Abstract
This study examines whether, and how, multiple risks in early childhood are associated with an increased likelihood of a poor language or literacy outcome in early adolescence. Using data from 210 participants in the longitudinal Twins Early Developmental Study, we focus on the following risk factors at age 4: family risk, and poor language, speech, emergent literacy and nonverbal skills. The outcomes of interest at age 12 are language, reading fluency and reading comprehension. We contrast a ‘cumulative risk’ model, counting the presence or absence of each risk factor (breadth), with a model that also considers the severity of the early deficits (depth). A ‘cumulative risk index’ correlated modestly but significantly with outcome (r = 0.32–0.40). Odds ratios confirmed that having many risk factors (3–6) confers a higher probability of a poor outcome (OR 7.86–17.71) than having one or two (OR 3.65–7.28). Logistic regression models showed that predictive validity is not improved by including information about the severity of each deficit. Even with rich information on children's risk status at age 4, we can make only a moderately accurate prediction of the likelihood of a language or literacy disorder 8 years later (Area Under the Curve = 0.74–0.84; Positive Predictive Value = 0.33–0.55, Negative Predictive Value = 0.86–0.91). Taken together, and consistent with the idea of ‘cumulative risk’, these results suggest that the breadth of risk is a core predictor of outcome, and furthermore, that the severity of early deficits does not add significantly to this prediction.
Keywords: language, outcome, prediction, reading, risk
The greater the number of risk factors in early childhood, the greater the likelihood of a poor language or literacy outcome in early adolescence. However, the severity of early deficits may not in itself be very informative: depth (severity) adds little to prediction beyond breadth (number of risk factors).
Research highlights.
The greater the number of risk factors in early childhood, the greater the likelihood of a poor language or literacy outcome in early adolescence.
The severity of early deficits may not in itself be very informative: depth (severity) adds little to prediction beyond breadth (number of risk factors).
Difficulties may emerge in later childhood despite the absence of red flags in the early years.
1. INTRODUCTION
There is growing recognition that common neurodevelopmental disorders such as reading disorders and developmental language disorder (DLD) may be the consequence of multiple underlying neurocognitive deficits. This has been explicitly proposed in the ‘multiple deficit’ model of dyslexia (Pennington, 2006; Pennington et al., 2012), and is in line with current thinking in developmental psychology, as well as behavioural and psychiatric genetics, where multiple genetic and environmental risk factors are thought to accumulate, resulting in the behavioural manifestation of a disorder (Evans, Li, & Whipple, 2013). An important corollary of this approach is that the greater the number of risk factors present, the greater the likelihood of a poor outcome: that is, breadth matters. An intuitive, alternative (but not mutually exclusive) possibility is that the severity of the deficits is important in determining the likelihood of a poor outcome: that is, depth matters. In the current paper, we investigate whether the presence of a greater number of risk factors in young children (aged 4) is associated with a greater likelihood of a poor outcome in language and literacy skills at 12. We then consider whether this long‐range prediction is enhanced if the severity of early deficits, beyond just their presence or absence, is taken into account.
The outcomes of interest are oral language difficulties, poor reading fluency and poor reading comprehension at 12. Difficulties in these domains are associated with poor academic performance (Stothard, Snowling, Bishop, Chipchase, & Kaplan, 1998), as well as worse outcomes in broader life domains such as mental health and employment (Law, Rush, Schoon, & Parsons, 2009). The transition to secondary school is challenging for many children, and especially for those with communication and literacy difficulties (Dockrell & Lindsay, 2007). In the current study, we examine the extent to which risk factors identifiable when children first enter formal education (age 4–5) are informative about children's outcomes at the age of transition from primary to secondary school.
1.1. Risk factors for poor language and literacy outcomes
It has long been known that both language and literacy disorders run in families, and family aggregation studies clearly show an increased incidence in the relatives of children with dyslexia or language disorder (Flax et al., 2003). A meta‐analysis of prospective studies showed that 45% of children with a first‐degree relative who has dyslexia are likely to develop dyslexia, compared to 11.6% of children who are not at family risk (Snowling & Melby‐Lervåg, 2016). Similarly, children born into families with a first‐degree relative with language disorder have been found to have significantly poorer language skills at the age of 3 years, compared to children not at family risk, as well as some weaknesses in emergent literacy skills at ages 5 and 7 (Flax et al., 2003). Taken together, this literature provides strong evidence that family history is an important marker of risk of language and literacy difficulties. Evidence from genetically sensitive designs such as twin studies suggests a significant role for genetic factors in this familial transmission, particularly for dyslexia (Olson, Keenan, Byrne, & Samuelsson, 2014), and for language disorder that is accompanied by speech difficulties (Bishop & Hayiou‐Thomas, 2008).
There is substantial evidence that speech and language difficulties in the preschool years are likely to translate into long‐term problems. Several longitudinal studies have reported that young children with identified language disorders are likely to have poorer outcomes in both oral language and literacy than their peers (Beitchman et al., 1994; Catts, Fey, Tomblin, & Zhang, 2002; Stothard et al., 1998; Snowling, Nash, Gooch, Hayiou‐Thomas, & Hulme, 2019). In line with this literature, previous work from the longitudinal Twins Early Development Study (TEDS), the sample that the current study is based on, has found that poor language skills at age 4.5 are moderately predictive of reading at the ages of 7, 9 and 10 (Hayiou‐Thomas, Harlaar, Dale, & Plomin, 2010), and of poor oral language skills at the age of 12 (Hayiou‐Thomas, Dale, & Plomin, 2014).
Speech difficulties (speech sound disorder; SSD) have also been linked to later literacy problems (Bird, Bishop, & Freeman, 1995). This association seems to be strongest when the difficulties are persistent (Bishop & Adams, 1990), when speech errors indicate ‘disordered’ rather than delayed phonology (Leitao & Fletcher, 2004) and particularly when SSD co‐occurs with broader language disorder (Hayiou‐Thomas, Carroll, Leavett, Hulme, & Snowling, 2017; Peterson, Pennington, & Shriberg, 2009), a finding which underlines the importance of considering multiple deficits in the same sample.
The most strongly empirically supported risk markers in early childhood are in the domain of emergent literacy. There is robust, well‐replicated evidence across many different samples and languages that skills such as phonological awareness, letter knowledge and rapid automatized naming are important predictors of subsequent literacy development (Caravolas et al., 2012), and that deficits in these areas are important risk markers for poor literacy outcomes (Kamhi & Catts, 2012).
Finally, and somewhat controversially, is the role of nonverbal factors in language and reading disorders: should nonverbal ability be part of the definition of reading disorders and DLD (Bishop, Snowling, Thompson, Greenhalgh, & CATALISE Consortium, 2016; Siegel, 2003)? We have chosen not to prejudge this question by excluding children with low nonverbal skills; instead, we include this as a risk factor for later difficulties, as suggested by a modest but consistent literature (van Bergen et al., 2014; Chow, Ho, Wong, Waye, & Bishop, 2013; Fuchs et al., 2012). Similarly, our classification of low language and literacy skills in early adolescence is independent of nonverbal level.
