Skip to main content
BMC Medical Education logoLink to BMC Medical Education
. 2024 Oct 19;24:1177. doi: 10.1186/s12909-024-06181-9

Promoting motivation and reducing stress in medical students by utilizing self-determination theory – a randomized controlled trial in practical psychiatry courses

Nina Triebner 1, Franziska Sonnauer 1, Miriam Rauch 2, Gian-Marco Kersten 1, Christoph Rauch 3, Stefan Mestermann 1, Maximillian Bailer 1, Johannes Kornhuber 1, Janine Utz 1,#, Philipp Spitzer 1,✉,#
PMCID: PMC11491017  PMID: 39427147

Abstract

Background

Medical students experience high levels of stress and related mental health problems. Students’ autonomous and controlled motivation and their mental well-being are interconnected. This study aimed to investigate whether an innovative teaching concept based on self-determination theory (SDT) could improve students’ motivation and thereby reduce their stress levels, ultimately providing a healthier framework for learning.

Methods

In a week-long practical psychiatry course for medical students, a new didactic concept was implemented in half the groups (n = 73) and compared with the preexisting concept (n = 75) as a randomized controlled trial (RCT). To promote the SDT-target factors of perceived autonomy, competence, and relatedness, the methods used included team building, exclusively positive feedback, group discussions, and choice in task distribution. Significant group differences in motivation, stress, performance, and their relationships were analyzed through t-tests, multiple linear regression analyses, mediation analyses, and hierarchical linear modeling (HLM) using questionnaires collected before (t0) and after (t1) the course, and students’ exam results (t2).

Results

In the innovation group (n = 53), intrinsic motivation/interest (d = 0.41; p = .019) and perceived choice/autonomy (d = 0.33; p = .048) were greater than in the control group (n = 52). While autonomous regulation remained stable, the innovation group showed reduced controlled regulation (d = -0.36; p = .033) and reported significantly lower stress (d = -0.55; p = .003). The observed changes in motivation collectively mediated the stress reduction. However, students in the innovation group achieved lower exam scores, which seemed to result from the absence of critical feedback, but not from the observed differences in motivation or stress.

Conclusions

This study demonstrated that enhancing intrinsic motivation through SDT-based teaching can effectively reduce stress in medical students. Exclusively strengths-based positive feedback may have hindered exam performance, but optimizing educational concepts to promote motivation and reduce stress will be a valuable step toward improving medical students’ mental well-being.

Supplementary Information

The online version contains supplementary material available at 10.1186/s12909-024-06181-9.

Keywords: Intrinsic motivation, Extrinsic motivation, Self-determination theory (SDT), Stress, Medical education, Psychiatry, Randomized controlled trial (RCT)

Background

The demanding environment of medical schools and the high levels of distress experienced by medical students are topics frequently discussed both in specialist circles as well as the public debate. High rates of depression, burnout, and anxiety among medical students have been reported in various studies worldwide over the past decades [1, 2]. Considering students’ mental well-being and its effects on patient care, it is therefore imperative that educators and institutions find effective solutions to provide students with a learning environment that promotes mental well-being and reduces stress [3, 4].

Intrinsic motivation is known to be a decisive factor in promoting students’ mental and physical well-being [59]. The self-determination theory (SDT) encompasses a basic needs theory, which asserts that intrinsic motivation is sustained by satisfying three fundamental psychological needs: autonomy, competence, and relatedness [1014]. Autonomy refers to the need for individuals to experience self-direction and personal agency in their actions and decisions. When learners feel they can make meaningful choices, they are more likely to engage in learning activities out of genuine interest [1014]. Competence is the feeling of being effective and capable in one’s endeavors. In an academic setting, providing students with tasks that are challenging but achievable, along with positive reinforcement, fosters their sense of proficiency and mastery [1014]. Finally, relatedness emphasizes the importance of interpersonal connections. When students feel understood, valued, and supported by their peers and instructors, they are more likely to be motivated and engaged [1014]. The integration of these three needs in a teaching environment, which encourages students’ interests, provides supportive feedback, choices, and room for collaboration, as well as reflection, can foster intrinsic motivation [1519] and reduce stress in an educational setting [20, 21]. Furthermore, it has been demonstrated that a lack of intrinsic motivation is associated with depression and stress in undergraduate students [22]. Moreover, intrinsic motivation leads to the use of more deep learning [2325] as well as better academic performance [5, 9, 11, 2528].

The aim of our study was to investigate whether intrinsic motivation could be promoted in medical education by implementing a set of didactic methods designed to support students’ basic psychological needs in accordance with SDT. These included moderated group discussions, team building exercises, giving students freedom of choice regarding their individual focal topics, and implementing exclusively strengths-based positive feedback. We also examined whether intrinsic motivation would in turn contribute to a reduction in medical students’ experience of stress during a one-week psychiatric practical course. Additionally, we analyzed whether the new course concept would enable students to achieve exam results equal to their peers in the control group despite dedicating a part of the course time to these methods and not giving specific critical feedback.

Hypotheses:

  1. An innovative teaching method optimized according to the SDT can promote students’ intrinsic motivation.

  2. Furthermore, it can facilitate lower stress levels.

  3. A favorable development of motivational factors can lead to a reduction in stress.

  4. The level of performance in exams remains unchanged.

Methods

Participants, course structure and ethical approval

Fourth-year medical students completing a mandatory five-day practical course at our psychiatric clinic participated in this study in the summer semester of 2022.

The one-week course was offered during most weeks throughout the semester and in the early part of the semester break. Students were free to sign up for their preferred time slot online on a first-come, first-serve basis at the beginning of the semester. The groups of five to eight students were subsequently randomized so that half of them were taught according to the usual didactic concept and the other half using an innovative concept improved according to the principles of the SDT. Lecturers were informed which study arm they were teaching. To ensure consistent implementation of the study protocol, they received a briefing on the procedures relevant to their group, along with a handbook containing precise instructions, dos and don’ts, and a comprehensive timetable.

The students were blinded in the sense that they were not informed which study arm they were participating in. All students took part in the survey voluntarily and were explicitly informed that participation would not impact their exam grades and that the data would be processed anonymously.

This study was submitted to the Institutional Review Board (ethical committee) of the Friedrich-Alexander University Erlangen-Nuremberg and received a designation of exempt according to § 15 BO (professional code of conduct for the physicians of Bavaria). Therefore, a need for consent to participate was deemed unnecessary by the afore mentioned university’s review board according to national regulations.

Course concept

The course comprised five consecutive afternoons of practical teaching (Monday to Friday, 4–5 h daily, overall 21 h) at our psychiatric clinic and concluded with an objective structured clinical examination (OSCE) the following Monday. Prior to on-site practical education, students completed a multimedia online course (10 h) at their own pace, providing a theoretical background for the practical part.

Every day during on-site teaching, real and simulated patients were each interviewed by one of the students. Thereafter, the simulated patients, fellow students, and the lecturer provided feedback on the student’s performance. During the OSCE, students were given a standardized patient scenario and graded on taking the medical history, applying communication models, and giving a case report.

Our innovation encompassed four newly implemented methods (Table 1).

Table 1.

