This cohort study evaluates sleep, well-being, and cognition between interns working irregular, extended shifts and those working a more regular schedule with restricted hours.
Key Points
Question
Is a night float schedule with consecutive 12-hour night shifts associated with reduced burden on intern sleep, well-being, and performance compared with a 24-hour overnight call schedule?
Findings
In this cohort study of 96 participants, tracking across 8 weeks showed more regular and better-quality sleep on a float schedule than on a call schedule. A 24-hour overnight call was adversely associated with mood, motivation, sleepiness, and impaired cognition; participants on the float schedule did not have these associations, and naps benefitted vigilance in both schedules.
Meaning
These findings suggest a float schedule may have fewer negative outcomes for sleep, well-being, and cognition than a call schedule, and that naps during night work may be beneficial.
Abstract
Importance
Extended work hours and night shifts are essential in health care, but negatively affect physician sleep, well-being, and patient care. Alternative schedules with shorter work hours and/or reduced irregularity might mitigate these issues.
Objective
To compare sleep, well-being, and cognition between interns working irregular, extended shifts (call schedule), and those working a more regular schedule with restricted hours (float schedule).
Design, Setting, and Participants
In this observational longitudinal cohort study, interns in a Singapore-based teaching hospital were studied for 8 weeks from January 2022 to July 2023. Data were analyzed from July 2023 to July 2024.
Exposure
Participants worked either regular approximately 10-hour workdays, interspersed with 24 hour or more overnight calls 4 to 5 times a month, or a float schedule, which included regular approximately 10-hour workdays, and 5 to 7 consecutive approximately 12-hour night shifts every 2 months. Exposure was based on departmental training and operational needs.
Main Outcomes and Measures
Sleep was measured with wearable sleep trackers and an electronic diary. Day-to-day well-being and cognitive assessments were collected through a smartphone application. Assessments included the Sleep Regularity Index (SRI; determines the probability of an individual being in the same state [sleep or wake] at any 2 time points 24 hours apart, with 0 indicating highly random sleep patterns and 100 denoting perfect regularity) and Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Inventory (PSQI; scores ranges from 0 to 21, with higher scores indicating poorer sleep; a score greater than 5 suggests significant sleep difficulties).
Results
Participants (mean [SD] age, 24.7 [1.1] years; 57 female participants [59.4%]; 41 on call schedule [42.7%]; 55 on float schedule [57.3%]) provided 4808 nights of sleep (84.2%) and 3390 days (59.3%) of well-being and cognition assessments. Participants on a float schedule had higher SRI scores (mean [SD] score, 69.4 [6.16]) and had better quality sleep (PSQI mean [SD] score, 5.4 [2.3]), than participants on call schedules (SRI mean [SD] score, 56.1 [11.3]; t91 = 6.81; mean difference, 13.3; 95% CI, 9.40 to 17.22; P < .001; PSQI mean [SD] score, 6.5 [2.3]; t79 = 2.16; 95% CI, 0.09 to 2.15; P = .03). Overnight call shifts, but not night float shifts, were associated with poorer mood (−13%; β = −6.79; 95% CI, −9.32 to −4.27; P < .001), motivation (−21%; β = −10.09; 95% CI, −12.55 to −7.63; P < .001), and sleepiness ratings (29%; β = 15.96; 95% CI, 13.01 to 18.90; P < .001) and impaired vigilance (21 ms slower; β = 20.68; 95% CI, 15.89 to 25.47; P < .001) compared with regular day shifts. Night shifts with naps were associated with better vigilance (16 ms faster; β = −15.72; 95% CI, −28.27 to −3.17; P = .01) than nights without naps.
Conclusions and relevance
In this cohort study, 24-hour call schedules were associated with poorer sleep, well-being, and cognition outcomes than float schedules. Naps during night shifts benefited vigilance in both schedules.
Introduction
Extended work hours and night shifts disrupt sleep and circadian rhythms1,2 but are essential for the continuity of high-quality health care. The first year of postgraduate medical training engages physicians in long duty hours and overnight shifts that negatively impact sleep and neurocognitive function, as well as physical and mental health.3,4,5,6,7,8,9,10 To reduce physician workload while preserving educational and patient care goals, alternative schedules have been designed.11,12,13,14,15,16
On a traditional call schedule, interns work 24-hour or longer shifts (overnight call) in addition to regular daytime work. A popular alternative is the float schedule, where multiple consecutive night shifts are strung together (night float week), with no daytime work (maximum 16 hours continuous shift). This allows physicians to rest between night shifts and to adapt their sleep schedules over consecutive nights.
Following duty hour reforms, many US hospitals have adopted restricted duty hour shifts (16-hour shifts) for interns.15 However, equivocal data on their effectiveness have led the Accreditation Council for Graduate Medical Education to reverse an earlier mandate and allow for continuous shifts of a maximum of 24 hours.16,17,18,19 Moreover, 24-hour shifts are common in many countries worldwide.20
Detailed studies are needed to inform the strategic implementation of such schedules. However, continuous objective monitoring of sleep and performance is difficult to achieve, given the demanding schedules and high workload.21 Advances in wearable health tracking and smartphone-based ecological momentary assessments (EMA) have made the long-term intensive monitoring of sleep, cognitive performance, and mental well-being more feasible.22,23,24,25,26 Here, we monitored first year postgraduate (PGY1) physicians through 8 weeks of either a traditional call schedule or a float schedule using a wearable sleep tracker and smartphone-based EMAs.
