Skip to main content
F1000Research logoLink to F1000Research
. 2024 Oct 11;12:446. Originally published 2023 Apr 26. [Version 3] doi: 10.12688/f1000research.131906.3

An Insight into developmental changes in reasoning skills among Indian Preschoolers: A cross-sectional study using a story-based approach

Aparna Prasanna 1,2, Gagan Bajaj 2,a, Malavika Anakkathil Anil 2,3, Jayashree S Bhat 4
PMCID: PMC11582391  PMID: 39584014

Version Changes

Revised. Amendments from Version 2

In this revised version, the discussion and future directions of the study has been revisited with the explanation for the test-retest reliability findings. Additional references quoted are added in the bibliography.

Abstract

Background: Considering the importance of exploring the development of reasoning skills during preschool period and the suitability of using a culturally linguistically relevant story-based approach for the same, the present research intended to profile the reasoning skills in typically developing Indian preschool children between 36 and 72 months using a story-based approach. The specific objectives were to determine the test-retest and inter-rater reliability of reasoning tasks within an existing story-based cognitive-communicative assessment tool and to use this tool to assess the reasoning skills of typically developing Indian preschool children.

Method: Reasoning tasks across explanation, prediction and inference domains were evaluated for its psychometric properties and administered to 63 typically developing Indian preschool children attending English medium schools in Mangalore. The preschoolers were equally divided into three age groups. The responses obtained across the age groups were analyzed quantitatively and qualitatively.

Results: The developed tasks were confirmed to have good psychometric properties like test-retest and inter-rater reliability. The age comparisons of reasoning abilities using one-way ANOVA suggested an increase in reasoning abilities with age during the preschool period. The qualitative analysis further suggested that with increasing age, the nature of reasoning changed from content-based reasoning to reasoning based on prior knowledge which was integrated with the story content.

Conclusion: The study describes reasoning skill development using a story-based task in Indian preschoolers. The study findings further provide clinical and educational implications to assess and foster reasoning abilities among preschoolers.

Keywords: explanation, prediction, inference, reasoning, development, story, preschoolers, India

Introduction

Reasoning is a cognitive process of making inferences, drawing conclusions, or evaluating a proposed conclusion ( Andrews, 2020). Reasoning skills enable individuals to understand and learn about the physical and social environment on a daily basis ( Khemlani, 2018). The ability to appropriately reason across different life situations is essential for a successful professional and everyday life ( Bronkhorst et al., 2020). It was long believed that reasoning skills do not develop until adolescence ( Piaget, 1952). Although preschoolers are known as ‘little scientists’ with abundant curiosity and an innate drive to know the world ( Alvarez & Booth, 2014; Piaget, 1952), reasoning skills are considered to be a higher-level cognitive skill that is beyond the reach of preschoolers ( Whittaker & McMullen, 2014). Thinking abilities among preschoolers have often been described as “egocentric, pre-logical, affective, undifferentiated, pre-causal, personal, vague and unanalysed” ( Piaget, 1952). However, some recent researchers indicate that the development of reasoning skills begins during the preschool years and continues through adolescence ( Amsterlaw, 2006; Gopnik et al., 2004; Legare et al., 2010; Säre, Luik & Fisher, 2016a; Whittaker & McMullen, 2014).

During the preschool period, as children actively explore their environment through social interactions and physical manipulations, they achieve significant language and cognitive developmental milestones. This period marks crucial language developments such as the onset of sentence-level communication with simple, active declarative utterances, the use of functional and auxiliary words, pronouns, questions, tenses, conjunctions, possessives, initiation of conversations, event narrations, spurts of “why” questions, and “because” sentences ( Conti-Ramsden & Durkin, 2012; Visser-Bochane et al., 2020). These language milestones facilitate expressing their thoughts as verbal reasoning ( Socher et al., 2022). Likewise, cognitive abilities such as reasoning, processing speed, and cognitive flexibility significantly contribute to the comprehension and development of language in children ( Willinger et al., 2019). Research postulates that questioning and exploratory talk with children trigger reasoning responses by encouraging them to explain their ideas, draw conclusions, and make predictions in general communication contexts. Thus, language and reasoning during developmental phases share a bidirectional relationship where development in one domain facilitates or enhances the other ( Van de Sande et al., 2019; Willinger et al., 2019). This underscores the crucial role of understanding the development of reasoning skills during the preschool period, given its significant role in facilitating the development of other critical skills like language ( Bauer & Booth, 2019; Nussipzhanova et al., 2018). Understanding reasoning skills during preschool years is also vital for several other critical decisions, such as preparing children to perceive and understand real-life scenarios, determining the right time for school entry, and laying the foundation for academic achievement and success ( Niklas et al., 2018; Pasnak et al., 2015).