Differences in the didactical concepts of the innovation group and the control group and the respective target factors of self-determination theory (SDT)

Innovation Control Target factor
1) Framework Moderated group discussions twice daily No structured framework

relatedness,

autonomy

2) Breaks Team building tasks Breaks at students’ free disposal relatedness
3) Case distribution Cases distributed by the students Cases assigned by the lecturer autonomy
4) Feedback Strengths-based positive feedback Critical constructive feedback competence, relatedness
  1. At the beginning and end of each course day, students gathered with their lecturer for a five-minute structured group discussion with an extended 30-minute introductory round on their first day and a 15-minute concluding discussion at the end of the last day. The aim was to provide an open, supportive environment where students felt comfortable expressing their emotions and concerns. Additionally, they were given a space to reflect on expectations for the course, their learning process, and takeaways from each day, as well as positive and potentially difficult experiences throughout the course and with psychiatric patients encountered in their education and career thus far. The method was implemented to support the basic need for relatedness by promoting group cohesion and psychological safety, thereby enhancing students’ sense of belonging. This process also contributed to autonomy support by encouraging students to experience task relevance and understand the rationale of their learning activities in their personal and professional development. In contrast, the control group did not engage in structured group discussions, reducing opportunities for students to connect with each other and the lecturer on a personal level and to reflect on task relevance and rationale.

  2. Instead of having a scheduled break to use as they wish, all students engaged in a group activity to further support the need for relatedness by promoting cooperation and train their communication skills. All activities were designed to create a space for teamwork and reflection on collaborative learning. Students were, for instance, asked to coordinate through non-verbal communication when counting up to 30 or to work together in lowering a long stick to the floor while supporting it on both their pointer fingers at all times. The control group, by contrast, did not engage in these structured activities, which may have limited the development of interpersonal connections.

  3. To strengthen their sense of autonomy, our innovation group students were able to freely distribute the topics, patients, time slots, and number of interviews among themselves. This allowed students to have a sense of choice and ownership over their learning experience, which is a key factor in fostering intrinsic motivation. On the other hand, the control group had their interviews assigned by the lecturer in advance, which restricted their autonomy and may have reduced their sense of control over the learning process.

  4. Lastly, students, teachers, and simulated patients in the innovation group were instructed to give only strengths-based positive feedback. Receiving positive feedback has been shown to enhance students’ perceived competence [8] as well as motivation and confidence [29]. By pointing out which aspects were particularly well handled by the interviewer, without giving critical or negative feedback on weaker aspects of their performance, we aimed to improve the student’s sense of competence. Additionally, the goal was to reduce pressure and stress caused by fear of negative responses to their performance, thereby also strengthening the group’s relatedness. The control group followed the guideline of giving critical constructive feedback to the interviewer, mentioning both positive and negative aspects of their performance. The method may fall short in fully supporting competence, as students may leave feeling less sure of their abilities after receiving detailed critiques of their performance and weaknesses by the group.

OSCE testing

Simulated patients performed one of four standardized clinical cases (depression, dementia, schizophrenia, or obsessive-compulsive disorder). Students were graded by examiners that differed from their lecturers. To facilitate a fair exam, we used a standardized rubric, rating the student’s performance out of 60 points in conducting a mental status examination, including taking the psychopathological history, using communication models, and giving a conclusory case presentation. The point score is used for research purposes and to monitor the didactic quality of the course, while only a pass (≥ 36 points) or fail grade is registered on students’ grade record. The same OSCE had been conducted for several past semesters at our psychiatric university hospital, showing no improvement in the overall results over time, which indicates that previous knowledge of the exam structure is not advantageous to students’ performance; rather, their actual competence is examined [30].

Questionnaires

The students answered previously published and well-established questionnaires online before starting their first day of practical teaching and again at the end of the last day of the course, three days before the OSCE (Fig. 1). All questionnaires were administered in German, the language of instruction, to prevent potential language-related inaccuracies.

Fig. 1.

Fig. 1

Data collected before (t0) and after (t1) the course week included the revised 2-factor study process questionnaire (R-SPQ-2 F), the learning self-regulation questionnaire (LSRQ), the task evaluation questionnaire of the intrinsic motivation inventory (IMI) and the perceived autonomy support learning climate questionnaire (LCQ) as well as self-reported stress on a visual analog scale (Stress VAS) and the course grade achieved in the objective structured clinical exam (OSCE) on Monday after the course week

  • Before the course, we collected the revised 2-factor study process questionnaire (R-SPQ-2 F) with 20 items to measure students’ deep and surface learning approaches [31]. It comprises the subscales of deep and surface motive, as well as deep and surface strategy, which are evaluated via a 5-point Likert scale [1 (never/rarely true) to 5 (always/almost always true)] and added up to calculate each approach score [31]. The R-SPQ-2 F has been validated and shown to be reliable, with reported Cronbach’s alpha values of 0.73 for deep approach and 0.64 for the surface approach subscale [31]. The questionnaire was used to evaluate how students’ deep and surface learning approaches would influence students’ performance compared to factors directly targeted by our course concept.

  • We collected the interviewing version of the learning self-regulation questionnaire (LSRQ) [32] both before and after the course. It comprises 14 items on the subscales “autonomous regulation”, reflecting intrinsic motivation, and “controlled regulation”, reflecting extrinsic motivation, based on a 7-point Likert scale [1 (not at all true) to 7 (very true)]. In past studies the alpha reliabilities for these two subscales have been approximately 0.75 for controlled regulation and 0.80 for autonomous regulation. Validation was done at the level of these two categories [32]. The questionnaire allows the calculation of a “relative autonomy index” by subtracting controlled from autonomous regulation subscale scores [32]. It was used examine differences in students’ styles of regulation in our two study arms after the course while being able to control for their initial values from before the course.

  • Before and after the course, we assessed stress levels during the past week using a visual analog scale (VAS) operationalized from 0 to 100, so that we could compare the values after the course in the two study arms and account for their base line before the course. Construct validity of VAS for assessing stress has been established through comparison with other well-established tools for measuring stress [33, 34].

  • At the end of the course, students answered the task evaluation questionnaire of the intrinsic motivation inventory (IMI), which comprises four subscales [35]. The “interest/enjoyment” subscale is considered a self-reported measure of intrinsic motivation. “Perceived competence”, “perceived choice”, which reflects a sense of autonomy, and “pressure/tension” are considered predictors of intrinsic motivation. Each of the 22 items is evaluated on a 7-point Likert scale [1 (not at all true) to 7 (very true)] [35]. There has been found strong support for the validity of the IMI as well as adequate reliability of its subscales [36]. The questionnaire was used to examine differences between the two study arms after the course, as well as interactions between the different factors and motivation.

  • Additionally, we collected the perceived autonomy support learning climate questionnaire (LCQ) with 15 items after the course, which also uses a 7-point Likert scale [1 (not at all true) to 7 (very true)] [37]. The validated questionnaire has shown to be reliable, with reported Cronbach’s alpha values of 0.71 [38]. It was used to evaluate whether the differences in didactical concepts between the two groups could be detected in their perceived autonomy support by and sense of relatedness to the lecturer.

  • Finally, we asked the students to give freeform feedback about their experience with the course with a separate question each for positive and negative remarks to clearly distinguish between their evaluation of the mentioned factors. Meanwhile, without limiting answers to certain topics or aspects of the course, students were free to report on exactly the points they felt were important to them.

Data analysis

The data were analyzed through t-tests, multiple linear regressions, simple and parallel mediations, and hierarchic linear modeling.

Regression analyses and t-tests were performed using SPSS (SPSS® 29, IBM®, Armonk, USA). Assumptions of linearity, normality, and homoscedasticity were assessed, ensuring the appropriateness of the models. We calculated effect sizes (d) for group differences [39] with their confidence intervals (CI) and applied the empirically derived guidelines of 0.15 being a small effect, 0.36 a medium effect, and 0.65 a large effect in social psychology [40]. The models’ goodness-of-fit was interpreted according to Cohen [39].