The objectives of this study were to compare sleep (duration, regularity, quality, and sleepiness), well-being (mood, motivation, and stress), and cognitive performance (speed of processing, working memory, and vigilance) between interns who worked on a call schedule vs those who worked on a float schedule. We hypothesized that more beneficial outcomes would be observed for the float group compared with the call group in all these domains. Importantly, the daily sampling of these outcomes over 8 weeks per participant allowed us to compare night shifts (overnight call or night float shifts) with each person’s baseline on regular day shifts.
Methods
Recruitment
Incoming interns were recruited from a large training hospital in Singapore (National University Hospital) from departments that followed either a traditional call schedule (Paediatrics, Obstetrics, or Gynaecology) or a float schedule (Internal Medicine, General Surgery, or Orthopaedics). Participants were assigned to departments before recruitment in our study. Assignment was established by a centrally coordinated national agency based on indicated preference, staffing requirements, and availability of training positions. Each department followed only 1 schedule type based on department-level operational considerations, such as staffing and work requirements.
All procedures were approved by the National University of Singapore institutional review board and all participants signed informed consent before data collection. Out of a total of 326 interns, 98 consented to study participation. Two participants who initially consented withdrew before data collection. This report follows the Strengthening the Reporting of Observational Studies in Epidemiology (STROBE) reporting guidelines for cohort studies.
Shift Schedules: Call vs Float Schedules
Participants were recruited from departments that followed either a traditional call schedule (from here onwards termed the call group) or a float schedule (float group). In the call group, interns worked from 7 am to 5 pm on most days.21 For 5 to 7 days a month, they performed an overnight call shift that commenced at 7 am and ended between 8 am to 1 pm the following day (with the rest of the day off). In the float group, interns worked from 7 am to 5 pm every day for most weeks. During designated periods occurring once every 2 to 3 months (night float week), they worked for 5 to 7 consecutive nights (night float shift) from 8 pm to 8 am.
Data Collection Protocol
Upon study enrollment, participants provided demographic data (sex, age, ethnicity, height, weight, and body mass index [BMI]) through self report. Ethnicity data was collected as part of standard practice in Singapore (categories: Chinese, Malay, Indian, or Other). Over 8 weeks, participants recorded their sleep and physical activity using an Oura Ring 3 wearable device (Oura Health). Additionally, they completed daily well-being and cognitive assessments on an EMA smartphone application (Z4IP, Sleep and Cognition Laboratory) and reported their daily activities in an electronic time-use diary embedded in this application. EMA sessions were completed once between 5 am and 10 pm, to allow for completion on irregular work schedules. Data collection through wearable and phone-based methods allowed for high-resolution sampling of passive and active measures, while minimizing the participant’s burden. In cases where no night float shifts occurred within the 8 weeks (eg, due to rescheduling of the night float week), the data collection period was extended. Participants were financially remunerated for participation (up to $230 depending on completion rates).
Sleep Measurement
For high-fidelity assessment of sleep, we integrated wearable-derived sleep and physical activity measurements with electronic time-use diary input (Figure 1). This enabled us to (1) impute missing data from wearable or e-diary channels, and (2) arbitrate when there was discrepancy between different channel outputs, such as detecting short naps not detected by the wearable.27 Details of this procedure are described in eFigure 1 in Supplement 1. Briefly, an automated pipeline was created that calculated the proportion of time spent asleep in 15-minute time windows for each channel (wearable device sleep, wearable device activity, and sleep diary). These proportions were combined into a sleep probability score (ranging from 0 = likely wake to 1 = likely sleep), where the contribution of each channel was weighted by its correlation with the other channels. For each day, sleep duration was computed separately for nocturnal (8 pm to 8 am) and daytime periods (8 am to 8 pm). Only periods with at least 75% data were included for analyses. Furthermore, the Sleep Regularity Index (SRI)28 was calculated, which determines the probability of an individual being in the same state (sleep or wake) at any 2 time points 24 hours apart. A score of 0 indicates highly random sleep patterns, while 100 denotes perfect regularity.
Daily Cognitive and Well-Being Assessment
Each day, participants completed a short cognitive and well-being assessment through an EMA mobile application (details in eFigure 2 in Supplement 1). In brief, each session comprised 3 short cognitive tasks evaluating speed of processing (symbol search task), working memory (dot-memory task), and vigilance (abbreviated psychomotor vigilance test [PVT-B]).29 Following this, participants filled in their bedtime, wake time, and sleep quality (5-point scale with 1 indicating very poor and 5 indicating very good) for the preceding sleep period. They also indicated their current mood (sliding scale with 0 indicating negative and 100 indicating positive), sleepiness, stress, motivation, and loneliness (sliding scale with 0 indicating not at all and 100 indicating very much) levels.
10-Minute PVT
To validate the EMA-delivered 3-minute PVT, a subset of participants additionally completed the more established 10-minute PVT on a laptop computer.30 Participants completed 3 PVT sessions on days after their night shift (between 8 am and 2 pm; before recovery sleep), and 3 control sessions after a full night of sleep. The median of the individual reaction time distribution (medRT), attentional lapses (responses >500ms), and false starts (responses before target onset or <150 after target onset) were extracted as outcome metrics.
