Abstract
Background:
Our study aimed to investigate the factors associated with mothers’ shaking behavior of their babies.
Methods:
Sixty-three mothers who stated that they shook their babies (Group B) and 91 mothers who stated that they did not (Group A) among those who applied to or were followed up from the Pediatric Outpatient Clinics of our University Hospital were included. The mothers (with a baby aged 0-30 months) completed the Brief Symptom Inventory (BSI) and psychiatric rating scales measuring attachment styles, empathic tendency, perceived social support, and self-esteem. The severity of the simulated shaking of the mothers was measured by an accelerometer placed on an infant manikin and separately scored on a Numerical Rating Scale by the researchers and an Illustrated Numeric Rating Scale by the mothers.
Results:
In group B, babies’ crying frequency was higher (P = .008); soothing the babies when they cried was more difficult (P = .019), mothers reported that they were exposed to physical violence more in their childhood (P = .003), BSI hostility scores (P = .004) and BSI-anxiety scores (P = .034) of the mothers were higher when compared with the group A. The age of the babies (P = .002, OR = 1.096), moderate crying frequency (P = .035, OR = 2.900), the mothers’ difficulty in soothing their babies (sometimes P = .018, OR = 3.705, often/always P = .014, OR = 7.777), and the mothers’ experience of physical violence in childhood (P = .002, OR = 5.674) were found to be factors affecting the shaking behavior.
Conclusion:
Clarifying the factors associated with the mothers’ shaking behavior of their babies would be helpful in terms of protecting future generations.
Main Points
In the group of mothers who reported that they shook their babies, the babies had poorer sleep patterns; babies’ crying frequency was higher; soothing the babies when they cried was more difficult and the mothers reported being exposed to physical violence more often in their childhood.
Brief Symptom Inventory—hostility and anxiety scores of mothers who shook their babies were higher.
The mothers’ difficulty in soothing their babies and the mothers’ experience of physical violence in childhood were found to be the most important factors affecting the shaking behavior of the mothers.
Introduction
Shaken baby syndrome (SBS) is a severe form of child abuse that may lead to death and severe brain damage.1,2 As cited by Miehl,3 SBS was first defined in 1946 by Dr. John Caffey, a pediatric radiologist. With the term “whiplash shaken baby syndrome,” in his 1974 article, Dr. Caffey described the collection of infantile subdural and subarachnoid hemorrhage, traction-type metaphyseal fractures, and retinal hemorrhage within this syndrome. Shaken baby syndrome is accepted as a subset of Abusive Head Trauma (AHT) diagnosis.4 A range of traumatic forces, such as blunt force trauma, acceleration/deceleration (inertial) forces, penetrating trauma, and asphyxiation, lead to neural damage in SBS.2
This syndrome is mainly encountered in children between 0 and 2 years but may affect children up to 5 years.1 The incidence of AHT in children under 1 year old between 2000 and 2009 was 33.4-38.8 per 100,000 annually in the USA.5 Approximately one-fourth of children with AHT under 2 years die.5 Abusive head trauma is often associated with significant rotational acceleration-deceleration force through violent shaking.6 Subdural and/or subarachnoid hemorrhages, various neurologic signs and symptoms, retinal hemorrhages, upper cervical spine injuries, and skeletal injuries are the results of SBS.2 Prolonged unsoothable crying may evoke frustration and anger in the caregiver and lead to shaking behavior and abuse toward the infant.7 Mortality rates due to SBS were between 13% and 26%8-10 and rates for neurological abnormality were 74% in a literature review by Barlow et al11 (2004) which included 489 cases; moderate-to-significant disability was 2-100%.8,9,12
Early detection of AHT is essential to prevent severe disability and death.13 The signs and symptoms of SBS include irritability, changes in sleep or feeding patterns, vomiting, paleness, pain, poor contact, decrease in the child’s capabilities, inability to suck or swallow, interrupted breathing, symptoms of a viral illness, severe malaise, scalp swelling, bruising, impaired vigilance (extending to coma), severe apnoea, bradycardia, cardiac arrest, convulsions, fixed upward gaze, signs suggesting acute intracranial hypertension and tentorial herniation, and death.14-17 Long-term consequences of SBS are developmental delay, learning disabilities, physical disabilities, seizures, paralysis, communication deficits, visual deficits, blindness, hearing impairment, behavioral disorders, cognitive impairment, and death.14,15,18 As cited by Miehl,3 according to Becker et al (2002), long-term impairment of SBS can also manifest itself as Alzheimer’s disease, Parkinson’s disease, dementia pugilistica, and post-traumatic dementia.
Risk factors related to the child for AHT include infantile colic and inconsolable crying, being under the age of one, being one of multiple births, prematurity, and disability of the child. Risk factors related to the parent/caregiver include being a single parent, young parental age, lack of support, frustration intolerance, lack of experience with childcare, low education level, domestic violence, caregiver’s criminal history, prior child protective services involvement, untreated mental disorder, substance use disorder, unrealistic expectations, attachment problems, and having been abused or neglected in childhood. Risks at the community level are isolation, poverty, and insufficient recreational facilities.3,6,17,19-21 These factors increase the risk of child physical abuse.
As aforementioned, acceleration/deceleration forces are 2 factors related to neural damage in SBS.2 Accelerometers are used to provide an objective evaluation of movement. Accelerometers assess the accelerations of an object’s motion throughout specified reference axes.22 Free fall from 3 feet (approximately 1 m) produces a velocity of 4.09 m/s and normalized impact energy of 0.85 Nm/N; shaking at 11-15.5 km/h produces a velocity of 4.31 m/s and normalized impact energy of 0.95 Nm/N23 (impact energy corresponds to the kinetic energy of a striking object at the moment of impact).24 Since these acceleration and velocity levels/numbers are very close to a free fall from 1 m, it is obvious that an infant’s neck cannot withstand these acceleration and velocity levels without injury.23 Adding an objective measurement tool, such as an accelerometer, to research related to simulated baby shaking is essential in attracting the attention of multidisciplinary fields, which may contribute in various ways to the subject.
In this study, we aimed to investigate the factors related to mothers’ shaking behavior of their babies. We hypothesized that mothers who stated that they shake their babies would be more likely to have babies with a poor sleeping pattern and/or frequent crying episodes, would have difficulty in soothing their babies while they are crying, would be more likely to be exposed to physical violence in their childhood, would be more likely to have low self-esteem, an insecure attachment profile, low empathic tendency, more psychiatric symptoms, would perceive their social support as inadequate when compared with mothers who stated that they do not shake their babies. For the mothers who stated that they shake their babies, the accelerometer peak resultant values would be correlated with the scores of the Illustrated Numeric Rating Scale (INRS) and Numerical Rating Scale (NRS).
