Abstract
We studied the impact of humor on the Iowa Gambling Task (IGT) decision-making performance and the cognitive control exerted during this task, considering sex as a moderator, and examined whether cognitive control mediated the influence of humor on decision-making. Sixty participants (30 females) performed an extended version of the IGT (500 trials divided into 20 blocks). We randomly assigned them to either an experimental group (Humor Group; Hg; n = 30), where humorous videos were interspersed in the decision-making trials or a control group (Non-Humor Group; NHg; n = 30), where nonhumorous videos were interspersed in the decision-making trials. We recorded participant performance and feedback-related negativity (FRN) and P3b event-related potentials (ERP) during IGT feedback as task monitoring and attention allocation indicators, respectively. We expected that whereas humor would improve IGT decision-making under risk in females during the last blocks (17–20) as well as cognitive control (specifically attention allocation and task monitoring) across the entire IGT, it would impair them in males. Contrary to our expectations, humor improved IGT decision-making under risk for both sexes (specifically at blocks 19 and 20) and attention allocation for most IGT blocks (P3b amplitudes). However, humor impaired IGT decision-making under ambiguity in males during the block six and task monitoring (FRN amplitudes) for most IGT blocks. Attention allocation did not mediate the beneficial effect of humor on decision-making under risk in either sex. Task monitoring decrements fully mediated the humor's detrimental influence on men's decision-making under ambiguity during block six.
Supplementary Information
The online version contains supplementary material available at 10.3758/s13415-024-01210-y.
Keywords: Humor, Iowa Gambling Task, Decision-making, Gender, Cognitive control
Introduction
The Iowa Gambling Task (IGT; Bechara et al., 1994) captures the intricate interplay between emotions and cognition in real-life decision-making. Originally developed to assess decision-making impairments in individuals with prefrontal cortex damage (Bechara, 2005), the IGT has been employed to study impulsivity and compromised decision-making in conditions, such as substance abuse and pathological gambling (Kovács et al., 2017). It also has gained widespread use in research with nonclinical participants, revealing significant associations with psychological factors, such as reward responsiveness, fun-seeking, impulsivity, and physiological markers linked to inhibitory control (Giustiniani et al., 2019; Suhr & Tsanadis, 2007).
Participants playing the IGT face four card decks and try to maximize gains while minimizing losses without any a priori information. Each deck represents different reward-to-punishment ratios, which participants must learn as they play. According to the Somatic Marker hypothesis (Bechara, 2005), both emotional and cognitive processes are needed to achieve superior performance; however, what these processes exactly mean has been substantially challenged across time (Bowman et al., 2005; Fernie & Tunney, 2013). Brevers et al. (2013) updated terms into hot and cool executive processes. Thus, during early blocks1 people require using hot processing, because the probabilities are unknown and cannot be estimated rationally, so the decision-making is to some extent emotionally guided (decision-making under ambiguity). By contrast, the last block operates under a cool executive control, because at that point probabilities are known and rationally guided (decision-making under risk). During hot processing, participants engage in exploring various deck options to grasp the associations between decks and the reward and punishment outcomes (i.e., the emotional processing of monetary feedback; Bechara, 2005; van den Bos et al., 2012). Players first prioritize immediate substantial rewards, often disregarding the risk of accumulating losses. Near the end of the task, successful participants depart from this high-risk default strategy and settle into making choices that lead to smaller gains but also result in smaller losses. As a result, we see a transition from ambiguity to decision-making under risk; the task depends more on cool executive function, because the deck’s probabilities are known (Brevers et al., 2013).
Growing research highlights sex's crucial role in untangling the intricate interplay between emotion and cognition2 (Bolla et al., 2004; Flores-Torres et al., 2019, 2022; Overman et al., 2006; Reavis & Overman, 2001; Weller et al., 2010). While the influence of cultural and social contexts should not be dismissed, consistent findings emphasize fundamental processes that deserve attention. For instance, females typically manifest higher emotional intensity and responsiveness to negative valence stimuli compared with males (Van Hasselt et al., 2012; Overman et al., 2011; Flores-Torres et al., 2022). Moreover, sex differences in brain activation during cognitive reappraisal indicate that females recruit prefrontal resources typically indicative of cognitive control to a greater extent than males, while simultaneously recruiting less activation in regions associated with reward (McRae et al., 2008). Therefore, there is reason to predict that the interaction between positive emotion and cognitive control is moderated by sex. However, our previous IGT study collected only task performance, so the neural processes engaged during this interaction remained unclear.
Considering the factors mentioned, it is not surprising that consistent evidence supports sex differences on IGT performance (Bolla et al., 2004; Flores-Torres et al., 2019, 2022; Overman et al., 2006; Reavis & Overman, 2001; Weller et al., 2010). Women typically earn less money and take more trials to select consistently advantageous decks with long-term benefits compared with men (Bolla et al., 2004; Flores-Torres et al., 2019; Flores-Torres et al., 2022; Weller et al., 2010). These sex differences primarily emerge during the second half of the task, indicating that they are related to maintaining (cool) cognitive control rather than solely the (hot) learning of the task.
Cognitive control is a neural mechanism crucial for goal-directed behaviors, which enables us to engage in complex behaviors designed to achieve goals by overriding or modifying automatic and habitual responses (Mäki-Marttunen et al., 2019; Miller, 2000). Notably, cognitive control is not only involved during IGT performance but also during humor processing. The relationship between humor and cognitive control is clear during the cognitive processes involved in creating, understanding, and responding to humor. For instance, humor arises from incongruities, surprises, or deviations from expectations, such as when we understand something that could be interpreted in different ways and purposedly we choose to play with those meanings or by inhibiting the expression of something not funny (Beaty & Silvia, 2012; Martin, 2007; Zabelina & Ganis, 2018). Given that cognitive control is necessary for both solving the IGT (Bolla et al., 2004; Flores-Torres et al., 2019; Flores-Torres et al., 2022; Weller et al., 2010) and processing humor (Beaty & Silvia, 2012; Zabelina & Ganis, 2018), it is conceivable that participants who engage in alternating tasks involving humorous stimuli and the IGT might experience a competition for cognitive resources. Consequently, this competition could lead to a decline in performance on the IGT. However, it is important to note that this assertion assumes that men and women process humor and the IGT in a similar manner, which is not the case.
First, functional neuroanatomical sex differences in humor processing have been observed, with greater left prefrontal cortex activation in females when exposed to humorous stimuli compared with males (Azim et al., 2005). Another study found that females tend to use an emotional/limbic pathway while processing humor, whereas males rely more on a cognitive/executive pathway (Kohn et al., 2011). Second, sex differences in neuroanatomy and function also have been associated with variations in performance on the IGT. Females have shown hypoactivity in limbic structures related to affective and reward control, as well as lower emotional control compared with males (Van Hasselt et al., 2012; Overman et al., 2011; Bechara, 2005; van den Bos et al., 2012). These findings indicate significant sex differences in the mechanisms of cognitive and emotional control. Evidence suggests that females may achieve better cognitive control through emotion regulation, whereas males may achieve cognitive control directly without depending on emotion regulation (Flores-Torres et al., 2022; Van Hasselt et al., 2012; Overman et al., 2011; Kohn et al., 2011). However, further research in this regard is still needed.
In a previous study, we investigated the impact of humor on decision-making during the IGT, considering the moderating effect of sex (Flores-Torres et al., 2019). Participants were exposed to either brief humorous or nonhumorous videos before each IGT decision. One-half of the sample was exposed to 100 brief humorous videos (humor group, Hg); the other half was exposed to 100 brief nonhumorous videos (nonhumor group, NHg)—see supplementary material. Women in the Hg demonstrated higher IGT performance than women in the NHg in the last block of the task, suggesting that humor had a beneficial effect on females’ IGT performance; by contrast, males in the Hg demonstrated lower IGT performance than males in the NHg in the last block of the task, indicating that humor impaired males IGT performance.
As far as we know, our previous study remains the only investigation that has delved into the intricate relationship among humor, sex, and decision-making on the IGT. Given this unique position, it is essential to replicate our findings to ensure their validity. Furthermore, while cognitive control may explain, at least partially, the effect of humor in decision-making, to our knowledge, no studies measuring neural correlates of cognitive control have been conducted yet. Therefore, this research has three main objectives. First, we attempt to reevaluate how humor and sex influence decision-making with a new sample. Second, we intend to explore how humor and sex impact the EEG activity associated with cognitive control during IGT performance as it evolves over time. Our third objective is to explore whether cognitive control mediates the influence of humor on decision-making and to explore whether this relationship also is moderated by sex.
