Skip to main content
PLOS ONE logoLink to PLOS ONE
. 2024 Nov 21;19(11):e0309789. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0309789

Perception, regulation, and effects on longevity of pollen fatty acids in the honey bee, Apis mellifera

Marielle C Schleifer 1,2, Fabian A Ruedenauer 2, Johanna Ziegler 1, Sara D Leonhardt 2,, Johannes Spaethe 1,‡,*
Editor: Olav Rueppell3
PMCID: PMC11581215  PMID: 39570936

Abstract

For successful cross-pollination, most flowering plants rely on insects as pollinators and attract them by offering rewards, predominantly nectar and pollen. Bees–a highly important pollinator group—are especially dependent on pollen as their main source of essential nutrients, including proteins, lipids, and sterols. Fatty acids (FAs) in particular play a pivotal role as fundamental energy source, contributing to membrane structure integrity, cellular homeostasis, and cognitive processes. However, overconsumption of FAs can have detrimental effects on fitness and survival. Thus, bees need to precisely modulate FA intake. To better understand how Apis mellifera, the globally predominant managed pollinator, regulate FA intake, we conducted controlled feeding experiments with newly hatched honey bee workers by providing pollen with different FA concentrations. We additionally investigated the honey bee’s capacity to perceive individual FAs by means of chemotactile proboscis extension response (PER) conditioning. We tested both natural concentrations and concentrations exceeding those typically found in pollen. Given the dose-dependent importance of FAs observed in other bee species, we hypothesized that (i) a high FA concentration in pollen would reduce honey bee longevity, and (ii) honey bees are able to perceive individual FAs and differentiate between different FA concentrations via antennal sensation prior to consumption. Our study revealed that elevated FA concentrations in pollen resulted in reduced consumption rates and increased mortality in Apis mellifera. Workers can detect and discriminate between saturated and unsaturated FAs utilizing their antennae. Moreover, they were able to distinguish between individual FAs and also between different concentrations of the same FAs. Our results suggest a high sensitivity of A. mellifera towards both the concentration and composition of individual FAs, which greatly impacts their foraging decisions and fitness. These insights contribute to the growing evidence highlighting the importance of balanced nutrient ratios, in particular of FAs, for bees and other organisms.

Introduction

All animals, including humans, need to balance their intake of macronutrients to optimize their health and fitness [1]. However, not all resources provide an adequate nutrient balance due to variation in nutritional compositions. Animals consuming non-complementary, nutritionally imbalanced diets must therefore reach a suitable compromise between overconsumption of certain nutrients and underconsumption of others [2, 3].

Florivores, like bees, rely exclusively on flowering plants as their food source. While adult bees primarily consume nectar as their source of energy (carbohydrates), they also collect pollen, especially for feeding their larvae [46]. Pollen provides them with all other needed nutrients, i.e. proteins (in the form of amino acids (AA)), lipids (comprising fatty acids (FA) and sterols), minerals, vitamins and various plant secondary metabolites (PSMs, e.g. nicotine or caffeine) [7]. However, pollen nutrient content (often referred to as nutritional quality) can significantly vary within and among different plant species [812]. For example, protein to lipid (P:L) ratio in honey bee collected pollen can range from 1.3 to 13.1 [1315]. Differences in pollen nutrient composition have been demonstrated to strongly impact bee physiology [16, 17]. Especially under challenging environmental conditions and when facing stressors, such as insecticides, low P:L ratios appear to support bee health [13]. These findings underscore the importance of nutritionally appropriate diets for bee health and fitness and point to a prominent role of lipids, such as FAs.

There are four distinct categories of FAs: i) saturated FA (SFAs), ii) mono-unsaturated FAs (MUFAs) or omega-9 poly-unsaturated FAs (PUFAs), iii) omega-6 PUFAs and iv) omega-3 PUFAs. Among these, only two types are essential nutrients for many animals: omega-6 PUFAs (e.g., linoleic acid, primarily found in seeds) and omega-3 PUFAs (e.g., linolenic acid, primarily found in green leaves). They are essential, because they cannot be synthesized by animals and cannot be converted into one another [18]. Honey bee physiology (i.e., brood development, adult longevity and body FA composition) is notably affected by dietary lipid concentrations and omega-6:3 ratios: Comparatively higher concentrations of 8% of total dietary lipids were found to increase brood production, whereas high omega-6:3 ratios increased mortality and diminished brood rearing [19]. Similarly, in bumble bees, diets high in FAs also decreased reproductive fitness and survival [20]. Moreover, a balanced omega-6:3 FA ratio supported the cognitive capabilities of young nurse bees [21, 22], whereas an omega-3 deficiency was linked to impaired learning [23]. Additionally, omega-3 deficiency resulted in smaller hypopharyngeal glands, which are responsible for the production of royal jelly [23]. On top, diets high in FAs were found to modulate the composition of gut microbiota in honey bees and cause overweight [24]. Beyond their nutritional and physiological roles, some FAs possess antimicrobial properties that can significantly impact honey bee health. In particular, FAs with up to 14 carbon atoms and/or with one or more double bounds show antimicrobial activity [25], which can protect bees against pathogenic microorganisms.

Consequently, optimal bee development, health and cognitive functioning appear to require a minimum absolute quantity of essential FAs alongside a balanced omega-6:3 ratio [26]. Overall, omega-6 PUFAs and omega-3 PUFAs are only needed in very small quantities [18]. Their main role is as constitutes of membrane lipids, with species-specific membrane FA compositions [18]. In fact, the omega-6:3 ratios observed in pollen mixtures collected by bees show less variability (ranging from 0.3 to 0.9) compared to hand-collected pollen sourced from 28 distinct plant species, where ratios ranged from 0.09 to 5.34 [23], which indicates that honey bees regulate both the quantity and ratio of FA intake.

Such a nutrient-sensitive regulation can only be achieved when the bees are able to perceive specific nutrients. Indeed, both honey bees and bumble bees were found to discriminate between pollen differing in nutrient content by means of their antennae prior to consumption [27, 28]. Utilizing their antennae as sensory organs, these bees employ chemotactile sensation, also referred to as taste, to evaluate pollen quality prior to ingestion [27]. This pre-ingestive assessment is crucial, given that most bee species, with the exception of a few Hylaeus spp. [29], transport collected pollen for larval provisioning externally on their bodies, e.g. on their corbiculae or scopae [30], rather than consuming it themselves. Ruedenauer [20] additionally showed that bumble bees can differentiate between pollen enriched with single FAs, indicating a fine-tuned perception for FAs. Interestingly, while bumble bees could detect single FAs, they were unable to distinguish between pollen samples with varying AA concentrations [20]. Likewise, bumble bees could perceive sterols when presented in isolation (single sterol solved in chloroform), but not when presented in varying concentrations in pollen [31]. These findings suggest that FAs play a particularly important role for bees, with direct effects on their foraging behavior, development, survival and cognition.

It remains unclear whether honey bees are also able to perceive different FAs–a prerequisite for pre-ingestive assessment of FA profiles and hence nutrient regulation. Given the diverse nutrient composition of pollen, even within plant species [7, 10], it is unlikely for bees to regulate all pollen nutrients comprehensively, considering the substantial resources such as time and metabolic energy required for reception (e.g., receptors), perception (e.g. neuronal circuits), and behavioral responses (e.g., learning and memory) [32]. Therefore, we postulate that only those nutrients or compound groups are perceived and regulated by bees, that are detrimental to their health and fitness when over- and/or underconsumed, whereas compounds that are not maleficent are not perceived or at least may receive less attention from bees [32].

In this study, we investigated whether honey bees regulate the intake of diets varying in FA content and how high amounts of FAs affected the bees’ longevity by conducting no-choice feeding assays. Furthermore, we examined whether honey bee workers were able to perceive and discriminate different individual FAs (presented in isolation within a solvent) as well as different concentrations of FA mixtures using chemotactile proboscis extension response (PER) conditioning. Based on the known importance of FAs and on findings in bumble bees [20, 28], we hypothesized that (i) honey bees are able to perceive and discriminate different FAs via chemotactile sensation of their antennae (prior to consumption), and that (ii) variation in FA concentrations in pollen affects the bees’ longevity, e.g., higher FA concentrations have a detrimental effect on bee health.

Material and methods

Colonies

The experiments were conducted with workers of the Western honey bee (Apis mellifera carnica) kept in Dadant bee hives at the Biocenter of the University of Würzburg, Germany. The surrounding landscape comprised hedges, gardens, grassland, and orchards, which provided the colonies with a diverse range of different flowering plants [33].

FA analysis

The FA content of the honey bee collected pollen that was used for the feeding experiments was analyzed according a protocol recently established in our lab using gas chromatography-mass spectrometry [34]. For a detailed protocol see S1 Text.