Material and Methods
Participants
The literate mothers with babies aged between 0 months and 30 months, who applied to or were followed up by the pediatric outpatient clinics of Marmara University Pendik Training and Research Hospital, stated that they shook their babies to soothe them when they cried and volunteered to participate in the present study were included. The age-matched mothers who stated that they did not shake their babies constituted the control group. Both groups of mothers were recruited as consecutive referrals from our University Hospital’s pediatric outpatient clinics. Mothers who were illiterate and had difficulties with comprehension were excluded from this study. This study was conducted after the approval of Marmara University Research Ethics Review Committee (date: December 06, 2013; protocol code: 09.2013.0347). Written informed consent was provided by the mothers.
The participants were divided into 2 groups. Group A included mothers who stated that they did not shake their babies; mothers who stated that they shook their babies were in group B.
Research Design
This research was conducted in the Pediatric Outpatient Clinics of Marmara University Pendik Training and Research Hospital. Mothers who volunteered to participate were included in this study. The mothers completed the psychiatric rating scales. Afterwards, the mother and the researcher completed related parts of the sociodemographic questionnaire, respectively.
As the mother was being inquired about the crying frequency of the baby and difficulties regarding soothing the baby, she was asked whether she had ever shaken her baby, as shown by the researcher. Mothers who admitted shaking their babies were further requested to demonstrate the intensity of a maximum shake using the infant manikin. The accelerometer recorded the acceleration generated by this movement. Meanwhile, the observed shaking intensity of the mother was noted on the researchers’ Numerical Rating Scale (NRS) by the researchers. Finally, the mothers were asked to score their shaking intensity on the mothers’ Illustrated Numeric Rating Scale (INRS). Afterwards, each mother was informed about the health risks of shaking a baby. None of the mothers reported any history of head trauma.
We encountered a similar experimental approach that employed simulated shaking as a method by Yamazaki et al25 (2014) which investigated several modes of violent shaking by volunteers who imitate perpetrators using a dummy doll with an eyeball model that can lead to retinal hemorrhages observed in SBS.
Data Collection Tools
Sociodemographic questionnaire (SQ): The researchers composed the SQ for this study. The relevant parts of the SQ were filled in separately by the mother and the researcher.
Adult Attachment Style Questionnaire (AASQ): AASQ consists of 2 parts. The first part, developed by Hazan and Shaver26 (1987), has 3 statements that are used to classify the attachment style of the participant as secure, anxious/ambivalent, or avoidant. These statements contain definitions of the characteristics of the relationship with the parents in childhood and the general behavioral characteristics of the participant. The second part of the scale, developed by Mikulincer et al27 (1990), consists of 15 items, each of which is scored between 1 and 7. Each attachment style is represented by 5 items, and the highest score determines the attachment style of the individual who completed the scale. In Mikulincer et al’s27 (1990) study, Cronbach’ alpha coefficients for each factor, on which secure, avoidant, and ambivalent items were loaded, ranged from 0.79 to 0.83. In Sabuncuoğlu and Berkem’s28 study (2006), in which AASQ was used, the internal consistency of the second part of the scale was found to be acceptable for avoidant and anxious/ambivalent attachment (Cronbach’s α = 0.61 and 0.66, respectively), while it was found to be poor for secure attachment (Cronbach’s α = 0.42). The validity and reliability study of the Turkish version of the scale was performed by Kesebir et al29 (2012), during which the authors made some changes to increase the reliability of the Turkish version of the scale. For the second part of the scale, the items that were thought to be incomprehensible were divided, and the number of items increased from 15 to 18. Participants were asked to answer the items of the second part as “true” or “false.” In Kesebir et al’s29 study (2012), Cronbach’s alpha coefficients for secure, avoidant, and anxious/ambivalent attachment were 0.72, 0.82, and 0.85, respectively. In our study, Cronbach’s alpha coefficients for secure, avoidant, and anxious/ambivalent attachment were 0.55, 0.51, and 0.53, respectively.
Empathic Tendency Scale (ETS): ETS was developed by Dökmen30 (1988) to measure the empathic tendency of the participants. It is a 20-item 5-point Likert type scale. Reported test-retest reliability was 0.82.30 High scores indicate high levels of empathic tendency. ETS was used to measure the empathic tendency of the mothers. In Dökmen’s30 study (1988) study, ETS was administered to a group of 70 students twice with a 3-week interval, and a correlation of r = 0.82 was found between the scores obtained from these 2 applications. In our study, Cronbach’s alpha coefficient was found to be 0.58.
Brief Symptom Inventory (BSI): The BSI was developed by Derogatis31 (1992). It is a 53-item self-report scale used to evaluate psychopathological and psychological symptoms. The BSI is a 5-point Likert scale with 9 subscales (somatization, obsession–compulsion, interpersonal sensitivity, depression, anxiety, hostility, phobic anxiety, paranoid ideation, and psychoticism) and 3 global indices as it is the short form of SCL-90.31 These indices are the Global Severity Index (GSI), the Positive Symptom Distress Index, and the Positive Symptom Total. Higher scores indicate greater psychological distress. Derogatis31 (1992) reported that the internal consistency coefficients for the 9 subscales of the BSI ranged between 0.71 and 0.85. The validity and reliability study of the Turkish version of the scale was performed by Şahin and Durak.31 In Şahin and Durak’s32 study (1994), the Cronbach’s alpha coefficient obtained from the entire BSI was found to be 0.95. The reliability coefficients obtained from the original 9 subscales of the scale were as follows: Obsessive-Compulsive, α = 0.69; Paranoid Thoughts, α = 0.72; Hostility, α = 0.74; Phobic Anxiety, α = 0.63; Psychoticism, α = 0.55; Somatization, α = 0.82; Interpersonal Sensitivity, α = 0.69; Depression, α = 0.82; Anxiety, α = 0.82; Additional Items, α = 0.63.31 In our study, the Cronbach’s alpha coefficient retrieved from the entire BSI was 0.95. The Cronbach’s alpha coefficients retrieved from the original 9 subscales were: Obsessive-Compulsive, α = 0.72; Paranoid Thoughts, α = 0.70; Hostility, α = 0.66; Phobic Anxiety, α = 0.62; Psychoticism, α = 0.59; Somatization, α = 0.79; Interpersonal Sensitivity, α = 0.64; Depression, α = 0.78; Anxiety, α = 0.82; Additional Items, α = 0.50.