Cognitive control has been linked to several event-related potentials (ERPs). Although names and functional descriptions differ, a rather uniform sequence of ERPs is found during the IGT: an early frontocentral negativity named feedback-related negativity (FRN), which is especially relevant during the beginning of the task (Bianchin & Angrilli, 2011; Chandrakumar et al., 2018; Cohen et al., 2007; Holroyd et al., 2005) and a later positive deflection mostly at parietal sites—named P3b—which is especially informative late in the task (Kok, 2001; Polich, 2007). The FRN component is expected, especially after punishments, and is interpreted as a signal indicating incoming evidence for the potential necessity of action adaptation (Wetzels et al., 2010; Ullsperger et al., 2010, Steinhauser & Yeung, 2010). As the evidence of past choices accumulates, a later posterior positivity appears—P3b—which is considered a biomarker of the amount of attention available for task performance (Kahneman, 1973; O’Connell & Hofmann, 2012). In the context of the IGT, P3b amplitude is an index of attentional resource allocation to process cognitive aspects of the task feedback, which is fundamental to solve the task (Turetsky et al., 2009; O’Connell & Hofmann, 2012; Polich, 2007). Therefore, we assessed these two ERPs components—FRN and P3b during reward and punishments—as indicators of cognitive control. The first of these components will inform us about task learning, that is, how participants use the information provided through feedback to correct and improve their future decisions. The second one will inform us about attentional resource allocation toward IGT performance improvement.
Participants were exposed to either brief humorous or nonhumorous videos during the IGT. Building on previous research, we predicted that men in the humor group (Hg) would exhibit lower IGT performance during the final blocks (decision-making under risk), specifically from block 17 to 20, compared with men in the nonhumor group (NHg). Men in the Hg were anticipated to have reduced amplitudes in the EEG correlates of cognitive control (FRN and P3b) across the entire task compared to the men in the NHg. By contrast, we expected that women in the Hg would exhibit higher IGT performance, especially during the final blocks (17–20), compared with women in the NHg. We anticipated that women in the Hg would exhibit increased amplitudes in FRN and the P3b components throughout the task, in contrast to the women in the NHg. Lastly, we hypothesized that cognitive control would mediate the relationship between humor and IGT performance and tested whether this was moderated by sex.
Materials and methods
Participants
Inclusion criteria for participation were (1) being an undergraduate student, (2) speaking Spanish, and (3) having normal or corrected-to-normal vision. Exclusion criteria were (1) reporting current diagnosis of a neuropsychiatric disorder, (2) scores on the Self-Report Questionnaire (SRQ) (Harding et al., 1980; Vielma et al., 1994) indicating severe depressive symptomatology, (3) reporting a history of drug or alcohol abuse and/or consumption of drugs within 24 h of participating in the experimental task.
The sample size was determined before data collection, based on our previous study (Flores-Torres et al., 2019). According to analyses performed with the software G* Power 3.1, in that study for a sample of 68 participants [34 women (17 experimental and 17 control) and 34 men (17 experimental and 17 control)], we obtained a large effect size of Fs (0.47), and a statistical power of 0.97 using a probability error α (0.05) for hypothesized differences in the interaction (Sex by Condition by Blocks). Because we are using the same experimental design, we calculated the sample size for the present study for a large effect size Fs (0.40), a probability error α (0.05), and a statistical power of (0.80), which leads to 60 participants (30 women and 30 men). Finally, 64 participants (33 women and 31 men) completed the study. Data from four of these participants (1 man and 3 women) were not included in the analyses, because it was determined after participating that they did not fulfill participation criteria. One of the participants reported having consumed drugs before the experiment, and three of them presented severe depressive symptomatology according to the SRQ. Therefore, data from 60 participants (30 men; mean age 23.05, SD = 4.6 and 30 women; mean age 23.6, SD = 4.4) were included in the final analyses.
Questionnaires and instruments
The Self-Report Questionnaire (Harding et al., 1980)
The SRQ was used to assess depressive symptomatology. It consists of 25 yes/no questions. The SRQ has been validated for the Chilean population (Vielma et al., 1994). Subjects answering affirmatively to questions 21 to 25 (elevated probability of depressive symptomatology) were not included in the study sample, because depression has been shown to affect decision-making (Battersby et al., 2006; Dalgleish et al., 2004).
The State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI) (Spielberger, 1970)
The STAI was used to assess anxiety symptoms. It consists of 40 questions divided into two subscales: state anxiety and trait anxiety. The STAI has been validated for the Chilean population (Vera-Villarroel et al., 2007). We assessed this construct because higher trait anxiety scores have been previously associated with impairments in decision-making and could potentially affect our results (Miu et al., 2008).
Humorous and nonhumorous videos
We used a selection of 200 videos (100 humorous and 100 nonhumorous). These videos were used in our previous study, in which they were rated as equally funny by women and men. For a detailed description, see supplementary material and Flores-Torres et al., 2019.
The IGT
The IGT was designed as a realistic decision-making task (Bechara et al., 1994; Hooper et al., 2004). On each trial, participants choose a card from one of four card decks (A, B, C, and D). After each choice, participants might be rewarded with either virtual money (reward) or punished with a loss of virtual money (punishment). Participants must learn as they play which are the advantageous and disadvantageous decks to solve the task and maximize earnings. Participants can change decks at will; however, they are warned that some decks are worse than others in terms of total payment and that the win/loss proportions and amounts stay fixed within each deck. Likewise, they are informed that the goal is to win as much money as they can or to avoid losing money as much as possible.
Card decks A and B are monetarily risky/disadvantageous, and C and D are monetarily safe/advantageous. Card decks A and B are associated with large, immediate rewards (e.g., $100), but continuing to select from these decks results in accumulating less profit, or loss, because of occasional, large monetary punishments. Choosing from card decks A and B leads to a net loss of $250 during the first 10 trials. By contrast, card decks C and D are associated with small immediate rewards (e.g., $50) but with small monetary punishments. Continuing to select from these decks results in accumulating more profit, and choosing from decks C and D leads to a net gain of $250 during the first 10 trials.
For the present research, to measure reliably the event-related potentials (ERP), we modified the original IGT following Cui et al. guidelines (2013). Namely, we increased the number of trials from 100 to 500 and modified the feedback shown to participants, such that the presentation of win and losses observed in the original task was replaced by net scores. According to previous studies (Flores-Torres et al., 2019, 2022; Weller et al., 2010), we expected to find differences in the last blocks for the present study, blocks 17 to 20.
EEG data acquisition and preprocessing
EEG data were recorded on a Biosemi Active two system. We used 64 active electrodes placed according to the international 10/20 extended system. Horizontal and vertical eye movements were monitored using four external electrodes. Horizontal EOG was recorded bipolarly from the outer canthi of both eyes and vertical EOG was recorded from above and below of the participant’s right eye. Data pre-processing was performed by using MATLAB v8.3.0.532 (Matlab, 2014) with EEGLAB v13.6.5b toolbox (Delorme & Makeig, 2004) and ERPLAB v9.20 toolbox (Lopez-Calderon & Luck, 2014). Following Cui et al.’s guidelines (2013), the signal was down-sampled offline at 512 Hz, and all electrodes were referenced to averaged mastoids. Following Tanner et al.’s guidelines (2016), a second-order infinite impulse response Butterworth filter was used for band-pass filtering continuous EEG data, with a half amplitude cutoff frequency of 0.1 Hz and 30 Hz. For channels and artifact rejection, we performed a semiautomated channel rejection coupled with visual inspection applying EEGLAB “trim outlier” function. We set a high upper bound rejection threshold of 200 μV. The total average of rejected channels was 0.6 channels per subject. Commonly recorded artifactual potentials (CRAP: Luck, 2014) detection was performed by setting a high rejection criterion (± 100 μV) threshold for the moving window peak-to-peak algorithm. This procedure allowed rejecting extremely noisy data, which never exceeded 15% of total trials (75/500 trials). We also performed visual inspection to detect muscle-like activity. Then, we performed independent component analysis (ICA; Delorme & Makeig, 2004) decomposition on continuous data, through infomax RunICA algorithm, to eliminate an average of two ocular stereotypical artifactual components per EEG recording. RunICA is an iterative algorithm that minimizes the non-Gaussianity of the components, which allows for its separation and, in our case, facilitates the subsequent identification of artifactual components. Then, we performed a channel spherical interpolation function.
Electrophysiological assessment
The EEG signal was segmented into 500 trials. For each trial, we analyzed the 200 ms immediately prior to feedback until 1,000 ms after feedback onset. The average number of epochs after artifactual rejection and correction was 460 (SD = 10). The prestimulus window of 200 ms was used to correct baseline activity in each trial. Epochs were averaged for each subject, and then averaged by sex (males vs. females), condition (humor vs. nonhumor), feedback (reward vs. punishment), and Blocks (Block 1 [trials 1–100], Block 2 [trials 101–200], Block 3 [trials 201–300]), Block 4 [trials 301–400], and Block 5 [trials 401–500], based in Garrido-Chaves et al. (2021). We have chosen to work with five blocks of 100 trials each, rather than 20 blocks with 25 trials each, mirroring the original segmentation performed in Bechara’s behavioral analyses. This decision is based on recent studies on ERPs where in between-group designs, statistical power doubles when transitioning from 45 to 90 trials, even when having just 12 subjects per condition (Fischer et al., 2017; Boudewyn et al., 2018).