Each of the early risk factors discussed above is known to be individually predictive of language and literacy outcomes, but it is not yet clear what their collective contribution is. In part this is because few studies have considered more than one or two of them within the same sample, especially in relation to multiple outcomes, as we do here. It is likely that many of these risk factors reflect overlapping underlying genetic and neurocognitive mechanisms (Pennington & Bishop, 2009). If they are essentially indexing a single underlying dimension of liability, then there should little additional predictive value when including multiple predictors. If they index overlapping but not identical dimensions of risk, then we should see a pattern of cumulative risk.
1.2. Relationships among language and literacy outcomes
In the studies reviewed above, early difficulties in spoken language often manifest in later problems in written language, even when the oral language difficulties themselves appear to have resolved (Stothard et al., 1998). Family aggregation and family risk studies strongly suggest that what is shared in the family is not specifically dyslexia or DLD, but rather weak language skills more broadly defined: a child with DLD is as likely to have parents or siblings who have dyslexia, as s/he is to have family members with oral language difficulties (Flax et al., 2003). Similarly, a child with dyslexia is likely to have family members who display weaknesses in spoken language (Snowling & Melby‐Lervåg, 2016). Although they often co‐occur, dyslexia and DLD are not the same disorder: children with dyslexia often have good language skills, and children with DLD may have intact reading (particularly decoding) skills (Bishop, McDonald, Bird, & Hayiou‐Thomas, 2009; Catts, Adlof, & Ellis Weismer, 2006; Snowling et al., 2019). Both the heterogeneity within groups and the overlap between groups sit comfortably within a multiple deficit framework: if there are overlapping liabilities, it may be that some constellations of risk factors are likely to lead to poor language outcomes, others to poor reading fluency and yet others to poor reading comprehension.
1.3. The present study
Few studies in this area have included all or most of the identified risk factors, addressed multiple outcomes and covered a substantial developmental range. One aim of the present study is to address these limitations. Another aim is to explicitly address the breadth versus depth question by using early measures in both a categorical (deficit as a unitary concept) or continuous (severity of impairment, in a regression design) fashion.
Research Questions:
Does the likelihood of a poor language or literacy outcome increase with the number of risk factors assessed at 4 years?
Does including the severity of the early difficulties lead to a better prediction of outcome than a simple count of the number of categorical risk factors?
2. METHODS
2.1. Participants
The sampling frame for the present study is the Twins Early Development Study (TEDS), a longitudinal study of twins born in England and Wales in 1994, 1995 and 1996. After checking for infant mortality, all families identified by the UK Office for National Statistics (ONS) as having twins born in these years were invited to participate in TEDS when the twins were about 18 months old. The twins have been assessed on measures of language, cognitive and behavioural development from the age of 2 into adulthood, using a variety of methods, including parent questionnaires, telephone testing and web‐based assessment. The TEDS sample has continued to be reasonably representative of the UK population with respect to ethnicity, maternal education and employment and paternal employment (Haworth, Davis, & Plomin, 2013). Informed written consent was obtained from all families participating in TEDS (Institute of Psychiatry ethical approval).
Twin pairs were excluded where either member of the pair had any major medical or perinatal problems, documented hearing loss or organic brain damage. English was the only language spoken at home in all selected families. The sample for the current study was derived from children who had participated in the 4½ (N = 1,672) and 12‐year (N = 15,038) data collection waves, and was limited to children with complete data on all six risk factors, and data on either language or literacy measures at 12. Only one member of each twin pair was included in analysis to ensure independence of data, yielding a final sample of 210 (103F: 107M). This subsample was slightly upwardly biased in terms of socioeconomic status, as indicated by maternal education: 44.4% of the children had mothers with A‐level qualifications or above (national examinations taken at 18 years of age in the UK). This is similar to the rest of the TEDS sample in adolescence (40.6%) but greater than the national average for children of that generation (35%). Children's mean age at the time of 4‐year data collection was 4.42 years (SD = 0.19) and was 12.03 years (SD = 0.37) for the 12‐year measures. Further characterization of the sample is provided in Table S1.
2.2. Measures
2.2.1. Family history
When children were 9, parents provided information about family history. If any first‐degree relative was reported as having either early language or reading difficulty, we classified this as a positive family history.
Measures of children's language, nonverbal, pre‐literacy and literacy skills were assessed at the ages of 4 and 12, as described below. All composites were created by averaging z‐scores that were first corrected for age. Psychometric reliability is reported for all composites included in analyses, and for individual measures where available; we report both Cronbach's alpha as an index of internal reliability and the MZ twin–co‐twin correlation, which provides a lower bound estimate of test–retest reliability.
2.3. Child measures at 4
2.3.1. Language composite
An extensive in‐home test battery administered to a subset of TEDS twins at the age of 4 included measures of both receptive and expressive language, assessing lexical, morphological, syntactic and narrative skills (Bus Story Information, Renfrew 1997a; Action Pictures Test Grammar, Renfrew 1997b; Verbal Comprehension, British Ability Scales, Elliot, Smith, & McCulloch, 1996; and Word Knowledge, Verbal Fluency, and Verbal Memory from the McCarthy Scales of Children's Abilities, McCarthy, 1972). Previous analysis has shown that variance across these measures is captured by a single factor, both phenotypically and aetiologically (Hayiou‐Thomas et al., 2006). Based on that finding, we averaged z‐scores for the individual tests to create a composite. This measure shows good internal consistency and reliability, with a Cronbach's alpha calculated from individual test scores of α = 0.85, as well as an MZ correlation of r = 0.76.
2.3.2. Speech composite
This was computed by averaging two measures in the battery requiring accurate speech production, the Goldman–Fristoe articulation (Goldman & Fristoe, 1986; α = 0.96) and nonword repetition (Gathercole, Willis, Baddeley, & Emslie, 1994; α = 0.88). These measures had previously been found to be highly intercorrelated, and to load onto a separate factor than the language composite described above (Hayiou‐Thomas et al., 2006). The Cronbach's alpha for this composite is α = 0.80, and the lower bound estimate of test–retest reliability based on MZ correlations is r = 0.66.
2.3.3. Nonverbal composite
Block Building, Puzzle Solving, Tapping Sequence and Draw‐a‐Design from the McCarthy Scales of Children's Ability (McCarthy, 1972) loaded onto a nonverbal factor (Viding et al., 2003). Cronbach's alpha for this factor is α = 0.70, and the MZ correlation is r = 0.64.
2.3.4. Phonological awareness
This was indexed by a bespoke 8‐item measure of rhyme judgement, based on a receptive task developed by Bird et al., 1995. Children were introduced to puppets, and asked to point to the picture, out of a choice of four, that sounded like that puppet's name; for example, ‘Which of these things would Dan like?’, ‘Spoon?’, ‘Ring?’, ‘Pan?’, ‘Key?’ (α = 0.72).
2.3.5. Letter knowledge
Parents reported on their children's knowledge of letter sounds and names at the age of 4: ‘If shown a letter, can your child name it?’ and ‘If shown a letter, does your child know what sound it makes?’ These two items were combined (Spearman's rho = 0.57).