The Multidimensional Scale of Perceived Social Support (MSPSS) is a 12-item self-report scale used to evaluate social support given by a significant other, family, and friends. It was developed by Zimet et al33 (1988) and is a 7-point Likert-type scale. Higher scores reflect higher perceived social support. In Zimet et al’s33 study (1988), the Cronbach’s alpha coefficients for the significant other, family, and friends subscales, and the entire scale were 0.91, 0.87, 0.85, and 0.88, respectively. A validity and reliability study of the Turkish version of the scale was conducted by Eker and Arkar.33 In Eker and Arkar’s34 study (1995), the Cronbach’s alpha coefficients of the whole scale for the normal group (randomly selected patient visitors) and the psychiatry clinic patient group (half inpatient and half outpatient) were 0.87 and 0.88, respectively. The alpha coefficients for the significant other subscale were 0.89 for both groups, while the alpha coefficients for the family subscale were 0.83 for both groups. The alpha coefficients for the friends subscale were 0.90 and 0.86, respectively. In our study, Cronbach’s alpha coefficients for the entire scale and the significant other, family, and friends subscales were found to be 0.89, 0.86, 0.85, and 0.87, respectively.
Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale (RSES): RSES was developed by Rosenberg35 (1965). It is a self-report scale used to measure self-esteem. It has 10 items and is scored on a 4-point Likert scale. Higher scores indicate low self-esteem. The RSES’s reproducibility was found to be 0.92 and scalability was found to be 0.72 by Rosenberg.35 A validity and reliability study of the Turkish version of the scale was carried out by Çuhadaroğlu.36 In Çuhadaroğlu’s36 study (1986), the test-retest reliability method was used, and the reliability coefficient of RSES was found to be 0.75. In our study, Cronbach’s alpha coefficient of RSES was found to be 0.66.
Numerical Rating Scale (NRS) and Illustrated Numeric Rating Scale (INRS): The NRS is a 0-10 scale completed by the researcher after the mother made a shaking simulation with the infant CPR manikin. The NRS was located in the researcher’s part of the sociodemographic questionnaire. Higher scores indicate more severe shaking.
The illustrated numeric rating scale (INRS) (Figure 1) was designed by the authors for this study. The baby’s head movement pictures were located above a 0-10 NRS at points 0, 2, 4, 6, 8, and 10 on the scale. It has been enriched with pictures describing the baby’s back-and-forth head movements with increasing intensity so that mothers can accurately mark the severity of their shaking. Point 0 is “no shaking,” 5 is “moderate shaking,” and 10 is “most severe shaking.” After the shaking simulation with the infant CPR manikin, the INRS completed by the mother to rate her own shaking intensity.
Figure 1.
Illustrated numeric rating scale.
Accelerometer: Accelerometer measures the instantaneous acceleration applied to an object. In our study, Node modular wireless sensor (Variable Inc.), a 3-axis accelerometer fixed to the chest of the infant CPR manikin, was used to measure the mothers’ shaking intensity of their babies. Since the neck of the infant CPR manikin was not flexible enough, we placed the accelerometer on the chest instead of the head. The data obtained during the shaking movement was saved via Bluetooth to the Node-specific application on the iOS operating system. The peak (maximum) resultant acceleration vector value is calculated by the formula
. The accelerometer peak resultant value for each mother was used as a variable in the analysis.
Infant CPR manikin: The infant (6-month-old) CPR manikin provided by the Clinical Skills Laboratory of our Faculty was used in the shaking simulation. As previously explained, a 3-axis wireless accelerometer was preferred to be placed on the chest of the infant CPR manikin.
Statistical Analysis
IBM SPSS Statistics 27 (IBM SPSS Statistics for Windows, Version 27.0. Armonk, NY: IBM Corp) software was used for statistical analysis; PASS 2011 (NCSS Corp. Released 2011. Power Analysis and Sample Size for Windows, Version 11.0. (Utah, USA) software was used for power analysis. Reliability analyses of psychiatric rating scales were evaluated with Cronbach’s alpha coefficient. The Kolmogorov-Smirnov test was used to determine the normality of the data distribution. Descriptive analyses were performed to reveal the characteristics of the study sample. Continuous variables are expressed as mean ± SD or median (IQR) according to the normality of the distribution, and categorical data are expressed as n (%). Independent samples t-test was used to compare normally distributed continuous variables; Mann–Whitney U-test was used to compare non-normally distributed continuous variables. Pearson’s chi-square test and Fisher’s exact test were used to compare the categorical variables between the groups of mothers who stated that they shook their babies and those who did not. Pearson’s correlation coefficient or Spearman’s rank-order correlation coefficient depending on the normality of the variables was used to examine the relationships between accelerometer peak resultant values, researchers’ NRS, and mothers’ INRS and psychiatric rating scale scores of mothers who stated that they shook their babies. We conducted a multivariable logistic regression analysis to investigate the risk factors related to mothers’ shaking behavior of their babies, including only those identified in the univariate analyses with a P-value of .05 or less. All predictor variables were simultaneously entered into the logistic regression model using the enter method. The appropriateness of the model was analyzed using the Hosmer-Lemeshow test. The significance level was accepted as P < .05.
Outliers: Mothers who performed more than 3 SDs above group means for the psychiatric rating scale total scores were accepted as outliers and excluded from the analysis. One of the mothers from the group B, BSI-psychoticism subscale score was a major outlier. Even though this was a subscale, to be on the safer side regarding the accurate completion of the psychiatric rating scales, her data were removed from the analysis.
Results
Sixty-three (40.9%) mothers who stated that they shook their babies to soothe them when they cried and 91 (59.1%) mothers who stated they did not were included in this study. When the variable “mothers’ exposure to physical violence in childhood” was taken into account, the power of the study was found to be 84.8%.
Some of the significant findings we would like to emphasize were (Table 1): In group B, babies had poorer sleep patterns; babies’ crying frequency was higher; soothing the babies when they cried was more difficult, and the mothers reported being exposed to physical violence more often in their childhood when compared with group A. The mean ages of the babies were higher in group B.
Table 1.