The existing literature suggests that reasoning abilities among preschoolers have been explored in domains like reasoning associated with improbable and less logical events like growing money on a tree, going back in time ( Shtulman & Carey, 2007), counterfactual events ( Muller et al., 2007), judgments on human abilities ( Heyman et al., 2003), temporal ordering of events ( McCormack & Hoerl, 2005), teleological functions ( Kelemen et al., 2003), consistent and inconsistent events ( Legare et al., 2010), pre-energy reasoning ( Koliopoulos et al., 2009), analogies ( Simms et al., 2018), and causal reasoning concerning scientific literacy associated with space, structures, tools, etc. ( Bauer & Booth, 2019). However, the findings from these studies remain inconclusive in understanding the general reasoning development in preschoolers that could be fostered through education during the preschool period ( Säre, Luik, & Fisher, 2016a). These studies seem to focus less on how children verbally reason in a general living context and how education could be the mediator to foster reasoning among preschoolers in such contexts. We could find one such recent initiative by Säre, Luik, and Fisher (2016a), wherein a valid and reliable tool ‘Younger Children Verbal Reasoning Test’ was developed to aid educators in assessing the general verbal reasoning of older preschoolers between five and six years of age. The tool utilized scenarios related to a theme on ‘bravery’ with illustrations and prompted children to judge how brave the characters are in the given scenario based on their actions. However, the authors of the work highlighted the psychometric properties of the developed tool and provided limited description regarding application of their tool in understanding general reasoning skills development in preschoolers.

The stimuli used to assess the reasoning abilities among preschoolers is a significant aspect to consider while discussing the development of general reasoning skills in this age group. In this context, Kendeou et al. (2019) put forth an integrated theoretical framework, inferential Language Comprehension (iLC), guiding and recommending the effective use of static or dynamic visual narratives (Stories) as a stimulus for evaluating reasoning skills among younger children. The study recommended that reasoning skills, such as inferencing opportunities, could be increased using static or dynamic visual narrative with questioning techniques in younger children who are non-readers. The story-based materials are reported to aid in better representation of general reasoning abilities in everyday communication contexts as they are more relatable to the natural learning context for the children ( Killick & Boffey, 2012; O’Reilly et al., 2022). Stories also seem to provide an opportunity for children to provide explanations, form predictions, and make inferences that would help them to form logical, causally sequenced plots ( Paris & Paris, 2003). Since a long, stories are known to constitute an inevitable part of childhood education, as children are intrinsically drawn to stories, and stories facilitate the development of thinking and learning in children ( Paley, 2013). Preschool teachers highly appreciate the use of stories for fostering preschool child development and have recommended for its use as a stimuli for assessments ( Prasanna et al., 2024b). Recent research by Dawes et al. (2019) advocates the potential and sensitivity of a story-based approach to obtain insight into preschoolers’ developmental changes for skills such as inferential comprehension. The application of story-based stimuli to assess reasoning skills during preschool years has ranged from asking questions in an open-ended format after a short story sequence with picture cards ( Muller et al., 2007; Shtulman & Carey, 2007) or after short fragments of story scenarios depicted through illustrations and objects ( Bauer & Booth, 2019; Heyman et al., 2003; McCormack & Hoerl, 2005; Simms et al., 2018). A recent review by O’Reilly et al. (2022) highlighted the effectiveness of stories in fostering critical thinking, such as reasoning and problem-solving among preschoolers, and advocated the utility of story-based approaches in the field of critical thinking for future researchers.