Mediation analyses were performed using the PROCESS macro by Hayes (PROCESS Procedure for SPSS® Version 4.2, Andrew F. Hayes, Calgary, Canada), which uses ordinary least squares regression, yielding unstandardized path coefficients for total, direct, and indirect effects. Bootstrapping with 5000 samples together with heteroscedasticity-consistent standard errors [41] was employed to compute the confidence intervals and inferential statistics. The effects were deemed significant when the confidence interval for the indirect effect did not include zero. Visual inspection of scatterplots after LOESS smoothing indicated an approximately linear relationship between all variables involved.

We used hierarchic linear modeling or HLM (HLM® 8, Scientific Software International, Inc., Skokie, USA) with level-1 variables describing data collected from a single student and level-2 variables comprising data collected in the same group (same lecturer, time slot, examiner, study arm). The results of the hierarchic linear modeling are reported as the difference between slopes (b) of the regression curves and its significance (p).

Statistical significance was set at p ≤ .05.

Results

Participant characteristics

In the summer semester of 2022, 148 medical students participated in the course, 73 of whom participated in a group with the new didactical concept and 75 of whom participated in a control group with our conventional concept. We obtained complete sets of data from 105 students (return rate: 71%), comprising the questionnaires taken before (t0) and after the course (t1) as well as the results from the OSCE (t2). Notably, for variables assessed at t0, no statistically significant group differences were found using two-sided t-tests (Table 2). Nonetheless, preexisting non-significant but potentially relevant group differences, such as stress scores, were controlled for in our subsequent data analysis.

Table 2.

Characteristics of the study participants at t0 before the course; values are given as the mean (± standard deviation)

Innovation Control
N (female/male/other) 53 (41/11/1) 52 (37/15/0)
Age [years] 24.0 (± 3.1) 24.3 (± 2.9)
Study duration [semesters] 8.1 (± 0.3) 8.1 (± 0.2)
R-SPQ-2 F Deep motive 3.40 (± 0.57) 3.36 (± 0.52)
Deep strategy 3.13 (± 0.49) 3.19 (± 0.49)
Surface motive 2.38 (± 0.57) 2.53 (± 0.59)
Surface strategy 2.72 (± 0.63) 2.93 (± 0.61)
LSRQ Autonomous regulation 6.02 (± 0.66) 5.86 (± 0.72)
Controlled regulation 3.97 (± 1.00) 3.96 (± 0.91)
Relative autonomy index 2.05 (± 1.23) 1.90 (± 1.16)
Stress 42.89 (± 25.09) 52.23 (± 23.55)

Greater intrinsic motivation, higher perceived autonomy, and less controlled regulation in the innovation group

With respect to our hypothesis that a teaching concept according to the SDT could promote students’ intrinsic motivation, we conducted one-sided t-tests to examine group differences at the end of the course. Our control group served as the reference group.

After the course (t1), students in the innovation group showed significantly greater levels of intrinsic motivation regarding the course, as measured by the IMI (MC = 5.67 ± 0.78; MI = 6.00 ± 0.81; p = .019), with a standard error difference of 0.16 (Fig. 2A). The effect size for this difference was medium (d = 0.41; 95% CI [0.02, 0.80]).

Fig. 2.

Fig. 2

Significant differences between the control and innovation group were found after the course week (t1) regarding intrinsic motivation measured on the interest/enjoyment subscale of the intrinsic motivation inventory (IMI) (p = .019) and perceived choice (p = .048) measured on the IMI’s choice subscale. Further significant differences were found on the learning self-regulation questionnaire (LSRQ) subscale of controlled regulation (p = .033) and its relative autonomy index (p = .037), which is calculated by subtracting the controlled regulation subscore from the questionnaire’s autonomous regulation subscore. All (sub)scores shown are measured on a 7-point Likert scale

Similarly, a greater sense of choice (IMI) was observed in the innovation group at t1 (MC = 4.09 ± 1.17; MI = 4.50 ± 1.32; p = .048), with a standard error difference of 0.24 (Fig. 2B). The effect size was small (d = 0.33; 95% CI [-0.06, 0.71]).

Additionally, the innovation group scored significantly lower on the LSRQ’s controlled regulation subscale at t1 (MC = 4.01 ± 0.92; MI = 3.66 ± 1.00; p = .033), with a standard error difference of 0.19 (Fig. 2C). The effect size for this difference was medium (d = -0.36; 95% CI [-0.75, 0.02]).

Correspondingly, the relative autonomy index of the LSRQ was significantly higher in the innovation group at t1 (MC = 2.26 ± 1.12; MI = 2.66 ± 1.17; p = .037), with a standard error difference of 0.22 (Fig. 2D). The effect size was small (d = 0.35; 95% CI [-0.03, 0.74]).

Furthermore, we conducted a multiple linear regression analysis with controlled regulation (LSRQ) at the end of the course as the dependent variable. The independent variables considered were the study arm and the initial controlled regulation at t0 to control for preexisting individual differences (Supplementary material 1). The model demonstrated a high level of goodness-of-fit, with an R² of 0.44 (adjusted R² = 0.42). Both the initial controlled regulation at t0 (p < .001) and the study arm (p = .016) were significant predictors of the controlled regulation at t1, with the overall model being statistically significant (F(2, 102) = 39.36, p < .001). The β coefficients for t0 controlled regulation was 0.64, indicating that higher initial values were associated with higher controlled regulation at t1. The study arm had a β coefficient of -0.18, meaning that participants in the innovation group showed lower levels of controlled regulation at t1. The model’s statistical power was 1.00.

When conducting the same regression analysis for the relative autonomy index (LSRQ) at t1, only the initial relative autonomy index was a significant predictor (p < .001), while the study arm was only trend-significant (p = .064).

Among the variables assessed at t1, no statistically significant group differences were found regarding.

  • IMI competence (MD = 0.08, p = .322).

  • IMI pressure (MD = -0.18, p = .213).

  • LSRQ autonomous regulation (MD = 0.05, p = .333).

  • LCQ learning climate, with this questionnaire being trend-significant (MD = 0.21, p = .070).

When assessing freeform feedback in the post-course survey at t1, we found a distinct difference regarding the participants’ perceptions of the group climate. Specifically, 19 of the students in the innovation group actively mentioned it as a positive aspect in their feedback, while only 7 in the control group did so. No negative remarks were made in either group.

Lower stress levels in the innovation group at the end of the course

Regarding our hypothesis that an SDT-based teaching concept could reduce students’ stress, a one-sided t-test revealed significantly lower stress levels in the innovation group after the course (MC = 52.10 ± 22.15; MI = 40.43 ± 20.65; p = .003), with a standard error difference of 4.18 (Fig. 3). The effect size indicated a medium effect (d = -0.55; 95% CI [-0.93, -0.15]).

Fig. 3.

Fig. 3

A significant difference between the control and innovation group was found regarding stress levels throughout the last week (p = .003) reported at t1 after the course on a visual analog scale from 0–100

To also account for stress levels reported at t0, which showed a trend-significant difference (p = .054) despite randomization, we conducted a multiple linear regression analysis considering the study arm and t0 stress as independent variables and t1 stress as the dependent variable (Supplementary material 2). The R² for the overall model was 0.27 (adjusted R² = 0.25), indicating a high goodness-of-fit. The regression analysis suggested that both stress levels at t0 (p < .001) and the study arm (p = .040) significantly contributed to predicting stress levels at t1 (F(2, 102) = 18.66, p < .001). The β coefficients for the independent variables were 0.45 for t0 stress and − 0.18 for the study arm, suggesting that higher stress before the course was associated with higher stress levels reported at the end, while being in the innovation group was linked to lower reported stress levels after the course week. A power analysis was conducted, and the statistical power of the model was estimated to be 0.99.