Sociodemographic Characteristics Related to the Mother, the Family, and the Baby
Group A# (n = 91) | Group B# (n = 63) | P | ||
---|---|---|---|---|
Mothers’ age | 27.70 ± 5.47 | 27.92 ± 5.34 | .807a | |
Age of first motherhood | 23.00 (6.00) | 23.00 (7.00) | .884b | |
Mothers’ education | Primary school | 27 (29.7%) | 22 (34.9%) | .191c |
Secondary school | 26 (28.6%) | 17 (27.0%) | ||
High school | 22 (24.2%) | 20 (31.7%) | ||
Undergraduate and higher education | 16 (17.6%) | 4 (6.3%) | ||
Mothers’ employment status | Employed | 6 (6.6%) | 3 (4.8%) | .085c |
Unemployed/on leave | 12 (13.2%) | 2 (3.2%) | ||
Homemaker | 73 (80.2%) | 58 (92.1%) | ||
Family’s total monthly income | Below 1000 ₺ | 26 (28.6%) | 12 (19.0%) | .367c |
1000-1999 ₺ | 36 (39.6%) | 33 (52.4%) | ||
2000-2999 ₺ | 20 (22.0%) | 11 (17.5%) | ||
3000 ₺ and above | 9 (9.9%) | 7 (11.1%) | ||
Number of children | 1 | 41 (45.1%) | 20 (31.7%) | .129c |
2 | 33 (36.3%) | 33 (52.4%) | ||
3 and above | 17 (18.7%) | 10 (15.9%) | ||
Difficulty in soothing the baby when crying | Never | 30 (33.0%) | 8 (12.7%) | .019*, c |
Sometimes | 51 (56.0%) | 41 (65.1%) | ||
Often | 8 (8.8%) | 10 (15.9%) | ||
Always | 2 (2.2%) | 4 (6.3%) | ||
Mothers’ exposure to physical violence in childhood | Yes | 8 (8.8%) | 17 (27.0%) | .003**, c |
No | 83 (91.2%) | 46 (73.0%) | ||
Gender of the baby | Girl | 44 (48.4%) | 30 (47.6%) | .929c |
Boy | 47 (51.6%) | 33 (52.4%) | ||
Age of the baby (month) | 7.00 (9.00) | 11.00 (14.00) | .015*, b | |
The person who takes care of the baby the most | Mother | 84 (92.3%) | 62 (98.4%) | .091d |
Relative | 7 (7.7%) | 1 (1.6%) | ||
Baby’s sleep pattern | Good | 30 (33.0%) | 21 (33.3%) | .029*, c |
Moderate | 46 (50.5%) | 21 (33.3%) | ||
Poor | 15 (16.5%) | 21 (33.3%) | ||
Baby’s crying frequency | Very little | 13 (14.3%) | 1 (1.6%) | .008**, c |
Little | 21 (23.1%) | 9 (14.3%) | ||
Moderate | 41 (45.1%) | 35 (55.6%) | ||
Much | 10 (11.0%) | 6 (9.5%) | ||
Very much | 6 (6.6%) | 12 (19.0%) |
Descriptive data were presented as mean ± SD, median (IQR), n (%). For group A, the sum of all subcategories is 91 and 100 %; for group B, the sum of all subcategories is 63 and 100%.
aIndependent samples t-test.
bMann–Whitney U-test.
cPearson’s chi-squared test.
dFisher’s exact test.
*P < .05.
**P < .01.
#Group A: mothers who stated that they did not shake their babies; Group B: Mothers who stated that they shook their babies.
Psychiatric rating scale comparisons of group A and group B, and accelerometer peak resultant values, researchers’ NRS, and mothers’ INRS scores for group B are presented in Table 2.
Table 2.
Psychiatric Rating Scale Comparisons of Group A and Group B, Accelerometer Peak Resultant Values, Researchers’ NRS, and Mothers’ INRS Scores for Group B
Group A# | Group B# | P | |
---|---|---|---|
AASQ | n = 89 | n = 63 | |
Avoidant attachment | 3.00 (3.00) | 4.00 (3.00) | .204b |
Anxious/ambivalent attachment | 2.00 (2.00) | 1.00 (2.00) | .225b |
Insecure attachment | 2.50 (1.50) | 2.50 (1.50) | .998b |
Secure attachment | 3.00 (3.00) | 4.00 (2.00) | .449b |
ETS | n = 91 | n = 63 | .059a |
69.74 ± 8.12 | 67.22 ± 7.98 | ||
MSPSS | n = 91 | n = 63 | |
Family | 27.00 (4.00) | 27.00 (8.00) | .505b |
Significant other | 28.00 (6.00) | 28.00 (6.00) | .926b |
Friends | 22.00 (12.00) | 22.00 (12.00) | .817b |
Total | 72.00 (17.00) | 74.00 (19.00) | .971b |
RSES | n = 91 | n = 63 | |
1.00 (1.00) | 1.00 (1.00) | .289b | |
BSI | n = 91 | n = 63 | |
Somatization | 4.00 (9.00) | 5.00 (8.00) | .084b |
Obsession-compulsion | 7.00 (8.00) | 8.00 (8.00) | .227b |
Interpersonal sensitivity | 4.00 (6.00) | 6.00 (6.00) | .193b |
Depression | 4.00 (7.00) | 5.00 (8.00) | .142b |
Anxiety | 4.00 (8.00) | 7.00 (7.00) | .034* b |
Hostility | 4.00 (5.00) | 6.00 (6.00) | .004** b |
Phobic anxiety | 3.00 (5.00) | 3.00 (5.00) | .269b |
Paranoid ideation | 5.00 (7.00) | 6.00 (6.00) | .418b |
Psychoticism | 3.00 (5.00) | 3.00 (5.00) | .634b |
Additional items | 3.00 (5.00) | 4.00 (4.00) | .138b |
Global Severity Index | 0.75 (0.94) | 1.11 (0.98) | .066b |
Positive Symptom Total | 25.30 ± 12.00 | 27.68 ± 10.45 | .203a |
Positive Symptom Distress Index | 1.75 (1.27) | 2.06 (1.04) | .110b |
Accelerometer peak resultant value | – | n = 57 | |
1.86 ± 0.85 | |||
Researchers’ NRS | – | n = 63 | |
3.00 (1.00) | |||
Mothers’ INRS | – | n = 63 | |
2.00 (2.00) |
Descriptive data were presented as mean ± SD, median (IQR).
AASQ, Adult Attachment Questionnaire; ETS, Empathic Tendency Scale; MSPSS, Multidimensional Scale of Perceived Social Support; RSES, Rosenberg Self Esteem Scale.
BSI: Brief Symptom Inventory, NRS: Numerical Rating Scale, INRS: Illustrated Numeric Rating Scale, Accelerometer peak resultant value: .
#Group A: Mothers who stated that they did not shake their babies; Group B: Mothers who stated that they shook their babies.
aIndependent samples t-test
bMann–Whitney U-test.
*P < .05.