Cognitive assessments, in general, have been recommended to be culturally and linguistically relevant to the age and community of the participants for yielding accurate findings ( Tanveer et al., 2022). Likewise, though stories offer significant potential in assessing reasoning abilities among preschool age children, it is important to note that stories should be culturally and linguistically relevant for getting a true insight into the developmental trends of reasoning abilities among preschoolers ( Rao et al., 2021; Sternberg, 2004).

Some other crucial aspects while exploring general reasoning skills among preschoolers, besides the stimuli, are the ways reasoning based responses are elicited with the children and the different tasks employed to assess their abilities. An interventional research on facilitating reasoning among preschoolers used undirected, shared picture book narration within a peer-group setting ( Reed et al., 2015). The study qualitatively analyzed the narration discourse by preschoolers while they freely generated stories from picture books and observed the emergence of reasoning categories such as explanation, prediction, and inference. The findings of this research informed that spontaneously elicited reasoning categories in pre-schoolers without guidance could better represent the reasoning developmental changes in the preschool period . Regarding the tasks for assessing reasoning among preschoolers, different studies have employed tasks based on explanation, predictions, and inferencing. In the explanation-based reasoning tasks, participants are expected to explain the reason behind an outcome ( Legare et al., 2010) whereas, in the prediction-based reasoning tasks, participants are required to predict the outcome of an event assuming a cause ( Bonawitz et al., 2010). The inference-based reasoning task expects the participants to form a conclusion about a connection between an antecedent and an outcome with rational justification ( Gopnik et al., 2004). Similar reasoning tasks based on explanation, prediction, and inference were utilized in preschoolers to explore reasoning skills related to the physical causality of objects by Blank et al. (1981). It seems most promising to incorporate explanation, prediction, and inference based-reasoning tasks in a story-based approach while studying reasoning development during preschool years.

Thus, recognizing the critical role of reasoning in language development and the mutually facilitating nature of these skills, alongside the scarcity of studies on general reasoning development among preschoolers, the current research was initiated. Understanding the importance of using a culturally and linguistically sensitive tool and the suitability of a story-based approach, this research employed a story-based method to explore reasoning among preschoolers. Due to the lack of such culturally and linguistically relevant story-based tools in the Indian context, our previous work developed a tool for assessing cognitive-communication skills among Indian preschoolers based on a story and evaluated its content validity ( Prasanna, et al., 2024b). The present research extends this work by utilizing the reasoning component (explanation, prediction, and inference-based reasoning tasks) of the developed tool to explore reasoning skills development among preschoolers. The aim of this research was to profile the reasoning skills of typically developing Indian preschool children aged 36 to 72 months using a story-based approach. The specific objectives were to determine the test-retest and inter-rater reliability of reasoning tasks within an existing story-based cognitive-communicative assessment tool and to use this tool to assess the reasoning skills of typically developing Indian preschool children. The study did not expect the developmental trends of reasoning abilities to be gender-specific, based on evidence from previous literature ( Ardila et al., 2011).

Methods

The study followed a cross-sectional research design following STROBE guidelines and commenced after obtaining Institutional Ethical Clearance from Kasturba Medical College, Mangalore, Manipal Academy of Higher Education (IEC KMC MLR 02-19/51). The data collection was conducted between June 2021 to January 2022 in Mangalore, India. The present study serves as an evaluation phase of our previous work that developed a tool for assessing cognitive-communication skills among preschoolers in the Indian context based on a story ( Prasanna et al., 2024b) and hence, the current study utilized the story-based cognitive-communication assessment tool developed in our previous work ( Prasanna et al., 2024b) for exploring reasoning skills among preschoolers.