Optimizing motivational factors reduced stress

We conducted further analyses to examine whether the favorable outcome regarding motivational factors facilitated the lower stress levels found among students in the innovation group.

To identify predictors of stress at t1 among the preexisting individual differences present at t0 and to examine the cumulative influence of the study arm yielded during the course, t0 stress, age, gender, t0 R-SPQ-2 F, t0 LSRQ, and the study arm were considered as potential independent variables in a multiple linear regression analysis (Supplementary material 3). Variables were included via a stepwise approach. The final model included 3 statistically significant variables, namely, t0 stress (p < .001), the t0 LSRQ relative autonomy index (p = .029), and the study arm (p = .040). With an R² of 0.30 (adjusted R² = 0.28), the model provided a strong model fit. The three variables were collectively significant in predicting t1 stress (F(3, 101) = 14.55, p < .001), with β coefficients of 0.41 for t0 stress, -0.19 for the t0 LSRQ relative autonomy index, and − 0.18 for the study arm. The model thus indicated that higher t0 stress predicted higher reported stress at the end of the course week (t1), while a more internalized locus of motivation (Fig. 4A) and participation in the innovation group were associated with lower stress reported at t1. The statistical power was 0.99.

Fig. 4.

Fig. 4

The learning self-regulation questionnaire (LSRQ) relative autonomy index at t0 before the course, which is calculated by subtracting the controlled regulation subscore from the questionnaire’s autonomous regulation subscore, showed a significant correlation with students’ stress reported at t1 after the course week. A significant correlation with stress at t1 was also found concerning the questionnaire’s controlled regulation subscale at t1. Furthermore, there were significant simple correlations between stress at t1 and the intrinsic motivation inventory (IMI) subscales of pressure, interest/enjoyment, and choice, whereas the competence subscale did not reach significance. Stress was measured on a visual analog scale from 0–100, while all other variables were reported on a 7-point Likert scale

To identify how factors assessed at the end of the course week were related to t1 stress, we constructed another model including t0 stress, age, gender, t0 R-SPQ-2 F (considered stable at t1), t1 LSRQ, t1 IMI, t1 LCQ, and the study arm as potential independent variables (Supplementary material 4). The study arm was included to account for the cumulative effect of our intervention otherwise not measured by the questionnaires collected at t1. Variables were again included using a stepwise approach (entry and removal criteria: p = .050).

IMI pressure (p < .001), t0 stress (p < .001), the study arm (p = .024), and IMI competence (p = .027) were eventually shown to be significant predictors of t1 stress. The model demonstrated a high level of goodness-of-fit with an R² of 0.67 (adjusted R² = 0.45). Collectively, the 4 variables were able to predict t1 stress with statistical significance (F(4, 100) = 20.42, p < .001). The respective β coefficients for these predictors were 0.46 for IMI pressure, 0.33 for t0 stress, -0.17 for the study arm, and 0.18 for IMI competence, suggesting that higher levels of perceived pressure (Fig. 4C) and competence (Fig. 4B) as well as higher stress at t0 predicted more elevated stress levels reported at t1, while being in the innovation group was associated with lower stress at t1. The statistical power was 1.00.

When excluding the study arm as an independent variable from this regression analysis under the assumption that it would have influenced the other variables at t1, the predictors included as significant remained the same, with the additional inclusion of t1 IMI interest (Supplementary material 5). This model demonstrated a high level of goodness-of-fit with an R² of 0.45 (adjusted R² = 0.43), and the 4 variables were able to predict t1 stress with statistical significance (F(4, 100) = 20.37, p < .001). The respective β coefficients for these predictors were 0.44 for IMI pressure, 0.35 for t0 stress, 0.22 for IMI competence, and − 0.19 for IMI interest, suggesting that in addition to our earlier findings, higher intrinsic motivation (Fig. 4D) was associated with lower stress at t1, indicating that this factor might have had the most influential effect on stress among the variables influenced by the innovation. The statistical power was 1.00.

We conducted a series of mediation analyses to explore whether the relationship between the study arm and students’ stress levels at the end of the course (t1) could be explained through differences in motivation.

An initial total effect of the study arm on t1 stress was observed (B = -11.66; p = .007). When t0 stress was added as a covariate to this model to account for the students’ baseline of stress before the course, the total effect of the study arm on t1 stress remained significant (B = -7.91; p = .043). The covariate t0 stress was also included in all further mediation analyses as a baseline control (Fig. 5).

Fig. 5.

Fig. 5

The relationship between the study arm and stress levels throughout the last week reported at t1 after the course week are fully mediated by intrinsic motivation measured on the interest/enjoyment subscale of the intrinsic motivation inventory (IMI interest) and the learning self-regulation questionnaire’s controlled regulation subscale (LSRQ CR) as parallel mediators. Both parallel mediators are measured on a 7-point Likert scale, and stress was reported on a visual analog scale from 0–100. Students’ stress reported at t0 before the course was taken into consideration as a covariate

The variables that our prior analyses had shown to be influenced by the study arm did not show significant mediation effects individually. However, the relationship between these variables and t1 stress was significant for IMI interest (B = -5.90; p = .019), t1 LSRQ controlled regulation (B = 4.754; p = .034), and t1 LSRQ relative autonomy index (B = -5.00; p = .009), while this was not the case for IMI choice.

Additionally, we tested models with multiple (parallel) mediators to investigate potential combined mediation effects.

When considering both IMI interest and t1 LSRQ controlled regulation simultaneously as mediator, the influence of the study arm on these two mediators, previously shown to be significant through t-tests and regression analyses, did not reach significance in this model. However, the impact of both IMI interest (B = -6.07; p = .016) and t1 SRC controlled regulation (B = 4.91; p = .025) on t1 stress was significant (Fig. 5).

There was no statistically significant direct effect of the study arm on t1 stress when accounting for the combined effect of interest and controlled regulation (B = -4.91; p = .208), indicating that the effect of the study arm on stress at t1 was fully mediated by these two variables. Furthermore, the total indirect effect, representing the overall mediation through IMI interest and t1 SRC controlled regulation, was significant and estimated at -2.99 (95%-CI [-6.56, -0.18]), which further supports a significant mediation. This finding suggested that higher levels of intrinsic motivation (Fig. 4D) and lower levels of controlled regulation (Fig. 4E) collectively mediated the relationship between students participating in the innovation group and them showing lower stress levels at t1.

When IMI choice (Fig. 4F) was added as a third mediator to the previous model, the total indirect effect of the three mediators was also significant, while the direct effect of the study arm on t1 stress was not, suggesting a full mediation. However, neither the effect of the study arm on IMI choice, nor the effect of IMI choice on t1 stress were significant in this combined model.

When replacing controlled regulation as a mediator with the relative autonomy index in either of the two mediation models, no significant indirect effects were found.

Inferior OSCE results in the motivation group due to structural rather than individual factors such as stress or motivation

When examining whether both study arms had achieved equal exam results, a two-sided t-test revealed significantly lower overall OSCE scores in the innovation group (MC = 48.03 ± 4.14; MI = 45.81 ± 4.12; p = .007), with a standard error difference of 0.81 (Fig. 6A). Notably, the significant difference also persisted when the most and least highly scoring topics and/or examiners were excluded as possible confounders. The effect size was medium (d = -0.54; 95% CI [-0.93, -0.15]).