**P < .01.
A significantly positive correlation was found between accelerometer peak resultant values and researchers’ NRS scores, and between researchers’ NRS, and mothers’ INRS scores (Table 3).
Table 3.
Correlation Between Accelerometer Peak Resultant Value and Researchers’ Numerical Rating Scale, and Mothers’ Illustrated Numeric Rating Scale in Group B
Accelerometer Peak Resultant Value | Researchers’ NRS | Mothers’ INRS | ||
---|---|---|---|---|
Accelerometer peak resultant value | r | 1.000a | ||
P | <.001 | |||
n | 57 | |||
Researchers’ NRS | r | 0.475a | 1.000a | |
P | <.001** | <.001 | ||
n | 57 | 63 | ||
Mothers’ INRS | r | 0.204a | 0.482a | 1.000a |
P | .128 | <.001** | <.001 | |
n | 57 | 63 | 63 |
Group B: Mothers who stated that they shook their babies.
a Spearman’s rank-order correlation coefficient.
NRS, Numerical Rating Scale; INRS, Illustrated Numeric Rating Scale.
*P < .05.
**P < .01.
Correlations between psychiatric rating scale scores and accelerometer peak resultant values, researchers’ NRS, and mothers’ INRS scores are presented in Tables 4 and 5.
Table 4.
Correlation Between Accelerometer Peak Resultant Value, Researchers’ Numerical Rating Scale, Mothers’ Illustrated Numeric Rating Scale, and Adult Attachment Questionnaire, Empathic Tendency Scale, Multidimensional Scale of Perceived Social Support, Rosenberg Self Esteem Scale Scores in Group B
Accelerometer Peak Resultant Value | Researchers’ NRS | Mothers’ INRS | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
r | P | r | P | r | P | |
AASQ | ||||||
Avoidant attachment | 0.172a | .201 | 0.123a | .337 | 0.131a | .307 |
Anxious/ambivalent attachment | −0.157a | .243 | 0.048a | .709 | 0.138a | .282 |
Insecure attachment | 0.059a | .661 | 0.164a | .198 | 0.216a | .089 |
Secure attachment | 0.054a | .690 | −0.022a | .863 | −0.044a | .731 |
ETS | 0.189b | .159 | 0.079a | .539 | 0.057a | .655 |
MSPSS | ||||||
Family | 0.072a | .594 | 0.005a | .967 | −0.041a | .750 |
Significant other | 0.179a | .182 | 0.039a | .761 | −0.079a | .540 |
Friends | 0.247a | .064 | −0.003a | .980 | 0.169a | .184 |
Total | 0.213a | .112 | 0.038a | .770 | 0.058a | .651 |
RSES | −0.239a | .073 | 0.310a | .013* | 0.313a | .013* |
Group B, mothers who stated that they shook their babies.
AASQ, Adult Attachment Questionnaire; ETS, Empathic Tendency Scale; INRS, Illustrated Numeric Rating Scale; MSPSS, Multidimensional Scale of Perceived Social Support; NRS, Numerical Rating Scale; RSES, Rosenberg Self Esteem Scale.
aSpearman’s rank-order correlation coefficient.
bPearson's correlat’on coefficient.
*P < .05.
**P < .01.
Table 5.
Correlation Between Accelerometer Peak Resultant Value, Researchers’ Numerical Rating Scale, Mothers’ Illustrated Numeric Rating Scaleand Brief Symptom Inventory Scores in Group B
Accelerometer Peak Resultant Value | Researchers’ NRS | Mothers’ INRS | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
r | P | r | P | r | P | |
BSI | ||||||
Somatization | −0.011a | 0.936 | 0.119a | .351 | 0.195a | .125 |
Obsession-compulsion | 0.094a | 0.486 | 0.174a | .173 | 0.256a | .043* |
Interpersonal sensitivity | 0.046a | 0.735 | 0.187a | .143 | 0.339a | .007** |
Depression | −0.049a | 0.715 | 0.376a | .002** | 0.401a | .001** |
Anxiety | 0.054a | 0.692 | 0.279a | .027 | 0.284a | .024* |
Hostility | −0.021a | 0.877 | 0.180a | .157 | 0.353a | .005** |
Phobic anxiety | −0.086a | 0.524 | 0.054a | .675 | 0.226a | .075 |
Paranoid ideation | 0.082a | 0.545 | 0.113a | .376 | 0.242a | .056 |
Psychoticism | 0.038a | 0.779 | 0.017a | .894 | 0.180a | .158 |
Additional items | −0.047a | 0.727 | 0.221a | .081 | 0.158a | .217 |
Global severity index | −0.010a | 0.939 | 0.209a | .100 | 0.327a | .009** |
Positive symptom total | −0.039b | 0.773 | 0.237a | .061 | 0.205a | .107 |
Positive symptom distress index | 0.029a | 0.831 | 0.081a | .527 | 0.340a | <.006** |
Group B: Mothers who stated that they shook their babies.
BSI, Brief Symptom Inventory; INRS, Illustrated Numeric Rating Scale; NRS, Numerical Rating Scale.
a Spearman’s rank-order correlation coefficient.
b Pearson's correlation coefficient.
*P < .05.
**P < .01.
Significant positive correlations were found between mothers’ INRS and BSI-depression scores, researchers’ NRS and BSI-depression scores, mothers’ INRS and BSI-hostility scores, mothers’ INRS scores and BSI-Positive Symptom Distress Index, and mothers’ INRS and BSI-interpersonal sensitivity scores (Table 5).
Risk factors we found related to mothers’ shaking behavior of their babies with a P-value of .05 or less in the univariate analyses were included in the multivariable logistic regression analysis (Table 6). The goodness-of-fit of the logistic regression model was evaluated using the Hosmer-Lemeshow test, which yielded a significance value of 0.917, indicating that the model adequately fits the data. Significant factors associated with mothers’ shaking behavior were as follows: each monthly increase in the age of the babies increased the mothers’ shaking tendency 1.096 times. In mothers who stated that the babies’ crying frequency was moderate, the tendency to shake increased 2.900 times compared to the mothers who stated that the babies’ crying frequency was low or very low. Compared to mothers who stated that they never had difficulty in soothing their babies, the tendency to shake increased 3.705 times in mothers who said that they sometimes had difficulty in soothing their babies; the tendency to shake increased 7.777 times in mothers who said that they often or always had difficulty in this issue. In mothers who experienced physical violence in their childhood, the tendency to shake their babies increased 5.674 times compared to mothers who did not experience physical violence in their childhood (Table 6).
Table 6.