Feeding experiments

The feeding experiments were conducted in June 2022 and lasted for 21 days. A frame of mature brood was taken from a colony and transferred to an emergence box stored in an incubator (33°C, 55% RH, darkness) [19, 21]. Due to a shortage in mature brood frames, we obtained bees from only one colony. On the next day, we collected newly emerged bees (0-24hrs old), since the consumption of pollen by adult bees declines rapidly after the first week [35]. Every bee was exposed to only one out of four different treatments, with each treatment comprising ten replicates. Each replicate consisted of 30 bees placed in a wooden cage (14x8x18cm), resulting in 300 newly emerged bees per treatment. Wooden cages were equipped with wire mesh at the bottom for air ventilation, a removable glass front panel, and two small openings at the top for food provisioning. The caged bees were provided with 50% w/w sucrose solution ad libitum and with one of the following four dietary treatments: pure pollen (as a control), low FA–pollen (same pollen enriched with a FA mix resulting in a 1.5 times higher concentration than the control), medium FA–pollen (fivefold increase of FA concentration) and high FA–pollen (tenfold increase of FA concentration). The diets comprised bee-collected pollen (Naturwaren Niederrhein GmbH, Goch-Asperden, Germany), which was ground into a fine powder using a coffee grinder (Bosch, Germany) and mixtures of FAs. The FA mixture comprised several saturated FAs (arachidic, behenic, capric, lauric, myristic, palmitic, and stearic acid), two poly-unsaturated FAs (PUFAs, linolenic (omega-3, essential) and linoleic (omega-6, essential)) and one mono-unsaturated FA (MUFA, oleic (omega-9) acid, for details, see Table 1). The pollen–FA- mixture was thoroughly homogenized, and the resulting pollen powder was blended 1:1 with deionized water to achieve uniform texture. Fresh diets were prepared daily and offered to bees on plastic weighing dishes (4cm in diameter). Daily pollen consumption was recorded by weighing the dishes before and after exposure to the colonies and averaged per bee. To account for evaporation-induced weight loss, a control weighing dish containing pollen paste was placed in an empty wooden cage. Sugar water was replaced daily with freshly prepared solution. All procedures were conducted under red light conditions to minimize disturbance. Dead bees were counted and removed without replacement. Bees tested in the feeding experiment were not used for the PER experiments.

Table 1. Fatty acid (FA) concentrations.

Total FA concentrations [mg/g] of honey bee collected pollen (i.e., pure pollen and the same pollen enriched with the 1.5 fold, 5 fold and 10 fold FA concentration as found in pure pollen), percentage of total FAs in pollen and company from which the FAs were obtained.

FA mg/g pollen Pollen pure x1.5 FA x5 FA x10 FA % of total FA content Source company
Capric 0.58 0.87 2.90 5.79 2% Carl Roth, Karlsruhe, Germany
Lauric 0.70 1.06 3.52 7.04 2% Sigma-Aldrich, Saint Louis, MO
Myristic 2.68 4.02 13.40 26.79 8% TCI-Europe, Zwijndrecht, Belgium
Palmitic 18.05 27.08 90.27 180.54 56% Sigma-Aldrich, Saint Louis, MO
Stearic 6.09 9.14 30.46 60.92 19% Sigma-Aldrich, Saint Louis, MO
Arachidic 0.55 0.82 2.74 5.49 2% Sigma-Aldrich, Saint Louis, MO
Behenic 0.86 1.29 4.31 8.61 3% Fluka AG (Honeywell), Morristown,NJ
Oleic + Linolenic 1.84 2.76 9.20 18.41 6% Sigma-Aldrich, Saint Louis, MO
Linoleic 0.81 1.22 4.07 8.14 3% Sigma-Aldrich, Saint Louis, MO
Total FA content 32.17 48.26 160.87 321.73

PER experiments

The PER experiments were conducted from April until September in 2022 and 2023 with foragers sourced from ten different unrelated honey bee colonies. On sunny and warm days, five leaving foragers were randomly collected from each of three hives (15 specimens in total per day) and stored in separate containers. Since the cognitive abilities of honey bees are primarily affected by their social role in the colony rather than their chronological age [3639], we did not control for the foragers’ age. The containers were put on ice for ten minutes to temporally immobilize the bees. The individuals were then transferred into metal tubes and fixed with a 1mm wide adhesive tape placed directly behind the head of the bee. Another tape was wrapped around the tube and the body to prevent further movement. Harnessed like this, the bees could only move their antennae and extend their proboscis. Five minutes after harnessing, the bees were fed with 30% w/w sucrose solution and then starved for three hours in complete darkness. Prior to the experiment, the motivational state of each bee was tested by touching its antenna with a toothpick soaked in 30% w/w sucrose solution. Only bees that showed a proper proboscis extension response (PER) were used for the experiment.

Experimental procedure

To investigate whether bees can detect and discriminate FAs, we used the differential chemotactile proboscis extension response (PER) conditioning approach [27]. It is based on the principles of classical conditioning [40], which had been adapted for bees by Bitterman et al. [41]. Honey bees (as several other bee species) show an innate response towards sucrose solution, i.e. they extend their proboscis upon contact of their antennae, tarsi, or parts of their mouthparts with a sugar solution (= unconditioned stimulus, US). Through pairing this unconditioned stimulus with a conditioned stimulus (= CS), such as an olfactory, visual, or chemical cue, previously not leading to a PER, the bee can learn to associate the CS with the US. Following repeated training bouts, the bee can learn to elicit a PER solely towards the CS—provided it perceives the CS. To test if the bees can differentiate between two stimuli (e.g., two different FAs), only one stimulus is paired with sucrose solution (= rewarded CS, CS+), while the other remains unrewarded (= unrewarded CS, CS-). If the bee can differentiate between the two stimuli, it will, following a training period, extend its proboscis only to the rewarded stimulus (CS+).

Each day, eight new bees were tested individually and for one stimulus pairing only. The tested bee was placed in a forward-shifted rack for easy access to the antennae. It was then allowed to rest for 15 seconds. Next, the CS was presented using a small filter paper soaked with the stimulus solution (e.g. a specific FA in a solvent, see below). The filter paper was placed on a copper plate and moved towards the bee’s right antennae using a micromanipulator. Six seconds after the bee’s initial antennal contact with the filter paper, a toothpick was presented to the other antennae for three more seconds. When a rewarded stimulus (CS+) was presented, the toothpick was soaked in sucrose solution and the bee was allowed to lick from the toothpick. When an unrewarded stimulus (CS-) was presented, the toothpick was plain. After a total of nine seconds of stimulus presentation (six seconds CS alone plus three seconds CS and US combined), the filter paper and the toothpick were removed. The bee was then allowed to rest for another 15 seconds. The copper plates were cleaned in 75% ethanol and thoroughly dried before being reused. A new filter paper was placed on the copper plate for each test phase. Each bee was tested in an eight-minute interval (inter-trial interval, ITI) and ten times each for the rewarding and the unrewarding stimulus (resulting in a total of 20 trials). The sequence of stimulus presentation was pseudorandomized to ensure that no stimulus was presented consecutively more than twice. Each stimulus was used as CS+ as well as CS-. Both CS+ and CS- were used as first trial stimulus in separate experimental rounds. The proboscis extension responses were documented using the software “Timing protocol” v2.0 [42]. A response to the stimulus was scored as “1”, no response to the stimulus was scored as “0”, and no response to sucrose solution was scored as “NA”. Bees failing to respond to the sucrose solution five or more times were excluded from the experiment.

Stimuli

The stimuli used for the experiment comprised FAs dissolved in chloroform. These solutions were prepared on a weekly basis and stored at -20°C until required. We used three unsaturated FAs with 18 carbon atoms each (linolenic acid (omega-3, essential FA), linoleic acid (omega-6, essential FA) and oleic acid (omega-9)) along with two saturated FAs (stearic acid (also 18 carbon atoms) and capric acid (10 carbon atoms)). We selected these five FA, to test if the bees can differentiate between FAs based on the chain length, saturation state or the number of double bounds. The FAs were dissolved individually in chloroform at a ratio of 1:1000. Furthermore, a mixture of ten FAs in chloroform at the concentration of 1:100, 1:1000, and 1:10000 were prepared (see Table 1). The selected concentrations were based on natural FA concentrations found in honey bee collected pollen (see Table 1). Ten minutes before the experiment, 5 μL of the respected stimulus (or pure chloroform as a solvent control) was applied to small filter paper snippets (5 x 5 mm). At least 100 snippets were prepared per stimulus to ensure sufficient amounts for testing, as we used a new snippet for every round and bee. Before the filter papers were presented to the bees, the snippets were thoroughly dried (i.e., the chloroform evaporated completely).

Statistical analysis

Statistical analyses were performed using R version 4.1.2 [43]. Prior to further analysis, normal distribution of all data was assessed using a Shapiro-Wilk test, and homogeneity of variance was assessed using a Levene’s test, both included in the R package ’car’ [44]. The α‐level was set to 5%.

Feeding experiments

The consumption of pollen per individual per day was calculated as follows: The amount of pollen consumed per cage was divided by the number of bees alive. To evaluate the effect of different concentrations of FAs in pollen consumption (per individual day), we used generalized additive models (GAM). Given the pronounced daily (nonlinear) variability in consumption, we opted for an additive modelling approach that applies smoothers to the data over time. Where necessary, data were log-transformed to ensure normal distribution and homogeneity of variances. To more specifically assess differences in mean consumption between different diets during the critical initial seven-day period (when newly emerged honey bees consumed the highest amount of the provided pollen), we used generalized linear models (GLM) and Tukey Honest Significant Differences (TukeyHSD) as post-hoc test. We finally assessed the quantity of FAs consumed per diet, considering variation in the concentration among pollen diets, also using a GLM and TukeyHSD for post-hoc comparison.

To assess differences in the longevity of honey bees exposed to different diets, we used Kaplan-Meier survival statistics to compare median survival times for each diet individually against all others. To account for multiple testing, we adjusted the α-levels using the Bonferroni correction method. The analyses were conducted using the ’survival’ package [45] and the ’KMsurv’ package [46].

PER experiments

First, each stimulus pair was tested against the inverted one using a Wilcoxon rank-sum test with continuity correction to assess whether the type of substance that was rewarded, i.e. used as CS+, influenced the bees’ responses (please note that each substance was used as CS+ as well as CS-). If no significant differences were found among the substances, the data were combined and analyzed together. Subsequently, a paired Wilcoxon signed rank test with continuity correction was used to test whether the bees could discriminate between the rewarded and the unrewarded stimulus (i.e., whether there is a difference in the number of responses towards CS+ and CS-). In instances where combining the data was inappropriate, separate analyses were conducted for each stimulus pair (e.g., omega-3 vs chloroform: trials with omega-3 as CS+ and chloroform as CS+ were tested separately).

