insel K, Morrow D, Brewer B, Figueredo A. Executive function, working memory, and medication adherence among older adults. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 2006; 61: P102–7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Obermeit LC, Morgan EE, Casaletto KB, Grant I, Woods SP, HIV NRPG. Antiretroviral Non-Adherence is Associated With a Retrieval Profile of Deficits in Verbal Episodic Memory. Clin Neuropsychol. 2015; 29: 197–213. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Stilley CS, Bender CM, Dunbar-Jacob J, Sereika S, Ryan CM. The impact of cognitive function on medication management: three studies. Health Psychol. 2010; 29: 50–55. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Zogg JB, Woods SP, Weber E et al. HIV-associated prospective memory impairment in the laboratory predicts failures on a semi-naturalistic measure of health care compliance. Clin Neuropsychol. 2010; 24: 945–962. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Garvie PA, Brummel SS, Allison SM et al. Roles of Medication Responsibility, Executive and Adaptive Functioning in Adherence for Children and Adolescents With Perinatally Acquired HIV. Pediatr Infect Dis J. 2017; 36: 751–757. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Armstrong A, Nagata JM, Vicari M et al. A Global Research Agenda for Adolescents Living With HIV. J Acquir Immune Defic Syndr. 2018; 78 Suppl 1: S16–S21. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Mbalinda SN, Bakeera-Kitaka S, Lusota DA, Magongo EN, Musoke P, Kaye DK. Barriers and facilitators for transitioning of young people from adolescent clinics to adult ART clinics in Uganda: unintended consequences of successful adolescent ART clinics. BMC Health Serv Res. 2020; 20: 835. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Meloni ST, Agaba P, Chang CA et al. Longitudinal evaluation of adherence, retention, and transition patterns of adolescents living with HIV in Nigeria. PLoS One. 2020; 15: e0236801. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Mohammed P, Linden A, Reilly M. Quantifying pediatric patient need for second- and third-line HIV treatment: A tool for decision-making in resource-limited settings. PLoS One. 2019; 14: e0224226. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Tadesse BT, Foster BA, Latour E et al. Predictors of Virologic Failure Among a Cohort of HIV-infected Children in Southern Ethiopia. Pediatr Infect Dis J. 2021; 40: 60–65. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Adams HR, Mwanza-Kabaghe S, Mbewe EG et al. The HIV-Associated Neurocognitive Disorders in Zambia (HANDZ) Study: Protocol of a research program in pediatric HIV in sub-Saharan Africa. medRxiv. 2019; 19003590. [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Dean O, Buda A, Adams HR et al. Brain Magnetic Resonance Imaging Findings Associated With Cognitive Impairment in Children and Adolescents With Human Immunodeficiency Virus in Zambia. Pediatr Neurol. 2020; 102: 28–35. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Molinaro M, Adams HR, Mwanza-Kabaghe S et al. Evaluating the Relationship Between Depression and Cognitive Function Among Children and Adolescents with HIV in Zambia. AIDS Behav. 2021; [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Schneider CL, Mohajeri-Moghaddam S, Mbewe EG et al. Cerebrovascular Disease in Children Perinatally Infected With Human Immunodeficiency Virus in Zambia. Pediatr Neurol. 2020; 112: 14–21. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Buda A, Dean O, Adams HR et al. Neighborhood-Based Socioeconomic Determinants of Cognitive Impairment in Zambian Children With HIV: A Quantitative Geographic Information Systems Approach. J Pediatric Infect Dis Soc. 2021; piab076. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Mbewe EG, Kabundula PP, Mwanza-Kabaghe S et al. Socioeconomic Status and Cognitive Function in Children With HIV: Evidence From the HIV-Associated Neurocognitive Disorders in Zambia (HANDZ) Study. JAIDS Journal of Acquired Immune Deficiency Syndromes. 2022; 89: 56–63. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Organization WH. WHO child growth standards: length/height-for-age, weight-for-age, weight-for-length, weight-for-height and body mass index-for-age: methods and development. World Health Organization; 2006. [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Gioia GA, Isquith PK, Guy SC, Kenworthy L. Test review behavior rating inventory of executive function. Child Neuropsychology. 2000; 6: 235–238. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Chernoff MC, Laughton B, Ratswana M et al. Validity of Neuropsychological Testing in Young African Children Affected by HIV. J Pediatr Infect Dis. 2018; 13: 185–201. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Akshoomoff N, Newman E, Thompson WK et al. The NIH Toolbox Cognition Battery: results from a large normative developmental sample (PING). Neuropsychology. 2014; 28: 1–10. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Hessl D, Sansone SM, Berry-Kravis E et al. The NIH Toolbox Cognitive Battery for intellectual disabilities: three preliminary studies and future directions. J Neurodev Disord. 2016; 8: 35. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Tulsky DS, Heinemann AW. The clinical utility and construct validity of the NIH Toolbox Cognition Battery (NIHTB-CB) in individuals with disabilities. Rehabil Psychol. 2017; 62: 409–412. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Zelazo PD, Anderson JE, Richler J, Wallner-Allen K, Beaumont JL, Weintraub S II.. NIH Toolbox Cognition Battery (CB): measuring executive function and attention. Monogr Soc Res Child Dev. 2013; 78: 16–33. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Zelazo PD, Anderson JE, Richler J et al. NIH Toolbox Cognition Battery (CB): validation of executive function measures in adults. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2014; 20: 620–629. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Matafwali B, Serpell R. Design and validation of assessment tests for young children in Zambia. New directions for child and adolescent development. 2014; 2014: 77–96. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 72.Mwanza-Kabaghe S. Preschool, executive functions and oral language as predictors of literacy and numeracy skills in first grade. 2015; [Google Scholar]
  • 73.Mwanza-Kabaghe S. Zambian preschools: A boost for early literacy. 2015; [Google Scholar]
  • 74.Tassiopoulos K, Patel K, Alperen J et al. Following young people with perinatal HIV infection from adolescence into adulthood: the protocol for PHACS AMP Up, a prospective cohort study. BMJOpen. 2016; 6: e011396. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Data Availability Statement

Data is available upon reasonable request from David R. Bearden at david_bearden@urmc.rochester.edu


Articles from medRxiv are provided here courtesy of Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory Preprints

RESOURCES