Investigation of the Risk Factors Related to Mothers’ Shaking Behavior of their Babies
B | P | OR | 95% CI for OR | |||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Lower | Upper | |||||
Age of the baby (month) | 0.092 | .002** | 1.096 | 1.033 | 1.162 | |
BSI-anxiety subscale | −0.001 | .974 | 0.999 | 0.915 | 1.090 | |
BSI-hostility subscale | 0.097 | .131 | 1.102 | 0.971 | 1.251 | |
Baby’s sleep pattern | Good | ref | ||||
Moderate | −0.758 | .106 | 0.468 | 0.187 | 1.176 | |
Poor | 0.385 | .480 | 1.470 | 0.505 | 4.279 | |
Baby’s crying frequency | Very little or little | ref | ||||
Moderate | 1.065 | .035* | 2.900 | 1.080 | 7.788 | |
Much or very much | 0.751 | .282 | 2.119 | 0.540 | 8.318 | |
Difficulty in soothing the baby when crying | Never | ref | ||||
Sometimes | 1.310 | .018* | 3.705 | 1.246 | 11.017 | |
Often or always | 2.051 | .014* | 7.777 | 1.519 | 39.818 | |
Mothers’ exposure to physical violence in childhood | 1.736 | .002** | 5.674 | 1.887 | 17.058 | |
Constant | −1.240 | .014* | 0.289 |
Logistic regression model P < .001.
BSI, Brief Symptom Inventory; OR, odds ratio, logistic regression analyses.
*P < .05.
**P < .01.
Discussion
In the group of mothers who stated that they shook their babies, the babies had poorer sleep patterns, babies’ crying frequency was higher, and soothing the babies when they cried was more difficult when compared to the group of mothers who stated that they did not shake their babies. Prolonged, unsoothable crying episodes and misperceptions by the caregiver that the reason for crying is pain may lead to frustration and consequently to shaking of the baby.7,37
In the group of mothers who stated that they shook their babies, mothers reported being exposed to physical violence more often in their childhood when compared to the group of mothers who stated that they did not. This finding is in accordance with previous studies that highlight the mothers’38,39 or parents’40 own abuse history as a risk factor for their children to be exposed to abuse. It was reported that parents with an abuse history in childhood are more likely to display hostile parenting towards their children during early childhood.39
The BSI hostility subscale scores of the mothers who said that they shook their babies were higher than those of the mothers who stated that they did not. It was suggested that hostile parental feelings may cause an increased risk for abuse toward preterm infants or infants with poor fetal growth.41 It was also suggested that exposure to high levels of hostility in the home may be related to important mechanisms linking child maltreatment to adverse outcomes.42 Brief Symptom Inventory anxiety subscale scores of the mothers who stated that they shook their babies were higher than those of the mothers who stated that they did not. Mental health problems can lead to AHT.19,20 In terms of the BSI’s other subscale scores, global severity index, positive symptom total, and positive symptom distress index, we could not find any statistically significant difference between the mothers who stated that they shook their babies and the mothers who stated that they did not. We could not find any significant differences in terms of attachment styles, empathic tendency levels, perceived social support, and self-esteem levels between the mothers who stated that they shook their babies and those who did not. Mental health problems of a parent were found to be associated with an increased risk of child maltreatment43,44 or AHT.19,20 Mothers with a high risk of physically abusing their children were found to have a lack of empathy and a higher presence of negative affectivity.45 Deficits in parental empathy/low levels of parental empathy were found to be related to abuse potential.46,47 Attachment styles of the parents may contribute to the development of risk factors, which may lead to child maltreatment. Cooke et al48 (2019) revealed that mothers who had adverse childhood experiences were at increased risk of having children with behavioral and emotional difficulties in early childhood; this effect was thought to be conveyed via mothers’ attachment styles and depressive symptoms. Perceived lack of social support or lack of support was shown to be associated with an increased risk of child physical abuse.43,49,50
In the group of mothers who stated that they shook their babies, a significant positive correlation was found between accelerometer peak resultant values and researchers’ NRS scores, researchers’ NRS, and mothers’ INRS scores. The results indicated significant positive correlations between mothers’ INRS and BSI-depression scores, researchers’ NRS and BSI-depression scores, mothers’ INRS and BSI-hostility scores, mothers’ INRS scores and BSI-Positive Symptom Distress Index, and mothers’ INRS and BSI-interpersonal sensitivity scores. Higher depressive scores or higher psychiatric symptoms of mothers may have contributed to the severity of shaking their babies. Depression and other mental health disorders of the caregivers are among the risk factors for physical abuse or SBS.3,19,20 We could not encounter a study measuring the mothers’ shaking severity with INRS and comparing this parameter with various psychiatric rating scale scores.
The age of the babies, moderate crying frequency of the babies, the mothers’ difficulty in soothing their babies, and the mothers’ experience of physical violence in their own childhood were found to be significant factors affecting the shaking behavior. SBS is encountered mostly under the age of 2.1 Moderate crying frequency had an effect on shaking behavior. To our surprise, higher frequencies of crying did not reveal a significant effect. This finding may be explained by the possibility that since kinship relations are strong in our country, mothers may have found a solution to cope with a higher frequency of crying, such as arranging family support (e.g., inviting a relative to live with them).
After this study, our Faculty’s Department of Forensic Medicine and Child Protection Unit composed and distributed brochures related to SBS to inform the families who referred to the pediatric outpatient clinics about the risks related to infant shaking.
Our study has several limitations. First of all, if the sample size was larger, it would be easier to reveal the differences between the mothers who stated that they shook their babies and those who stated that they did not. We had to attach the accelerometer to the infant manikin’s chest instead of the head; this may have prevented us from measuring the shaking intensity accurately, and the actual shaking intensity may be higher. There was a possibility that some mothers may have withheld information about shaking their babies.
To our knowledge, this is the first study in the literature that examines various parameters, such as attachment style, empathic tendency, psychiatric symptoms, and perceived social support, as well as self-esteem in relation to mothers’ shaking behavior of their babies. Our study also includes a shaking simulation with an infant manikin and an accelerometer.
Conclusion
The shaking behavior of mothers is related to various factors associated with babies and mothers themselves. Education regarding babies’ crying patterns during their development, ways for soothing the babies or calming the mothers/caregivers, and risks of shaking the babies are crucial for preventing the development of SBS. Steps for the prevention of SBS should be undertaken to protect new generations.
Funding Statement
The authors declare that this study received no financial support.
Footnotes
Ethics Committee Approval: This study was approved by the Ethics Committee of Marmara University School of Medicine (approval number: 09.2013.0347; date: December 06, 2013).