Results

Feeding experiments

Freshly emerged honey bee workers showed significantly different consumption patterns between pollen diets varying in FA concentrations (Fig 1). Throughout the entire experimental period (21 days), we observed a higher pollen consumption of the control (pure pollen) and low FA diet compared to the medium and high FA diets (F1,3 = 30.99, P = 0.001, Fig 1). After seven days, overall pollen consumption decreased with minor fluctuations (Fig 1). Therefore, our analysis focused on the first seven days. We found that during this period, bees consumed significantly more pure pollen and low FA diets compared to medium and high FA diets (P<0.001, Fig 2A and S1 Table). Bees also consumed significantly less pollen from the high FA diet than from the medium FA diet (P<0.001, Fig 2A and S1 Table). When analyzing the consumption of total amounts of FAs, bees consumed similar amounts of FAs in the control and low FA diet (P = 0.179), as well as in the medium and high FA diet (P = 0.999, Fig 2B and S1 Table), but significantly less FAs in the medium and high FA diets compared to the pure and low FA diets (P<0.001, Fig 2B and S1 Table). For more information regarding the pollen and FA consumption in the second and third week see Supplementary Material S1, S2 Figs and S1 Table.

Fig 1. Mean consumed food per individual and day of Apis mellifera workers fed with pollen diets differing in FA content (i.e. pure pollen, x1.5, x5 and x10 times the natural concentration) (N = 40).

Fig 1

Each dot represents the average pollen consumption per individual and day for one replicate. Continuous lines represent smoothers calculated by generalized additive models (GAMs) with dashed lines as upper and lower 95% confidence intervals. The different FA concentrations did affect the amount of food consumed per individual in (F1,3 = 30.99, P = 0.001).

Fig 2.

Fig 2

Mean consumption of (A) pollen and (B) FAs per individual and day during the first seven days after Apis mellifera worker bee emergence. Freshly emerged bees were fed with four different pollen diets differing in FA content (i.e. pure honey bee collected pollen as a control and the same pollen enriched with a FA-mix to achieve low FA pollen (1.5 times higher FA concentration), medium FA pollen (5 times higher FA concentration) and high FA pollen (10 times higher FA concentration)). Different letters above boxplots indicate significant differences in the mean consumption of pollen.

The diets also differed significantly in their effect on the longevity of honey bees (Fig 3). The highest survival probability was found in the low FA diet, followed by the control diet and the medium FA diet. The lowest survival probability was observed for the high FA diet. Significant differences were found between the low and medium FA diets (χ2 = 4, P = 0.05), pure and high FA diets (χ2 = 9.9, P = 0.002) and the low and high FA diets (χ2 = 6.1, P = 0.01).

Fig 3. Survival of freshly emerged Apis mellifera workers fed pollen diets differing in FA content (i.e., pure pollen or pollen enriched with x1.5, x5 or x10 the natural concentrations of a FA mixture) over a period of 21 days.

Fig 3

Different letters show significant differences regarding the survival probability for different diets (P = 0.006).

PER experiments

Honey bees were able to perceive most FAs when presented in isolation (FA vs chloroform): omega-3 (linolenic acid), omega-6 (linoleic acid), omega-9 (oleic acid) and capric acid. Stearic acid was the only FA, which was not detected by the bees (Fig 4 and S2 Table).

Fig 4. Percentage of A. mellifera workers that responded with a proboscis extension response (PER) to one out of five FA when individually tested against the solvent control (chloroform).

Fig 4

From left to right and top to bottom: Omega-3, omega-6, omega-9, capric acid and stearic acid. CS+ (black, diamonds) represents the rewarded stimulus and CS- (grey, circles) the unrewarded stimulus. Both FA and solvent were tested as CS+ and CS-. Statistical differences are marked with different letters at the right side of the curves (P < 0.05).

They could also differentiate between different individual FAs, i.e. omega-3 vs omega-6, omega-3 vs omega-9, omega-3 vs capric acid, omega-6 vs omega-9, omega-6 vs stearic acid, omega-6 vs capric acid, omega-9 vs stearic acid, omega-9 vs capric acid, and stearic acid vs capric acid (Fig 5 and S2 Table). The bees also showed a trend to differentiate between omega-3 and stearic acid, but this was not significant (Fig 5 and S2 Table). When capric acid was tested, the type of rewarded stimulus influenced the learning curves (S3 Fig and S2 Table).

Fig 5. Percentage of A. mellifera workers that responded with a PER to one out of ten FAs combinations.

Fig 5

From left to right and top to bottom: Omega-3 vs omega-6, omega-3 vs capric acid, omega-6 vs omega-9, omega-3 vs stearic acid, omega-6 vs capric acid, omega-6 vs stearic acid, omega-9 vs stearic acid, stearic acid vs capric acid and omega-9 vs. capric acid. CS+ (black, diamonds) represents the rewarded stimulus and CS- (grey, circles) the unrewarded stimulus. Both FA and solvent were tested as CS+ and CS-. Statistical differences are marked with different letters at the right side of the curves (P < 0.05).

Furthermore, the bees were able to perceive different concentrations of a FA mixture (1:100 FA mix and 1:1000 FA mix vs chloroform; Fig 6 and S2 Table). Interestingly, the lowest FA concentration (1:10000) was only perceived when the mixture was used as the rewarded stimulus (Fig 6 and S2 Table). Finally, the bees could also discriminate between high and medium FA mix concentrations (Fig 6 and S2 Table).

Fig 6. Percentage of A. mellifera workers that responded with a PER to one out of four different concentrations of aFAs mixture against the solvent control chloroform.

Fig 6

From left to right and top to bottom: 1:100 FA mix vs chloroform, 1:1000 FA mix vs chloroform, 1:10000 FA mix as CS+ vs chloroform as CS- and 1:10000 FA mix as CS- vs chloroform as CS+. The last panel shows 1:100 FA mix vs 1:1000 FA mix. CS+ (black, diamonds) represents the rewarded stimulus and CS- (grey, circles) the unrewarded stimulus. Both FA and solvent were tested as CS+ and CS-. Statistical differences are marked with different letters at the right side of the curves (P < 0.05).

Discussion

Our results show that Apis mellifera workers can perceive FAs and use this ability to regulate FA intake, i.e., they reduce the consumption of pollen enriched with medium to high concentrations of FAs. This led to a reduced survival probability of workers compared to workers feeding on low FA or control diets. Our results agree with findings of Ruedenauer [20], who showed similar effects in bumble bees offered pollen enriched with FA mixtures.

These findings indicate that high concentrations of FAs in pollen may be detrimental to young honey bees, explaining why their intake is strongly regulated by the bees, resulting in a reduced overall pollen consumption. This result adds to the increasing body of evidence that bees in general are adversely affected by an excess fat consumption at significantly lower levels as compared to proteins [47]. Elevated concentrations of FAs in food can restrict the absorption rate of FAs by midgut cells [47], potentially leading to the damage of cell membranes [48]. The composition and concentration of FAs consequently play an important role in bee health, which is analogous to human health, where saturated and trans-FAs increase the risk of coronary heart diseases, while mono- and polyunsaturated FAs (MUFAs and PUFAs) are linked with reduced risk [49, 50]. In comparatively low concentrations, low omega-6:3 ratios are also crucial for bee cognition [19, 23, 26]. In humans, the amount of omega-6 FAs consumed can modulate the amount of omega-3 FAs consumed and thereby reduce the amount of omega-3 available in the body [51], because excessive intake of omega-6 FAs interferes with the desaturation and elongation of omega-3 [52]. Humans therefore also favor an overall low omega-6:3 FA ratio.

The negative effects of high concentrations of specific FAs may explain why, in our study, bees exposed to medium and high FA diets drastically decreased their pollen intake and thereby consumed significantly less FAs compared to bees fed low FA or control diets. As a consequence, the reduced pollen intake may have led to an underconsumption of other essential nutrient compounds, like AAs, sterols, minerals and vitamins. Interestingly, bees seem to prioritize avoiding overconsumption of FAs even at the expense of potentially underconsuming other vital nutrients, which may explain why the medium and high FA groups showed a significant higher mortality compared to the control and low FA groups.

The presence and concentrations of different saturated FAs (SFAs) in pollen can strongly vary. For instance, myristic acid can constitute up to 47% of the total FA concentration in Heliantheus annus pollen [53]. Similarly, palmitic acid reaches up to 36% of the total pollen FA content in Onobrychis viciifolia pollen [53]. In contrast, SFAs, like lauric or stearic acid, are less commonly found but can still account for up to 13% of the total pollen FAs. Arachidic acid, on the other hand, is generally present in lower concentrations, with levels up to 5% of the total FA content observed in the perennial monocot Xanthorrhoea preissii [53]. These concentrations align with concentrations found in the honey bee collected pollen used in our study. The concentrations used in our feeding experiments consequently were within the naturally occurring range observed in pollen, with the exception of the 10-fold higher concentration that exceeded natural levels.

The varying importance of different FAs may further explain why honey bees were able to perceive and differentiate between FAs and different concentrations of the same FAs both in isolation and when added to pollen [28]. Moreover, honey bees could also discriminate among some PUFAs (linolenic, linoleic and oleic acid) which only differed in the number of double bounds but not in chain length. This precise discrimination implies that honey bees invest considerable resources into the reception and perception of FAs and subsequent behavioral responses. Across insects, perception of FAs relies on ionotropic (IR) and gustatory receptors (GR) [5456]. In Drosophila, for example, the IR56d contributes to the perception of short-, medium-, and long-chained FAs [54, 55], while discrimination among chain lengths probably involves one or more receptor classes, such as IR56d+ and GR64f [55]. However, the receptors involved in FA reception in bees are completely unknown to date.