Statistical Analysis
The analysis specifically focused on comparing sleep, well-being, and cognitive performance between the different schedule groups (call group vs float group), on the different shift events during their posting (regular day shifts vs night shifts [ie, overnight call shift for the call group or night float shift for the float group]). Linear mixed models (LMM) were constructed with the factors group (call or float), shift (regular day shift or night shift), and their interaction. Where a significant group × shift interaction was found, pairwise comparisons were calculated to estimate the difference between groups for each regular shift and night shift separately. All models controlled for participant demographics (age, gender, and BMI), and for any changes over the study period (day-of-study). To control for various possible influencing factors, a set of control analyses were performed. In control analysis 1, the variable timing of the well-being and cognitive assessments was accounted for. Control models were constructed with total sleep duration in the prior 24 hours, and time awake since the last sleep episode as additional control variables. Control analysis 2 examined whether outcomes associated with well-being and/or cognition were present before the start of a work shift, or whether they developed over the course of the shift by selecting EMA sessions completed before the work shift (preshift) or after work shift (postshift). Separate LMMs were constructed with group (call or float), shift (regular days shift or night shift), and timing (preshift or postshift), while controlling for demographics and day-of-study. Control analysis 3 specifically assessed whether the outcomes associated with night shifts could be mitigated when interns had an opportunity to sleep during their night shift. Postshift assessments for night shifts with and without an on-shift nap were compared. Lastly, performance on the 10-minute PVT was examined with an LMM using group (call, float) and day (control, night shift) as factors, and controlling for demographics and day-of-study. All analyses were conducted in SPSS version 29.0.1.0(171) (IBM Corp). Statistical tests were corrected for multiple comparisons using the Benjamini-Hochberg method,31 with corrected significance threshold set to a 2-sided α = .05. Data were analyzed from July 2023 to July 2024.
Results
Sample Characteristics
Demographics and baseline characteristics for the 96 participants (mean [SD] age, 24.7 [1.1] years; 57 female participants [59.4%]) are displayed in Table 1. Forty-one participants were on a call schedule and 55 participants were on a float schedule. The call and float groups did not significantly differ in age, ethnicity, and BMI. However, there were more female participants in the call group (31 participants [75.6%]) than in the float group (26 participants [47.3%]). The 2 groups had similar sleep quality, chronotype, and self-rated depression and burnout measures at study commencement. Over the study period, participants on a call schedule reported a mean (SD) of 64.4 (8.52) work hours per week, with a mean (SD) of 8.6 (2.0) night shifts (overnight call shifts). Participants on a float schedule, over their observed period, reported a comparable number of working hours per week (mean [SD], 64.2 [8.06] hours), with about 1 night float week during the study period (mean [SD], 6.2 [2.3] night shifts per night float week). For 10 participants from the float group, no night float week had occurred within the original study period, and data collection was extended (mean [SD], 2.7 [1.9] weeks extension).
Table 1. Sample Demographics and Baseline Characteristics for the Night Call and Float Groups.
Characteristic | Participants, No (%) | t Value, U-statistic, or χ2 | ||
---|---|---|---|---|
Overall | Call group | Float group | ||
Age, median (IQR), y | 24.3 (24.0-25.1) | 24.3 (23.9-25.0) | 24.3 (24.0-25.4) | U = 1079 |
Sex | χ2 = 7.82a | |||
Female | 57 (59.4) | 31 (75.6) | 26 (47.3) | |
Male | 39 (40.6) | 10 (24.4) | 29 (52.7) | |
Ethnicity | χ2 = 2.86 | |||
Chinese | 88 (91.7) | 38 (92.7) | 50 (90.9) | |
Non-Chinese | 8 (8.3) | 3 (7.3) | 5 (9.1) | |
Body mass index, median (IQR)b | 20.8 (19.2-22.8) | 20.8 (19.2-21.9) | 21.0 (19.2-23.1) | U = 1032 |
Baseline characteristics | ||||
Sleep quality (PSQI), mean (SD) | 5.8 (2.3) | 5.9 (2.4) | 5.8 (2.3) | t = 0.24 |
Chronotype (MEQ), mean (SD) | 49.4 (7.6) | 48.6 (6.6) | 49.9 (8.2) | t = 0.76 |
Depression (BDI), median (IQR) | 10.0 (5.0-16.8) | 10.0 (5.0-14.0) | 11.0 (5.3-18.0) | U = 843 |
Burnout (OLBI), mean (SD) | 43.1 (6.8) | 42.3 (6.1) | 43.6 (7.2) | t = 0.92 |
Abbreviations: BDI, Beck Depression Inventory; MEQ, Morningness-Eveningness Questionnaire; OLBI, Oldenburg Burnout Inventory; PSQI, Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Inventory.
P < .01.
Calculated as weight in kilograms divided by height in meters squared.
Sleep Outcomes
A total of 4808 days (84.2%) of sleep data met analysis criteria (mean [SD], 50.05 [12.54] days/person). Figure 1A shows example sleep profiles from 2 participants (float vs call schedule). Figure 1B shows the mean sleep probability for regular day shifts (starting from the night preceding the workday) and for night shifts. On regular days, a consolidated bout of nocturnal sleep was observed in both groups, while on night shift days, distinct bouts of nocturnal sleep (during the night shift hours) and recovery sleep (in the daytime after the night shift) were observed. LMMs were used to compare sleep duration between groups (call or float) and shifts (regular days or night shift).