For tmax permutation analysis (Groppe et al., 2011a, 2011b) (see Data Analyses), the data were down-sampled to 128 Hz to decrease the number of comparisons and increase statistical power (Luck, 2014). Following an examination of the permutation results, mean difference wave for the FRN amplitude was set within 100–250-ms time window relative to the 200-ms prestimulus baseline and computed for the three electrodes of interest (FZ, FCZ and CZ), selection based on existing literature (Chandrakumar et al., 2018; Cohen et al., 2007). Cluster mass permutation test (Blair & Karniski, 1993) procedure was performed for the P3b amplitudes, which was set within 350–450-ms time window relative to the 200-ms prestimulus baseline and computed for 36 electrodes of interest: FP1, AF7, F7, F5, FT7, C1, C3, C5, T7, CP1, CP3, TP7, P1, P3, P5, PO3, P9, FCZ, CZ, CPZ, PZ, POZ, OZ, C2, C4, T8, CP2, CP4, CP6, TP8, P2, P4, P6, P8, PO4, PO8. This resulted in a matrix of widely distributed electrodes for P3b activity derived from actual data, rather than relying on a preselected electrode matrix for P3b analysis, as shown in previous studies (Cui et al., 2013).
Procedure
Our research protocol was approved by the Ethics Committee of the Pontificia Universidad Católica de Chile (PUC). Participants provided written consent in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. All experiments were performed at the Neuro-dynamic Laboratory of the School of Psychology of the PUC. We recruited participants for the study through an advertisement published on the PUC student website. Those interested in participation were informed about the inclusion and exclusion criteria and provided with more study details via email. If they reported that they met the inclusion criteria, we invited them to come to the lab. Participants were given a movie ticket as compensation for their time used in the laboratory.
In-lab session. First, we provided participants with more details about the study and completed the informed consent process. Next, participants completed the SRQ and the STAI-t. Then, they sat down in a comfortable chair in front of a computer screen, and the electrodes were connected. After the electrode application was completed, participants started the experimental task. Task instructions were presented on the computer screen. The distance from participant’s eyes to the computer screen was 60 cm, visual angle 4.7°, refresh rate 144 frames per seconds. Study duration was approximately 1 h.
Each trial began with the word “video,” which appeared on the screen for 1,500 ms. Then, the video itself appeared (5,000 ± 1,000 ms of duration), followed by five decision-making trials. Because participants made five decisions for each video, by the end of the task, each participant completed 100 different videos and 500 IGT trials. During these trials, participants saw four deck options (labeled A, B, C, and D) and chose one by left clicking on their preference with a USB mouse. When participants selected a deck, its perimeter lit up in red. After that, the screen changed to black for 300 ms, which allowed for a clean baseline for ERP measurement. Then, feedback appeared for 2,000 ms, the onset of which corresponds to “0” time to evaluate ERPs. Feedback could be a net gain (e.g., + 100) or a net loss (e.g., − 50). Each card’s feedback depended on the probabilities according to the Bechara IGT manual (2007), using modifications for an electrophysiological adaptation (Cui et al., 2013). During the screen showing the four deck options, on the central superior area of the screen, two bars appeared. A green bar showed cumulative wins and losses and a red bar represented the amount of money they owed (all participants started the task with $2,000 CLP of virtual money [around $3 USD]). After feedback, these bars automatically updated according to the feedback on that trial. We emphasized to participants that positions and deck contingencies were fixed during the whole task, that they could change decks at will, and that there was no association whatsoever between the videos and the decks. Participants had no specific information about how to solve the task, nor did they know how long it would take. Participants completed 100 videos and 500 IGT trials (divided into 20 blocks of 25 trials each). We programmed two breaks (after 40% and 70% of total trials). As mentioned, we calculated IGT performance every 25 trials. We calculated individual block performance scores and not cumulative total ones, whereas we calculated mean FRN and P3b amplitudes every 100 trials. Therefore, we have five measurements of FRN and P3b and 20 measurements of IGT performance throughout the experiment.
Data analyses
To test our behavioral hypotheses, we conducted several three-way mixed ANOVAs and MANOVAS. Before them, we checked normality, linearity, and sphericity assumptions. We used the parameter ε Greenhouse–Geisser to correct for sphericity violations. Our significance threshold was p < 0.05, with Bonferroni corrections for post-hoc comparisons. Outliers were replaced using the mean plus two standard deviations method recommended by Field (2013).
We corrected for multiple comparisons by using sequential Bonferroni-Holm and Benjamini–Hochberg procedures (Cramer et al., 2016). To test our neurophysiological ERPs, we performed permutation tests based on tmax statistics (Blair & Karniski, 1993). One critical advantage of the tmax procedure is that it uses the characteristics of the actual data to evaluate statistical significance. This method was chosen, because it allows to determine, with strong confidence and without previous assumptions, the temporal dynamic and topographical distribution of an effect, controlling adequately for Type-1 and Type-2 error (Luck, 2014). We performed tmax permutation procedures on mean difference wave amplitudes for a global time window. We did this for FRN (100–250 ms) on three representative midline electrodes (FZ, FCZ, and CZ). For the P3b amplitude, we observed that previous studies have used an a priori highly distributed selection of electrodes (Cui et al., 2013). Thus, we preferred using a cluster mass permutation test to determine both topography and time distribution based on the actual data.
The cluster-based permutation tests operate by building a null hypothesis distribution from the most extreme statistic from each time point/electrode. First, it forms clusters of neighboring extreme t-scores and then builds the null hypothesis distribution from the most extreme cluster statistic (e.g., the sum of all the t-scores in the cluster).
The results for cluster mass permutation were set within 350–450-ms time window relative to the 200-ms prestimulus baseline and determined 36 electrodes of interest: FP1, AF7, F7, F5, FT7, C1, C3, C5, T7, CP1, CP3, TP7, P1, P3, P5, PO3, P9, FCZ, CZ, CPZ, PZ, POZ, OZ, C2, C4, T8, CP2, CP4, CP6, TP8, P2, P4, P6, P8, PO4, PO8.
To examine whether the effect of humor on IGT performance was mediated by cognitive control and whether this mediational effect was moderated by sex, we conducted several path analyses with standard errors, indirect effects, and nonsymmetric 3,000 bootstrap confidence intervals (CI) by using the software MPlus (Muthén & Muthén, 2017). We employed the mean cumulative scores of targeted IGT block performance and mean cognitive control scores (averaged amplitudes for FRN and P3b components), both transformed to z-scores, to construct comparable mediational models across multiple timepoints.
Results
Preliminary analyses
Because trait anxiety has been shown to affect IGT performance, and women report more trait anxiety than men (Miu et al., 2008), we examined differences in this variable between the groups. Namely, we conducted a two-way between subject factorial ANOVA (Sex [male and female] by Group [humor and nonhumor]), in which the dependent variable was trait anxiety. The main effect of Sex, the main effect of Group, and the interaction were not statistically significant, indicating that there were no significant differences in trait anxiety among groups. Therefore, we did not consider this variable in further analyses.
Differences in IGT Performance
To examine the effect of humor on IGT performance and whether the effect of humor differed by sex, we conducted a three-way repeated measures ANOVA (sex [male and female] by group [humor and non-humor] by blocks [from 1 to 20]) considering as the dependent variable the number of advantageous decks chosen (C + D). Results revealed a significant main effect of blocks F (3,132) = 3.83, p < 0.01; ηp2 = 0.08, 95% CI [0.31, 0.69] indicating that performance improved across the task. The blocks by group interaction was statistically significant (F (19,38) = 2.57, p < 0.01; ηp2 = 0.56, 95% CI [0.06, 0.54]), indicating that participants in the Hg exhibited higher IGT performance during the end of the task, during block 19 (trials 451–475; t = 4.46, p = 0.01) and block 20 (trials 476–500; t = 5.03, p < 0.01) than participants in the NHg (See Fig. 1). The triple interaction of sex by group by blocks was statistically significant F (19,38) = 1.88, p = 0.04; ηp2 = 0.48, 95% CI [0, 0.45]. Specifically, our analysis revealed that males in the Hg initially exhibited lower IGT performance than males in the NHg during block six (trials 126–150; t = -5.53, p < 0.01, 95% CI -9.47, -1.6). Nevertheless, this last result must be interpreted with caution since the triple interaction of sex, group and block did not survive our correction procedures, meaning it may not be reliable. Preserving specially the main effect of blocks and the interaction blocks by group (See Table 1).
Table 1.