Notably, no such effect on intake and perception was observed when bumble bees were fed with pollen enriched with mixtures of AAs [20, 21] or sterols [31]. Variations in the concentrations of these nutrients did not influence reproductive fitness (i.e., number of produced larvae), even when consumed in concentrations beyond those naturally occurring in pollen [20, 31]. This supports the hypothesis that only those nutrients or compound groups, that are toxic or detrimental when over- or underconsumed, are perceived by bees prior to consumption. Pollen concentrations of other compounds which are essential for bee health and larval growth [57, 58], like AAs or sterols, are likely within the optimal or tolerated range and do therefore not require specific attention by bees in terms of health effects [20, 27, 28, 31]. Interestingly, like bumble bees, honey bees did not differentiate between pollen diets enriched with different concentrations of sterols [28, 31]. However, unlike bumble bees, they discriminated between pollen diets enriched with different concentrations of AAs [28]. This difference between bumble bees and honey bees may suggest potential variations in nutrient-specific perception and likely even the importance of different nutrients for fitness across bee species. In fact, there is emerging evidence that taste receptor gene expression (for GR and IR) is even species-specific in bumble bees [59], which may reflect distinct nutritional requirements among bee species [60].

Overall, honey bees learned and discriminated different FAs equally well, except for stearic acid. Stearic acid, a saturated FA with a hydrocarbon chain length of 18 carbon atoms (same length as linoleic, linolenic and oleic acid), is one of the most common dietary FAs [61] including those found in pollen [62]. Interestingly, bumble bees were able to perceive stearic acid in isolation [20], but failed to regulate its intake as shown by no-choice feeding experiments, where diets were enriched with only one specific FA [63]. Compared to linoleic acid, stearic acid also scored lower intensity responses in humans when administered via edible taste stripes [64]. In fact, in mammals, stearic acid acts differently, i.e., generates a lower lipemic response (i.e., rise in triglycerides with lipoproteins following consumption) as compared to other shorter saturated FAs, like myristic or palmitic acid [65], which may be explained by the possible desaturation of stearic acid into the omega-9 MUFA oleic acid [65].

Honey bees were able to perceive and differentiate between capric acid both in isolation and when tested against other FAs. However, bees always learned to differentiate between the rewarded and unrewarded stimulus faster when capric acid was used as rewarded stimulus (see S3 Fig for an example of the differences in the learning curves). This explains why we had to always test the two complementary series involving capric acid (as CS+ and CS-) separately (S2 Table), and indicates that capric acid influenced the learning process. Capric acid is a medium-chain FA (ten carbon atoms), which can be rapidly absorbed and metabolized, likely rendering it a readily available and thus valuable energy source preferred by bees [66].

While our findings provide valuable insights into FA regulation by freshly emerged honey bees, we acknowledge the limitation of using only one colony in the feeding experiments, which may limit the generalizability of our results. However, our findings align with previous experiments conducted with bumble bees [20], supporting the validity of our data.

Conclusion

Our results demonstrate the capacity of A. mellifera workers to effectively detect and discriminate between both saturated and unsaturated FAs by means of their antennae. They used this ability to regulate the intake of FAs, and when fed with high FA diets, they showed increased mortality rates due to their reduction of pollen and thus FA intake. Apparently, bees are willing to tolerate insufficient amounts of other nutrients to prevent overconsumption of FAs. Our findings add to the growing evidence of the importance of nutrition in maintaining bee health, fitness and survival. They also highlight the importance of a constantly provided diverse flowering plant spectrum, which bees can easily navigate through aided by their nutrient-sensitive perception prior to consumption. This enables them to adjust and optimize the nutrient composition of their diet according to their specific requirements [23, 67, 68]. A pre-ingestional perception of nutrients may thus represent a vital strategy employed by bees and likely other pollinators to strike a balance between overconsuming certain nutrients and underconsuming others in an environment full of nutritionally imbalanced food sources.

Supporting information

S1 Text. FA analysis.

Analysis of FA contents of honey bee collected pollen using gas chromatography- mass spectrometry.

(DOCX)

pone.0309789.s001.docx (15.7KB, docx)
S1 Table. Differences in pollen consumption and FA consumption.

Results of TukeyHSD post-hoc test analyzing differences in pollen (white background) and FA consumption (grey background) between diets enriched with low, medium and high FA concentrations for the first seven days.

(DOCX)

pone.0309789.s002.docx (16.8KB, docx)
S2 Table. Results of the chemotactile PER conditioning experiments.

All FAs were solved 1:1000 in chloroform. Each stimulus was tested as CS+ as well as CS- to test whether the rewarded stimulus influenced learning performance. If this was the case, data was not pooled but analyzed separately, otherwise it was pooled. To test if the bees can differentiate between two stimuli a paired Wilcoxon signed-rank test with continuity correction was used. N represents the number of bees tested per stimulus pair.

(DOCX)

pone.0309789.s003.docx (16.8KB, docx)
S1 Fig. Pollen and FA consumption during the second week.

Mean consumption of (A) pollen and (B) FAs per individual and day during day seven to 14. Bees were fed with four different pollen diets differing in FA content (i.e. pure honey bee collected pollen as a control and the same pollen enriched with a FA-mix to achieve low FA pollen (1.5 times higher FA concentration), medium FA pollen (5 times higher FA concentration) and high FA pollen (10 times higher FA concentration)). Different letters above boxplots indicate significant differences in the mean consumption of pollen.

(TIF)

pone.0309789.s004.tif (52.7KB, tif)
S2 Fig. Pollen and FA consumption during the third week.

Mean consumption of (A) pollen and (B) FAs per individual and day during day 15 to 21. Bees were fed with four different pollen diets differing in FA content (i.e. pure honey bee collected pollen as a control and the same pollen enriched with a FA-mix to achieve low FA pollen (1.5 times higher FA concentration), medium FA pollen (5 times higher FA concentration) and high FA pollen (10 times higher FA concentration)). Different letters above boxplots indicate significant differences in the mean consumption of pollen.

(TIF)

pone.0309789.s005.tif (52KB, tif)
S3 Fig. Percentage of A. mellifera workers that responded with a PER when capric acid was tested against omega-3.

(A) Omega-3 was used as the rewarded stimulus and capric acid as the unrewarded stimulus. (B) Capric acid was used as rewarded stimulus and omega-3 as unrewarded stimulus. CS+ (black, squares) represents the rewarded stimulus and CS- (grey, circles) the unrewarded stimulus. N represents the number of individuals tested. Statistical differences are marked with different letters at the right side of the curves (P < 0.05).

(TIF)

pone.0309789.s006.tif (46.5KB, tif)

Acknowledgments

We thank the entire TUM PII team and two anonymous reviewers for valuable feedback on a previous version of this manuscript.

Data Availability

All relevant data is available on https://doi.org/10.17605/osf.io/geuzv

Funding Statement

Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (DFG project: LE 2750/5-2 to SDL and SP1380/1-2 to JS).