Participants

Typically developing children aged 36 to 72 months attending English medium schools in Mangalore were selected as participants. Written informed consent was obtained from school authorities, and parents of all the participants before their inclusion in the study. Initially, 76 children were screened for typical sensorimotor, cognitive, and language development using the Ten Questions Screen (TQS) ( Durkin et al., 1995) and Assessment of Language Development (ALD) ( Lakkanna et al., 2021). As TQS doesn’t have any cut-off scoring, children who screened negative across all the 10 items of the TQS, and those who exhibited age-adequate receptive and expressive language skills according to ALD were recruited. Since socioeconomic status highly correlates with the cognitive development of young children ( Greenfield & Moorman, 2019), only those participants who belonged to middle socioeconomic status according to the modified Kuppuswamy socioeconomic scale ( Saleem, 2019) were included in this study. Only those children who had attended nursery before joining preschool were included in the study to ensure homogeneity concerning exposure to literacy skills. The English language proficiency of the children was ascertained on Child Language Experience and Proficiency Questionnaire (LEAP-Q-Child version) and those who obtained seven or greater on a 10-point rating scale in the questionnaire were included in the study ( Marian et al., 2007; Rochanavibhata et al., 2019). Thus, from a total of 76 children, 13 children were excluded from participation due to either history or complaint of deficits in sensory, motor, and/or language development, and only 63 participants were considered for the present study. The age and gender of the participants confirmed based on parental report, and the participants were equally divided into three groups according to age, i.e., Group I (Age range: 36–48 months, Mean age: 43 months, Standard deviation: 3.8 months, 10 Females and 11 Males), Group II (Age range: 49–60 months, Mean age: 54 months, Standard deviation: 3.8 months, 10 Females and 11 Males) and Group III (Age range: 61–72 months, Mean age: 65 months, Standard deviation: 3.2 months, 10 Females and 11 Males). All the shortlisted children had English as their second language, and among them, 89% were bilingual (L1-Kannada/Malayalam, L2-English), and 11% were multilingual (L1-Konkani/Hindi/Malayalam, L2-English, L3-Kannada). The sample size of the study was calculated using the formula n=Z1-α/22SD2/d2 with a 95% confidence level, 20% absolute error, and 80% power. The anticipated standard deviation of 0.789 was based on pilot study results.

Material

Present research utilized the story-based cognitive-communication assessment tool developed in our previous work for Indian preschoolers ( Prasanna et al., 2024b). The tool was developed following Delphi procedure in its conceptualization and construction, and the processes involved is illustrated in our previous work. The content validity index of greater than 0.78 for all the items of the tool revealed an excellent content validity for the developed tool as per the standard CVI criteria ( Almanasreh et al., 2019; Polit & Beck, 2006).

Story description

The story of the tool was titled ‘A Day at Grandparents House’ ( Prasanna et al., 2021b; Prasanna et al., 2024b) themed on a boy visiting grandparents’ house and the events happening at the house. The story was divided into three story sections with an equal distribution of 14 story elements per section with corresponding two pictures. Thereby the story contained a total of six pictures ( Prasanna et al., 2021a; Prasanna et al., 2024b) presenting the story elements equally across auditory and auditory-visual modality ( Table 1). The story was designed in English to maintain uniformity among participants from different native languages.

Table 1. Details of story and reasoning tasks with examples.

Section Story Number of pictures Number of story elements Reasoning Tasks
Explanation (Open-ended questions) Prediction (Open-ended questions) Inference (Closed-ended yes/no question and open-ended question)
S1 Virat was a 6-year-old boy. On a week day, Virat’s parents dropped him at his grandparents’ house in a scooter. Virat’s grandparents were waiting for him at the gate. Virat ran towards his grandparents and hugged them. Suddenly, Virat saw a dog coming towards him barking. Virat got scared and started crying. His grandpa pulled and locked the dog in the metal cage. After this, Virat entered the house happily with grandparents. 2 14
For example, Grandparents were waiting at the gate
2
For example, Why did Virat get scared?
2
For example, What would have happened if grandma hadn't locked the dog in the cage?
2
For example, Did Virat like his grandparents? (Inference) What made you feel so? (Justification of inference)
S2 It was raining. Hence, grandpa told Virat not to play outside. Virat looked through the window. He saw the dog in the cage getting wet. Water was leaking through a hole on its roof. Dog was sleeping at the corner. Seeing this, Virat took umbrella from cupboard and ran to the cage. He kept umbrella above the cage. Then water stopped leaking. Dog started wagging tail. Seeing this, Virat started running to the house. Suddenly Virat heard a strange sound from the side of the house. 2 14
For example, He kept umbrella above the cage
2
For example, Why did Virat keep an umbrella on the cage?
2
For example, What would have happened if Virat had played in the rain?
2
For example, Did Virat help the dog? (Inference) What made you feel so? (Justification of inference)
S3 Virat ran towards the side where the sound came. A robber wearing a black mask caught Virat and closed his mouth. Seeing this, the dog started barking loudly. Everybody came outside the house and the robber ran away. Virat slowly went to the cage and tried to touch the dog. The dog started licking his hand. Virat asked grandpa to open the cage and the dog came out happily. Virat gave biscuits to the dog. They became friends. 2 14
For example, Dog started barking loudly
2
For example, Why did the dog bark loudly?
2
For example, What would have happened if Grandparents were not at home when the robber came?
2
For example, Did the Grandparents catch the robber? (Inference) What made you feel so?
(Justification of inference)