Effect | p | αadjSeqB | αadjSeqBH | H0 seqB | H0 seqBH |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Blocks | .0001 | .0071 | .0071 | rejected | Rejected |
Blocks x Group | .006 | .0083 | .0142 | rejected | Rejected |
Blocks x Sex x Group | .04 | .0100 | .0214 | retained | Retained |
Blocks x Sex | .05 | .0125 | .0285 | retained | Retained |
Sex x Group | .3 | .0166 | .0357 | retained | Retained |
Sex | .6 | .0250 | .0428 | retained | Retained |
Group | .7 | .0500 | .0500 | retained | Retained |
αadjSeqB = the adjusted alpha level with the sequential Bonferroni procedure; αadjSeqBH the adjusted alpha level with the Benjamini–Hochberg procedure; H0 seqB = evaluation of the null hypothesis with the sequential Bonferroni procedure; H0 seqBH = evaluation of the null hypothesis with the Benjamini–Hochberg procedure
Differences in FRN
Given that the FRN component exhibits a well-determined EEG topography, we conducted a series of tmax permutation tests on key midline electrodes for assessing FRN (FZ, FCZ, and CZ). We applied an average difference waveform within the time interval of 100–250 ms (identified through tmax permutation analysis). Our analysis encompassed sex and group as intergroup factors, and type of feedback (monetary reward and monetary punishment) and EEG-blocks [E-block one (trials 1–100), E-block two (trials 101–200), E-block three (trials 201–300), E-block four (trials 301–400), and E-block five (trials 401–500)] as intraparticipant factors. A comprehensive summary of the outcomes of tmax permutation tests on FRN component disparities can be found in Table 2.
Table 2.
Comparison | Type of feedback | Block | Trials IGT | Time window | Electrodes | t | p |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
MHg-MNHg | Reward | 1 | 1–100 | 100–250 | FZ, FCZ, and CZ | 1.89 | [.008, .002] |
2 | 101–200 | FZ | 1.94 | .02 | |||
3 | 201–300 | None | 0 | .05 (NS) | |||
4 | 301–400 | None | 0 | .06 (NS) | |||
5 | 401–500 | FZ, FCZ, and CZ | 1.93 | [.04, .01] | |||
Punishment | 1 | 1–100 | None | 0 | .15 (NS) | ||
2 | 101–200 | None | 0 | .06 (NS) | |||
3 | 201–300 | FZ | 1.98 | .02 | |||
4 | 301–400 | FZ, FCZ, and CZ | 1.95 | [.04, .007] | |||
5 | 401–500 | FZ | 2.01 | .01 | |||
FHg-FNHg | Reward | 1 | 1–100 | 100–250 | None | 0 | .14 (NS) |
2 | 101–200 | None | 0 | .49 (NS) | |||
3 | 201–300 | None | 0 | .50 (NS) | |||
4 | 301–400 | None | 0 | .54 (NS) | |||
5 | 401–500 | None | 0 | .33 (NS) | |||
Punishment | 1 | 1–100 | None | 0 | .51 (NS) | ||
2 | 101–200 | None | 0 | .47 (NS) | |||
3 | 201–300 | None | 0 | .23 (NS) | |||
4 | 301–400 | None | 0 | .23 (NS) | |||
5 | 401–500 | None | 0 | .47 (NS) | |||
MHg-FHg | Reward | 1 | 1–100 | 100–250 | None | 0 | .11 (NS) |
2 | 101–200 | FZ | 1.86 | .03 | |||
3 | 201–300 | FZ, FCZ, and CZ | 1.88 | [.04, .007] | |||
4 | 301–400 | FZ, FCZ, and CZ | 1.89 | [.03, .001] | |||
5 | 401–500 | FZ and FCZ | 1.85 | [.03, .033] | |||
Punishment | 1 | 1–100 | FZ, FCZ, and CZ | 1.87 | [.04, .02] | ||
2 | 101–200 | None | 0 | .15 (NS) | |||
3 | 201–300 | FZ | 1.99 | .01 | |||
4 | 301–400 | FZ, FCZ, and CZ | 1.93 | [.03, .01] | |||
5 | 401–500 | FZ and FCZ | 1.97 | [.04, .007] | |||
MNHg-FNHg | Reward | 1 | 1–100 | 100–250 | None | 0 | .43 (NS) |
2 | 101–200 | None | 0 | .43 (NS) | |||
3 | 201–300 | None | 0 | .32 (NS) | |||
4 | 301–400 | None | 0 | .10 (NS) | |||
5 | 401–500 | None | 0 | .18 (NS) | |||
Punishment | 1 | 1–100 | None | 0 | .35 (NS) | ||
2 | 101–200 | None | 0 | .49 (NS) | |||
3 | 201–300 | None | 0 | .12 (NS) | |||
4 | 301–400 | None | 0 | .08 (NS) | |||
5 | 401–500 | None | 0 | .20 (NS) |
NS = not significant
Tmax permutation test results for FRN amplitude component during humor and nonhumor conditions, across sex, and in response to reward and punishment feedback are as follows (5,000 permutations): MHg = male in humor group; MNHg = male in nonhumor group; FHg = female in humor group; and WNHg = female in nonhumor group
Significant t-scores (t) and corrected p-values (p) within the [higher and lower p range value] range
When comparing males in the Hg to males in the NHg, significant differences emerged in the FRN component. Contrary to our expectations, males in the Hg displayed a significant decrease in FRN amplitude during reward feedback compared with males in the NHg. This reduction was evident in the E-block one (trials 1–100) across the three electrodes measured (FZ, FCZ, and CZ) and in E-block two (trials 101–200) at the FZ electrode. Furthermore, a significant decrease in FRN amplitude was observed in the E-block five (trials 401–500), impacting all measured electrodes. Critically, during punishment instances, a statistically significant decrease in FRN amplitude was observed in males in the Hg compared with males in the NHg. This effect manifested in E-block three (trials 201–300) at the FZ electrode, block four (trials 301–400) across all measured electrodes, and E-block five (trials 401–500), specifically at the FZ electrode.
We were interested in sex-related effects of humor on FRN amplitude components differences, particularly during the final E-blocks (decision-making under risk). Figures 2 and 3 display representative waveform data and tmax permutation results, respectively, from E-block four during punishments, which helps to illustrate the essential cognitive control processes involved to implement corrective measures after experiencing punishments.
Upon examining FRN amplitude differences between females in the Hg and females in the NHg, no statistically significant differences were found during either reward or punishment scenarios. Comparing males and females in the Hg, it was observed that, during reward feedback, males exhibited a considerable reduction in FRN amplitude compared with females during E-block two (trials 101–200) at electrode FZ; then during E-block three (trials 201–300) and E-block four (301–400) at the three electrodes measured, during E-block five (trials 401–500) at electrodes FZ and FCZ. Additionally, during punishment feedback, males in the Hg displayed a decreased FRN amplitude compared with females in the Hg: in E-block one (trials 1–100) at electrode FZ, FCZ, and CZ; in E-block three at electrode FZ; in E-block four a decrease in amplitude in the three electrodes measured; and in E-block five at electrodes FZ and FCZ. Lastly, in assessing differences between males and females within the nonhumor context (males in the NHg and females in the NHg), no statistically significant differences were identified during either reward or punishment delivery.
Differences in P3b
To identify clusters of adjacent values across electrodes and timepoints, we employed a series of cluster mass permutation tests. We applied an average difference waveform within the time interval of 350–450 ms, encompassing 36 electrodes, which was identified through the permutation analysis: FP1, AF7, F7, F5, FT7, C1, C3, C5, T7, CP1, CP3, TP7, P1, P3, P5, PO3, P9, FCZ, CZ, CPZ, PZ, POZ, OZ, C2, C4, T8, CP2, CP4, CP6, TP8, P2, P4, P6, P8, PO4, PO8. Within this framework, sex and group were considered as intergroup variables, whereas Type of Feedback (monetary reward and monetary punishment) and EEG-blocks [E-block one (trials 1–100), E-block two (trials 101–200), E-block three (trials 201–300), E-block four (trials 301–400), and E-block five (trials 401–500)] were regarded as intraparticipant factors. A comprehensive summary of the findings from the cluster permutation tests on P3b component variations is presented in Table 3.
Table 3.