References

  • 1.Raubenheimer D, Simpson SJ. Integrative models of nutrient balancing: application to insects and vertebrates. Nutr Res Rev. 1997;10(1):151–79. doi: 10.1079/NRR19970009 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Raubenheimer D, Simpson SJ. Integrating nutrition: a geometrical approach. Proceedings of the 10th International Symposium on Insect-Plant Relationships: Springer Netherlands; 1999. p. 67–82. [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Simpson SJ, Raubenheimer D. The nature of nutrition: a unifying framework from animal adaptation to human obesity: Princeton university press; 2012. [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Stabler D, Paoli PP, Nicolson SW, Wright GA. Nutrient balancing of the adult worker bumblebee (Bombus terrestris) depends on the dietary source of essential amino acids. J Exp Biol. 2015;218(Pt 5):793–802. doi: 10.1242/jeb.114249 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Grund-Mueller N, Ruedenauer FA, Spaethe J, Leonhardt SD. Adding Amino Acids to a Sucrose Diet Is Not Sufficient to Support Longevity of Adult Bumble Bees. Insects. 2020;11(4). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Brodschneider R, Crailsheim K. Nutrition and health in honey bees. Apidologie. 2010;41(3):278–94. [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Leonhardt SD, Chui SX, Kuba K. The role of non-volatile chemicals of floral rewards in plant-pollinator interactions. Basic and Applied Ecology. 2024;75:31–43. [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Roulston TaH, Cane JH, Buchmann SL. What Governs Protein Content of Pollen: Pollinator Preferences, Pollen–Pistil Interactions, or Phylogeny? Ecological Monographs. 2000;70(4):617–43. [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Egan PA, Adler LS, Irwin RE, Farrell IW, Palmer-Young EC, Stevenson PC. Crop Domestication Alters Floral Reward Chemistry With Potential Consequences for Pollinator Health. Front Plant Sci. 2018;9:1357. doi: 10.3389/fpls.2018.01357 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Ruedenauer FA, Spaethe J, van der Kooi CJ, Leonhardt SD. Pollinator or pedigree: which factors determine the evolution of pollen nutrients? Oecologia. 2019;191(2):349–58. doi: 10.1007/s00442-019-04494-x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Eilers EJ, Kleine S, Eckert S, Waldherr S, Muller C. Flower Production, Headspace Volatiles, Pollen Nutrients, and Florivory in Tanacetum vulgare Chemotypes. Front Plant Sci. 2020;11:611877. doi: 10.3389/fpls.2020.611877 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Zu P, Koch H, Schwery O, Pironon S, Phillips C, Ondo I, et al. Pollen sterols are associated with phylogeny and environment but not with pollinator guilds. New Phytol. 2021;230(3):1169–84. doi: 10.1111/nph.17227 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Crone MK, Grozinger CM. Pollen protein and lipid content influence resilience to insecticides in honey bees (Apis mellifera). J Exp Biol. 2021: jeb242040. doi: 10.1242/jeb.242040 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Vaudo AD, Dyer LA, Leonard AS. Pollen nutrition structures bee and plant community interactions. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2024;121(3):e2317228120. doi: 10.1073/pnas.2317228120 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Vaudo AD, Tooker JF, Patch HM, Biddinger DJ, Coccia M, Crone MK, et al. Pollen Protein: Lipid Macronutrient Ratios May Guide Broad Patterns of Bee Species Floral Preferences. Insects. 2020;11(2). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Di Pasquale G, Salignon M, Le Conte Y, Belzunces LP, Decourtye A, Kretzschmar A, et al. Influence of pollen nutrition on honey bee health: do pollen quality and diversity matter? PLoS One. 2013;8(8):e72016. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0072016 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Leroy C, Brunet J-L, Henry M, Alaux C, Cooke S. Using physiology to better support wild bee conservation. Conservation Physiology. 2023;11(1). doi: 10.1093/conphys/coac076 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Hulbert AJ, Abbott SK. Nutritional ecology of essential fatty acids: an evolutionary perspective. Australian Journal of Zoology. 2011;59(6):369–79. [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Arien Y, Dag A, Yona S, Tietel Z, Lapidot Cohen T, Shafir S. Effect of diet lipids and omega-6:3 ratio on honey bee brood development, adult survival and body composition. J Insect Physiol. 2020;124:104074. doi: 10.1016/j.jinsphys.2020.104074 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Ruedenauer FA, Raubenheimer D, Kessner-Beierlein D, Grund-Mueller N, Noack L, Spaethe J, et al. Best be(e) on low fat: linking nutrient perception, regulation and fitness. Ecol Lett. 2020;23(3):545–54. doi: 10.1111/ele.13454 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Lajad R, Moreno E, Arenas A. Young honeybees show learned preferences after experiencing adulterated pollen. Sci Rep. 2021;11(1):23327. doi: 10.1038/s41598-021-02700-6 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Bennett MM, Welchert AC, Carroll M, Shafir S, Smith BH, Corby-Harris V. Unbalanced fatty acid diets impair discrimination ability of honey bee workers to damaged and healthy brood odors. Journal of Experimental Biology. 2022;225(7). doi: 10.1242/jeb.244103 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Arien Y, Dag A, Zarchin S, Masci T, Shafir S. Omega-3 deficiency impairs honey bee learning. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2015;112(51):15761–6. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1517375112 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Wang X, Zhong Z, Chen X, Hong Z, Lin W, Mu X, et al. High-Fat Diets with Differential Fatty Acids Induce Obesity and Perturb Gut Microbiota in Honey Bee. Int J Mol Sci. 2021;22(2). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Feldlaufer ML, Knox DA, R. LW, H. S. Antimicrobial activity of fatty acids against Bacillus larvae, the causative agent of American foulbrood disease. Apidologie 1993;24:95–9. [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Arien Y, Dag A, Shafir S. Omega-6:3 Ratio More Than Absolute Lipid Level in Diet Affects Associative Learning in Honey Bees. Frontiers in Psychology. 2018;9:1001. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01001 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Ruedenauer FA, Spaethe J, Leonhardt SD. How to know which food is good for you: bumblebees use taste to discriminate between different concentrations of food differing in nutrient content. J Exp Biol. 2015;218(Pt 14):2233–40. doi: 10.1242/jeb.118554 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Ruedenauer FA, Biewer NW, Nebauer CA, Scheiner M, Spaethe J, Leonhardt SD. Honey Bees Can Taste Amino and Fatty Acids in Pollen, but Not Sterols. Frontiers in Ecology and Evolution. 2021;9:684175. [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Daly HV, Magnacca KN. Insects of Hawaii, vol. 17: Hawaiian Hylaeus (Nesoprosopis) Bees (Hymenoptera, Apoidea): University of Hawaii Press; 2003. [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Thorp RW. The collection of pollen by bees. Pollen and pollination. 2000:211–23. [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Nebauer CA, Schleifer MC, Ruedenauer FA, Leonhardt SD, Spaethe J. Perception, regulation, and fitness effects of pollen phytosterols in the bumble bee, Bombus terrestris. Am J Bot. 2023;110(6):e16165. doi: 10.1002/ajb2.16165 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Ruedenauer FA, Parreno MA, Grunwald Kadow IC, Spaethe J, Leonhardt SD. The ecology of nutrient sensation and perception in insects. Trends Ecol Evol. 2023;38(10):994–1004. doi: 10.1016/j.tree.2023.05.006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Kriesell L, Hilpert A, Leonhardt SD. Different but the same: bumblebee species collect pollen of different plant sources but similar amino acid profiles. Apidologie. 2017;48(1):102–16. [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Villagómez GN, Brachvogel R-C, Kárpáti Z, Leonhardt SD, Schmitt T, Ruedenauer FA. A common protocol for reliable comparison of pollen fatty acid profiles: highlighting pitfalls and proposing a methodology for ecological research. Frontiers in Ecology and Evolution. 2023;11. [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Szolderits MJ, Crailsheim K. A comparison of pollen consumption and digestion in honeybee (Apis mellifera carnica) drones and workers. Journal of Insect Physiology. 1993;39(10):877–81. [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Scheiner R, Amdam GV. Impaired tactile learning is related to social role in honeybees. J Exp Biol. 2009;212(Pt 7):994–1002. doi: 10.1242/jeb.021188 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Behrends A, Scheiner R, Baker N, Amdam GV. Cognitive aging is linked to social role in honey bees (Apis mellifera). Exp Gerontol. 2007;42(12):1146–53. doi: 10.1016/j.exger.2007.09.003 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Rueppell O, Christine S, Mulcrone C, Groves L. Aging without functional senescence in honey bee workers. Curr Biol. 2007;17(8):R274–5. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2007.02.015 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Ray S, Ferneyhough B. The effects of age on olfactory learning and memory in the honey bee Apis mellifera. NeuroReport. 1997;8(3):789–93. doi: 10.1097/00001756-199702100-00042 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Pavlov IP. Conditioned Reflexes Oxford: Oxford University Press; 1927. [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Bitterman ME, Menzel R, Fietz A, Schäfer S. Classical conditioning of proboscis extension in honeybees (Apis mellifera). Journal of comparative psychology. 1983;97(2):107. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Lichtenstein L, Sommerlandt FM, Spaethe J. Dumb and Lazy? A Comparison of Color Learning and Memory Retrieval in Drones and Workers of the Buff-Tailed Bumblebee, Bombus terrestris, by Means of PER Conditioning. PLoS One. 2015;10(7):e0134248. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0134248 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.R Core Team. R: A language and environment for statistical computing. 2021.
  • 44.Fox J, Weisberg S. An R Companion to Applied Regression, 3rd ed. Thousand Oaks, CA, USA: Sage; 2019. [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Therneau TM, Grambsch PM. Modeling survival data: extending the Cox model. NY, USA: Springer; 2013. [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Klein JP, Moeschberger ML. Survival analysis: techniques for censored and truncated data: Springer; 2003. [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Canavoso LaE Jouni ZE, Karnas KJ Pennington JE, Wells MA. Fat metabolism in insects. Annual review of nutrition. 2001;21(1):23–46. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Haddad LS, Kelbert L, Hulbert AJ. Extended longevity of queen honey bees compared to workers is associated with peroxidation-resistant membranes. Exp Gerontol. 2007;42(7):601–9. doi: 10.1016/j.exger.2007.02.008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Williams CM, editor Dietary fatty acids and human health. Annales de zootechnie; 2000: EDP Sciences. [Google Scholar]
  • 50.White B. Dietary fatty acids. Am Fam Physician. 2009. 80(4):345–50. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Taha AY, Cheon Y, Faurot KF, Macintosh B, Majchrzak-Hong SF, Mann JD, et al. Dietary omega-6 fatty acid lowering increases bioavailability of omega-3 polyunsaturated fatty acids in human plasma lipid pools. Prostaglandins Leukot Essent Fatty Acids. 2014;90(5):151–7. doi: 10.1016/j.plefa.2014.02.003 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Patterson E, Wall R, Fitzgerald GF, Ross RP, Stanton C. Health implications of high dietary omega-6 polyunsaturated Fatty acids. J Nutr Metab. 2012;2012:539426. doi: 10.1155/2012/539426 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Manning R. Fatty acid composition of pollen and the effect of two dominant fatty acids (linoleic and oleic) in pollen and flour diets on longevity and nutritional composition of honey bees (Apis mellifera): Murdoch University; 2006. [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Ahn JE, Chen Y, Amrein H. Molecular basis of fatty acid taste in Drosophila. Elife. 2017;6. doi: 10.7554/eLife.30115 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Brown EB, Shah KD, Palermo J, Dey M, Dahanukar A, Keene AC. Ir56d-dependent fatty acid responses in Drosophila uncover taste discrimination between different classes of fatty acids. Elife. 2021;10. doi: 10.7554/eLife.67878 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Hou XQ, Zhang DD, Powell D, Wang HL, Andersson MN, Lofstedt C. Ionotropic receptors in the turnip moth Agrotis segetum respond to repellent medium-chain fatty acids. BMC Biol. 2022;20(1):34. doi: 10.1186/s12915-022-01235-0 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Svoboda J, Thompson M, Robbins W, Kaplanis J. Insect steroid metabolism. Lipids. 1978;13(10):742–53. doi: 10.1007/BF02533755 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.de Groot AP. Protein and Amino Acid Requirements of the Honeybee (Apis Mellifica L.). Physiol Comp Oecol 1953;3:197–285. [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Sun C, Huang J, Wang Y, Zhao X, Su L, Thomas GWC, et al. Genus-Wide Characterization of Bumblebee Genomes Provides Insights into Their Evolution and Variation in Ecological and Behavioral Traits. Mol Biol Evol. 2021;38(2):486–501. doi: 10.1093/molbev/msaa240 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Barraud A, Barascou L, Lefebvre V, Sene D, Le Conte Y, Alaux C, et al. Variations in Nutritional Requirements Across Bee Species. Frontiers in Sustainable Food Systems. 2022;6. [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Acid L. Final report on the safety assessment of oleic acid, laurie acid, palmitic acid, myristic acid, and stearic acid. J Am Coll Toxicol. 1987;6:321–401. [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Manning R. Fatty acids in pollen: a review of their importance for honey bees. Bee World. 2015;82(2):60–75. [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Ruedenauer FA, Schaeffler AA, Scheider T, Rakonic G, Spaethe J, Leonhardt SD. Does fat identity matter? The effect of saturated and unsaturated fatty acids on bumble bee consumption and fitness. Ecological Entomology. in press. [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Ebba S, Abarintos RA, Kim DG, Tiyouh M, Stull JC, Movalia A, et al. The examination of fatty acid taste with edible strips. Physiol Behav. 2012;106(5):579–86. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2012.04.006 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Sampath H, Ntambi JM. The fate and intermediary metabolism of stearic acid. Lipids. 2005;40(12):1187–91. doi: 10.1007/s11745-005-1484-z [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Bernard A, Carlier H. Absorption and intestinal catabolism of fatty acids in the rat: effect of chain length and unsaturation. Exp Physiol. 1991;76(3):445–55. doi: 10.1113/expphysiol.1991.sp003511 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Vaudo AD, Patch HM, Mortensen DA, Tooker JF, Grozinger CM. Macronutrient ratios in pollen shape bumble bee (Bombus impatiens) foraging strategies and floral preferences. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2016;113(28):E4035–42. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1606101113 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Kraus S, Gomez-Moracho T, Pasquaretta C, Latil G, Dussutour A, Lihoreau M. Bumblebees adjust protein and lipid collection rules to the presence of brood. Curr Zool. 2019;65(4):437–46. doi: 10.1093/cz/zoz026 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Decision Letter 0