Fig. 6.

Fig. 6

A significant difference between the control and innovation group was found regarding the exam score (p = .007) in the objective structured clinical exam (OSCE), which was graded from 0–60 at t2 on Monday after the course week. The revised 2-factor study process questionnaire (R-SPQ-2 F) subscale of surface motive, which is evaluated on a 5-point Likert scale at t0 before the course, shows a significant simple correlation with students’ exam scores in the objective structured clinical exam (OSCE), which was graded from 0–60 at t2 on Monday after the course week

To analyze the influence of preexisting individual differences present at t0 as well as the cumulative influence of the study arm on the OSCE exam results, we conducted a multiple regression analysis (Supplementary material 6). The study arm, gender, age, t0 LSRQ, t0 R-SPQ-2 F, and t0 stress were considered as potential independent factors. The OSCE result served as the dependent variable. We employed a stepwise approach for variable inclusion. The final model included the study arm and R-SPQ-2 F surface motive as independent variables and demonstrated a moderate goodness-of-fit, with an R² of 0.13 (adjusted R² = 0.12). Both the study arm (p = .002) and t0 R-SPQ-2 F surface motive (p = .007) as well as the overall model showed statistically significant predictive power for the OSCE result (F(2, 102) = 7.79, p < .001), with corresponding β coefficients of -0.29 for the study arm and − 0.26 for the R-SPQ-2 F surface motive. This finding suggested that being in the innovation group and having more surface learning motivation (Fig. 6B) were associated with poorer exam results. The statistical power was 0.95.

Simple and parallel mediations were performed to analyze whether this direct path of the study arm predicting students’ OSCE results was mediated by any of the group differences in motivation or stress that we observed. t1 IMI interest, t1 IMI choice, t1 LSRQ controlled regulation, t1 LSRQ relative autonomy index, and t1 stress, being the factors influenced by the study arm, were considered as potential simple mediators of the relationship between the study arm and the OSCE result (B = -2.22; p = .008). For parallel mediations, we considered both IMI interest and IMI choice in combination with either of the LSRQ variables as well as the respective models with t1 stress as an additional mediator. No significant total indirect effect of the mediator(s) was found in any of the models, and the direct effect of the study arm on the OSCE results remained significant in all models after adding the mediator(s), indicating that the relationship was neither partially nor fully mediated by stress or motivation.

Hierarchical linear modeling was employed to examine the predictive factors of students’ OSCE scores, with students (level 1) nested within teaching groups (level 2).

The intercept-only model with OSCE results as the outcome variable indicated that variance existed at both levels of the data structure (χ² = 60.38, p < .001). We found an intraclass correlation coefficient (ICC) of 0.71; thus, 71% of the variance in total OSCE scores was between groups, and 29% was between students within a given group.

We tested a combined level 1 and 2 model to examine which structural and individual factors had significant predictive value for the OSCE results.

Based on our prior results and hypotheses, we included the t0 R-SPQ-2F surface motive and surface strategy reflecting a surface learning approach, Δ stress, Δ LSRQ controlled regulation, and Δ LSRQ autonomous regulation to account for students’ baseline as well as final levels of stress and motivation and the exam topic as individual variables at level 1. At level 2, the study arm, time slot, lecturer, and examiner were included as structural variables. When testing this two-level model, the regression coefficients with robust standard errors relating the predictors to the OSCE results were significant for the study arm (b = -2.89, p < .001), time slot (b = -0.84, p = .002) and lecturer (b = 0.38, p = .016) at level 2 and the exam topic at level 1 (b = -1.03, p = .006). All other level 1 variables as well as the examiner (b = -0.08, p = .327) were not significant. This indicates that the exam results were effectively predicted by structural rather than individual factors and the exam topic. A decrease or increase in students’ stress or motivation as well as the degree of surface learning approach, on the other hand, had no significant predictive value. The predictors included in the combined HLM accounted for approximately 52% of the reduction in unexplained variance in the exam results (pseudo-R2 = 0.52).

By analyzing students’ free-form feedback, we found that 28 of the students in the innovation group (corresponding to 53%) mentioned that the exclusive use of strengths-based positive feedback hindered their learning process, while three students noted that it helped them feel more at ease. In the control group, six students mentioned that they enjoyed feedback, including positive and negative constructive criticism, while none mentioned that they were dissatisfied with the principle.

Discussion

The data presented in this study show that a course concept optimized according to the SDT can increase intrinsic motivation for the task, foster students’ perceived autonomy, reduce controlled regulation, and ultimately mitigate the sense of stress in medical students during a one-week psychiatric course. Contrary to our hypothesis, the course concept led to a weaker performance in the exam, which did however not seem to be a result of the differences in motivation and stress.

The innovation succeeded in promoting an improved sense of perceived choice, as a measure of autonomy, being one of the three targeted base factors strengthening intrinsic motivation according to the SDT.

The IMI revealed superior intrinsic motivation among medical students taught according to the new concept, which was shown to play a decisive role in reducing their stress levels. This reflects the importance of engaging students in an autonomy-supportive learning process by giving them task choice and providing task relevance and rationale to promote interest in a subject and vice versa [42], including effects as far-reaching as the choice of their future specialty [14, 43, 44].

We also found a shift in the balance from autonomous to controlled regulation, while improvements in autonomous regulation itself did not reach significance in the LSRQ. The fact that the IMI detected significantly higher intrinsic motivation in our innovation group, while the LSRQ did not show a significant difference for autonomous regulation between the study arms, reflects the intricacy of measuring changes in intrinsic motivation over a short period. In our study, this might additionally be explained by the already high scores on the LSRQ subscale for autonomous regulation in both groups at t0, possibly leading to a ceiling effect.

We did not observe significant improvements in perceived competence or relatedness, as the other two targeted SDT factors, or in pressure. Regarding relatedness, students’ freeform feedback did suggest that the innovation group experienced an improved group climate. With the LCQ focusing mainly on relatedness to and autonomy support by the lecturer, our questionnaires might not have been able to detect this difference. The IMI subscale of “relatedness”, which has not been fully validated according to the Center for Self-Determination Theory, has shown satisfactory validity in some studies [45, 46] and might be able to better assess the targeted changes in future studies.

The innovation’s impact on stress perception was notable, with significantly lower stress reported in the innovation group after the course than in the control group. This was also true when accounting for differences in pre-course stress levels. Successful initiatives to reduce stress in an educational setting at medical schools or universities in general have been described by various studies, with the most effective methods being mindfulness practices, cognitive-behavioral and relaxation strategies, and social ability training [4749]. The positive correlation between intrinsic motivation and mental well-being (including lower stress levels) is well established, and it is known that they can successfully be promoted bidirectionally by the aforementioned measures [50]. Nevertheless, research examining active approaches to promoting motivation with the explicit aim of thereby reducing stress in university students is limited.

Prior research indicates that stress levels in university students are influenced by a range of internal and external factors [51], among which cognitive processes and self-esteem [52] as well as the SDT and the job demands-resources model [53] have been described as key factors in understanding and counteracting students’ distress. As anticipated, we found that heightened intrinsic motivation mitigated stress, whereas increased controlled regulation exacerbated stress levels. This seemingly mediated the positive effect of the didactical innovation on stress levels reported at the end of the course. This finding aligns with previous research showing that greater autonomy support promotes positive affect and better performance through autonomous motivation, while increased self-criticism leading to negative affect is mediated by controlled motivation [54]. Although we identified perceived competence and pressure to be among the most impactful predictors of stress at t1, our innovation did not succeed in significantly improving these factors by providing only strengths-based positive feedback. Therefore, other methods should be explored to that end. Furthermore, greater perceived competence was associated with heightened stress, seemingly contrary to SDT, possibly reflecting greater self-imposed expectations to perform well.