Task description

The reasoning tasks of the tool had been constructed based on the iLC framework proposed by Kendeou et al. (2019) which emphasized the utility of a static visual narrative and questioning technique for assessing reasoning abilities among preschoolers. The reasoning skills were designed across explanation, prediction, and inference-based reasoning tasks formulated for each of the sections of the story. Considering the potential of questioning as a strategy to assess reasoning among young children ( Kendeou et al., 2019; Säre, Luik, & Tulviste, 2016b), the tasks were designed in a question based format. The questions regarding explanation and prediction based reasoning abilities were framed in an open-ended format (Eg: Why did Virat get scared? [explanation], What would have happened if grandpa had not locked the dog in the cage? [prediction]), whereas the questions for the inference based reasoning comprised of a closed-ended polar question (Yes/No type) to elicit the inference followed by an open-ended question to prompt the justification for the same (Eg: Did Virat love grandparents? [inference] followed by What made you feel so? [justification for the inference]).

Pilot testing

Though the tool revealed the representation of the targeted construct of assessment with adequate content validity index in the previous work, other psychometric properties were not evaluated. Hence, in the present research, for further evaluation of the psychometric properties and to confirm the feasibility of the reasoning tasks, a pilot study among 15 typically developing children aged between 36 to 72 months was conducted. Since diverse answers could be possible for some of these reasoning tasks, especially among preschoolers, all the probable responses obtained during the pilot study were reviewed and discussed by the researchers to formulate an answer key. To obtain the test-retest reliability, the tasks were re-administered after two weeks among the 15 preschoolers (five from each age group), and to obtain the inter-rater reliability, two raters rated the data obtained from the 15 preschoolers. Since the tool developed is planned to be commercially released in the future, the optimum representation of the story text and reasoning tasks has been provided in Table 1 for the readers.

Procedure

The data collection was carried out within the home premises, wherein the participants were seated in a well-lit, quiet room with only the researcher and the child present during the assessment. The evaluation was scheduled in the morning, considering the children’s active time according to the mother’s feedback. The researcher initially built rapport with the children and developed their interest in listening to the story through engaging in an active conversation about stories (For example: Do you like to listen to a story? Does your mother tell you stories? I am going to tell you a story. Would you like to know what happened in that story?). The researcher then instructed the child to carefully listen to the story and answer the questions, which was rewarded with reinforcement (stickers). The researcher also explained that if the child did not know the answer or wanted to think longer, they could freely say that. A practice trial was given to the child by listening to another short story (different from the story used in present assessment) and answering three reasoning-based questions based on the story to familiarise with the assessment procedure.

After the practice trial, the story was presented using a laptop and headphones. The reasoning tasks were performed after the presentation of each story section to ensure that excessive recall load did not dilute the findings. The order of the presentation of the questions in each section was randomized for each participant using a random number generator. Children were encouraged to respond verbally to all the questions and were rewarded with reinforcement (stickers). The assessment took around 20 minutes per participant, including a one-minute rest after each story section and task.

Data processing and analysis

The content validation of the tool was estimated in the previous work ( Prasanna et al., 2024b), hence in the present research further evaluation interms of the test-retest reliability and inter-rater reliability was established using the Intra class correlation coefficient (ICC) analysis at 95% confidence interval with absolute agreement using two-way mixed model.