Informed Consent: Written informed consent was obtained from the patients/patient who agreed to take part in the study.
Peer-review: Externally peer-reviewed.
Author Contributions: Concept – A.T.B., E.B.H.Ö., S.D.A., K.A., T.B.A., E.Ç., M.A.I.; Design – .A.T.B., E.B.H.Ö., S.D.A., K.A., T.B.A., E.Ç., M.A.I; Supervision – .A.T.B., M.A.I.; Resources – A.T.B., M.A.I.; Materials – .A.T.B., M.A.I.; Data Collection and/or Processing – .A.T.B., E.B.H.Ö., S.D.A., K.A., T.B.A., E.Ç., C.A., M.A.I.; Analysis and/or Interpretation – A.T.B., C.A., M.A.I.; Literature Search – A.T.B., E.B.H.Ö., S.D.A., K.A., T.B.A., E.Ç., M.A.I.; Writing – A.T.B., E.B.H.Ö., S.D.A., K.A., T.B.A., E.Ç., C.A., M.A.I.; Critical Review – A.T.B., E.B.H.Ö., S.D.A., K.A., T.B.A., E.Ç., M.A.I.
Declaration of Interests: The authors have no conflicts of interest to declare.
References
- 1. American Academy of Pediatrics: Committee on Child Abuse and Neglect: Committee on Child Abuse and Neglect. Shaken baby syndrome: rotational cranial injuries-technical report. Pediatrics. 2001;108(1):206 210. ( 10.1542/peds.108.1.206) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2. Chiesa A, Duhaime AC. Abusive head trauma. Pediatr Clin North Am. 2009;56(2):317 331. ( 10.1016/j.pcl.2009.02.001) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3. Miehl NJ. Shaken baby syndrome. J Forensic Nurs. 2005;1(3):111 117. ( 10.1111/j.1939-3938.2005.tb00027.x) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4. Narang SK, Fingarson A, Lukefahr J, Council on Child Abuse and Neglect. Abusive head trauma in infants and children. Pediatrics. 2020;145(4):e20200203. ( 10.1542/peds.2020-0203) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5. Shanahan ME, Zolotor AJ, Parrish JW, Barr RG, Runyan DK. National, regional, and state abusive head trauma: application of the CDC algorithm. Pediatrics. 2013;132(6):e1546 e1553. ( 10.1542/peds.2013-2049) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6. Hung KL. Pediatric abusive head trauma. Biomed J. 2020;43(3):240 250. ( 10.1016/j.bj.2020.03.008) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7. Barr RG. Preventing abusive head trauma resulting from a failure of normal interaction between infants and their caregivers. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2012;109(suppl 2):17294 17301. ( 10.1073/pnas.1121267109) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8. Haviland J, Russell RI. Outcome after severe non-accidental head injury. Arch Dis Child. 1997;77(6):504 507. ( 10.1136/adc.77.6.504) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9. King WJ, MacKay M, Sirnick A. Shaken baby syndrome in Canada: clinical characteristics and outcomes of hospital cases. CMAJ. 2003;168(2):155 159. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10. Miller Ferguson N, Sarnaik A, Miles D, et al. Abusive head trauma and mortality-an analysis from an international comparative effectiveness study of children with severe traumatic brain injury. Crit Care Med. 2017;45(8):1398 1407. ( 10.1097/CCM.0000000000002378) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11. Barlow K, Thompson E, Johnson D, Minns RA. The neurological outcome of non-accidental head injury. Pediatr Rehabil. 2004;7(3):195 203. ( 10.1080/13638490410001715331) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12. Chevignard MP, Lind K. Long-term outcome of abusive head trauma. Pediatr Radiol. 2014;44(suppl 4):S548 S558. ( 10.1007/s00247-014-3169-8) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13. Depallens S, Favrod C, Maeder P, San Millan D, Cheseaux JJ. Early diagnosis of abusive head trauma to avoid repetitive shaking events. Glob Pediatr Health. 2022;9:2333794X211067037. ( 10.1177/2333794X211067037) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14. Canadian Public Health Association. Joint statement on shaken baby syndrome. Available at: https://www.cpha.ca/sites/default/files/assets/policy/sbs_e.pdf. Accessed December 7, 2021 [Google Scholar]
- 15. National Center on Shaken Baby Syndrome. Learn more. Available at: https://www.dontshake.org/learn-more. Accessed July 21, 2021 [Google Scholar]
- 16. Laurent-Vannier A, Nathanson M, Quiriau F, et al. A public hearing “shaken baby syndrome: guidelines on establishing a robust diagnosis and the procedures to be adopted by healthcare and social services staff”. Guidelines issued by the Hearing Commission. Ann Phys Rehabil Med. 2011;54(9-10):600 625. ( 10.1016/j.rehab.2011.10.002) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17. Kazmir S, Rosado N. Abusive head trauma: a review of current knowledge. Clin Pediatr Emerg Med. 2020;21(3):100791. ( 10.1016/j.cpem.2020.100791) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 18. Nuño M, Ugiliweneza B, Zepeda V, et al. Long-term impact of abusive head trauma in young children. Child Abuse Negl. 2018;85:39 46. ( 10.1016/j.chiabu.2018.08.011) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19. Ricci L, Giantris A, Merriam P, Hodge S, Doyle T. Abusive head trauma in Maine infants: medical, child protective, and law enforcement analysis. Child Abuse Negl. 2003;27(3):271 283. ( 10.1016/s0145-2134(03)00006-1) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20. Notrica DM, Kirsch L, Misra S, et al. Evaluating abusive head trauma in children <5 years old: risk factors and the importance of the social history. J Pediatr Surg. 2021;56(2):390 396. ( 10.1016/j.jpedsurg.2020.10.019) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21. Keenan HT, Runyan DK, Marshall SW, Nocera MA, Merten DF, Sinal SH. A population-based study of inflicted traumatic brain injury in young children. JAMA. 2003;290(5):621 626. ( 10.1001/jama.290.5.621) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22. Yang CC, Hsu YL. A review of accelerometry-based wearable motion detectors for physical activity monitoring. Sensors (Basel). 2010;10(8):7772 7788. ( 10.3390/s100807772) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23. Bandak FA. Shaken baby syndrome: a biomechanics analysis of injury mechanisms. Forensic Sci Int. 2005;151(1):71 79. ( 10.1016/j.forsciint.2005.02.033) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24. Feraboli P, Kedward KT. Enhanced evaluation of the low velocity impact response of composite plates. AIAA J. 2004;42(10):2143 2152. ( 10.2514/1.4534) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 25. Yamazaki J, Yoshida M, Mizunuma H. Experimental analyses of the retinal and subretinal haemorrhages accompanied by shaken baby syndrome/abusive head trauma using a dummy doll. Injury. 