Olav Rueppell

10 Jun 2024

PONE-D-24-20571Perception, regulation, and fitness effects of pollen fatty acids in the honey bee, Apis melliferaPLOS ONE

Dear Dr. Leonhardt,

Thank you for submitting your manuscript to PLOS ONE. After careful consideration, we feel that it has merit but does not fully meet PLOS ONE’s publication criteria as it currently stands. Therefore, we invite you to submit a revised version of the manuscript that addresses the points raised during the review process.

Please submit your revised manuscript by Jul 25 2024 11:59PM. If you will need more time than this to complete your revisions, please reply to this message or contact the journal office at plosone@plos.org. When you're ready to submit your revision, log on to https://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/ and select the 'Submissions Needing Revision' folder to locate your manuscript file.

Please include the following items when submitting your revised manuscript:

  • A rebuttal letter that responds to each point raised by the academic editor and reviewer(s). You should upload this letter as a separate file labeled 'Response to Reviewers'.

  • A marked-up copy of your manuscript that highlights changes made to the original version. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Revised Manuscript with Track Changes'.

  • An unmarked version of your revised paper without tracked changes. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Manuscript'.

If you would like to make changes to your financial disclosure, please include your updated statement in your cover letter. Guidelines for resubmitting your figure files are available below the reviewer comments at the end of this letter.

If applicable, we recommend that you deposit your laboratory protocols in protocols.io to enhance the reproducibility of your results. Protocols.io assigns your protocol its own identifier (DOI) so that it can be cited independently in the future. For instructions see: https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/submission-guidelines#loc-laboratory-protocols. Additionally, PLOS ONE offers an option for publishing peer-reviewed Lab Protocol articles, which describe protocols hosted on protocols.io. Read more information on sharing protocols at https://plos.org/protocols?utm_medium=editorial-email&utm_source=authorletters&utm_campaign=protocols.

We look forward to receiving your revised manuscript.

Kind regards,

Olav Rueppell

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Journal Requirements:

When submitting your revision, we need you to address these additional requirements.

1. Please ensure that your manuscript meets PLOS ONE's style requirements, including those for file naming. The PLOS ONE style templates can be found at 

https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=wjVg/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_main_body.pdf and 

https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=ba62/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_title_authors_affiliations.pdf

2. Thank you for stating the following financial disclosure: 

"Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (DFG project: LE 2750/5-2 to SDL and SP1380/1-2 to JS)"

Please state what role the funders took in the study. If the funders had no role, please state: "The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript." If this statement is not correct you must amend it as needed. 

Please include this amended Role of Funder statement in your cover letter; we will change the online submission form on your behalf.

3. Please note that your Data Availability Statement is currently missing the repository name. If your manuscript is accepted for publication, you will be asked to provide these details on a very short timeline. We therefore suggest that you provide this information now, though we will not hold up the peer review process if you are unable.

Additional Editor Comments:

Most of the reviewers' concerns seem to stem from the need for more explanation/information (the most severe probably being the missing methods on FA determination) and a lack of attention to detail when preparing the manuscript (e.g., formatting). However, there are also a few comments that question your logic and I would encourage you to address everything very carefully because I will have to send the revised manuscript back to the reviewers for their input.

[Note: HTML markup is below. Please do not edit.]

Reviewers' comments:

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Author

1. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.

Reviewer #1: Partly

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

2. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

3. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #1: No

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

4. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

5. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #1: The manuscript by Schleifer et al. examines the ability of honey bees to detect the various fatty acids that are present in pollen, their ability to regulate this and the potential fitness effects. The manuscript is very interesting and adds to the existing literature of how honey bees are able to regulate nutrient intake, including fatty acids. My general comments are:

1. Please mention the full form before the use of abbreviations throughout the manuscript.

2. Replace animals with colonies in L114.

3. L120 why not use multiple colonies and multiple frames to avoid colony specific behavior and physiological results?

4. The total experimental duration for the feeding experiment should be included in the results. I found this eventually when reading the figure legends.

5. How was FA concentration measured in pollen and throughout the experiment? A whole section is missing on how the authors evaluated the FA concentrations. Was mass spec used and where is the raw data?

6. L146 is there a physiological difference between honey bee foragers and their PER due to the long experimental duration and changing months?

7. What about the ages of the foragers? How was forager age controlled for?

8. More clarity in the method section is needed for better understanding the methods and for the benefit of the readers. L 148 were sister queens used? What was the total number of hives eventually used? Were the same newly emerged honey bees tested for all the experimental replicates? How many were tested for each group?

9. Similar to the experimental duration, methods do not indicate that chloroform was tested as a solvent control for PER. I found this later on in the manuscript. Please include this to help improve the method section for further clarity.

10. L230 for the benefit of the readers the authors should indicate how they calculated FA consumption data.

11. Please add the weekly data for the entire experimental duration even though the first week showed most consumptions. It will be interesting to see what happens every week. This will also add more information on how much pollen was consumed for each experimental group. And how much FA was also consumed.

12. L252 please italicize scientific names.

13. L254 the graph shown does not seem to match 21 days for experimental duration based on the axis.

14. For the PER experiments in general, are honey bees not trained with a sugar reward as a positive stimulus response and no sugar reward as a negative response? So it is expected that honey bees will learn and not respond when sugar reward is not presented? Even if FA were presented? Also was the same honey bee tested for all experimental groups in all combinations (solvent control and each FA as CS+ and CS-)? If not how were honey bees age controlled/colony controlled to negate any other bias? How many training attempts and how many test attempts for each honey bee?

15. All raw data have not been presented and please include that.

16. While survival is a measure of fitness, not sure if the authors can truly use “fitness” in their manuscript title without actually measuring the various indices of fitness for honey bees (physiology and colony health).

Reviewer #2: The authors should make a few changes before the document is forwarded to reviewers.

- please check the reference format in the overall manuscript.

- I think chloroform is toxic to honey bees. Did you check the safety of this chemical for honey bees? We attached the PDF which is chloroform MSDS

-According to the PER experiment, except for the combination of omega-3 and stearic acid, honeybees were able to distinguish each fatty acid. However, concluding that honeybees distinguish between saturated and unsaturated fatty acids seems unreasonable.

- Line 105: Please change ‘overconsumption’ to ‘High amount of FA content’.

- Line 128: "0.5 times" means a decrease, not an increase. Therefore, please change all instances of 0.5 to 1.5.

- Line 132: Please explain more clearly about the FA mixture, for example, where is the source (which company?) or Did you mix yourself using each FAs then, please list the company of each FA, for example: palmitic (Sigma-Aldrich, St. Louis, MO).

- Line 142: In Table 1, How did you measure the fatty acids content in pollen? And what is FS? Maybe FS should be changed to FA.

- Line 237: Please add statistical results in Figure 2 (A and B). Please add the statistical result in the manuscript even present in the S1 Table.

- Line 252: Please check that 'Apis mellifera' is in italics in all manuscripts.

- Line 264: In Figure 3, please explain why CS- in stearic acid showed higher than other FAs.

- Line 268: Why did you choose to PER test linoleic, linolenic, oleic, capric, and stearic acids among the fatty acids?

- Line 271: Please check that 'P' is in italics in all manuscripts.

- Line 278: In Figure 4, Please list the full name of FAs, for example, ‘Omega-6 Capric’ change to ‘Omege-6 vs. Capric acid’. Please upgrade Figure 4.

- Line 291: Please change ‘1:1000 vs chloroform’ to ‘1:1000 FA mix vs chloroform’.

- In the discussion, please add more explanation of the percentage of each fatty acid (except Omega-3, Omega-6, and Omega-9) included in general pollen and explain the effect on honeybees.

- Line 300: Please delete the second author name.

- Line 305, 323, 332: Please delete the author's name.

**********

6. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #1: No

Reviewer #2: No

**********

[NOTE: If reviewer comments were submitted as an attachment file, they will be attached to this email and accessible via the submission site. Please log into your account, locate the manuscript record, and check for the action link "View Attachments". If this link does not appear, there are no attachment files.]