2.4. Child outcome measures at 12
With the exception of the TOWRE, all 12‐year measures were administered online; further information on the testing procedure and validation of this battery is provided in Haworth et al. (2007).
2.4.1. Language composite
Receptive language was assessed at 12 using a battery of audio‐streamed, web‐administered measures indexing vocabulary (WISC‐III‐PI, Kaplan, Fein, Kramer, Delis, & Morris, 1999; α = 0.88), syntax (Listening Grammar, Test of Adolescent & Adult Language‐3, Hammill, Brown, Larsen, & Wiederholt, 1994; α = 0.94), non‐literal semantics and understanding of inferences (Test of Language Competence‐Level 2, Wiig, Secord, & Sabers, 1989; α = 0.66 and α = 0.58 respectively). Previous analysis showed substantial phenotypic and genetic overlap among these four measures (Dale, Harlaar, Hayiou‐Thomas, & Plomin, 2010). Cronbach's alpha for this composite α = 0.65; the MZ correlation is r = 0.66.
2.4.2. Reading fluency composite
Children completed an online adaptation of the Woodcock–Johnson III Reading Fluency test (W‐J III; Woodcock, 2001; α = 0.96). In addition, the Test of Word Reading Efficiency, Form B (Torgesen, Wagner, & Rashotte, 1999; alternative forms r = 0.73) was included in a test booklet sent to families by mail (one test booklet for each twin), and administered to each twin separately by telephone. Previous work with the TEDS sample at age 7 established strong concurrent validity for telephone administration of the TOWRE (Dale, Harlaar, & Plomin, 2005). The W‐J III and the word and nonword subtests of the TOWRE were combined; Cronbach's alpha for this composite is α = 0.82; the MZ correlation is r = 0.77.
2.4.3. Reading comprehension composite
Sentence‐level reading comprehension was assessed using a web‐based version of the Reading Comprehension subtest of the Peabody Individual Achievement Test (PIAT; Markwardt, 1997; α = 0.94), in which children read a sentence and chose the matching picture from a set of four. In addition, children completed a web version of the GOAL Formative Assessment in Literacy for Key Stage III (GOAL plc, 2002; α = 0.91), which includes a wide range of literal and inferential comprehension questions based on a set of stimulus sentences and short paragraphs. These measures were combined; Cronbach's alpha for this composite is α = 0.67; the MZ correlation is r = 0.64.
2.5. Defining risk factors and outcomes
A key aim of the current study is to contrast the predictive value of using categorical versus continuous measures of risk. With the exception of family risk, which was coded as present or absent, the risk factors we examine at 4 are based on continuous measures, so cut‐off criteria had to be determined for the categorical analysis. Based on common practice in studies of early vocabulary (see, e.g. multiple studies included in Rescorla & Dale, 2013), we chose a criterion of – 1.25 SD for the language, speech and nonverbal composites and the phonological awareness measure, and falling below the 10th centile on the letter knowledge measure, given its high skewness.
With respect to the outcomes of interest, it is increasingly acknowledged that the great majority of language and literacy impairments are best thought of as lying on a continuum of language ability, rather than reflecting a qualitatively different category. For this reason, cut‐offs are inherently somewhat arbitrary. Nevertheless, they are essential clinically for decisions concerning service delivery, and essential in research to focus attention on the interplay of factors at the low end of the continuum, which may differ from that in the centre or high end. We have chosen a broad criterion for defining poor language, reading fluency and reading comprehension outcomes, based on scoring at least −1 SD below the mean on the relevant composite. This cut‐off was chosen both because it has been widely used in previous research (e.g. language: Armstrong et al., 2018; Rice, Taylor, & Zubrick, 2008; Snowling, Duff, Nash, & Hulme, 2016; dyslexia: Catts, Adlof, Hogan, & Weismer, 2005; poor reading comprehension: Nation, Clarke, Marshall, & Durand, 2004), and also to maximize the sample of children with weak language or literacy skills at 12.
2.6. Analysis
To address the first research question, concerning the relation of number of risk factors with poor outcomes, the primary analysis method is the computation of Odds Ratios. Odds Ratios are the accepted benchmark in medical research, because they provide a measure of association between a risk factor and the outcome of interest by comparing the odds of a particular outcome occurring when the risk is present, to the odds of that outcome occurring in the absence of risk. In addition to Odds Ratios, we also computed Spearman's correlations between the number of risk factors and each of the three outcomes. The second research question is addressed by comparison of the accuracy of predictions based on dichotomizing a simple count of risk factors with logistic regressions based on continuous measures of risk factors. In each case, a criterion was set for prediction of poor outcome, which approximately matched the actual proportion in the sample. Logistic regressions were calculated with continuous measures of risk factors, and the residual ‘estimated probability of positive outcome’ was saved and dichotomized. Four conventional measures of diagnostic validity were then calculated: sensitivity, specificity, positive predictive value (PPV) and negative predictive value (NPV). In addition, ROC analysis was carried out based on these models, and the Area Under the Curve statistic was used to indicate overall predictive validity independent of a specific criterion.
3. RESULTS
3.1. Outcomes
Of the 210 children in our sample, 34 (13F: 21M) met criteria for a poor outcome in oral language at 12, 34 (13F: 21M) in reading fluency and 38 in reading comprehension (14F: 24M). Several children met criteria for more than one outcome: this overlap is illustrated in the Venn diagram in Figure 1, for the subset of children for whom we had data on all three outcomes (N = 163), and who met criteria for at least one poor outcome at 12 (N = 61).
3.2. Risk factors
Approximately 40–50 children (20%–25% of the sample) met criteria for each of the individual risk factors, with a much smaller proportion (2%–7%) meeting criteria for only that risk factor (Table 1). We used the cumulative risk index (min:max 0–6) to examine the distribution of risk factors in our sample. Eighty‐nine (42%) of the 210 children in the sample did not have any risk factors present at 4, and as expected, in general, the greater the number of risk factors, the fewer the cases: 45 children (21%) had one risk factor, 27 (13%) had two, 34 (16%) had three, 8 (4%) had four and 7 (3%) had five. No children had all six risk factors. The risk factors were modestly associated with one another, as indicated by the phi correlations presented in Table 2.
TABLE 1.
Risk factor at 4 | N (%) of children with this risk factor at 4 (categorical) | N (%) of children with ONLY this risk factor at 4 (categorical) | Mean level (M; SD) on this measure at 4, for the whole sample (continuous) |
---|---|---|---|
Family risk | 56 (26.67%) | 15 (7.14%) | 56 (26.67%) |
Language | 47 (22.38%) | 5 (2.38%) | −0.34 (1.16) |
Speech | 42 (20.00%) | 4 (1.90%) | −0.26 (1.15) |
Phonological awareness | 40 (19.05%) | 8 (3.81%) | −0.32 (1.03) |
Letter knowledge | 46 (21.90%) | 7 (3.33%) | 1.70 (1.17) |
Nonverbal ability | 37 (17.62)% | 6 (2.86%) | −0.23 (1.16) |
TABLE 2.