Sleep Duration and Regularity
Nocturnal sleep duration showed a group × shift interaction (β = 48.11; 95% CI, 35.67-60.54; P < .001) (Figure 2A). On regular days, participants in the call group had a mean of 16.98 minutes longer nocturnal sleep (mean [SE], 6.6 hours [6.32 minutes]) than the float group (mean [SE], 6.34 hours [5.04 minutes]; β = 16.98; 95% CI, 0.83 to 33.13; P = .04), with no differences for daytime sleep (call group mean [SE], 7.13 [3.30] minutes; float group mean [SE], 5.02 [2.57] minutes; β = 2.11; 95% CI, −6.22 to 10.45; P = .62). On night shifts, nocturnal sleep indicated naps taken during the work shift. The call group obtained longer naps during their night shift (mean [SE], 2.08 hours [7.06 minutes]) than the float group (mean [SE], 59.78 [6.17] minutes; β = 65.09; 95% CI, 46.51 to 83. 67; P < .001). In contrast, during the daytime following night shifts, the float group obtained more sleep (mean [SE], 5.16 hours [3.92 minutes]) than the call group (mean [SE], 4.22 hours [4.27 minutes]; β = 56.20; 95% CI, 44.79 to 67.62; P < .001). Across all qualifying 24-hour periods analyzed, interns in the float group had significantly higher sleep regularity (SRI mean [SD], 69.4 [6.16]) (Figure 2B) than the call group (mean [SD], 56.1 [11.3]; t91 = 6.81; P < .001).
Questionnaires
Participants in the call group reported poorer subjective sleep quality (PSQI mean [SD], 6.5 [2.3]) compared with the float group (mean [SD], 5.4 [2.3]; t = 2.16; P = .03) (Figure 2B). However, no significant differences in depression, burnout, or shift work–related insomnia were reported 8 weeks after living under these schedules (Table 2).
Table 2. Well-Being Questionnaires at Study Exit (After 8 Weeks of Night Call and Night Float Schedule Work).
Questionnaire | Mean (SD) | t Value | P value | |
---|---|---|---|---|
Call group | Float group | |||
Sleep quality (PSQI) | 6.5 (2.3) | 5.4 (2.3) | 2.16 | .03 |
Depression (BDI) | 10.5 (8.1) | 10.6 (10.1) | 0.06 | .96 |
Burnout (OLBI) | 41.0 (6.5) | 42.4 (7.1) | 0.87 | .20 |
Bergen Shift Work Sleep Questionnaire | ||||
Day shifts | 10.70 (3.37) | 9.63 (3.21) | −1.45 | .15 |
Night shifts | 12.10 (3.91) | 11.13 (4.28) | −0.99 | .32 |
Rest days | 7.00 (3.43) | 5.67 (2.72) | −1.91 | .06 |
Abbreviations: BDI, Beck Depression Inventory; OLBI, Oldenburg Burnout Inventory; PSQI, Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Inventory.
Daily Well-Being and Cognitive Assessments
Linear mixed models yielded significant group × shift interactions for self-reported sleep quality (Figure 3) (β = −0.89; 95% CI, −1.05 to −0.73), sleepiness (β = 14.82; 95% CI, 10.96 to 18.69), mood (β = −6.56; 95% CI, −9.88 to −3.25), and motivation (β = −8.31; 95% CI, −11.54 to −5.08). Pairwise-comparisons showed that following an overnight call shift, participants reported poorer sleep quality (β = −0.98; 95% CI, −1.11 to −0.86; P < .001) (eTable 1 and eTable 2 in Supplement 1), increased sleepiness (β = 15.96; 95% CI, 13.01 to 18.90; P < .001), poorer mood (β = −6.79; 95% CI, −9.32 to −4.27; P < .001) (eTable 3 and eTable 4 in Supplement 1), and lower motivation (β = −10.09; 95% CI, −12.55 to −7.63; P < .001) relative to their regular day shifts. For float participants, these ratings were not significantly different between regular day shifts and night float shifts (sleep quality: β = −0.094; 95% CI, −0.19 to 0.003; P = .06; sleepiness: β = 1.1; 95% CI, −1.38 to 3.65; P = .38; mood: β = −0.23; 95% CI, −2.39 to 1.93; P = .83; motivation: β = −1.8; 95% CI, −3.88 to −0.32; P = .10). No significant interactions were found for stress and loneliness ratings (eTable 5 and eTable 6 in Supplement 1).
For cognitive performance, significant group × shift interactions were found for the working memory (β = −1.42; 95% CI, −2.52 to −0.31) and vigilance tasks (β = 21.01; 95% CI, 14.80 to 27.21). For both tasks, performance was worse after the overnight call shift compared with regular days for the call group (working memory: β = −1.11; 95% CI, −1.95 to −0.26; P = .01; PVT: β = 20.68; 95% CI, 15.89 to 25.47; P < .001) but not for the float group (night float shift vs regular days nonsignificant). No significant main effects of group or shift were found for the speed of processing tasks (eTable 7 and eTable8 in Supplement 1).