While revising your submission, please upload your figure files to the Preflight Analysis and Conversion Engine (PACE) digital diagnostic tool, https://pacev2.apexcovantage.com/. PACE helps ensure that figures meet PLOS requirements. To use PACE, you must first register as a user. Registration is free. Then, login and navigate to the UPLOAD tab, where you will find detailed instructions on how to use the tool. If you encounter any issues or have any questions when using PACE, please email PLOS at figures@plos.org. Please note that Supporting Information files do not need this step.

PLoS One. 2024 Nov 21;19(11):e0309789. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0309789.r002

Author response to Decision Letter 0


20 Jul 2024

Thank you very much for the possibility to submit a revised version of our manuscript to PLOS ONE. We also thank the two reviewers for their valuable and constructive comments on the manuscript. We have addressed all points raised by the reviewers and provide a detailed response to each comment individually below (starting with --).

Reviewer #1: The manuscript by Schleifer et al. examines the ability of honey bees to detect the various fatty acids that are present in pollen, their ability to regulate this and the potential fitness effects. The manuscript is very interesting and adds to the existing literature of how honey bees are able to regulate nutrient intake, including fatty acids.

-- Thank you very much for this remark, and also for taking the time to review our manuscript and for providing constructive feedback.

My general comments are:

1. Please mention the full form before the use of abbreviations throughout the manuscript.

-- Done.

2. Replace animals with colonies in L114.

-- Done.

3. L120 why not use multiple colonies and multiple frames to avoid colony specific behavior and physiological results?

-- Thank you for pointing that out, and we agree that this would be the optimal procedure. Unfortunately, at the time period of the experiment, we had only one colony with one mature brood frame available. Since this single brood frame had a sufficient number of bees emerged on the same day, we decided to stick with only one brood frame from a single colony. We now clarified this aspect in the manuscript and discuss the limitations of using only one colony.

4. The total experimental duration for the feeding experiment should be included in the results. I found this eventually when reading the figure legends.

-- The experimental duration is mentioned in the first sentence of the ‘Feeding experiment’ section (L127), but for clarification, we now also added the duration in the ‘Results’ section.

5. How was FA concentration measured in pollen and throughout the experiment? A whole section is missing on how the authors evaluated the FA concentrations. Was mass spec used and where is the raw data?

-- Thank you for pointing this out! We added the information of the FA analysis to the ‘Material & Method’ section and included a detailed protocol in the Supplementary Material section!

6. L146 is there a physiological difference between honey bee foragers and their PER due to the long experimental duration and changing months?

-- We could see differences in the response of foragers during the season. For example, early in the season, when many flowering plants were available, less bees were willing to show a PER when touching the antenna during the pre-test, compared to later in the season. However, only bees that showed a proper PER and thus a high motivational state, were used in the experiments. We additionally tested all stimuli randomly throughout the season to avoid any bias.

7. What about the ages of the foragers? How was forager age controlled for?

-- We randomly caught the bees while leaving the hive, which is why we could not control for age. However, studies showed, that honey bees’ cognitive abilities are influenced by their social role rather than the chronological age. We have added this information in the manuscript.

8. More clarity in the method section is needed for better understanding the methods and for the benefit of the readers. L 148 were sister queens used? What was the total number of hives eventually used? Were the same newly emerged honey bees tested for all the experimental replicates? How many were tested for each group?

-- In total, we used bees from ten hives (L158), but no sister queens were used.

Newly emerged bees were only used for the feeding experiment since the consumption of pollen by adult bees declines rapidly after the first week. Each bee was assigned to only one dietary treatment. We had 4 treatments with 10 replicates each. Each replicate comprised 30 newly emerged bees – so 300 bees for each dietary treatment (L131-133). Bees used in the feeding experiments were not used for the learning / PER experiments; only foragers that were about to leave the hive, were used for PER experiments. All this information has now been added/clarified in the methods.

9. Similar to the experimental duration, methods do not indicate that chloroform was tested as a solvent control for PER. I found this later on in the manuscript. Please include this to help improve the method section for further clarity.

-- Thank you for the hint! We now added this information to the ’Stimuli’ paragraph of the ‘Material and Method’ section.

10. L230 for the benefit of the readers the authors should indicate how they calculated FA consumption data.

-- Good point. This is now included in the beginning of the ‘Statistical analysis’ section.

11. Please add the weekly data for the entire experimental duration even though the first week showed most consumptions. It will be interesting to see what happens every week. This will also add more information on how much pollen was consumed for each experimental group. And how much FA was also consumed.

-- We agree and have now added the respective figures in the Supplementary Material

12. L252 please italicize scientific names.

-- Done.

13. L254 the graph shown does not seem to match 21 days for experimental duration based on the axis.

-- Thank you for the hint. We have revised the figure.

14. For the PER experiments in general, are honey bees not trained with a sugar reward as a positive stimulus response and no sugar reward as a negative response? So it is expected that honey bees will learn and not respond when sugar reward is not presented? Even if FA were presented? Also was the same honey bee tested for all experimental groups in all combinations (solvent control and each FA as CS+ and CS-)? If not how were honey bees age controlled/colony controlled to negate any other bias? How many training attempts and how many test attempts for each honey bee?

-- You are correct, the rewarded stimulus (CS+) is combined with sugar water (e.g. fatty acid A + sugar water), and the unrewarded stimulus (CS-) is not paired with anything but the stimulus alone (e.g. fatty acid B).

Naïve, untrained bees will only elicit a PER towards sugar water and not towards a FA alone. In a classical absolute conditioning experiment, when the FA is paired with the sugar water, the bee associates the sugar reward with this FA after repeated presentations and will elicit a PER to the FA alone, but only if the bee can perceive the FA. In a classical differential conditioning experiment, one FA will be rewarded (CS+), a second FA will be unrewarded (CS-). After repeated random presentations of the CS+ and CS-, the bee will elicit a PER only to the rewarded one, but no PER to the unrewarded one.

Regarding testing: an individual bee was tested only once (described in L184), otherwise the bee would already have learned one stimulus and cannot be considered a naïve, untrained bee.

We randomly took bees from different colonies to exclude any colony bias, but we did not check for age (although leaving forager bees were considered to be between 16 and 35 day old). Each bee was tested ten times for the rewarding and ten times for the unrewarded stimulus, in total 20 trials (L 196-197).

We have tried to clarify these details throughout the manuscript.

15. All raw data have not been presented and please include that.

-- The raw data for the FA analysis of the pollen has now been added and is available on https://doi.org/10.17605/osf.io/geuzv.

16. While survival is a measure of fitness, not sure if the authors can truly use “fitness” in their manuscript title without actually measuring the various indices of fitness for honey bees (physiology and colony health).

-- We agree with the reviewer and have changed the title to: Perception, regulation, and fitness effects on longevity of pollen fatty acids in the honey bee, Apis mellifera

Reviewer #2: The authors should make a few changes before the document is forwarded to reviewers.

1. please check the reference format in the overall manuscript.

-- Thank you for this hint; we checked for all mistakes and have now a consistent reference format.

2. I think chloroform is toxic to honey bees. Did you check the safety of this chemical for honey bees? We attached the PDF which is chloroform MSDS

-- This is correct; however, we have made sure that the chloroform evaporates completely before the filter paper was presented to the bees. We now added this important information to the Method section.

3. According to the PER experiment, except for the combination of omega-3 and stearic acid, honeybees were able to distinguish each fatty acid. However, concluding that honeybees distinguish between saturated and unsaturated fatty acids seems unreasonable.

-- We agree that our conclusion may have been too far-fetched and have adjusted our wording in the revised version (L358-359).

4. Line 105: Please change ‘overconsumption’ to ‘High amount of FA content’.

-- Done.

5. Line 128: "0.5 times" means a decrease, not an increase. Therefore, please change all instances of 0.5 to 1.5.

-- Done.

6. Line 132: Please explain more clearly about the FA mixture, for example, where is the source (which company?) or Did you mix yourself using each FAs then, please list the company of each FA, for example: palmitic (Sigma-Aldrich, St. Louis, MO).

-- Thank you for the hint. We added the source of the single FAs.

7. Line 142: In Table 1, How did you measure the fatty acids content in pollen? And what is FS? Maybe FS should be changed to FA.

-- Thank you for pointing this out. Of course, FS should be stated as FA, which we have changed now. We now also mention the FA analysis in the Material and Methods section and added the full protocol in the Supplementary Material.

8. Line 237: Please add statistical results in Figure 2 (A and B). Please add the statistical result in the manuscript even present in the S1 Table.

-- Done.

9. Line 252: Please check that 'Apis mellifera' is in italics in all manuscripts.

-- Done.

10. Line 264: In Figure 3, please explain why CS- in stearic acid showed higher than other FAs.

-- We do not know why the bees did not perceive stearic acid, which we mention and discuss in the text.

11. Line 268: Why did you choose to PER test linoleic, linolenic, oleic, capric, and stearic acids among the fatty acids?

-- We added our rationale to the Material and Method section, which now reads: “We selected these five FA to test if the bees can differentiate between FAs based on the chain length, saturation state or the number of double bounds”.

12. Line 271: Please check that 'P' is in italics in all manuscripts.

-- Done.

13. Line 278: In Figure 4, Please list the full name of FAs, for example, ‘Omega-6 Capric’ change to ‘Omege-6 vs. Capric acid’. Please upgrade Figure 4.

-- Done.

14. Line 291: Please change ‘1:1000 vs chloroform’ to ‘1:1000 FA mix vs chloroform’.

-- Done.

15. In the discussion, please add more explanation of the percentage of each fatty acid (except Omega-3, Omega-6, and Omega-9) included in general pollen and explain the effect on honeybees.

-- Thank you for mentioning it. We have added this information in the discussion

16. Line 300: Please delete the second author name.

-- Done.

17. Line 305, 323, 332: Please delete the author's name.

-- Done.