Language | Speech | Phonological Awareness | Letter Knowledge | Nonverbal | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Family risk | 0.167* | 0.237** | 0.091 | 0.175* | 0.004 |
Language | 0.246** | 0.089 | 0.351** | 0.351** | |
Speech | 0.273** | 0.311** | 0.269** | ||
Phonological awareness | 0.124 | 0.126 | |||
Letter knowledge | 0.208** |
p < .05;
p < .01.
3.2.1. Number of risk factors and the likelihood of a poor outcome (research question 1)
Spearman's correlations indicated that there was a modest but significant (p < .001) linear relationship between the number of risk factors present at age 4, and outcomes at 12: Language r = 0.36, Reading fluency r = 0.32; Reading comprehension r = 0.40. These relationships remained significant when including only the subset of cases with at least one risk factor, that is, within the group having some degree of risk: Language r = 0.19 (p = .050), Reading fluency r = 0.26 (p = .004); Reading Comprehension r = 0.32 (p < .001).
Odds ratios were used to estimate the odds of a poor outcome in each domain in the presence of either few (1–2) or many (3–6) risk factors, relative to the odds of a poor outcome when no risk factors were present. Table 3 shows the proportion of children in each of these ‘no’, ‘low’ or ‘high‐risk’ groups who had a poor outcome at the age of 12; the overall pattern is illustrated in Figure 2. Only a small proportion of children who had no risks at all at 4 went on to meet our criteria for a poor outcome in oral language (4.28%), reading fluency (5.81%) or reading comprehension (4.94%) at age 12. A larger proportion of the group with 1–2 risk factors had poor outcomes (roughly 15%–25%), and a larger proportion still in the group with 3–6 risk factors (roughly 35%–45%).
TABLE 3.
Good outcome (N) | Poor outcome (N) | Total (N) | % poor outcome | Odds ratios (95% CI) Chi‐sq (df, p‐value) | ||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Oral language | ||||||
0 risk | 67 | 3 | 70 | 4.28% | ||
1–2 risks | 46 | 15 | 61 | 24.59% | 7.28 (1.99–26.59) | 11.34 (df = 1, p = .001) |
3–6 risks | 27 | 16 | 43 | 37.21% | 13.24 (3.56–49.13) | 20.64 (df = 1, p < .001) |
TOTAL N | 140 | 34 | 174 | 19.54% | ||
Reading fluency | ||||||
0 risks | 81 | 5 | 86 | 5.81% | ||
1–2 risks | 57 | 13 | 70 | 18.57% | 3.70 (1.25–10.94) | 6.15 (df = 1, p = .013) |
3–6 risks | 33 | 16 | 49 | 32.65% | 7.86 (2.66–23.19) | 17.12 (df = 1, p < .001) |
TOTAL N | 171 | 34 | 205 | 16.58% | ||
Reading comprehension | ||||||
0 risk | 77 | 4 | 81 | 4.94% | ||
1–2 risk | 58 | 11 | 69 | 15.94% | 3.65 (1.11–12.05) | 5.01 (df = 1, p = .025) |
3–6 risks | 25 | 23 | 48 | 47.92% | 17.71 (5.59–56.14) | 33.64 (df = 1, p < .001) |
TOTAL N | 160 | 38 | 198 | 19.19% |
The odds ratios confirm that the presence of at least some risk factors is significantly associated with a poor language or reading outcome, with ORs ranging from 3.65 to 17.71 (Table 3). It is also notable that for all three outcomes, the odds ratios are substantially larger when many risk factors are present, as compared to one or two, although the overlapping confidence intervals between 1–2 risks and 3–6 risks show that these odds ratios are not statistically different from each other with this sample size. However, the existence of the overall association between number of risk factors and poor outcomes is supported by the positive and significant correlations between them reported above.
3.2.2. Does depth add to breadth with respect to predictive accuracy? (research question 2)
Having established an association between number of early risks and later outcomes, we examined the utility of the early measures for predicting risk at the level of the individual child. Specifically, we compared two types of models in which different levels of information about the predictor variables were included. In the first, only the presence or absence of each risk factor was considered. The second type of model made use of continuous rather than categorical measures of the risk factors, as well as allowing differential weight of predictors in logistic regressions. Table 4 summarizes and compares the main results of the analyses, particularly diagnostic validity for the models in terms of sensitivity, specificity and positive and negative predictive value.
TABLE 4.
Prediction model | Outcome at 12 | Criterion # | Sensitivity | Specificity | Positive predictive value | Negative predictive value | AUC (95% CI) |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Number of risk factors | Oral language | ≥3 | 0.47 (0.30–0.65) | 0.81 (0.73–0.87) | 0.37 (0.27–0.49) | 0.86 (0.82–0.90) | 0.75*a (0.66–0.84) |
Reading fluency | ≥3 | 0.47 (0.30–0.65) | 0.81 (0.74–0.863) | 0.33 (0.23–0.44) | 0.88 (0.85–0.91) | 0.74*b (0.65–0.83) | |
Reading comprehension | ≥3 | 0.61 (0.43–0.76) | 0.84 (0.78–0.90) | 0.48 (0.37–59) | 0.90 (0.86–0.93) | 0.78*c (0.70–0.86) | |
Continuous measures of risk factors | Oral language | 0.370 | 0.50 (0.32–0.68) | 0.89 (0.83–0.94) | 0.53 (0.39–0.67) | 0.88 (0.84–0.91) | 0.84*a (0.66–0.84) |
Reading fluency | 0.339 | 0.53 (0.35–0.70) | 0.91 (0.859–0.95) | 0.55 (0.40–0.68) | 0.91 (0.87–0.93) | 0.77*b (0.68–0.87) | |
Reading comprehension | 0.340 | 0.55 (0.38–0.71) | 0.89 (0.84–0.94) | 0.55 (0.42–0.68) | 0.89 (0.85–0.92) | 0.83*c (0.75–0.90) |
For the second model, the criterion is the critical value of the logistic residual ‘estimated probability of positive outcome; values ≥ the criterion are predictions of poor outcome.
All AUC statistics had asymptotic significance < 0.001. The AUC values for each pair of models (categorical vs. continuous risk) for the three outcomes were formally compared using a critical ratio z value. These indicated no significant differences: a z = 0.32 p = .626; bz = 0.10, p = .540; cz = 0.18, p = .571.
We also report the results of a receiver operating characteristic (ROC) curve analysis, which provides a more comprehensive assessment of the validity of prediction because it is not tied to any specific cut‐off of risk. Instead, it plots sensitivity against specificity for each possible predictor cut‐off. The area under the curve (AUC) statistic is a measure of the extent to which the ROC curve diverges from the diagonal, which indicates chance performance. ROC curves are presented in Figure 3, and the associated AUC statistics are included in Table 4.
Finally, we report the results for individual predictors in Table 5. The Odds Ratios show the level of risk associated with each predictor (regardless of the presence of other risk factors), when this is defined categorically. The B weights are derived from the logistic regression model (Model 2, described below), in which the predictors are continuous measures.
TABLE 5.