Control Analyses
As EMA reports could be made from 5 am to 10 pm, to allow for completion on irregular work schedules, 2 sets of control analyses were performed (eFigure 3 in Supplement 1). First, we ran a set of linear mixed models with total sleep duration in the prior 24 hours and time awake since the last sleep episode as control variables. Importantly, while prior sleep duration explained some variance in sleep ratings (sleep quality and sleepiness), well-being (mood and motivation), and cognition (working memory and vigilance), the negative outcomes of night shifts on the call group (but not float group) remained significant (eTable 1, eTable 2, eTable 3, eTable 4, eTable 5, eTable 6, eTable 7, and eTable 8 in Supplement 1).
Second, we performed LMM analyses comparing the EMA sessions that were completed before starting the work shift, with sessions completed at the end of the work shift. These analyses showed that for most measures, detrimental outcomes were particularly observed at the end of night shifts (eTable 9, eTable 10, eTable 11, eFigure 4, and eFigure 5 in Supplement 1). Following up on this, the outcome of naps taken during night shifts on vigilance was examined by comparing night shifts that included a nap vs those that did not include a nap. Results showed that vigilance performance was better after a night shift with a nap (mean [SE] medRT, 309.1 [4.94] ms) than after a night shift without a nap (mean [SE] medRT = 324.8 [6.76] ms) in both groups (nap main effect: β = −15.72; 95% CI, −28.27 to −3.17; P = .01 with no nap × group interaction: β = 12.16; 95% CI, −12.91 to 37.22; P = .34) (eTable 12 and eFigure 6 in Supplement 1).
10-Minute Psychomotor Vigilance Task
Seventy-three participants (34 in the call group and 39 in the float group) additionally performed a standardized 10-minute PVT30 (3 times after a full night of sleep [control days] and 3 times after a night shift). Both groups had poorer performance after their night duties compared with control days. However, this impairment was larger in the call group than in the float group (group × shift interaction: lapses, β = −5.56; 95% CI, −8.60 to −2.51; median RT: β = −40.92; 95% CI, −77.30 to −4.53; false starts: β = −1.29; 95% CI, −2.24 to −0.33) (eTable 13 and eFigure 7 in Supplement 1).
Discussion
Over 8 weeks, the float schedule was associated with more regular sleep patterns and better self-reported sleep quality compared with the call schedule. Additionally, day-to-day mental well-being and cognitive assessments were relatively unperturbed after night float shifts, while 24-hour overnight call shifts were associated with poorer sleep and well-being ratings and impaired cognitive performance (poorer working memory and vigilance).
The call and float schedules differed in several key aspects. First, the call schedule required interns to work extended hours (≥24 hours vs 12 hours on night float) with associated accumulation of sleep deprivation and fatigue. Second, it involved frequent rotations from day to night shifts (ie, 8 to 10 shifts over the 8-week study period). In the float schedule, night shifts were concentrated in a week occurring once or at most twice over the same period, while the majority of weeks consisted of regular day shifts.
Although the call group in our study had equal or longer sleep duration compared with the float group, their sleep schedule was characterized by higher sleep irregularity. Sleep irregularity is increasingly recognized as a risk factor for health and well-being.32,33 In the short term, sleep irregularity has been associated with mental health issues in the general population,34,35,36 as well as in physicians.22 Longer term, high sleep irregularity raises the risk for cardiovascular disease, hypertension, and premature death37,38 more than short sleep duration.39
Accordingly, daily ratings of mood, motivation, and sleepiness were worse after an overnight call shift. This triad has been associated with an individual’s readiness to perform in relation to the prior night’s sleep duration40,41 and could affect learning and interpersonal interactions. Relatedly, physician empathy has a positive influence on patient perceptions about the professionalism of their care,42 and it is diminished by fatigue. Reduction of empathy in association with night work could also alter therapeutic decision-making; for example, being less willing to prescribe analgesics to alleviate pain.43
Objective assessment of working memory and vigilance were also relatively poorer after overnight calls, confirming prior findings.4,8,10 Importantly, impaired working memory has been associated with medical errors in the face of sleep loss.44 Impaired vigilance relates to missing critical signals45 and involuntarily falling asleep.46 Given that the greatest impairments were found at the end of night shifts, this may not only relate to on-duty functioning, but may also confer a risk immediately postduty, affecting interns who may have to drive home.47,48 As supported by our data, on-shift napping may mitigate the vigilance deficits associated with night shifts.49,50
Patient outcomes and educational outcomes were not collected in this study. It has been suggested that long duty hours may allow for more professional education and decrease adverse patient outcomes arising from more frequent hand offs.51 Against these concerns, a recent study involving PGY1 interns in 2 training hospitals in Singapore examined official records of educational outcomes and medical errors. No significant differences in learning attainment or patient safety were found.52 A separate nationwide survey study reported that 78.9% of interns found a float schedule less disruptive to learning practice and 86.9% felt that a float schedule helped to reduce medical errors.53
Strengths and Limitations
The current study has several strengths. Objectively assessing sleep by combining wearable and smartphone-based sources of sleep measurement reduced the impact of missing data. Also, as wearable sleep measurement algorithms have not been designed for persons with irregular sleep schedules, this approach provided superior sleep assessments by complementing the strengths of different sources of sleep information.27,54,55,56,57
The regular sampling of mental well-being, and cognitive performance outcomes throughout the interns’ different shift events is unprecedented in participants on busy schedules, such as medical interns. Evaluation over 8 weeks provided a representative depiction of interns’ day-to-day experiences across a mixture of regular and night-shift nights. While only first-year interns were included in this study, it could be expected that the effects of the float and call schedules on sleep, well-being and cognitive performance extend to other junior physicians working similar schedules.