Comparison | Type of feedback | Block | Trials IGT | Time window | Cluster | Electrodes | t | p |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
MHg-MNHg | Reward | 1 | 1–100 | 350–450 | 1 | P2 | 4.26 | .03 |
2 | 101–200 | 350–450 | 1 | P2 | 2.8 | .03 | ||
3 | 201–300 | 350–450 | 1 | P2 | 3.02 | .04 | ||
4 | 301–400 | 350–450 | 0 | 0 | 0 | .05 (NS) | ||
5 | 401–500 | 350–450 | 1 | P2 | 3.22 | .03 | ||
Punishment | 1 | 1–100 | 350–450 | 1 | P2 | 2.71 | .04 | |
2 | 101–200 | 350–450 | 1 | P2 | 2.71 | .04 | ||
3 | 201–300 | 350–450 | 1 | P2 | 3.0 | .04 | ||
4 | 301–400 | 350–450 | 0 | 0 | 0 | .05 (NS) | ||
5 | 401–500 | 350–450 | 1 | P2 | 3.62 | .03 | ||
FHg-FNHg | Reward | 1 | 1–100 | 350–450 | 4 | Fp1, AF7, FT7, C5, T7, TP7, P9, and P2 | 2.6 | [.03, .01] |
2 | 101–200 | 350–450 | 2 | Fp1, AF7, and P9 | 2.61 | [.04, .01] | ||
3 | 201–300 | 350–450 | 4 | Fp1, AF7, T7, P9, and P2 | 2.71 | [.04, .01] | ||
4 | 301–400 | 350–450 | 3 | Fp1, AF7, and P9 | 3.03 | [.03, .02] | ||
5 | 401–500 | 350–450 | 1 | Fp1 and AF7 | 4.2 | .01 | ||
Punishment | 1 | 1–100 | 350–450 | 3 | Fp1, AF7, FT7, C5, T7, and P9 | 2.68 | [.03, .01] | |
2 | 101–200 | 350–450 | 6 | Fp1, AF7, F7, C1, C5, T7, CP1, TP7, P9, C2, C4, and P2 | 2.47 | [.04, .01] | ||
3 | 201–300 | 350–450 | 3 | Fp1, AF7, C5, T7, and P9 | 2.99 | [.02, .01] | ||
4 | 301–400 | 350–450 | 5 | Fp1, AF7, FT7, C1, C5, T7, CP1, CP3, TP7, P3, P5, P9, Pz, and P2 | 2.48 | [.03, .01] | ||
5 | 401–500 | 350–450 | 2 | Fp1, AF7, and T7 | 2.81 | [.04, .01] | ||
MHg-FHg | Reward | 1 | 1–100 | 350–450 | 0 | 0 | 0 | 1.0 (NS) |
2 | 101–200 | 350–450 | 0 | 0 | 0 | 1.0 (NS) | ||
3 | 201–300 | 350–450 | 0 | 0 | 0 | 1.0 (NS) | ||
4 | 301–400 | 350–450 | 0 | 0 | 0 | 1.0 (NS) | ||
5 | 401–500 | 350–450 | 0 | 0 | 0 | 1.0 (NS) | ||
Punishment | 1 | 1–100 | 350–450 | 0 | 0 | 0 | 1.0 (NS) | |
2 | 101–200 | 350–450 | 0 | 0 | 0 | 1.0 (NS) | ||
3 | 201–300 | 350–450 | 0 | 0 | 0 | 1.0 (NS) | ||
4 | 301–400 | 350–450 | 0 | 0 | 0 | 1.0 (NS) | ||
5 | 401–500 | 350–450 | 0 | 0 | 0 | 1.0 (NS) | ||
MNHg-FNHg | Reward | 1 | 1–100 | 350–450 | 0 | 0 | 0 | 1.0 (NS) |
2 | 101–200 | 350–450 | 0 | 0 | 0 | 1.0 (NS) | ||
3 | 201–300 | 350–450 | 0 | 0 | 0 | 1.0 (NS) | ||
4 | 301–400 | 350–450 | 0 | 0 | 0 | 1.0 (NS) | ||
5 | 401–500 | 350–450 | 0 | 0 | 0 | 1.0 (NS) | ||
Punishment | 1 | 1–100 | 350–450 | 0 | 0 | 0 | 1.0 (NS) | |
2 | 101–200 | 350–450 | 0 | 0 | 0 | 1.0 (NS) | ||
3 | 201–300 | 350–450 | 0 | 0 | 0 | 1.0 (NS) | ||
4 | 301–400 | 350–450 | 0 | 0 | 0 | 1.0 (NS) | ||
5 | 401–500 | 350–450 | 0 | 0 | 0 | 1.0 (NS) |
Cluster permutation tests results for P3 amplitude component during humor and nonhumor, across sex, and in response to reward and punishment feedback are as follows (5,000 permutations): MHg = male in humor group, MNHg = male in non-humor group, FHg = female in humor group, and FNHg = female in nonhumor group
Significant t-scores (t) and corrected p-values (p) within the [higher and lower p range value] range. Max distance between adjacent electrodes corresponds to approximately 3.88 cm (assuming a 56-cm head circumference)
As summarized in Table 3, significant P3b amplitude differences were observed when comparing the males in the Hg with the males in the NHg. Contrary to our expectations, the males in the Hg exhibited increased P3b amplitudes during reward feedback compared with males in the NHg across E-block one (trials 1–100), E-block two (101–200), E-block three (201–300), and E-block five (trials 401–500), with a distinct cluster. Similar patterns were seen in the case of punishment, where males in the Hg consistently showed elevated P3b amplitudes compared with males in the NHg across E-blocks one, two, three, and five, again pinpointing the same cluster. Upon examining P3b amplitude disparities between females in the Hg and females in the NHg, and in line with our expectations, females in the Hg exhibited increased and highly distributed P3 amplitude activity during reward feedback compared with females in the NHg across all E-blocks; with four clusters in E-block one, two clusters in E-block two, four clusters in E-block three, three clusters in E-block four, and one cluster in E-block five. During punishment feedback, we also identified distributed increases in P3b amplitude activity across all E-blocks; with three clusters in E-block one, six clusters in E-block two, three clusters in E-block three, five clusters in E-block four, and two clusters in E-block five. Figures 4 and 5 display representative waveform data and cluster permutation results from E-block four during punishment. Therefore, E-block four serves to illustrate how cognitive control is allocating attentional resources to IGT performance when experiencing negative emotions.
Comparing males and females within the Hg, no statistically significant differences during either reward or punishment feedback were observed. Lastly, in assessing differences between males and females within the nonhumor group, no statistically significant differences were identified during either reward or punishment situations.
Mediational analysis results
Are the FRN amplitudes mediating the impairing effect of humor on IGT performance observed in males at IGT block six?
We conducted a path analysis by using only the sample of males to examine whether cognitive control, specifically FRN amplitudes, was mediating the impairing effect of humor on the IGT performance in males in Hg compared with males in the NHg at IGT block six. We considered humor as the independent variable, IGT performance during block six (IGT trials 126–150) as the dependent variable, and the mean of three electrodes (FZ, FCZ, and CZ) assessing FRN during reward on trial 1 to 100 as the mediator, because we had previously found differences in FRN amplitudes between males in the Hg and males in the NHg in these electrodes and trials. The indirect effect of humor on IGT performance through the FRN amplitudes was statistically significant (IE = 0.41; SE = 0.23; 95% CI = 0.08, 0.8). The direct effect of humor was not statistically significant (DE = 0.48; SE = 0.43; 95% CI = − 0.22, 1.21). Therefore, the FRN amplitudes during rewards were fully mediating the impairing effect of humor observed at block six in males.
Are the P3b amplitudes mediating the beneficial effect of humor in males during IGT block 20?
We conducted two path analyses on the sample of males to examine whether P3b amplitudes mediated the beneficial effect of humor on IGT during block 20. Humor was treated as the independent variable in both analyses, with IGT performance during block 20 (trials 476–500) as the dependent variable. In the first analysis, we considered the mean amplitude of electrode P2 assessing the P3b component during trials 201–300 in response to reward as the mediator, because we had previously found differences in P3b amplitudes between males in the Hg and males in the NHg in this particular electrode and trials. In the second analysis, the mean amplitude of electrode P2 in response to punishment during trials 201–300 was considered as the mediator.
Results for the first path analysis indicated that the indirect effect of humor on IGT performance through P3b amplitudes in response to rewards, was not statistically significant (IE = 0.11; SE = 0.23; CI 95% = -0.26, 0.48). Therefore, P3b amplitudes during rewards did not mediate the observed beneficial effect of humor during IGT block 20, among males. Similarly, results from the second path analysis did not reveal a statistically significant indirect effect of humor on IGT performance in response to punishment (IE = − 5.43; SE = 10.93; 95% CI = − 25.32, 8.87). Consequently, P3b amplitudes during punishments did not mediate the observed beneficial effect of humor during blocks 20, among males.
Are the P3b amplitudes mediating the beneficial effect of humor on IGT performance observed in females during IGT blocks 19 and 20?
We conducted two path analyses by using exclusively the sample of females to examine whether P3b amplitudes mediated the beneficial effect of humor on IGT performance among females in the Hg compared with females in the NHg during blocks 19 and 20. Humor was treated as the independent variable in both analyses, with IGT performance during blocks 19 and 20 (trials 451–500) as the dependent variable. In the first analysis, we considered the mean amplitude of three electrodes (FP1, AF7, and P9) assessing the P3b component during trials 301–400 in response to reward as the mediator, because we had previously found differences in P3b amplitudes between females in the Hg and females in the NHg in these electrodes and trials. In the second analysis, the mean amplitude of 14 electrodes (FP1, AF7, FT7, C1, C5, T7, CP1, CP3, TP7, P3, P5, P9, Pz, and P2) in response to punishment was considered as the mediator.