2014;45(8):1196 1206. ( 10.1016/j.injury.2014.04.014) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26. Hazan C, Shaver P. Romantic love conceptualized as an attachment process. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1987;52(3):511 524. ( 10.1037//0022-3514.52.3.511) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27. Mikulincer M, Florian V, Tolmacz R. Attachment styles and fear of personal death: a case study of affect regulation. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1990;58(2):273 280. ( 10.1037/0022-3514.58.2.273) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 28. Sabuncuoğlu O, Berkem M. Bağlanma biçemi ve doğum sonrası depresyon belirtileri arasındaki ilişki: Türkiye’den bulgular Relationship between attachment style and depressive symptoms in postpartum women: findings from Turkey. Turk Psikiyatri Derg. 2006;17(4):252 258 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29. Kesebir S, Kökçü F, Dereboy F, Ölçeği EBB. Geçerlilik ve güvenilirlik çalışması. Adult attachment style scale: adaptation, reliability and validity study. Yeni Symp. 2012;50:99 104 [Google Scholar]
- 30. Dökmen Ü. Empatinin yeni bir modele dayanılarak ölçülmesi ve psikodrama ile geliştirilmesi. Measurement of empathy based on a new model and its development with psychodrama. Ank Univ Eğitim Bilimleri Fak Derg. 1988;21(1):155 190. [Google Scholar]
- 31. Derogatis LR. The brief symptom inventory-BSI administration, scoring and procedures manual-II. USA: Clinical Psychometric Research ; Inc; 1992. [Google Scholar]
- 32. Şahin NH, Durak A. Kısa semptom envanteri (Brief Symptom Inventory-BSI): Türk gençleri için uyarlanması. [Brief symptom inventory (BSI): Adaptation for Turkish youth]. Turk Psikol Derg. 1994;9(31):44 56 [Google Scholar]
- 33. Zimet GD, Dahlem NW, Zimet SG, Farley GK. The multidimensional scale of perceived social support. J Pers Assess. 1988;52(1):30 41. ( 10.1207/s15327752jpa5201_2) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34. Eker D, Arkar H. Çok boyutlu algılanan sosyal destek ölçeği’nin faktör yapısı, geçerlik ve güvenirliği. Factor structure, validity and reliability of multidimensional scale of perceived social support. Turk Psikol Derg. 1995;34:45 55. [Google Scholar]
- 35. Rosenberg M. Society and the Adolescent Self-Image. Princeton : NJ: Princeton University Press; 1965. [Google Scholar]
- 36. Çuhadaroğlu F. Adolesanlarda Benlik Saygısı [Self-Esteem in Adolescents]. [Specialty thesis]. Ankara ( Türkiye: ): Hacettepe; University Medical Faculty, Department of Psychiatry; 1986. [Google Scholar]
- 37. Fujiwara T, Barr RG, Brant R, Barr M. Infant distress at five weeks of age and caregiver frustration. J Pediatr. 2011;159(3):425 430.e1 -2. ( 10.1016/j.jpeds.2011.02.010) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38. Appleyard K, Berlin LJ, Rosanbalm KD, Dodge KA. Preventing early child maltreatment: implications from a longitudinal study of maternal abuse history, substance use problems, and offspring victimization. Prev Sci. 2011;12(2):139 149. ( 10.1007/s11121-010-0193-2) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39. Pasalich DS, Cyr M, Zheng Y, McMahon RJ, Spieker SJ. Child abuse history in teen mothers and parent-child risk processes for offspring externalizing problems. Child Abuse Negl. 2016;56:89 98. ( 10.1016/j.chiabu.2016.04.011) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40. Romero-Martínez A, Figueiredo B, Moya-Albiol L. Childhood history of abuse and child abuse potential: the role of parent’s gender and timing of childhood abuse. Child Abuse Negl. 2014;38(3):510 516. ( 10.1016/j.chiabu.2013.09.010) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41. Spencer N, Wallace A, Sundrum R, Bacchus C, Logan S. Child abuse registration, fetal growth, and preterm birth: a population based study. J Epidemiol Community Health. 2006;60(4):337 340. ( 10.1136/jech.2005.042085) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42. Plate RC, Bloomberg Z, Bolt DM, Bechner AM, Roeber BJ, Pollak SD. Abused children experience high anger exposure. Front Psychol. 2019;10:440. ( 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00440) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43. Lo CKM, Tung KTS, Chan KL, et al. Risk factors for child physical abuse and neglect among Chinese young mothers. Child Abuse Negl. 2017;67:193 206. ( 10.1016/j.chiabu.2017.02.031) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44. O’Donnell M, Maclean MJ, Sims S, Morgan VA, Leonard H, Stanley FJ. Maternal mental health and risk of child protection involvement: mental health diagnoses associated with increased risk. J Epidemiol Community Health. 2015;69(12):1175 1183. ( 10.1136/jech-2014-205240) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45. Milner JS, Halsey LB, Fultz J. Empathic responsiveness and affective reactivity to infant stimuli in high- and low-risk for physical child abuse mothers. Child Abuse Negl. 1995;19(6):767 780. ( 10.1016/0145-2134(95)00035-7) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46. Rodriguez CM, Cook AE, Jedrziewski CT. Reading between the lines: implicit assessment of the association of parental attributions and empathy with abuse risk. Child Abuse Negl. 2012;36(7-8):564 571. ( 10.1016/j.chiabu.2012.05.004) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47. Perez-Albeniz A, de Paul J. Dispositional empathy in high- and low-risk parents for child physical abuse. Child Abuse Negl. 2003;27(7):769 780. ( 10.1016/s0145-2134(03)00111-x) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48. Cooke JE, Racine N, Plamondon A, Tough S, Madigan S. Maternal adverse childhood experiences, attachment style, and mental health: pathways of transmission to child behavior problems. Child Abuse Negl. 2019;93:27 37. ( 10.1016/j.chiabu.2019.04.011) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49. Black DA, Heyman RE, Slep AMS. Risk factors for child physical abuse. Aggress Violent Behav. 2001;6(2-3):121 188. [Google Scholar]
- 50. Yang MY, Maguire-Jack K. Individual and cumulative risks for child abuse and neglect. Fam Relat. 2018;67(2):287 301. ( 10.1111/fare.12310) [DOI] [Google Scholar]