The most notable limitation of this study is the observational nature of the design. Exposure conditions were not randomly assigned but were determined by the departments to which participants were allocated. The findings should be considered associations. Additional limitations include the inherently different work requirements between departments (for example, pediatrics vs surgery), although both exposure groups were composed of multiple specialties. Moreover, the day-to-day sampling allowed us to examine the within-participants outcomes of night shifts vs regular day shifts, with the individual participants serving as their own baseline. Second, participants who joined the study were possibly more motivated or more able to cope with the work pressures than those who did not sign up or those who adhere to the study procedures. The flexible timing and unsupervised nature of the EMA assessments may also have introduced additional variance. Third, light exposure, whose timing and intensity could influence circadian timing and outcomes of interest, was not measured. While it might not be possible to control all influential factors in field studies, the outcomes of timing were well accounted for in control analyses.
Conclusions
In this cohort study, a float schedule, where physicians worked 12-hour night shifts concentrated into 5 to 7 consecutive shifts, seemed to offer benefits over a traditional call schedule for physician sleep regularity, mood, and cognitive performance. Vigilance after night shifts, irrespective of schedule, was higher when naps were taken.
References
- 1.James SM, Honn KA, Gaddameedhi S, Van Dongen HPA. Shift work: disrupted circadian rhythms and sleep-implications for health and well-being. Curr Sleep Med Rep. 2017;3(2):104-112. doi: 10.1007/s40675-017-0071-6 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Kecklund G, Axelsson J. Health consequences of shift work and insufficient sleep. BMJ. 2019;355:i5210. doi: 10.1136/bmj.i5210 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Barger LK, Sullivan JP, Blackwell T, et al. ; ROSTERS Study Group . Effects on resident work hours, sleep duration, and work experience in a randomized order safety trial evaluating resident-physician schedules (ROSTERS). Sleep. 2019;42(8):zsz110. doi: 10.1093/sleep/zsz110 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Basner M, Dinges DF, Shea JA, et al. Sleep and alertness in medical interns and residents: an observational study on the role of extended shifts. Sleep. 2017;40(4):zsx027. doi: 10.1093/sleep/zsx027 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Chellappa SL, Morris CJ, Scheer FAJL. Effects of circadian misalignment on cognition in chronic shift workers. Sci Rep. 2019;9(1):699. doi: 10.1038/s41598-018-36762-w [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Drake CL, Roehrs T, Richardson G, Walsh JK, Roth T. Shift work sleep disorder: prevalence and consequences beyond that of symptomatic day workers. Sleep. 2004;27(8):1453-1462. doi: 10.1093/sleep/27.8.1453 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Mata DA, Ramos MA, Bansal N, et al. Prevalence of depression and depressive symptoms among resident physicians: a systematic review and meta-analysis. JAMA. 2015;314(22):2373-2383. doi: 10.1001/jama.2015.15845 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Rahman SA, Sullivan JP, Barger LK, et al. ; ROSTERS STUDY GROUP . Extended work shifts and neurobehavioral performance in resident-physicians. Pediatrics. 2021;147(3):e2020009936. doi: 10.1542/peds.2020-009936 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Sen S, Kranzler HR, Krystal JH, et al. A prospective cohort study investigating factors associated with depression during medical internship. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2010;67(6):557-565. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2010.41 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Anderson C, Sullivan JP, Flynn-Evans EE, Cade BE, Czeisler CA, Lockley SW. Deterioration of neurobehavioral performance in resident physicians during repeated exposure to extended duration work shifts. Sleep. 2012;35(8):1137-1146. doi: 10.5665/sleep.2004 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Philibert I, Friedmann P, Williams WT; ACGME Work Group on Resident Duty Hours. Accreditation Council for Graduate Medical Education . New requirements for resident duty hours. JAMA. 2002;288(9):1112-1114. doi: 10.1001/jama.288.9.1112 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Landrigan CP, Rothschild JM, Cronin JW, et al. Effect of reducing interns’ work hours on serious medical errors in intensive care units. N Engl J Med. 2004;351(18):1838-1848. doi: 10.1056/NEJMoa041406 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Lockley SW, Cronin JW, Evans EE, et al. ; Harvard Work Hours, Health and Safety Group . Effect of reducing interns’ weekly work hours on sleep and attentional failures. N Engl J Med. 2004;351(18):1829-1837. doi: 10.1056/NEJMoa041404 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Landrigan CP, Rahman SA, Sullivan JP, et al. ; ROSTERS Study Group . Effect on patient safety of a resident physician schedule without 24-hour shifts. N Engl J Med. 2020;382(26):2514-2523. doi: 10.1056/NEJMoa1900669 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Pilibert I, Amis S, eds. The ACGME 2011 Duty Hour Standards: Enhancing Quality of Care, Supervision, and Resident Professional Development. Accreditation Council for Graduate Medical Education. ACGME; 2011. [Google Scholar]