Results for the first path analysis indicated that the indirect effect of humor on IGT performance through P3b amplitudes in response to rewards was not statistically significant (IE = − 0.06; SE = 0.12; 95% CI = − 0.3, 0.06). Therefore, P3b amplitudes during rewards did not mediate the observed beneficial effect of humor during IGT blocks 19–20, in females. Similarly, results from the second path analysis did not reveal a statistically significant indirect effect of humor on IGT performance in response to punishment (IE = 0.13; SE = 0.21; 95% CI = − 0.24, 0.46). Consequently, P3b amplitudes during punishments did not mediate the observed beneficial effect of humor during blocks 19–20 in females.
Discussion
The fact that humor improved IGT performance in females using an extended version (500 trials) of the IGT is in line with the results of our previous study (Flores-Torres et al., 2019), in which we used the standard version (100 trials) of the IGT. Our results also are in line with the findings of our other study (Flores-Torres et al., 2022), where we found that cognitive reappraisal—an emotion regulation strategy that increases positive emotion—also improved IGT decision-making in females. The present study extends our findings by showing that humor not only improved decision-making under risk in females but also in males increasing their cognitive control, specifically their attention allocation toward the task. However, the increase in cognitive control did not mediate the effect of humor on female’s IGT decision performance. We previously found Flores-Torres et al. (2022) that—in females—the influence of positive cognitive reappraisal on IGT decision-making was mediated by a reduction in negative emotions. Both a decrease in negative and/or an increase in positive emotions may be good alternative candidate mediators for the effect of humor on IGT performance. Unfortunately, we did not assess emotions in the present research. Future studies considering emotions as potential mediators of this relationship are needed.
The fact that humor had a positive effect during decision-making under risk (specifically at blocks 19 and 20) also in males’ contrasts with our previous findings. In our previous study, we found that humor impaired IGT decision making during the last block of the task. The discrepancy between the current and previous studies may be attributed to the fact that in the previous one, participants conducted fewer trials (100 trials); therefore, we were not able to observe the beneficial effect of humor in males. We were only able to observe the early distracting and impairing effect of humor during decision-making under ambiguity, as the males in the humor group did not learn to perform the task, and as a result, they never took decisions under risk. We considered that they could have learned the task (and consequently, performed decisions under risk) if they would have been provided with more trials, as in the extended version. In fact, in the current study, with more trials available (500 trials) humor was beneficial for decision-making under risk among males.
In the present study, we also found a detrimental effect of humor among men during decision making under ambiguity (specifically at block six), similar to the one above described found in the previous study with the standard IGT version (Flores-Torres et al., 2019). Specifically, during trials 126 to 150, males exposed to humor exhibited poorer performance than males not exposed to humor. This detrimental effect was not found among females. Furthermore, we found a significant reduction in task monitoring during rewards that mediated the detrimental impact of humor among males during trials 126–150. This reduction in monitoring serves as the mechanism through which humor decreases IGT decision-making during ambiguity. Hence, our findings evidenced our initial assumption that humor would demand greater cognitive and attentional resources among males than among females; however, it is essential to note that this negative effect is temporary.
Unexpectedly, humor increased males’ attention allocation toward the task (P3b amplitude); nonetheless, these increases in attention allocation did not mediate the beneficial effect of humor on their IGT performance under risk. In fact, the positive influence of humor on performance in men contrasts with the result of Flores-Torres et al. (2022), where we observed that another emotion-regulation strategy (i.e., cognitive reappraisal) impaired IGT decision-making for males. The differences between humor and reappraisal suggests that humor may not predominantly operate as a cognitive process in males (as in reappraisal) and opens the possibility of a beneficial effect of humor driven by increased motivation to tolerate fatigue (Polich, 2007; Varazzani et al., 2015). However, this hypothesis remains to be tested. Previous studies have indicated that the amplitude of P3b could be associated with an increase in norepinephrine and dopamine activity (Polich, 2007), which may in turn promote attention and memory retrieval, and decrease fatigue, which may serve to maintain the focus of attention on a task despite tiredness or boredom (Varazzani et al., 2015).
We also observed a more intense and highly distributed effect of humor in P3b component among females. The increased amplitude observed, especially during punishments compared with females in the nonhumor group, also was lateralized such that left electrodes (from frontal to parietal sites) showed a strong activation. These findings are in line with Cunningham et al. (2005) and Cui et al. (2013), who suggest that according to the emotional asymmetry hypothesis, positive emotion is evoked more strongly in the left hemisphere, which also is more sensitive to reward learning and positive situations, which are reflected in an increase in left lateralized amplitude in P3b activity. Our findings suggest that females in the humor group, during trials 350–400, may have experienced more positive emotions as a result of humor induction, compared with females in the nonhumor condition, who were not exposed to humor. Therefore, during punishment feedback, they successfully signaled their choices and accordingly, changed their strategy to improve their performance. Additionally, during humor, females had larger P3b amplitudes than males; specifically, they showed an increase in bilateral parietal sites, which reflects comparatively better attention and working memory than males (Horowitz-Kraus, 2014).
The present study has some limitations. We did not measure subjective positive and negative emotions during the IGT. Future studies should consider including self-report measures, such as the PANAS to assess affect. Additionally, our study only considered sex as a binary variable, and it is recommended that future studies measure both—sex assigned at birth and gender identity—and incorporate a broader range of gender identities. It also is important to control for the potential influence of task-switching costs, because this variable may interact with sex. Some of our analyses may have lacked sufficient statistical power owing to the limited number of participants and conditions. For example, contrary to our expectations, we did not find a significant effect of humor in blocks 17 and 18, although we observed trend-level findings. It is imperative for future studies to replicate these analyses with a larger sample size. Finally, our sample comprised only undergraduate college students from several universities in Chile and may not generalize to other populations. Despite these limitations, our study has several important strengths. It partially replicates a previous study on the impact of humor on the IGT and provides insights into the cognitive and electrophysiological mechanisms underlying sex-specific effects. Importantly, this is the first study to identify a mechanism explaining that males exposed to humor at the beginning of the task experience a decrease in their monitoring ability, especially after reward feedback, which impaired their learning. Unlike previous studies focusing solely on emotional effects, our research considers both sex and humor, offering a more comprehensive understanding of decision-making. Furthermore, we employed rigorous statistical corrections, ensuring the reliability and validity of our behavioral and EEG findings.
Conclusions
Humor benefits decision-making under risk in both females and males (specifically at blocks 19 and 20) and attention allocation for most IGT blocks (P3b amplitudes). However, humor impaired men’s IGT decision-making under ambiguity during the block six and task monitoring (FRN amplitudes) for most IGT blocks. Attention allocation increases did not mediate the beneficial effect of humor on decision-making under risk neither among females nor males. Nonetheless, task monitoring decrements fully mediated the humor's detrimental influence on men's decision-making under ambiguity during block six.
Supplementary Information
Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.
Acknowledgements
The authors thank Eugenio Rodríguez, Vladimir López, Paulo Barraza, Iván Rubio, and Claudio Artigas for supporting the execution of the study.
Authors' Contributions
Jorge Flores-Torres: Conceptualization, Investigation, Methodology, Data Collection, Data Curation, Data Analysis, Writing – Original Draft, Review & Editing, and Data Visualization.
Kateri McRae: Writing—Review & Editing.
German Campos: Data analysis Advisory.
Lydia Gómez-Pérez: Data Analysis Advisory and Writing—Review & Editing, Visualization.
Each author has participated sufficiently in the manuscript to take public responsibility for the content. All authors have reviewed and approved the final manuscript.
Funding
Funding for open access publishing: Universidad de Málaga/CBUA. This research received support from the Agencia Nacional de Investigación y Desarrollo (ANID, Chile) through the Doctorado Nacional Grant 21140098 (design and experiment) and the Fondecyt Postdoc Grant 3220391 (behavioral and EEG analyses and manuscript writing). The sponsor had no role in the study design, collection, analyses, and interpretation of data, writing of the manuscript, nor in the article submission for publication.
Declarations
Conflicts of interest/Competing interests
The authors declare no conflicts of interest concerning research, authorship, and publication of this manuscript. The authors are committed to maintaining objectivity and transparency in their research process and reporting.
Ethics Approval
The Ethics Committee of the Pontificia Universidad Católica de Chile (PUC) approved the study. We conducted all the experiments at the Neuro-dynamic Laboratory of the School of Psychology of the PUC.
Consent to Participate
All study participants provided informed consent before their involvement in the research. We provided participants with a detailed explanation of the purpose of the study, procedures, potential risks, and benefits. We informed them that their participation was voluntary and that they had the right to withdraw from the study at any moment without facing any consequences. The Pontificia Universidad Católica de Chile Ethics Committee reviewed and approved the research protocol and consent forms, ensuring compliance with ethical standards for human research.