Outcome at 12 | Analysis type and statistic | Family risk | Language | Speech |
Phonological awareness |
Letter knowledge | Non verbal |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Oral language | Odds ratio (95% CI) | 2.56 (1.17–5.59) | 6.44 (2.86–14.50) | 1.92 (0.82–4.51) | 1.23 (0.50–3.01) | 2.99 (1.32–6.79) | 5.03 (2.19–11.55) |
B (S.E.) | −0.818 (0.480) | −1.118** (0.342) | −0.045 (0.259) | 0.448 (0.278) | 0.239 (0.252) | −0.633* (0.280) | |
Reading fluency | Odds ratio (95% CI) | 4.75 (2.20–10.26) | 3.43 (1.57–7.47) | 3.73 (1.68–8.28) | 1.69 (0.72–3.99) | 2.59 (1.18–5.70) | 2.67 (1.16–6.12) |
B (S.E.) | −1.288** (0.424) | −0.470 (0.282) | −0.422 (0.239) | −0.015 (0.241) | 0.073 (0.224) | −0.103 (0.258) | |
Reading comprehension | Odds ratio (95% CI) | 2.34 (1.11–4.89) | 6.37 (2.96–13.68) | 4.59 (2.12–9.94) | 1.61 (0.71–3.69) | 1.78 (0.81–3.89) | 10.00 (4.40–22.70) |
B (S.E.) | −0.760 (0.453) | −0.695* (0.283) | −0.129 (0.242) | −0.162 (0.254) | 0.427 (0.245) | −0.903** (0.275) |
Within each type of analysis, comparison across risk factors (columns) indicates the relative prediction weight of each factor. Because of differing assumptions in the two types of analysis, direct comparisons across rows (odds ratios vs. B weights) are not appropriate.
p < .05.
p < .01.
3.3. Model 1: Cumulative risk
The diagnostic validity measures for this model, which essentially weights all risk factors at the same level, were derived by dichotomizing the number of risk factors (0–2 and 3–6). This cut‐point was selected because it yielded the smallest high‐risk sample (23.3%) that was at least as large as the proportion actually observed at 12 years for a poor outcome (19.5% for language, 16.6% for reading fluency and 19.2% for reading comprehension). For all three outcomes, this model yielded moderate overall prediction, although it was notably poor with respect to sensitivity and positive predictive value at the selected cut‐point (Table 4). The AUC values ranged between 0.74 and 0.78, which can be considered ‘fair’ classification values (Caspi, Houts, Belsky, & Harrington, 2016).
3.4. Model 2: Continuous measures with differential weighting
A set of logistic regressions was run with the underlying continuous measures for all predictors except family history (which is inherently categorical). Thus, unlike Model 1 in which the same risk index (number of risk factors) was used for all three outcomes, in Model 2, a separate risk index was calculated for each outcome based on the logistic regression analysis for that outcome. As an indication of potential for differential prediction, correlations were computed between number of risk factors and each of the three new indices. They were 0.66, 0.81 and 0.71 for language, reading fluency and reading comprehension respectively. The correlation for reading fluency was significantly higher than that for the other two outcomes, using the r‐to‐z transformation evaluation procedure. Although these correlations were substantial, they were still low enough to allow for differential prediction of outcomes. Overall, the values of the classification indices for Model 2 were similar to those of Model 1, although there was a slight increase in positive predictive value, and in the AUC statistics (AUC 0.78–0.84; values of 0.80 and above can be considered ‘good’, Caspi et al., 2016). Taken together, these results show only modest improvement in prediction over cumulative risk even when full (continuous) information about the predictors is utilized and separate prediction formulas are used.
The ROC analysis allows a formal evaluation of the change in prediction from Model 1 to Model 2. The overlapping 95% confidence intervals for the AUC statistics in Table 4 indicated that for none of the three outcomes was the area under the curve significantly different between the analysis based on the cumulative risk index and that based on the estimated probability from the logistic regression. This is further confirmed by the non‐significant critical ratio z‐values for each pair of models (Hanley & McNeil, 1983).
In order to test the robustness of our findings that categorical versus continuous models yield similar predictive values, we carried out a supplementary analysis using a different statistical approach (reported in Supplementary Online Materials 2). Classification and regression tree (CART) models produce an easily interpretable visual output which shows the predicted outcome for individual children based on their score on each predictor variable (Figures [Link], [Link], [Link]). They also produce classification tables which can be used to derive diagnostic indices (sensitivity/specificity/PPV/NPV; Table S2). Applied to our data, CART models yielded overall comparable prediction of outcome for categorical versus continuous predictors: of the 12 pairs of diagnostic validity indices, only one had non‐overlapping confidence intervals (the sensitivity index for Reading Fluency).
Examining the individual predictors (Table 5), it can be seen that family history is a significant predictor of a poor reading fluency outcome at 12; that both early language and early nonverbal skills are significant predictors of a poor language outcome and that early language and nonverbal ability are again the only significant predictors of a poor reading comprehension outcome. This pattern of results is supported both by the logistic regressions for Model 2, which use continuous predictors, and by the Odds Ratios for individual risk factors defined categorically: the risk factors which were significant in the logistic regressions were also the ones which had the largest Odds Ratios for each outcome.
4. DISCUSSION
Our results show that the presence of a greater number of risk factors at the age of 4 increases the likelihood of a poor outcome in language and literacy at 12. Somewhat surprisingly, we find that adding information about the severity of the early predictors, and allowing the weighting of individual predictors to vary, adds little predictive value. At the same time, although the pattern of increased likelihood of a poor outcome associated with ‘cumulative risk’ is clear, we also show that even with rich information in early childhood, it is still not easy to predict which children will have a poor outcome at the age of 12.
4.1. Breadth versus depth in three outcome domains
Both of these aspects of our findings—the predominant role of breadth, and moderate individual prediction—cohere with, and extend, the existing literature. Previous studies have shown that the presence of additional early deficits is associated with poorer outcomes: children with language difficulties combined with poor nonverbal ability are more likely to have poor language and literacy outcomes in later childhood and adolescence (Bishop & Adams, 1990; Stothard et al., 1998). Similarly, speech deficits accompanied by language difficulties in preschool are more likely to be associated with poor literacy outcomes than isolated speech disorder (Peterson et al., 2009), and even more likely if there is also a family risk of dyslexia (Carroll, Mundy, & Cunningham, 2014; Hayiou‐Thomas et al., 2017). Children at family risk of dyslexia are in turn more likely to meet criteria for dyslexia if they also have weak language skills in early childhood (Snowling & Melby‐Lervag, 2016). Although in the current study, we have not delineated the specific combinations of risk factors which are associated with specific outcomes, we have demonstrated that this general pattern of cumulative risk seems to hold across a broad range of language‐relevant predictors and outcomes.
By itself, a correlation between number of risk factors and poor outcome is consistent with a depth interpretation; as the early predictors are often intercorrelated, they collectively might be indexing a single underlying dimension of risk, that is, breadth might be a proxy for depth. The failure to significantly improve prediction when (in Model 2) severity measures are included in the prediction equation provides support for the conclusion that the predictor variables here do primarily constitute measures of breadth of risk.