- 16.Burchiel KJ, Zetterman RK, Ludmerer KM, et al. The 2017 ACGME common work hour standards: promoting physician learning and professional development in a safe, humane environment. J Grad Med Educ. 2017;9(6):692-696. doi: 10.4300/JGME-D-17-00317.1 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Bolster L, Rourke L. The effect of restricting residents’ duty hours on patient safety, resident well-being, and resident education: an updated systematic review. J Grad Med Educ. 2015;7(3):349-363. doi: 10.4300/JGME-D-14-00612.1 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Reed DA, Fletcher KE, Arora VM. Systematic review: association of shift length, protected sleep time, and night float with patient care, residents’ health, and education. Ann Intern Med. 2010;153(12):829-842. doi: 10.7326/0003-4819-153-12-201012210-00010 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Fletcher KE, Underwood W III, Davis SQ, Mangrulkar RS, McMahon LF Jr, Saint S. Effects of work hour reduction on residents’ lives: a systematic review. JAMA. 2005;294(9):1088-1100. doi: 10.1001/jama.294.9.1088 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Maoz Breuer R, Waitzberg R, Breuer A, et al. Work like a Doc: A comparison of regulations on residents’ working hours in 14 high-income countries. Health Policy. 2023;130:104753. doi: 10.1016/j.healthpol.2023.104753 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Low J, Tan M, See K, Aw M. Sleep, activity and fatigue reported by postgraduate year 1 residents: a prospective cohort study comparing the effects of night float versus the traditional overnight on-call system. SMJ. 2018;59(12):652-655. doi: 10.11622/smedj.2018036 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Fang Y, Forger DB, Frank E, Sen S, Goldstein C. Day-to-day variability in sleep parameters and depression risk: a prospective cohort study of training physicians. NPJ Digit Med. 2021;4(1):28. doi: 10.1038/s41746-021-00400-z [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Kalmbach DA, Fang Y, Arnedt JT, et al. Effects of sleep, physical activity, and shift work on daily mood: a prospective mobile monitoring study of medical interns. J Gen Intern Med. 2018;33(6):914-920. doi: 10.1007/s11606-018-4373-2 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Massar SAA, Ng ASC, Soon CS, et al. Reopening after lockdown: the influence of working-from-home and digital device use on sleep, physical activity, and wellbeing following COVID-19 lockdown and reopening. Sleep. 2022;45(1):zsab250. doi: 10.1093/sleep/zsab250 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Wang J, Fang Y, Frank E, et al. Effectiveness of gamified team competition as mHealth intervention for medical interns: a cluster micro-randomized trial. NPJ Digit Med. 2023;6(1):4. doi: 10.1038/s41746-022-00746-y [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Lau T, Ong JL, Ng BKL, et al. Minimum number of nights for reliable estimation of habitual sleep using a consumer sleep tracker. Sleep Adv. 2022;3(1):zpac026. doi: 10.1093/sleepadvances/zpac026 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Chinoy ED, Cuellar JA, Jameson JT, Markwald RR. Daytime sleep-tracking performance of four commercial wearable devices during unrestricted home sleep. Nat Sci Sleep. 2023;15:151-164. doi: 10.2147/NSS.S395732 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Phillips AJK, Clerx WM, O’Brien CS, et al. Irregular sleep/wake patterns are associated with poorer academic performance and delayed circadian and sleep/wake timing. Sci Rep. 2017;7(1):3216. doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-03171-4 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Basner M, Mollicone D, Dinges DF. Validity and sensitivity of a brief psychomotor vigilance test (PVT-B) to total and partial sleep deprivation. Acta Astronaut. 2011;69(11-12):949-959. doi: 10.1016/j.actaastro.2011.07.015 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Dinges DF, Powell JW. Microcomputer analyses of performance on a portable, simple visual RT task during sustained operations. Behav Res Methods Instrum Comput. 1985;17(6):652-655. doi: 10.3758/BF03200977 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Benjamini Y, Hochberg Y. Controlling the false discovery rate: a practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. J R Stat Soc Series B Stat Methodol. 1995;57(1):289-300. doi: 10.1111/j.2517-6161.1995.tb02031.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Sletten TL, Weaver MD, Foster RG, et al. The importance of sleep regularity: a consensus statement of the National Sleep Foundation sleep timing and variability panel. Sleep Health. 2023;9(6):801-820. doi: 10.1016/j.sleh.2023.07.016 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Messman BA, Wiley JF, Feldman E, Dietch JR, Taylor DJ, Slavish DC. Irregular sleep is linked to poorer mental health: a pooled analysis of eight studies. Sleep Health. 2024;10(4):493-499. doi: 10.1016/j.sleh.2024.03.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Pye J, Phillips AJ, Cain SW, et al. Irregular sleep-wake patterns in older adults with current or remitted depression. J Affect Disord. 2021;281:431-437. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2020.12.034 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Mathew GM, Reichenberger DA, Master L, Buxton OM, Chang AM, Hale L. Actigraphic sleep variability is associated with lower positive mood in adolescents. J Adolesc Health. 2023;73(3):478-485. doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2023.04.019 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Burke TA, Hamilton JL, Seigel D, et al. Sleep irregularity and nonsuicidal self-injurious urges and behaviors. Sleep. 