Our parallel mediation analysis indicated that intrinsic motivation and controlled regulation fully mediated the relationship between the study arm and stress levels at t1. However, the lack of significance of the relationship between the study arm and the mediators in this combined model suggested that the changes induced by our innovation might not have been substantial enough to unequivocally determine their effects and interactions. While the innovation aimed to cater to students’ basic psychological needs, it is possible that not all SDT domains were addressed with enough depth or specificity to produce significant effects. For instance, autonomy-supportive teaching involves a broad range of actions, from offering meaningful choices to encouraging initiative and self-regulation, which may not have been fully implemented across all dimensions of the intervention [5, 55]. It is furthermore plausible that the interactions between a learning environment, motivational factors, and stress extend beyond the effects directly measured in our study and that other influential factors may have contributed to our results. Other studies have likewise found that mediating effects concerning intrinsic motivation and the satisfaction of basic psychological needs are multifaceted [5659]. Longitudinal studies with multiple assessment points over extended periods as well as in diverse settings could provide deeper insights into the complex interactions between autonomy-supportive teaching, the satisfaction of students’ basic psychological needs, and their motivation, stress, and performance.

Contrary to our hypothesis that our new course concept would not affect students’ performance in the OSCE, the innovation group achieved lower exam scores. However, our mediation and HLM results suggested that this was likely due to structural aspects, such as the lecturer, exam topic, time slot within the semester and the course concept itself. Neither the lower controlled regulation nor the lower stress levels in the innovation group appear to have led to a decrease in students’ performance. In line with SDT, this reflects that a focus on external rewards does not foster better academic performance, which corresponds to earlier research showing that successful performance in an exam is not promoted by external regulation [9]. Our finding that a greater degree of the R-SPQ-2 F’s surface learning motive was a significant negative predictor of exam results further suggests that having extrinsically motivated reasons for learning a subject does not lead to better academic performance. In fact, students with stronger surface motives performed worse than their peers who had fewer surface motives. A possible explanation for the weaker exam performance was brought up in the freeform feedback contributed by the students themselves: a majority of the students highlighted the lack of critical feedback as hindering their ability to address their weaknesses precisely and to work toward improving their interviewing skills more effectively. Generally, receiving feedback is a useful tool for improving performance [6063]. While exclusively positive feedback has been found to be beneficial to both performance and emotional responses in a workplace environment [64], the feedback we received rather reflects the theory that in particular high-achieving students benefit from feedback that challenges them [65]. The innovation group may have stayed within a “comfort zone” due to a lesser degree of challenging guidance to help them make difficult but achievable progress limiting in a more demanding “learning zone”, in line with the zone of proximal development theory [66]. Furthermore, the exclusively strengths-based positive feedback might have made students overconfident in their skills, which in turn might have led to them placing less focus on improving and preparing for the exam. While unbalanced negative critique can deter students’ motivation [29], criticism can also be motivational, when given with suggestions for future improvement and encouragement regarding the student’s ability to improve [67]. Therefore, the preexisting feedback method used in our control group, which included both positive reinforcement and constructive criticism, was likely already aligned with SDT principles. Our new approach may only constitute an improvement in settings where feedback is imbalanced or overly critical. In general, the effectiveness of feedback is highly context- and method-dependent [29, 60, 62, 63] Its influence on cognitive outcomes seems to be stronger than that on motivational and behavioral outcomes [62], possibly making other factors in educational concepts more suitable targets for enhancing motivational aspects and reducing pressure. Another interpretation of our findings could be that each student conducting only one to two interviews might not have given them enough time and opportunity to understand their mistakes, weaknesses, and potential for improvement through self-reflection and abstraction via model learning when exclusively giving strengths-based positive feedback. This finding corresponds to observations that positive-only feedback is more effective when applied to longer tasks rather than to shorter ones [68]. Complementing strengths-based positive feedback with constructive, change-oriented feedback delivered in an autonomy-supportive manner might function as the most effective approach because it encourages and motivates students while enabling them to make objective progress and simultaneously gain a sense of accomplishment. This would align with earlier findings with a focus on medical education [61, 69] as well as general higher education [70]. Moreover, research suggests that the way feedback is delivered can significantly impact student motivation and that autonomy-supportive feedback, which provides a clear rationale, acknowledges the student’s perspective, and fosters both competence and autonomy, is key to enhancing intrinsic motivation [71, 72].

Despite our students’ feedback and our data aligning with the theory that using exclusively strengths-based positive feedback did not lead to the desired outcomes, we cannot conclusively rule out that it might still have had a positive influence on students’ motivation and stress. To determine each method’s individual effects, further research with multiple study arms and partial implementation of the didactic innovation would be needed.

Further limitations in our study include the potential bias introduced by self-reported measures, retesting questionnaires, and the study’s singular focus on a psychiatric clinical setting. We found an even distribution of population characteristics in the two groups; however, despite randomizing and evenly distributing course groups to account for exam periods with generally greater stress, there may be additional decisive factors that we did not register. The effects observed in our study, given its relatively short duration, may not fully represent the long-term outcomes expected in longer longitudinal studies. Further research would be needed for validating our results over extended periods. Furthermore, the impact of motivation on questionnaire response rates could lead to potential biases through data collection, specifically when examining motivation as an outcome variable in the study; however, the return rates of the control group and innovation group were nearly equal.

Potential inconsistencies with the implementation of our teaching methods by different lecturers were counteracted by briefing them on the procedure of the respective group before the start of each week, accompanied by a handbook with precise instructions, by the randomization of the lecturers’ assignment to teaching groups and by matching those that taught more than one teaching group to both study arms.

Conclusions

In summary, our didactical concept, which was designed in accordance with SDT to promote the basic psychological needs of autonomy, competence, and relatedness, positively impacted motivation and stress in line with our expectations. The weaker OSCE performance of the innovation group seemed to be rooted in a lack of critical feedback, although further research is needed to explore to what extent that finding is valid in other educational settings. Our data, consistent with previous research, suggest that pairing positive strengths-based positive feedback with constructive criticism might be most efficient in providing the conditions for optimal academic performance.

The complexity of the relationships between motivation, stress, and performance highlights the need for a deeper understanding of their multifaceted nature and how they can be positively influenced within medical as well as general education. Incorporating multidimensional assessment tools and conducting longitudinal studies will be necessary to comprehensively examine their relationships and interactions. In light of this complexity, the results of our study call for future research to investigate the transferability of innovative SDT-optimized teaching methods to other medical specialties as well as to non-medical education. Meanwhile, they give us grounds to be optimistic that teaching concepts aligned with SDT can be an important step toward the promotion of intrinsic motivation and stress reduction. Based on our findings, educators can effectively improve learning conditions by integrating SDT-based didactic methods into already existing curricula. For instance, simply offering students more task choices and opportunities for self-regulation or creating a learning space where they can feel part of a group could help foster intrinsic motivation while reducing stress. To best support students’ motivation and performance, educators can pair strengths-based feedback with critical, constructive criticism. Eventually, focusing on providing a learning experience that meets students’ basic psychological needs and fosters motivation can help them thrive in their educational environment.

Electronic Supplementary Material

Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.

Acknowledgements

The present work was performed in partial fulfillment of the requirements for obtaining the degree ‘Dr. med.’ of Nina Triebner.