4.2. Prediction of poor outcomes from early measures
Turning to the prediction of individual risk, we found moderate levels of overall prediction over the long range from ages 4 to 12. The AUC values our models yielded were either just below or just above 0.80, which is at the lower end of what is considered an adequate value for clinical decision‐making about intervention (Caspi et al., 2016). This level of prediction is in line with other findings in the literature. For example, in predicting 7‐year outcomes in receptive language from 4‐year‐old language scores, McKean et al. (2017) reported an AUC value of 0.84, while in a study predicting reading disability at age 8 from pre‐literacy measures at ages 3 and 5, Puolakanaho et al. (2007) reported AUC values from 0.70 to 0.85. Interestingly, potentially more powerful statistical approaches utilizing methods which can take account of complex interactions among specific combinations of predictors do not find that these enhance the levels of prediction for language or reading outcomes (e.g. classification and regression tree [CART] models, Koon, Petscher, & Foorman, 2014; and machine learning based on neural net modelling, Armstrong et al., 2018).
This challenge in accurate long‐range prediction of language outcomes for individual children occurs despite high levels of stability in language and literacy skills in terms of individual variability, at least after the age of 5 (e.g. Bornstein, Hahn, & Putnick, 2016; Norbury et al., 2017), as well as reasonable levels of stability in diagnostic status, particularly when effects of regression to the mean are taken into account (Johnson et al., 1999; Tomblin, Zhang, Buckwalter, & O’Brien, 2003). Nonetheless, it is also the case that for many children, early language difficulties resolve over time, while for other children, language difficulties emerge later in childhood (Hayiou‐Thomas et al., 2014; Snowling et al., 2015). Reading difficulties also appear to be stable over the short term, with dyslexia in Grade 1 predicting dyslexia in Grade 2 with a PPV of 68.3% (Pennington et al., 2012), but the long‐range prediction of poor reading outcomes from pre‐reading skills is still challenging. It is possible that an even broader range of predictors than ours needs to be considered, for example, executive function (Thompson et al., 2015) or sensory and motor deficits (Carroll, Solity, & Shapiro, 2016).
4.3. Limitations and future directions
An important limitation to bear in mind when interpreting our findings is that of statistical power and sample size. Our initial sampling frame is from the large‐scale TEDS sample, and we had direct measures of speech and language at the age of 4 on a subset of 1672 children. Attrition over 8 years, our requirement of a broad range of both predictor and outcome measures, and the need to utilize only one twin from each pair to ensure independence of data reduced this sample to 210. While sufficient for addressing our main research questions, this sample size prevented us from examining the data at a more fine‐grained level, for example, comparing the contribution of individual predictors and specific combinations of predictors (there are 63 possible combinations of six predictors). The cumulative pattern of risk that we observed in our data is suggestive of an additive accumulation of risk, but it will also be important to explore the possibility of interactions among specific risk factors. That is, are there specific constellations of risk factors that are more likely to give rise to a specific outcome? And, if so, what are the causal pathways that mediate the relationship between sets of early risks and later outcomes, an issue that our correlational design cannot address?
We also note that in this study, we did not have a measure of Rapid Automatized Naming, which is an established predictor of reading (Caravolas et al., 2012), and our measure of letter knowledge was non‐standard, in that it was based on parental report of the level of letter knowledge, rather than a direct assessment of knowledge of each letter name or sound. In addition, our measure of phonological awareness focused on rhyme, but it is likely that phoneme awareness specifically is a more sensitive predictor than onset–rime analysis, which was assessed in the current study (Hulme et al., 2002).
Finally, we acknowledge that interpretation of results from (non‐experimental) longitudinal studies of language disorders is particularly challenging, given the potential role of intervention services. In the present study, information is available from only a subsample of the families, and it is limited to the provision of service, not the target of services, intensity and duration of service or therapeutic approach utilized. But even if more complete information were available, there is the problem that level of language ability is causally both an antecedent and a consequent of service provision. For this reason, comparison of language between children who have received services and those who have not is inherently ambiguous.
4.4. Clinical implications
Despite these limitations, the current study has clinical implications for the identification of children with language and literacy disorders. Firstly, the greater the number of early risk factors, the greater the likelihood of a poor outcome: that is, breadth matters. Secondly, and more surprisingly, the severity of early deficits may not in itself be very informative: depth adds little to prediction beyond breadth. And thirdly, it is important to be aware that difficulties may emerge in later childhood despite the absence of red flags in the early years.
CONFLICT OF INTEREST
No conflicts declared.
Supporting information
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
We gratefully acknowledge the ongoing contribution of the participants in the Twins Early Development Study (TEDS) and their families. TEDS is supported by a programme grant (MR/M021475/1 and previously G0901245) from the U.K. Medical Research Council.
Hayiou‐Thomas ME, Smith‐Woolley E, Dale PS. Breadth versus depth: Cumulative risk model and continuous measure prediction of poor language and reading outcomes at 12. Dev Sci. 2021;24:e12998. 10.1111/desc.12998
DATA AVAILABILITY STATEMENT
Data used for this submission may be made available on request to the Twins Early Development Study (TEDS), through their data access mechanism (see www.teds.ac.uk/research/collaborators‐and‐data/teds‐data‐access‐policy). They will then consider requests for sharing data for appropriate research purposes.