The responses obtained from the assessment of reasoning skills using the three tasks were analyzed quantitatively and qualitatively. For the quantitative analysis, the responses across the explanation, prediction, and justification components of the inference tasks were scored based on their appropriateness. The appropriateness of the answers was determined based on the answer key prepared by the researchers, considering logic and rationality. Children received a score of ‘one’ for the appropriate reasons and ‘zero’ for the inappropriate or no reasons for all the tasks. For example, for an explanation based question, ‘Why did Virat get scared?’, if the child’s response was ‘because he saw the dog’ or ‘because the dog would bite,’ it was interpreted as appropriate and received a score of ‘one.’ Whereas if the child’s response to the same question was unrelated (such as ‘because he saw a ghost’ or ‘don’t’ know’), then such answers were considered inappropriate and scored ‘zero’ accordingly. The closed-end polar questions (Yes/No type) of the inference based reasoning task were scored as ‘one’ for the correct answers and ‘zero’ for the incorrect answers. A child could thus obtain a maximum score of ‘six’ for each reasoning task. To prevent bias in the scoring, two independent researchers who were blind to the participants’ demographic details scored the responses, and any disagreements were resolved by reaching a consensus through discussion with a third independent researcher. The scored data were subjected to one-way ANOVA with post hoc Tukey test using SPSS V.25 software to investigate the quantitative changes in reasoning skills with age during the preschool period. Receiver Operating Characteristic (ROC) analysis was also carried out to determine the cut-off values distinguishing the significant quantitative changes in reasoning skills revealed during ANOVA analysis. Gender wise analysis of the task scores was carried out using Independent t test.

The data was further analyzed qualitatively, exploring the manner in which children reason with increase in age to get an insight into the typical pattern of reasoning during the preschool years. Such an analysis was expected to serve as a guide for stimulating typical reasoning development in clinical and educational settings. A general inductive approach ( Thomas, 2006), a systematic set of procedures for analyzing qualitative data that produce reliable and valid findings, was followed for qualitative analysis of the reasoning responses. Two independent researchers coded and categorized the responses for explanation, prediction, and justification of the inference tasks. The codes and categories obtained from the two researchers were compared and merged into a combined set to establish the extent of overlap. The agreement between the two coders was set at 95%, and disagreements were resolved through discussion with a third independent researcher. Based on this process and the consensus among the researchers, the responses of the participants were qualitatively categorized into ‘Appropriate reasoning with story content,’ ‘Appropriate reasoning integrating story content with prior knowledge,’ ‘Inappropriate reasoning with improper story content usage,’ and ‘Inappropriate reasoning with context associated with prior knowledge but improper to the story content’ and ‘No reason.’ An example of the qualitative analysis of one such response from the first section of the story is provided in Table 2. For more details of the associated data see underlying data ( Prasanna et al., 2023a).

Table 2. Examples of the data analysis based on story section 1.

Story Reasoning tasks Reasoning response analysis
Theme Tasks Question Appropriate reasons Score: 1 Inappropriate reasons Score: 0 No reasons Score: 0
Appropriate reasoning with story content Appropriate reasoning integrating story content with prior knowledge Inappropriate reasoning with improper story content usage Inappropriate reasoning with context associated with prior knowledge but improper to the story content No reasons
A boy visiting his grandparent's house gets scared of seeing a dog, and the dog is locked in the cage later. Explanation For example, Why did Virat get scared? For example, Because he saw the dog For example, Because Dog will bite For example, Scooter For example, He saw ghost For example, Don’t know
Prediction For example, What would have happened if grandpa hadn't locked the dog in the cage? For example, The boy will keep crying For example, Dog will come and bite For example, Locked the dog For example, Dog will fall down For example, Don’t know
Inference Did Virat like his grandparents? (Inference) For example: Y es For example: No For example, Don’t know
What made you feel so? (Justification of inference) For example, Because he hugged grandparents For example, Because I love my grandparents For example, Dog barked For example, He doesn’t like the car For example, Don’t know

Results

The psychometric properties of the tasks were determined using test-retest reliability and inter-rater reliability measures. The test-retest reliability measures revealed an ICC of 0.66 indicating moderate test-retest reliability and inter-rater reliability measures revealed an ICC of 0.91 indicating excellent inter-rater reliability at 95% confidence interval with absolute agreement using two-way mixed model.