Abbreviations

SDT

Self-determination theory

HLM

Hierarchic linear modeling

RCT

Randomized controlled trial

OSCE

Objective structured clinical exam

LSRQ

Learning self-regulation questionnaire

R-SPQ-2F

Revised 2-factor study process questionnaire

IMI

Intrinsic motivation inventory

LCQ

Perceived autonomy support learning climate questionnaire

VAS

Visual analog scale

Author contributions

NT, FS, MR, GMK, CR, SM, MB, JK, JU, and PS designed the study. NT, SM, MB, JU, and PS collected the data. The data were analyzed and evaluated by NT, FS, MR, CR, JK, JU, and PS. The statistical analyses were carried out by NT, FS, MR and PS. NT, JU, and PS drafted the manuscript. All the authors critically reviewed the manuscript and provided constructive comments to improve its quality. All authors have read and approved the final manuscript. All authors agree to be accountable for all aspects of the work.

Funding

Open Access funding enabled and organized by Projekt DEAL.

Data availability

The datasets used and/or analyzed during the current study are available from the corresponding author upon reasonable request.

Declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

This study was submitted to the Institutional Review Board (ethical committee) of the Friedrich-Alexander University Erlangen-Nuremberg and received a designation of exempt according to § 15 BO (professional code of conduct for the physicians of Bavaria). Therefore, a need for consent to participate was deemed unnecessary by the afore mentioned university’s review board according to national regulations. All students took part in the survey voluntarily and were explicitly informed that participation would not impact their exam grades and that the data would be processed anonymously.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing interests.

Footnotes

Publisher’s note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Janine Utza, Philipp Spitzer both authors contributed equally.