REFERENCES
- Armstrong, R. , Symons, M. , Scott, J. G. , Arnott, W. L. , Copland, D. A. , McMahon, K. L. , & Whitehouse, A. J. O. (2018). Predicting language difficulties in middle childhood from early developmental milestones: A comparison of traditional regression and machine learning techniques. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 61(8), 1926–1944. 10.1044/2018_JSLHR-L-17-0210 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Beitchman, J. H. , Brownlie, E. B. , Inglis, A. , Wild, J. , Mathews, R. , Schachter, D. , … Lancee, W. (1994). Seven‐year follow‐up of speech/language‐impaired and control children: Speech/language stability and outcome. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 33(9), 1322–1330. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bird, J. , Bishop, D. V. , & Freeman, N. H. (1995). Phonological awareness and literacy development in children with expressive phonological impairments. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 38(2), 446–462. 10.1044/jshr.3802.446 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bishop, D. V. , & Adams, C. (1990). A prospective study of the relationship between specific language impairment, phonological disorders and reading retardation. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 31(7), 1027–1050. 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1990.tb00844.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bishop, D. V. M. , & Hayiou‐Thomas, M. E. (2008). Heritability of specific language impairment depends on diagnostic criteria. Genes, Brain, and Behavior, 7(3), 365–372. 10.1111/j.1601-183X.2007.00360.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bishop, D. V. M. , McDonald, D. , Bird, S. , & Hayiou‐Thomas, M. E. (2009). Children who read words accurately despite language impairment: Who are they and how do they do it? Child Development, 80(2), 593–605. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bishop, D. V. M. , Snowling, M. J. , Thompson, P. A. , Greenhalgh, T. , & CATALISE Consortium . (2016). CATALISE: A multinational and multidisciplinary Delphi consensus study. Identifying language impairments in children. PLoS ONE, 11(7), e0158753. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bornstein, M. H. , Hahn, C.‐S. , & Putnick, D. L. (2016). Stability of core language skill across the first decade of life in children at biological and social risk. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 57(12), 1434–1443. 10.1111/jcpp.12632 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Caravolas, M. , Lervåg, A. , Mousikou, P. , Efrim, C. , Litavský, M. , Onochie‐Quintanilla, E. , … Hulme, C. (2012). Common patterns of prediction of literacy development in different alphabetic orthographies. Psychological Science, 23(6), 678–686. 10.1177/0956797611434536 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carroll, J. M. , Mundy, I. R. , & Cunningham, A. J. (2014). The roles of family history of dyslexia, language, speech production and phonological processing in predicting literacy progress. Developmental Science, 17(5), 727–742. 10.1111/desc.12153 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carroll, J. M. , Solity, J. , & Shapiro, L. R. (2016). Predicting dyslexia using prereading skills: The role of sensorimotor and cognitive abilities. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines, 57(6), 750–758. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Caspi, A. , Houts, R. M. , Belsky, D. W. , & Harrington, H. (2016). Childhood forecasting of a small segment of the population with large economic burden. Nature Human Behaviour, 1, 1–10. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Catts, H. W. , Adlof, S. M. , & Ellis Weismer, S. (2006). Language deficits in poor comprehenders: A case for the simple view of reading. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 49(2), 278–293. 10.1044/1092-4388(2006/023) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Catts, H. W. , Adlof, S. M. , Hogan, T. P. , & Weismer, S. E. (2005). Are specific language impairment and dyslexia distinct disorders? Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 48(6), 1378–1396. 10.1044/1092-4388(2005/096) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Catts, H. W. , Fey, M. E. , Tomblin, J. B. , & Zhang, X. (2002). A longitudinal investigation of reading outcomes in children with language impairments. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 45(6), 1142–1157. 10.1044/1092-4388(2002/093) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chow, B.‐W.‐Y. , Ho, C.‐S.‐H. , Wong, S.‐W.‐L. , Waye, M. M. Y. , & Bishop, D. V. M. (2013). Generalist genes and cognitive abilities in Chinese twins. Developmental Science, 16(2), 260–268. 10.1111/desc.12022 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dale, P. S. , Harlaar, N. , Hayiou‐Thomas, M. E. , & Plomin, R. (2010). The etiology of diverse receptive language skills at 12. Journal of Speech, Language & Hearing Research, 53, 982–992. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dale, P. S. , Harlaar, N. , & Plomin, R. (2005). Telephone testing and teacher assessment of reading skills in 7‐year‐olds: I. Substantial correspondence for a sample of 5544 children and for extremes. Reading and Writing, 18(5), 385–400. [Google Scholar]
- Dockrell, J. , & Lindsay, G. (2007). Identifying the educational and social needs of children with specific speech and language difficulties on entry to secondary school. Educational and Child Psychology, 24, 14. [Google Scholar]
- Elliot, C. D. , Smith, P. , & McCulloch, K. (1996). Verbal Comprehension Scale. British Ability Scales (2nd ed.). Windsor, England: nferNelson. [Google Scholar]
- Evans, G. W. , Li, D. , & Whipple, S. S. (2013). Cumulative risk and child development. Psychological Bulletin, 139(6), 1342–1396. 10.1037/a0031808 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Flax, J. F. , Realpe‐Bonilla, T. , Hirsch, L. S. , Brzustowicz, L. M. , Bartlett, C. W. , & Tallal, P. (2003). Specific language impairment in families: Evidence for co‐occurrence with reading impairments. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 46(3), 530–543. 10.1044/1092-4388(2003/043) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fuchs, D. , Compton, D. L. , Fuchs, L. S. , Bryant, V. J. , Hamlett, C. L. , & Lambert, W. (2012). First‐grade cognitive abilities as long‐term predictors of reading comprehension and disability status. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 45(3), 217–231. 10.1177/0022219412442154 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gathercole, S. E. , Willis, C. S. , Baddeley, A. D. , & Emslie, H. (1994). The children's test of nonword repetition: A test of phonological working memory. Memory, 2, 103–127. 10.1080/09658219408258940 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- GOAL plc . (2002). GOAL formative assessment in literacy (key stage 3). London, UK: Hodder & Stoughton. [Google Scholar]
- Goldman, R. , & Fristoe, M. (1986). Goldman fristoe test of articulation. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service. [Google Scholar]
- Hammill, D. D. , Brown, V. L. , Larsen, S. C. , & Wiederholt, J. L. (1994). Test of adolescent and adult language (TOAL‐3). Austin, TX: Pro‐Ed. [Google Scholar]
- Hanley, J. A. , & McNeil, B. J. (1983). A method of comparing the areas under receiver operating curves derived from the same cases. Radiology, 148, 839–843. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Haworth, C. M. A. , Davis, O. S. P. , & Plomin, R. (2013). Twins Early Development Study (TEDS): A genetically sensitive investigation of cognitive and behavioral development from childhood to young adulthood. Twin Research and Human Genetics, 16(1), 117–125. 10.1017/thg.2012.91 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Haworth, C. M. A. , Harlaar, N. , Kovas, Y. , Davis, O. S. P. , Oliver, B. R. , Hayiou‐Thomas, M. E. , … Plomin, R. (2007). Internet cognitive testing of large samples needed in genetic research. Twin Research and Human Genetics, 10(4), 554–563. 10.1375/twin.10.4.554 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hayiou‐Thomas, M. E. , Carroll, J. M. , Leavett, R. , Hulme, C. , & Snowling, M. J. (2017). When does speech sound disorder matter for literacy? The role of disordered speech errors, co‐occurring language impairment and family risk of dyslexia. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 58(2), 197–205. 10.1111/jcpp.12648 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hayiou‐Thomas, M. E. , Dale, P. S. , & Plomin, R. (2014). Language impairment from 4 to 12 years: Prediction and etiology. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 57(3), 850–864. 10.1044/2013_JSLHR-L-12-0240 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hayiou‐Thomas, M. E. , Harlaar, N. , Dale, P. S. , & Plomin, R. (2010). Preschool speech, language skills, and reading at 7, 9, and 10 years: Etiology of the relationship. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 53(2), 311–332. 10.1044/1092-4388(2009/07-0145) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hayiou‐Thomas, M. E. , Kovas, Y. , Harlaar, N. , Plomin, R. , Bishop, D. V. M. , & Dale, P. S. (2006). Common aetiology for diverse language skills in 4 1/2‐year‐old twins. Journal of Child Language, 33, 339–368. 10.1017/S0305000906007331 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hulme, C. , Hatcher, P. J. , Nation, K. , Brown, A. , Adams, J. , & Stuart, G. (2002). Phoneme awareness is a better predictor of early reading skill than onset‐rime awareness. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 82(1), 2–28. 10.1006/jecp.2002.2670 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Johnson, C. J. , Beitchman, J. H. , Young, A. , Escobar, M. , Atkinson, L. , Wilson, B. , … Wang, M. (1999). Fourteen‐year follow‐up of children with and without speech/language impairments: Speech/language stability and outcomes. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 42(3), 744–760. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kamhi, A. G. , & Catts, H. (2012). Language and reading disabilities (3rd ed.). Pearson. [Google Scholar]