The story-based tasks were then used to assess the reasoning abilities of 63 typically developing Indian preschool children. The responses obtained from the participants across the explanation, prediction, and inference based reasoning tasks were quantitatively and qualitatively analyzed. The mean and standard deviation of the total reasoning scores and the percentage of categories of responses for the explanation, prediction, and inference based reasoning tasks across the age groups are illustrated in Figure 1(a,b), Figure 2(a,b), and Figure 3(a-c) respectively.

Figure 1. (a) Mean and standard deviation of the reasoning scores and (b) the percentage of categories of reasoning responses for the explanation task across the age groups.

Figure 1.

Figure 2. (a) Mean and standard deviation of the reasoning scores and (b) the percentage of categories of reasoning responses for the prediction task across the age groups.

Figure 2.

Figure 3. (a) Mean and standard deviation of the reasoning scores under inference, (b) justification of inference, and (c) the percentage of categories of reasoning responses for the justification of inference task across the age groups.

Figure 3.

A comparison of the mean scores of the explanation based reasoning tasks revealed that the explanation based reasoning scores increased with age, F (2,60)=17.91, p<0.001, irrespective of the gender (t[61]=0.401,p=0.690). The post hoc pairwise comparison showed that the older groups (Groups II and III) performed significantly better (p<0.01) than the younger group (Group I); however, there was no statistically significant difference between the older groups (p=0.08). Receiver Operating Characteristic (ROC) analysis revealed a cut-off score of 2.5 discriminating between the younger (Group I) and older groups (Groups II and III) for explanation-based reasoning performance with 81% sensitivity, 62% specificity, and 0.8 Area under the curve (AUC) that is statistically significant (p<0.05).

The qualitative analysis using the percentage of categories of reasoning responses showed that with the increase in age, most children reasoned appropriately, integrating story content with prior knowledge. The codes identified under appropriate reasoning categories and their frequency across the age groups under the explanation task are presented in Table 3.

Table 3. Codes with its frequencies under appropriate reasoning categories for explanation task.

Question Group I Codes (Frequency) Group II Codes (Frequency) Group III Codes (Frequency)
Explanation Q1 Dog (10)
Dogs action (1)
Dog (9)
Dogs action (6)
Dog (16)
Dogs action (2)
- - Bite (2)
Opened and kept dog (1)
Explanation Q2 To see dog (2)
To see grandparents (1)
To see dog (2) To see dog (2)
To study/Play (2)
Parents have to go out (3)
Virat doesn't know the way (1)
To study/Play (1)
Parents have to go out (4)
Virat doesn't know the way (1)
Grandparents missed him (1)
Wanted to stay (1)
Naughty (2)
Safety (2)
To study/play (3)
Parents have to go out (6)
Virat doesn't know the way (1)
Grandparents missed him (1)
Likes grandparents (1)
Wanted to stay (1)
Naughty (1)
Explanation Q3 Rain (5) Rain (7) Rain (4)
Dog wet (2)
Good boy (2)
Prevent wetting (3)
Dog wanted (1)
Prevent wetting (7)
Rainfall on dog (1)
Prevent wetting (9)
Prevent being ill (2)
Explanation Q4 Kept Umbrella (1) Kept Umbrella (3) Kept Umbrella (2)
- Wanted (1)
Like (2)
Wind (1)
Like (6)
Happy (1)
Thanks for help (1)
Explanation Q5 Robber (3)
Robber caught and closed mouth (4)
Robber (4)
Robber caught and closed mouth (7)
Robber (4)
Robber caught and closed mouth (7)
Robber takes away the boy (1)
To come out of cage (1)
Robber takes away the boy (1)
To come out of cage (1)
Dog loved the boy (1)
Save boy (1)
Robber takes away the boy (2)
To come out of cage (1)
Save boy (6)
Explanation Q6 Since boy was crying (1) Boy went there (1)
Since boy went outside (1)
To take boy and go (1)
For not to scream (1)
Not to call grandparents (1)
Stop breathing (1)
All not to listen (2)
For not to scream (6)
Since boy went there alone (1)
Police not to come (1)
To die (1)
For not to scream (9)
Stop breathing and lose consciousness (1)
All not to listen (3)
Not to call grandparents (1)
To take boy and go (2)

Note: The writing on white background represents codes identified under the category of ‘appropriate reasoning with story content.’ The writings under grey background represent the codes identified under ‘appropriate reasoning integrating story content with prior knowledge’.