Consent for Publication
All individuals who contributed to this research, either as participants or co-authors, have provided explicit consent to the study finding publication. We informed participants that their anonymized data may appear in publications related to the research but without revealing any personally identifiable information. Co-authors, including those affiliated with the Pontificia Universidad Católica de Chile and the University of Denver have reviewed and agreed upon the final version of the manuscript and consented to its submission for publication. All involved parties are aware of and endorse the dissemination of the research outcomes. The Pontificia Universidad Católica de Chile Ethics Committee has overseen the publication process, ensuring adherence to ethical standards and data protection guidelines.
Data Availability
The datasets used in this study are available upon reasonable request. Researchers interested in accessing the data for replication, validation, or further investigation should contact the following email address of the corresponding author: lydiagomez@uma.es. We will share datasets in compliance with ethical standards, confidentiality requirements, and data protection regulations. The goal is to facilitate transparency, reproducibility, and the advancement of scientific knowledge. We will evaluate requests for data access to ensure that they align with the original research purpose and adhere to relevant ethical and legal considerations.
Code availability
Not applicable
Footnotes
There is no consensus in the exact timing where this process begins and ends (Bechara 1997, 2000; Dunn et al., 2006; Schiebener et al., 2011).
In the present manuscript, we use the word “sex” as our measure included the options of male and female. However, we did not measure sex and gender separately. When discussing previous findings, we will use the terms used by authors of those papers. We acknowledge that in many cases, females identify as women and males identify as men, but that previous research (as well as our own present measure) excludes some experiences of gender.
Publisher's Note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Open Practices Statements
Data or materials for the experiments are available upon request, and none of the experiments was preregistered.
References
- Azim, E., Mobbs, D., Jo, B., Menon, V., & Reiss, A. L. (2005). Gender differences in brain activation elicited by humor. PNAS,102, 16496–16501. 10.1073/pnas.0408456102 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Battersby, M., Tolchard, B., Scurrah, M., & Thomas, L. (2006). Suicide ideation and behaviour in people with pathological gambling attending a treatment service. International Journal of Mental Health,4, 233–246. 10.1007/s11469-006-9022-z [Google Scholar]
- Beaty, R., & Silvia, P. (2012). Why do ideas get more creative across time? An executive interpretation of the serial order effect in divergent thinking tasks. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts,6(4), 309. 10.1037/a0029171 [Google Scholar]
- Bechara, A. (2005). Decision making, impulse control and loss of willpower to resist drugs: A neurocognitive perspective. Nature Neuroscience,8, 1458. 10.1038/nn1584 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bechara, A. (2007). Iowa gambling task professional manual. Psychological Assessment Resources. [Google Scholar]
- Bechara, A., Damasio, A. R., Damasio, H., & Anderson, S. W. (1994). Insensitivity to future consequences following damage to the human prefrontal cortex. Cognition,50, 7–15. 10.1016/0010-0277(94)90018-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bechara, A., Damasio, H., Tranel, D., & Damasio, A. R. (1997). Deciding advantageously before knowing the advantageous strategy. Science,275(5304), 1293–1295. 10.1126/science.275.5304.1293 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bechara, A. Tranel., Damasio, D., & A,. (2000). Characterization of the decision-making deficit of patients with ventromedial prefrontal cortex lesions. Brain,123(11), 2189–2202. 10.1093/brain/123.11.2189 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bianchin, M., & Angrilli, A. (2011). Decision preceding negativity in the Iowa gambling task: An ERP study. Brain and Cognition,75(3), 273–280. 10.1016/j.physbeh.2011.05.019 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Blair, R., & Karniski, W. (1993). An alternative method for significance testing of waveform difference potentials. Psychophysiology,30(5), 518–524. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bolla, K., Eldreth, D., Matochik, J., & Cadet, J. (2004). Gender related differences in a gambling task and its neurological correlates. Cerebral Cortex,14, 1226–1232. 10.1093/cercor/bhh083 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Boudewyn, M. A., Luck, S. J., Farrens, J. L., & Kappenman, E. S. (2018). How many trials does it take to get a significant ERP effect? It Depends. Psychophysiology,55(6), e13049. 10.1111/psyp.13049 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bowman, C. H., Evans, C. E., & Turnbull, O. H. (2005). Artificial time constraints on the Iowa Gambling Task: The effects on behavioural performance and subjective experience. Brain and Cognition,57(1), 21–25. 10.1016/j.bandc.2004.08.015 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brevers, D., Bechara, A., Cleeremans, A., & Noël, X. (2013). Iowa Gambling Task (IGT): Twenty years after–gambling disorder and IGT. Frontiers in Psychology,4, 61282. 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00665 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chandrakumar, D., Feuerriegel, D., Bode, S., Grech, M., & Keage, H. A. (2018). Event-related potentials in relation to risk-taking: a systematic review. Frontiers in behavioral neuroscience, 111. 10.3389/fnbeh.2018.00111 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- Cohen, M., Elger, C., & Ranganath, C. (2007). Reward expectation modulate feedback-related negativity and EEG spectra. NeuroImage,35(2), 968–978. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.11.056 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cramer, A., van Ravenzwaaij, D., Matzke, D., Steingroever, H., & Wetzels, R. Grasman, R. P. … Wagenmakers, E. J. (2016). Hidden multiplicity in exploratory multiway ANOVA: Prevalence and remedies. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 23(2), 640–647. 10.3758/s13423-015-0913-5 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- Cui, J., Chen, Y., Wang, Y., Shum, D., & Chan, R. (2013). Neural correlates of uncertain decision making: ERP evidence from the Iowa Gambling Task. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience,7, 776. 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00776 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cunningham, W., Espinet, S., Deyoung, C., & Zelazo, P. (2005). Attitudes to the right-and left frontal ERP asymmetries associated with stimulus valence and processing goals. NeuroImage,28, 827–834. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.04.044 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dalgleish, T., Yiend, J., Bramham, J., Teasdale, J. D., Ogilvie, A. D., Malhi, G., et al. (2004). Neuropsychological processing associated with recovery from depression after stereotactic subcaudate tractotomy. American Journal of Psychiatry,161, 1913–1916. 10.1176/ajp.161.10.1913 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Delorme, A., & Makeig, S. (2004). EEGLAB: An open-source toolbox for analysis of single-trial EEG dynamics including independent component analysis. Journal of Neuroscience Methods,134(1), 9–21. 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2003.10.009 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dunn, B. D., Dalgleish, T., & Lawrence, A. D. (2006). The somatic marker hypothesis: A critical evaluation. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews,30(2), 239–271. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fernie, G., & Tunney, R. J. (2013). Learning on the IGT follows emergence of knowledge but not differential somatic activity. Frontiers in Psychology,4, 61285. 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00687 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Field, A. (2013). Discovering statistics using IBM SPSS statistics. Sage. [Google Scholar]
- Fischer, M., Heim, D., Janiesch, C., & Winkelmann, A. (2017). Assessing process fit in ERP implementation projects: A methodological approach. In Designing the Digital Transformation: 12th International Conference, DESRIST 2017, Karlsruhe, Germany, May 30–June 1, 2017, Proceedings 12 (pp. 3–20). Springer International Publishing.