2022;45(6):zsac084. doi: 10.1093/sleep/zsac084 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Makarem N, Zuraikat FM, Aggarwal B, Jelic S, St-Onge MP. Variability in sleep patterns: an emerging risk factor for hypertension. Curr Hypertens Rep. 2020;22(2):19. doi: 10.1007/s11906-020-1025-9 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Cribb L, Sha R, Yiallourou S, et al. Sleep regularity and mortality: a prospective analysis in the UK Biobank. Elife. 2023;12:RP88359. doi: 10.7554/eLife.88359 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Windred DP, Burns AC, Lane JM, et al. Sleep regularity is a stronger predictor of mortality risk than sleep duration: a prospective cohort study. Sleep. 2024;47(1):zsad253. doi: 10.1093/sleep/zsad253 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Ng ASC, Massar SAA, Bei B, Chee MWL. Assessing ‘readiness’ by tracking fluctuations in daily sleep duration and their effects on daily mood, motivation, and sleepiness. Sleep Med. 2023;112:30-38. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2023.09.028 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Massar SAA, Lim J, Huettel SA. Sleep deprivation, effort allocation and performance. In: Van Dongen HPA, Floyd ES, Whitney P, eds. Progress in Brain Research. Vol 246. Elsevier; 2019:1-26. doi: 10.1016/bs.pbr.2019.03.007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Licciardone JC, Tran Y, Ngo K, Toledo D, Peddireddy N, Aryal S. Physician empathy and chronic pain outcomes. JAMA Netw Open. 2024;7(4):e246026. doi: 10.1001/jamanetworkopen.2024.6026 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Choshen-Hillel S, Sadras I, Gordon-Hecker T, et al. Physicians prescribe fewer analgesics during night shifts than day shifts. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2022;119(27):e2200047119. doi: 10.1073/pnas.2200047119 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Westbrook JI, Raban MZ, Walter SR, Douglas H. Task errors by emergency physicians are associated with interruptions, multitasking, fatigue and working memory capacity: a prospective, direct observation study. BMJ Qual Saf. 2018;27(8):655-663. doi: 10.1136/bmjqs-2017-007333 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Basner M, Rubinstein J. Fitness for duty: a 3-minute version of the Psychomotor Vigilance Test predicts fatigue-related declines in luggage-screening performance. J Occup Environ Med. 2011;53(10):1146-1154. doi: 10.1097/JOM.0b013e31822b8356 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Ong JL, Asplund CL, Chia TTY, Chee MWL. Now you hear me, now you don’t: eyelid closures as an indicator of auditory task disengagement. Sleep. 2013;36(12):1867-1874. doi: 10.5665/sleep.3218 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Mulhall MD, Sletten TL, Magee M, et al. Sleepiness and driving events in shift workers: the impact of circadian and homeostatic factors. Sleep. 2019;42(6):zsz074. doi: 10.1093/sleep/zsz074 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Westley JA, Peterson J, Cook B. Drowsy driving among nurses: potential impetus to support napping. Workplace Health Saf. 2022;70(12):551-555. doi: 10.1177/21650799221111300 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Shea JA, Dinges DF, Small DS, et al. A randomized trial of a three-hour protected nap period in a medicine training program: sleep, alertness, and patient outcomes. Acad Med. 2014;89(3):452-459. doi: 10.1097/ACM.0000000000000144 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Volpp KG, Shea JA, Small DS, et al. Effect of a protected sleep period on hours slept during extended overnight in-hospital duty hours among medical interns: a randomized trial. JAMA. 2012;308(21):2208-2217. doi: 10.1001/jama.2012.34490 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.McGahan BG, Hatef J, Shaikhouni A, et al. Resident night float or 24-hour call hospital coverage: impact on training, patient outcome, and length of stay. J Surg Educ. 2022;79(3):732-739. doi: 10.1016/j.jsurg.2021.11.008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Tan MY, Koh ZJ, Kumar SK, et al. Achieving competency for year 1 doctors in Singapore: comparing night float or traditional call. TAPS. 2024;9(1):36-41. doi: 10.29060/TAPS.2024-9-1/OA3051 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Loo B, Ng C, Chin R, et al. Nationwide survey comparing residents’ perceptions of overnight duty systems in Singapore: night float versus full overnight call. SMJ. 2020;61(10):559-562. doi: 10.11622/smedj.2020149 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Depner CM, Cheng PC, Devine JK, et al. Wearable technologies for developing sleep and circadian biomarkers: a summary of workshop discussions. Sleep. 2020;43(2):zsz254. doi: 10.1093/sleep/zsz254 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.De Zambotti M, Goldstein C, Cook J, et al. State of the science and recommendations for using wearable technology in sleep and circadian research. Sleep. 2024;47(4):zsad325. doi: 10.1093/sleep/zsad325 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Massar SAA, Chua XY, Soon CS, et al. Trait-like nocturnal sleep behavior identified by combining wearable, phone-use, and self-report data. NPJ Digit Med. 2021;4(1):90. doi: 10.1038/s41746-021-00466-9 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Hassinger AB, Kwon M, Wang J, Mishra A, Wilding GE. Pilot study comparing sleep logs to a commercial wearable device in describing the sleep patterns of physicians-in-training. PLoS ONE. 2024;19(7):e0305881. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0305881 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.