Attachment

Submitted filename: Response to Reviewers_final.docx

pone.0309789.s007.docx (26.5KB, docx)

Decision Letter 1

Olav Rueppell

12 Aug 2024

PONE-D-24-20571R1Perception, regulation, and effects on longevity of pollen fatty acids in the honey bee, Apis melliferaPLOS ONE

Dear Dr. Leonhardt,

Thank you for re-submitting your manuscript to PLOS ONE. As you can see, the reviewer (and I) are satisfied with your revisions. However, before I can fully accept it, please fix the references at this stage. You might also want to give the manuscript an overall read through for catching minor errors. Finally, if you could please explain the authorship change?

Please submit your revised manuscript by Sep 26 2024 11:59PM. If you will need more time than this to complete your revisions, please reply to this message or contact the journal office at plosone@plos.org. When you're ready to submit your revision, log on to https://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/ and select the 'Submissions Needing Revision' folder to locate your manuscript file.

Please include the following items when submitting your revised manuscript:

  • A rebuttal letter that responds to each point raised by the academic editor and reviewer(s). You should upload this letter as a separate file labeled 'Response to Reviewers'.

  • A marked-up copy of your manuscript that highlights changes made to the original version. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Revised Manuscript with Track Changes'.

  • An unmarked version of your revised paper without tracked changes. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Manuscript'.

If you would like to make changes to your financial disclosure, please include your updated statement in your cover letter. Guidelines for resubmitting your figure files are available below the reviewer comments at the end of this letter.

If applicable, we recommend that you deposit your laboratory protocols in protocols.io to enhance the reproducibility of your results. Protocols.io assigns your protocol its own identifier (DOI) so that it can be cited independently in the future. For instructions see: https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/submission-guidelines#loc-laboratory-protocols. Additionally, PLOS ONE offers an option for publishing peer-reviewed Lab Protocol articles, which describe protocols hosted on protocols.io. Read more information on sharing protocols at https://plos.org/protocols?utm_medium=editorial-email&utm_source=authorletters&utm_campaign=protocols.

We look forward to receiving your revised manuscript.

Kind regards,

Olav Rueppell

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Journal Requirements:

Please review your reference list to ensure that it is complete and correct. If you have cited papers that have been retracted, please include the rationale for doing so in the manuscript text, or remove these references and replace them with relevant current references. Any changes to the reference list should be mentioned in the rebuttal letter that accompanies your revised manuscript. If you need to cite a retracted article, indicate the article’s retracted status in the References list and also include a citation and full reference for the retraction notice.

[Note: HTML markup is below. Please do not edit.]

Reviewers' comments:

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Author

1. If the authors have adequately addressed your comments raised in a previous round of review and you feel that this manuscript is now acceptable for publication, you may indicate that here to bypass the “Comments to the Author” section, enter your conflict of interest statement in the “Confidential to Editor” section, and submit your "Accept" recommendation.

Reviewer #2: All comments have been addressed

**********

2. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

3. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

4. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

5. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

6. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #2: Revision has been made according to reviewers' comments, but in information on references 13, 29, 30, no page numbers are found, please add it.

**********

7. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #2: Yes: Hyung Wook Kwon

**********

[NOTE: If reviewer comments were submitted as an attachment file, they will be attached to this email and accessible via the submission site. Please log into your account, locate the manuscript record, and check for the action link "View Attachments". If this link does not appear, there are no attachment files.]

While revising your submission, please upload your figure files to the Preflight Analysis and Conversion Engine (PACE) digital diagnostic tool, https://pacev2.apexcovantage.com/. PACE helps ensure that figures meet PLOS requirements. To use PACE, you must first register as a user. Registration is free. Then, login and navigate to the UPLOAD tab, where you will find detailed instructions on how to use the tool. If you encounter any issues or have any questions when using PACE, please email PLOS at figures@plos.org. Please note that Supporting Information files do not need this step.

PLoS One. 2024 Nov 21;19(11):e0309789. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0309789.r004

Author response to Decision Letter 1


18 Aug 2024

Dear Hyung Wook Kwon,

thank you very much for taking them time and effort to once again read our manuscript and for your valuable feedback on the manuscript.

We have changed the reference list and added the missing information.

Warmest regards,

Sara Leonhardt and all co-authors

Attachment

Submitted filename: Response to Editor_2nd revision.docx

pone.0309789.s008.docx (17.5KB, docx)

Decision Letter 2

Olav Rueppell

20 Aug 2024

Perception, regulation, and effects on longevity of pollen fatty acids in the honey bee, Apis mellifera

PONE-D-24-20571R2

Dear Dr. Leonhardt,

We’re pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been judged scientifically suitable for publication and will be formally accepted for publication once it meets all outstanding technical requirements.

Within one week, you’ll receive an e-mail detailing the required amendments. When these have been addressed, you’ll receive a formal acceptance letter and your manuscript will be scheduled for publication.

An invoice will be generated when your article is formally accepted. Please note, if your institution has a publishing partnership with PLOS and your article meets the relevant criteria, all or part of your publication costs will be covered. Please make sure your user information is up-to-date by logging into Editorial Manager at Editorial Manager® and clicking the ‘Update My Information' link at the top of the page. If you have any questions relating to publication charges, please contact our Author Billing department directly at authorbilling@plos.org.

If your institution or institutions have a press office, please notify them about your upcoming paper to help maximize its impact. If they’ll be preparing press materials, please inform our press team as soon as possible -- no later than 48 hours after receiving the formal acceptance. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information, please contact onepress@plos.org.

Kind regards,

Olav Rueppell

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Additional Editor Comments (optional):

Reviewers' comments:

Acceptance letter

Olav Rueppell

27 Aug 2024

PONE-D-24-20571R2

PLOS ONE

Dear Dr. Leonhardt,

I'm pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been deemed suitable for publication in PLOS ONE. Congratulations! Your manuscript is now being handed over to our production team.

At this stage, our production department will prepare your paper for publication. This includes ensuring the following:

* All references, tables, and figures are properly cited

* All relevant supporting information is included in the manuscript submission,

* There are no issues that prevent the paper from being properly typeset

If revisions are needed, the production department will contact you directly to resolve them. If no revisions are needed, you will receive an email when the publication date has been set. At this time, we do not offer pre-publication proofs to authors during production of the accepted work. Please keep in mind that we are working through a large volume of accepted articles, so please give us a few weeks to review your paper and let you know the next and final steps.

Lastly, if your institution or institutions have a press office, please let them know about your upcoming paper now to help maximize its impact. If they'll be preparing press materials, please inform our press team within the next 48 hours. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information, please contact onepress@plos.org.

If we can help with anything else, please email us at customercare@plos.org.

Thank you for submitting your work to PLOS ONE and supporting open access.

Kind regards,

PLOS ONE Editorial Office Staff

on behalf of

Dr. Olav Rueppell

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Associated Data

    This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

    Supplementary Materials

    S1 Text. FA analysis.

    Analysis of FA contents of honey bee collected pollen using gas chromatography- mass spectrometry.

    (DOCX)

    pone.0309789.s001.docx (15.7KB, docx)
    S1 Table. Differences in pollen consumption and FA consumption.

    Results of TukeyHSD post-hoc test analyzing differences in pollen (white background) and FA consumption (grey background) between diets enriched with low, medium and high FA concentrations for the first seven days.

    (DOCX)

    pone.0309789.s002.docx (16.8KB, docx)
    S2 Table. Results of the chemotactile PER conditioning experiments.

    All FAs were solved 1:1000 in chloroform. Each stimulus was tested as CS+ as well as CS- to test whether the rewarded stimulus influenced learning performance. If this was the case, data was not pooled but analyzed separately, otherwise it was pooled. To test if the bees can differentiate between two stimuli a paired Wilcoxon signed-rank test with continuity correction was used. N represents the number of bees tested per stimulus pair.

    (DOCX)

    pone.0309789.s003.docx (16.8KB, docx)
    S1 Fig. Pollen and FA consumption during the second week.

    Mean consumption of (A) pollen and (B) FAs per individual and day during day seven to 14. Bees were fed with four different pollen diets differing in FA content (i.e. pure honey bee collected pollen as a control and the same pollen enriched with a FA-mix to achieve low FA pollen (1.5 times higher FA concentration), medium FA pollen (5 times higher FA concentration) and high FA pollen (10 times higher FA concentration)). Different letters above boxplots indicate significant differences in the mean consumption of pollen.

    (TIF)

    pone.0309789.s004.tif (52.7KB, tif)
    S2 Fig. Pollen and FA consumption during the third week.

    Mean consumption of (A) pollen and (B) FAs per individual and day during day 15 to 21. Bees were fed with four different pollen diets differing in FA content (i.e. pure honey bee collected pollen as a control and the same pollen enriched with a FA-mix to achieve low FA pollen (1.5 times higher FA concentration), medium FA pollen (5 times higher FA concentration) and high FA pollen (10 times higher FA concentration)). Different letters above boxplots indicate significant differences in the mean consumption of pollen.

    (TIF)

    pone.0309789.s005.tif (52KB, tif)
    S3 Fig. Percentage of A. mellifera workers that responded with a PER when capric acid was tested against omega-3.

    (A) Omega-3 was used as the rewarded stimulus and capric acid as the unrewarded stimulus. (B) Capric acid was used as rewarded stimulus and omega-3 as unrewarded stimulus. CS+ (black, squares) represents the rewarded stimulus and CS- (grey, circles) the unrewarded stimulus. N represents the number of individuals tested. Statistical differences are marked with different letters at the right side of the curves (P < 0.05).

    (TIF)

    pone.0309789.s006.tif (46.5KB, tif)
    Attachment

    Submitted filename: Response to Reviewers_final.docx

    pone.0309789.s007.docx (26.5KB, docx)
    Attachment

    Submitted filename: Response to Editor_2nd revision.docx

    pone.0309789.s008.docx (17.5KB, docx)

    Data Availability Statement

    All relevant data is available on https://doi.org/10.17605/osf.io/geuzv


    Articles from PLOS ONE are provided here courtesy of PLOS

    RESOURCES