- Flores-Torres, J., Gómez Pérez, L., McRae, K., López, V., & Rubio., Rodríguez, E. F. (2019). Humor improves women’s but impairs men’s Iowa Gambling Task performance. Frontiers in Psychology, 10, 2538. 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.02538 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- Flores-Torres, J., Gómez-Pérez, L., & McRae, K. (2022). Positive cognitive reappraisal is beneficial for women’s but not for men’s IGT decision-making. Motivation and Emotion,46(3), 350–365. 10.1007/s11031-022-09927-4 [Google Scholar]
- Garrido-Chaves, R., Perez-Alarcón, M., Perez, V., Hidalgo, V., Pulopulos, M. M., & Salvador, A. (2021). FRN and P3 during the Iowa gambling task: The importance of gender. Psychophysiology,58(3), e13734. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Giustiniani, J., Joucla, C., Bennabi, D., Nicolier, M., Chabin, T., Masse, C. … Gabriel, D. (2019). Behavioral and electrophysiological arguments in favor of a relationship between impulsivity, risk-taking, and success on the Iowa gambling task. Brain sciences, 9(10), 248. 10.3390/brainsci9100248 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- Groppe, D., Urbach, T., & Kutas, M. (2011a). Mass univariate analysis of event related brain potentials/fields I: A critical tutorial review. Psychophysiology,48(12), 1711–1725. 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2011.01273.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Groppe, D., Urbach, T., & Kutas, M. (2011b). Mass univariate analysis of event related brain potentials/fields II: Simulation studies. Psychophysiology,48(12), 1726–1737. 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2011.01272.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Harding, T. W., De Arango, V., Baltazar, J., Climent, C. E., Ibrahim, H. H. A., Ladrido-Ignacio, L., et al. (1980). Mental disorders in primary health care: A study of their frequency and diagnosis in four developing countries. Psychological Medicine,10, 231–241. 10.1017/S0033291700043993 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Holroyd, C., Yeung, N., Coles, M. G., & Cohen, J. (2005). A mechanism for error detection in speeded response time tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General,134(2), 163. 10.1037/0096-3445.134.2.163 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hooper, C., Luciana, M., Conklin, H. M., & Yarger, R. S. (2004). Adolescents’ performance on the Iowa gambling task: Implications for the development of decision making and ventromedial prefrontal cortex. Developmental Psychology,40, 1148–1158. 10.1037/0012-1649.40.6.1148 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Horowitz-Kraus, T. (2014). Pinpointing the deficit in executive functions in adolescents with dyslexia performing the Wisconsin card sorting test: an ERP study. Journal of Learning Disabilities,47(3), 208–223. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kahneman, D. (1973). Attention and effort (Vol. 1063, pp. 218–226). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
- Kohn, N., Kellermann, T., Gur, R., Schneider, F., & Habel, U. (2011). Gender differences in the neural correlates of humor processing: Implications for different processing modes. Neuropsychologia,49(5), 888–897. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kok, A. (2001). On the utility of P3 amplitude as a measure of processing capacity. Psychophysiology,38(3), 557–577. 10.1017/S0048577201990559 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lee, S., Lee, H., Kweon, Y., Lee, C., & Lee, K. (2009). The impact of executive function on emotion recognition and emotion experience in patients with schizophrenia. Psychiatry Investigation,6(3), 156. 10.4306/pi.2009.6.3.156 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kovács, I., Richman, M. J., Janka, Z., Maraz, A., & Andó, B. (2017). Decision making measured by the Iowa Gambling Task in alcohol use disorder and gambling disorder: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Drug and Alcohol Dependence,181, 152–161. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lopez-Calderon, J., & Luck, S. J. (2014). ERPLAB: An open-source toolbox for the analysis of event-related potentials. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience,8, 213. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Luck, S. (2014). An introduction to the event-related potential technique. MIT press.
- Mäki-Marttunen, V., Hagen, T., & Espeseth, T. (2019). Proactive and reactive modes of cognitive control can operate independently and simultaneously. Acta Psychologica,199, 102891. 10.1016/j.actpsy.2019.102891 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Martin, R. (2007). The psychology of humor: An integrative approach. Elsevier Academic Press. [Google Scholar]
- Matlab, V. (2014). 8.3. 0.532 (R2014a). Natick, Massachusetts.
- McRae, K., Ochsner, K. N., Mauss, I. B., Gabrieli, J. J., & Gross, J. J. (2008). Gender differences in emotion regulation: An fMRI study of cognitive reappraisal. Group Processes & Intergroup Relations,11(2), 143–162. 10.1177/1368430207088 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Miller, E. (2000). The prefontral cortex and cognitive control. Nature Reviews Neuroscience,1(1), 59–65. 10.1038/35036228 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Miu, A., Heilman, R., & Houser, D. (2008). Anxiety impairs decision-making: Psychophysiological evidence from an Iowa gambling task. Biological Psychology,77, 353–358. 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2007.11.010 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Muthén, B., & Muthén, L. (2017). Mplus. In Handbook of item response theory (pp. 507–518). Chapman and Hall/CRC.
- O’Connell, L., & Hofmann, H. (2012). Evolution of a vertebrate social decision-making network. Science,336(6085), 1154–1157. 10.1126/science.12188 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Overman, W., Boettcher, L., Watterson, L., & Walsh, K. (2011). Effects of dilemmas and aromas on performance of the Iowa gambling task. Behavioral Brain Research,218, 64–72. 10.1016/j.bbr.2010.11.015 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Overman, W., Graham, L., Redmond, A., Eubank, R., Boettcher, L., Samplawski, O., et al. (2006). Contemplation of moral dilemmas eliminates gender differences on the Iowa gambling task. Behavioral Neuroscience,120, 817–825. 10.1037/0735-7044.120.4.817 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Polich, J. (2007). Updating P300: An integrative theory of P3a and P3b. Clinical Neurophysiology,118(10), 2128–2148. 10.1016/j.clinph.2007.04.019 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Reavis, R., & Overman, W. (2001). Adult gender differences on a decision-making task previously shown to depend on the orbital prefrontal cortex. Behavioral Neuroscience,115, 196–206. 10.1037/0735-7044.115.1.196 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schiebener, J., Zamarian, L., Delazer, M., & Brand, M. (2011). Executive functions categorization of probabilities and learning from feedback: What does really matter for decision making under explicit risk conditions? Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology,33(9), 1025–1039. 10.1080/13803395.2011.595702 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Spielberger, C. (1970). “STAI manual for the state-trait anxiety inventory” in Self evaluation questionnaire (Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press).
- Steinhauser, M., & Yeung, N. (2010). Decision processes in human performance monitoring. Journal of Neuroscience,30(46), 15643–15653. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1899-10.2010 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Suhr, J. A., & Tsanadis, J. (2007). Affect and personality correlates of the Iowa Gambling Task. Personality and Individual Differences, 43, 27–36 Cambridge (MA): The MIT Press.
- Tanner, D., Norton, J., Morgan-Short, K., & Luck, S. (2016). On high-pass filter artifacts (they’re real) and baseline correction (it’s a good idea) in ERP/ERMF analysis. Journal of Neuroscience Methods,266, 166–170. 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2016.01.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Turetsky, B. I., Bilker, W. B., Siegel, S. J., Kohler, C. G., & Gur, R. E. (2009). Profile of auditory information-processing deficits in schizophrenia. Psychiatry Research,165(1–2), 27–37. 10.1016/j.psychres.2008.04.013 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ullsperger, M., Harsay, H. A., Wessel, J., & Ridderinkhof, K. (2010). Conscious perception of errors and its relation to the anterior insula. Brain Structure and Function,214(5), 629–643. 10.1007/s00429-010-0261-1 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- van den Bos, R., De Visser, L., Van de Loo, A., Mets, M., Van Willigenburg, G. M., Homberg, J. R., & Verster, J. C. (2012). Sex differences in decision-making in adult normal volunteers are related to differences in the interaction of emotion and cognitive control. In K. O. Moore & N. P. Gonzalez (Eds.), Handbook on Psychology of Decision- Making (pp. 179–198). Nova Science Publisher Inc. [Google Scholar]
- Van Hasselt, F., De Visser, L., Tieskens, J., Cornelisse, S., Baars, A., Lavrijsen, M., et al. (2012). Individual variations in maternal care early in life correlate with later life decision-making and c-fos expression in prefrontal subregions of rats. PLoS ONE,7, e37820. 10.1371/journal.pone.0037820 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Varazzani, C., San-Galli, A., Gilardeau, S., & Bouret, S. (2015). Noradrenaline and dopamine neurons in the reward/effort trade-off: A direct electrophysiological comparison in behaving monkeys. Journal of Neuroscience,35(20), 7866–7877. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vera-Villarroel, P., Celis-Atenas, K., Córdova-Rubio, N., Buela-Casal, G., and Spielberger, C. D. (2007). Preliminary analysis and normative data of the state-trait anxiety inventory (STAI) in adolescents and adults of Santiago, Chile. Ter. Psicol. 25. 10.4067/S0718-48082007000200006
- Vielma, M., Vicente, B., Rioseco, P., Medina, E., Escobar, B., Saldivia, S., et al. (1994). Validación del Autorreportaje de Síntomas (SRQ) como instrumento de screening en estudios comunitarios. Revista De Psiquiatría,11, 180–185. [Google Scholar]
- Weller, J., Levin, I., & Bechara, A. (2010). Do individual differences in Iowa gambling task performance predicts adaptive decision making for risky gains and losses? Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychololy,32, 141–150. 10.1080/13803390902881926 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wetzels, R., Vandekerckhove, J., Tuerlinckx, F., & Wagenmakers, E. J. (2010). Bayesian parameter estimation in the expectancy valence model of the Iowa gambling task. Journal of Mathematical Psychology,54, 14–27. 10.1016/j.jmp.2008.12.001 [Google Scholar]
- Zabelina, D., & Ganis, G. (2018). Creativity and cognitive control: Behavioral and ERP evidence that divergent thinking, but not real-life creative achievement, relates to better cognitive control. Neuropsychologia,118, 20–28. 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2018.02.014 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
The datasets used in this study are available upon reasonable request. Researchers interested in accessing the data for replication, validation, or further investigation should contact the following email address of the corresponding author: lydiagomez@uma.es. We will share datasets in compliance with ethical standards, confidentiality requirements, and data protection regulations. The goal is to facilitate transparency, reproducibility, and the advancement of scientific knowledge. We will evaluate requests for data access to ensure that they align with the original research purpose and adhere to relevant ethical and legal considerations.