References

  • 1.Dyrbye LN, Thomas MR, Shanafelt TD. Systematic review of depression, anxiety, and other indicators of psychological distress among U.S. and Canadian medical students. Acad Medicine: J Association Am Med Colleges. 2006;81(4):354–73. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Erschens R, Keifenheim KE, Herrmann-Werner A, Loda T, Schwille-Kiuntke J, Bugaj TJ, et al. Professional burnout among medical students: systematic literature review and meta-analysis. Med Teach. 2019;41(2):172–83. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Dyrbye LN, Thomas MR, Shanafelt TD. Medical student distress: causes, consequences, and proposed solutions. Mayo Clin Proc. 2005;80(12):1613–22. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 4.Eley DS, Slavin SJ. Medical student mental health - the intransigent global dilemma: contributors and potential solutions. Med Teach. 2024;46(2):156–61. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Deci EL, Ryan RM. The what and why of goal pursuits: human needs and the self-determination of Behavior. Psychol Inq. 2000;11(4):227–68. [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Ryan RM, Deci EL. Intrinsic and extrinsic motivation from a self-determination theory perspective: definitions, theory, practices, and future directions. Contemp Educ Psychol. 2020;61:101860. [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Black AE, Deci EL. The effects of instructors’ autonomy support and students’ autonomous motivation on learning organic chemistry: a self-determination theory perspective. Sci Ed. 2000;84(6):740–56. [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Levesque C, Zuehlke AN, Stanek LR, Ryan RM. Autonomy and competence in German and American University students: a comparative study based on self-determination theory. J Educ Psychol. 2004;96(1):68–84. [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Howard JL, Bureau J, Guay F, Chong JXY, Ryan RM. Student Motivation and Associated outcomes: a Meta-analysis from self-determination theory. Perspect Psychol Science: J Association Psychol Sci. 2021;16(6):1300–23. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Deci EL, Ryan RM. Intrinsic motivation and self-determination in human behavior. Boston, MA: Springer US; 1985. [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Deci EL, Ryan RM. Self-determination theory: a macrotheory of human motivation, development, and health. Can Psychol / Psychologie Canadienne. 2008;49(3):182–5. [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Ryan RM, Deci EL. Self-determination theory and the facilitation of intrinsic motivation, social development, and well-being. Am Psychol. 2000;55(1):68–78. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Ryan RM, Deci EL. Self-determination theory: basic psychological needs in motivation, development, and wellness. New York: Guilford Press; 2017. [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Kusurkar RA, Cate TJ, van Asperen M, Croiset G. Motivation as an independent and a dependent variable in medical education: a review of the literature. Med Teach. 2011;33(5):e242–62. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Kusurkar RA, Croiset G. Autonomy support for autonomous motivation in medical education. Med Educ Online. 2015;20:27951. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Orsini C, Evans P, Binnie V, Ledezma P, Fuentes F. Encouraging intrinsic motivation in the clinical setting: teachers’ perspectives from the self-determination theory. Eur J Dent Education: Official J Association Dent Educ Europe. 2016;20(2):102–11. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Orsini C, Evans P, Jerez O. How to encourage intrinsic motivation in the clinical teaching environment? A systematic review from the self-determination theory. J Educational Evaluation Health Professions. 2015;12:8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Neufeld A, Hughton B, Muhammadzai J, McKague M, Malin G. Towards a better understanding of medical students’ mentorship needs: a self-determination theory perspective. Can Med Educ J. 2021;12(6):72–7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Vansteenkiste M, Soenens B, Waterschoot J. Catalyzing intrinsic and internalized motivation. In: O’Donnell AM, Barnes NC, Reeve J, editors. The Oxford handbook of educational psychology. New York: Oxford University Press; 2018. (Oxford library of psychology). [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Cate TJ, Kusurkar RA, Williams GC. How self-determination theory can assist our understanding of the teaching and learning processes in medical education. AMEE Guide 59 Med Teacher. 2011;33(12):961–73. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Neufeld A. Moving the Field Forward: Using Self-Determination Theory to Transform the Learning Environment in Medical Education. Teaching and learning in medicine 2023:1–6. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 22.Huang Y, Lv W, Wu J. Relationship between intrinsic motivation and undergraduate students’ depression and stress: the moderating effect of interpersonal conflict // relationship between intrinsic motivation and undergraduate students’ depression and stress. Psychol Rep. 2016;119(2):527–38. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Grolnick WS, Ryan RM. Autonomy in children’s learning: an experimental and individual difference investigation. J Personal Soc Psychol. 1987;52(5):890–8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Vansteenkiste M, Simons J, Lens W, Soenens B, Matos L. Examining the motivational impact of intrinsic versus extrinsic goal framing and autonomy-supportive versus internally controlling communication style on early adolescents’ academic achievement. Child Dev. 2005;76(2):483–501. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Vansteenkiste M, Zhou M, Lens W, Soenens B. Experiences of autonomy and control among Chinese learners: vitalizing or immobilizing? J Educ Psychol. 2005;97(3):468–83. [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Boggiano AK, Flink C, Shields A, Seelbach A, Barrett M. Use of techniques promoting students’ self-determination: effects on students’ analytic problem-solving skills. Motiv Emot. 1993;17(4):319–36. [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Vansteenkiste M, Simons J, Lens W, Sheldon KM, Deci EL. Motivating learning, performance, and persistence: the synergistic effects of intrinsic goal contents and autonomy-supportive contexts. J Personal Soc Psychol. 2004;87(2):246–60. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Orsini C, Binnie VI, Wilson SL. Determinants and outcomes of motivation in health professions education: a systematic review based on self-determination theory. J Educational Evaluation Health Professions. 2016;13:19. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Hattie J, Timperley H. The power of feedback. Rev Educ Res. 2007;77(1):81–112. [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Rauch C, Utz J, Rauch M, Kornhuber J, Spitzer P. E-Learning is not inferior to On-Site teaching in a Psychiatric Examination Course. Front Psychiatry. 2021;12:624005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Biggs J, Kember D, Leung DY. The revised two-factor Study Process Questionnaire: R-SPQ-2F 2001. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 32.The Self-Regulation. Questionnaires [cited 2024 Sep 22]. Available from: URL: https://selfdeterminationtheory.org/wp-content/uploads/2022/02/SRQ_Complete.pdf
  • 33.Lesage F-X, Berjot S, Deschamps F. Clinical stress assessment using a visual analogue scale. Occup Med (Lond). 2012;62(8):600–5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Lesage FX, Berjot S. Validity of occupational stress assessment using a visual analogue scale. Occup Med (Lond). 2011;61(6):434–6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Intrinsic Motivation Inventory (IMI). [cited 2024 Sep 22]. Available from: URL: https://selfdeterminationtheory.org/wp-content/uploads/2022/02/IMI_Complete.pdf
  • 36.McAuley E, Duncan T, Tammen VV. Psychometric properties of the intrinsic motivation inventory in a competitive sport setting: a confirmatory factor analysis. Res Q Exerc Sport. 1989;60(1):48–58. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.The Learning Climate Questionnaire (LCQ). [cited 2024 Sep 22]. Available from: URL: https://selfdeterminationtheory.org/wp-content/uploads/2022/02/PAS_LCQ_6and15.pdf
  • 38.Williams GC, Deci EL. Internalization of biopsychosocial values by medical students: a test of self-determination theory. J Personal Soc Psychol. 1996;70(4):767–79. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Cohen J. Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. 2nd ed. Hillsdale, N.J.: L. Erlbaum Associates; 1988. [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Lovakov A, Agadullina ER. Empirically derived guidelines for effect size interpretation in social psychology. Euro J Social Psych. 2021;51(3):485–504. [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Davidson J, Davidson R, MacKinnon JG. Estimation and inference in Econometrics. Economica. 1995;62(245):133. [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Großmann N, Wilde M. Promoting interest by supporting Learner Autonomy: the effects of Teaching Behaviour in Biology lessons. Res Sci Educ. 2020;50(5):1763–88. [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Williams GC, Wiener MW, Markakis KM, Reeve J, Deci E. L. PAS Learning Climate 1994. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 44.Reeve J, Jang H, Hardre P, Omura M. Motiv Emot. 2002;26(3):183–207. [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Kooiman BJ, Li W, Wesolek M, Heeja K. VALIDATION OF THE RELATEDNESS SCALE OF THE INTRINSIC MOTIVATION INVENTORY 2016.
  • 46.Ostrow KS, Heffernan NT. Testing the validity and reliability of intrinsic motivation inventory Subscales within ASSISTments; 2018. (vol 10947).
  • 47.Shapiro SL, Schwartz GE, Bonner G. Effects of mindfulness-based stress reduction on medical and premedical students. J Behav Med. 1998;21(6):581–99. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Warnecke E, Quinn S, Ogden K, Towle N, Nelson MR. A randomised controlled trial of the effects of mindfulness practice on medical student stress levels. Med Educ. 2011;45(4):381–8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Martineau M, Beauchamp G, Marcotte D. Efficacy of mental health prevention and promotion strategies in higher education. Smq. 2017;42(1):165–82. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Murad OS. Effectiveness of a cognitive-behavioral therapy program on reducing psychological stress and improving achievement motivation among University students. ujer. 2021;9(6):1316–22. [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Harahap NRA, Badrujaman A, Hidayat DR. Determinants of academic stress in students. Bisma T J Conseling. 2022;6(3):335–45. [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Anaz Kassim S. Determinants of perceived stress among University students. Curr Res Psychol Behav Sc. 2023;4(2):1–3. [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Naylor R. Key factors influencing psychological distress in university students: the effects of tertiary entrance scores. Stud High Educ. 2022;47(3):630–42. [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Clegg K-A, Levine SL, Zuroff DC, Holding AC, Shahar G, Koestner R. A multilevel perspective on self-determination theory: predictors and correlates of autonomous and controlled motivation. Motiv Emot. 2023;47(2):229–45. [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Reeve J. Why teachers adopt a Controlling Motivating Style toward students and how they can become more autonomy supportive. Educational Psychol. 2009;44(3):159–75. [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Jang H, Kim EJ, Reeve J. Longitudinal test of self-determination theory’s motivation mediation model in a naturally occurring classroom context. J Educ Psychol. 2012;104(4):1175–88. [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Orsini C, Binnie V, Wilson S, Villegas MJ. Learning climate and feedback as predictors of dental students’ self-determined motivation: the mediating role of basic psychological needs satisfaction. Eur J Dent Education: Official J Association Dent Educ Europe. 2018;22(2):e228–36. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Garn AC, Morin AJS, Lonsdale C. Basic psychological need satisfaction toward learning: a longitudinal test of mediation using bifactor exploratory structural equation modeling. J Educ Psychol. 2019;111(2):354–72. [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Rothes A, Lemos MS, Gonçalves T. The influence of students’ self-determination and personal achievement goals in Learning and Engagement: a mediation model for traditional and nontraditional students. Educ Sci. 2022;12(6):369. [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Kluger AN, DeNisi A. The effects of feedback interventions on performance: a historical review, a meta-analysis, and a preliminary feedback intervention theory. Psychol Bull. 1996;119(2):254–84. [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Mandhane N, Ansari S, Shaikh T, Deolekar S. Positive feedback: a tool for quality education in field of medicine. Int J Res Med Sci 2015:1868–73.
  • 62.Wisniewski B, Zierer K, Hattie J. The power of feedback revisited: a Meta-analysis of Educational Feedback Research. Front Psychol. 2019;10:3087. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Kukreja S, Singh T. Influence of Feedback on Learning. Indian Pediatr. 2019;56(9):733–4. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Choi E, Johnson DA, Moon K, Oah S. Effects of positive and negative feedback sequence on work performance and emotional responses. J Organizational Behav Manage. 2018;38(2–3):97–115. [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Shute VJ. Focus on formative feedback. Rev Educ Res. 2008;78(1):153–89. [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Vygotskij LS. Mind in society: the development of higher psychological processes. Mass. u.a.: Harvard Univ. Press;: Cambridge; 1978. [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Hu X, Chen Y, Tian B. Feeling better about self after receiving negative feedback: when the sense that ability can be improved is activated. J Psychol. 2016;150(1):72–87. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Verburg M, Snellings P, Zeguers MHT, Huizenga HM. Positive-blank versus negative-blank feedback learning in children and adults. Quarterly journal of experimental psychology (2006) 2019; 72(4):753–63. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 69.Drews R, Tani G, Lopes Cardozo P, Chiviacowsky S. Positive feedback praising good performance does not alter the learning of an intrinsically motivating task in 10-year-old children. Eur J Hum Mov. 2020;45:46–54. [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Faulconer E, Griffith J, Gruss A. The impact of positive feedback on student outcomes and perceptions. Assess Evaluation High Educ. 2022;47(2):259–68. [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Deci EL, Ryan RM, Williams GC. Need satisfaction and the self-regulation of learning. Learn Individual Differences. 1996;8(3):165–83. [Google Scholar]
  • 72.Núñez JL, León J. Autonomy support in the Classroom. Eur Psychol. 2015;20(4):275–83. [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Data Availability Statement

The datasets used and/or analyzed during the current study are available from the corresponding author upon reasonable request.


Articles from BMC Medical Education are provided here courtesy of BMC

RESOURCES