- Kaplan, E. , Fein, D. C. , Kramer, J. H. , Delis, D. , & Morris, R. (1999). Wechsler Intelligence Scale as a Process Instrumentt: ‐ WISC‐III PI. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation. [Google Scholar]
- Koon, S. , Petscher, Y. , & Foorman, B. R. (2014). Using evidence‐based decision trees instead of formulas to identify at‐risk readers (REL 2014–036). Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Education, Institute for Education Sciences, National Center for Education Evaluation and Regional Assistance, Regional Educational Laboratory Southeast. Retrieved from http://ies.ed.gov/ncee/edlabs [Google Scholar]
- Law, J. , Rush, R. , Schoon, I. , & Parsons, S. (2009). Modeling developmental language difficulties from school entry into adulthood: Literacy, mental health, and employment outcomes. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 52(6), 1401–1416. 10.1044/1092-4388(2009/08-0142) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Leitao, S. , & Fletcher, J. (2004). Literacy outcomes for students with speech impairment: Long‐term follow‐up. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 39(2), 245–256. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Markwardt Jr., F. C. (1997). Peabody individual achievement test‐revised (normative update) manual. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service. [Google Scholar]
- McCarthy, D. (1972). McCarthy scales of children's abilities. New York, NY: The Psychological Corporation. [Google Scholar]
- McKean, C. , Reilly, S. , Bavin, E. L. , Bretherton, L. , Cini, E. , Conway, L. , … Mensah, F. (2017). Language outcomes at 7 years: Early predictors and co‐occurring difficulties. Pediatrics, 139(3). 10.1542/peds.2016-1684 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nation, K. , Clarke, P. , Marshall, C. M. , & Durand, M. (2004). Hidden language impairments in children parallels between poor reading comprehension and specific language impairment? Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research: JSLHR, 47(1), 199–211. 10.1044/1092-4388(2004/017) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Norbury, C. F. , Vamvakas, G. , Gooch, D. , Baird, G. , Charman, T. , Simonoff, E. , & Pickles, A. (2017). Language growth in children with heterogeneous language disorders: A population study. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 58(10), 1092–1105. 10.1111/jcpp.12793 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Olson, R. K. , Keenan, J. M. , Byrne, B. , & Samuelsson, S. (2014). Why do children differ in their development of reading and related skills? Scientific Studies of Reading, 18, 38–54. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pennington, B. F. (2006). From single to multiple deficit models of developmental disorders. Cognition, 101(2), 385–413. 10.1016/j.cognition.2006.04.008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pennington, B. F. , & Bishop, D. V. M. (2009). Relations among speech, language, and reading disorders. Annual Review of Psychology, 60, 283–306. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pennington, B. F. , Santerre‐Lemmon, L. , Rosenberg, J. , MacDonald, B. , Boada, R. , Friend, A. , … Olson, R. K. (2012). Individual prediction of dyslexia by single versus multiple deficit models. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 121(1), 212–224. 10.1037/a0025823 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Peterson, R. L. , Pennington, B. F. , Shriberg, L. D. , & Boada, R. (2009). What influences literacy outcome in children with speech sound disorder? Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 52(5), 1175–1188. 10.1044/1092-4388(2009/08-0024) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Puolakanaho, A. , Ahonen, T. , Aro, M. , Eklund, K. , Leppänen, P. H. T. , Poikkeus, A.‐M. , … Lyytinen, H. (2007). Very early phonological and language skills: Estimating individual risk of reading disability. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 48(9), 923–931. 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2007.01763.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Renfrew, C. E. (1997a). Bus story test (4th ed.). Bicester, Oxon, England: Winslow Press. [Google Scholar]
- Renfrew, C. E. (1997b). Action picture test. (4th ed.). Bicester, Oxon, England: Winslow Press. [Google Scholar]
- Rescorla, L. , & Dale, P. H. (2013). Late talkers. Baltimore: Brookes. [Google Scholar]
- Rice, M. L. , Taylor, C. L. , & Zubrick, S. R. (2008). Language outcomes of 7‐year‐old children with or without a history of late language emergence at 24 months. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 51(2), 394–407. 10.1044/1092-4388(2008/029) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Siegel, L. S. (2003). IQ‐discrepancy definitions and the diagnosis of LD: Introduction to the special issue. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 36(1), 2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Snowling, M. J. , Duff, F. J. , Nash, H. M. , & Hulme, C. (2016). Language profiles and literacy outcomes of children with resolving, emerging, or persisting language impairments. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines, 57(12), 1360–1369. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Snowling, M. J. , & Melby‐Lervåg, M. (2016). Oral language deficits in familial dyslexia: A meta‐analysis and review. Psychological Bulletin, 142(5), 498–545. 10.1037/bul0000037 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Snowling, M. J. , Nash, H. M. , Gooch, D. C. , Hayiou‐Thomas, M. E. , & Hulme, C. , & Wellcome Language and Reading Project Team . (2019). Developmental outcomes for children at high risk of dyslexia and children with developmental language disorder. Child Development, 90(5), 548–564. 10.1111/cdev.13216 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stothard, S. E. , Snowling, M. J. , Bishop, D. V. , Chipchase, B. B. , & Kaplan, C. A. (1998). Language‐impaired preschoolers: A follow‐up into adolescence. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 41(2), 407–418. 10.1044/jslhr.4102.407 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thompson, P. A. , Hulme, C. , Nash, H. M. , Gooch, D. , Hayiou‐Thomas, E. , & Snowling, M. J. (2015). Developmental dyslexia: Predicting individual risk. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines, 56(9), 976–987. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tomblin, J. B. , Zhang, X. , Buckwalter, P. , & O’Brien, M. (2003). The stability of primary language disorder. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 46(6), 283–1296. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Torgesen, J. K. , Wagner, R. K. , & Rashotte, C. A. (1999). Test of word reading efficiency (TOWRE). Austin, TX: Pro‐ed. [Google Scholar]
- van Bergen, E. , de Jong, P. F. , Maassen, B. , Krikhaar, E. , Plakas, A. , & van der Leij, A. (2014). IQ of four‐year‐olds who go on to develop dyslexia. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 47(5), 475–484. 10.1177/0022219413479673 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Viding, E. , Price, T. S. , Spinath, F. M. , Bishop, D. V. M. , Dale, P. S. , & Plomin, R. (2003). Genetic and environmental mediation of the relationship between language and nonverbal impairment in 4‐year old twins. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 46(6), 1271–1282. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wiig, E. H. , Secord, W. , & Sabers, D. (1989). Test of language competence (Expanded ed.). San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation. [Google Scholar]
- Woodcock, W. , McGrew, K. S. , & Mather, N. (2001). Woodcock‐Johnson III. Itasca, IL: Riverside Publishing. [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
Data used for this submission may be made available on request to the Twins Early Development Study (TEDS), through their data access mechanism (see www.teds.ac.uk/research/collaborators‐and‐data/teds‐data‐access‐policy). They will then consider requests for sharing data for appropriate research purposes.