The comparison of the mean scores for the prediction based reasoning task also showed a significant increase with age F (2,60)=26.58, p<0.001, irrespective of the gender t (61)=0.564, p=0.575. Post hoc pairwise comparison revealed a significant difference in prediction scores between all three age groups (p<0.01). The ROC analysis revealed a cut-off prediction score of 4.5 discriminating between Group I and Group II with 52% sensitivity, 90% specificity, and 0.8 statistically significant AUC (p<0.001), and a cut-off prediction score of 5.5 discriminating Group II from Group III with 52% sensitivity, 86% specificity, and 0.7 AUC, that is statistically significant (p<0.05).

The qualitative analysis showed that similar to the explanation task, most children reasoned integrating story content with prior knowledge as their age increased. The codes identified under appropriate reasoning categories and their frequency across the age groups under the prediction task are presented in Table 4.

Table 4. Codes with its frequencies under appropriate reasoning categories for prediction task.

Question Group I Codes (Frequency) Group II Codes (Frequency) Group III Codes (Frequency)
Prediction Q1 Enter home (1)
Walk and go (1)
Will not run (2)
No cry (1)
Walk and go (2)
Will not run (1)
Enter home (2)
No cry (1)
Stand there itself (1)
Will bite (3)
Touch the dog (2)
Make noise (1)
Happy (3)
Touch the dog (2)
Dog will catch the boy (1)
Will play (1)
Loved the dog (1)
Will not bite (1)
Will bite (1)
Walk and go nicely (1)
Brave (4)
Will play (2)
Will bite (1)
Touch the dog (1)
Loved the dog (1)
Food to dog (1)
Prediction Q2 Run and go (3)
Scared (1)
Run and came (1)
Scared (4)
Run and come (1)
Bark (4)
Scared (1)
Stand there itself (1)
Run and go (1)
Boy cry (2)
Dog cry (1)
Bite (5)
Go out (2)
Bite (5)
Jump on him (1)
Will not do anything (1)
Go out (2)
Go out (4)
Bite (10)
Prediction Q3 Wet (5)
Rainfall (3)
Wet (6)
Rainfall (1)
Wet (6)
Ill with fever or cold (6)
Thunder and lightning will kill (1)
Ill with fever or cold (9)
Wet and mother shout (1)
Fall (1)
Ask grandpa umbrella (1)
Dog will make sound (1)
Fall (1)
Ill with fever or cold (13)
Prediction Q4 Rainfall (6)
Wet (7)
Run inside (1)
Wet (8)
Rainfall (2)
Call grandpa (1)
Wet (9)
Not able to keep umbrella (1)
Rainfall (2)
Cry (1)
Find out (1) Raincoat (5)
Cold (1)
Cage inside (1)
Wouldn't go (2)
Fever (1)
Raincoat (4)
Sad (1)
Prediction Q5 Bark (2)
Robber will throw (1)
Take and go (2)
Die (1)
Take and go (11)
Robber will take items from home (1)
Danger (1)
Robber put boy in jail (2)
Take and go (15)
Dog will inform barking, and they will come from back (1)
Prediction Q6 Will not catch (1)
Robber wouldn't have come (1)
Will not catch (3)
Will not bark (1)
Will not catch (9)
Danger (1) Play (2)
Will catch when went to Play with dog (1)
Escape (1)
Dog will bark and thus will get to know (1)
Ran away (1)

Note: The writing on white background represents codes identified under the category of ‘appropriate reasoning with story content.’ The writings under grey background represent the codes identified under ‘appropriate reasoning integrating story content with prior knowledge’.

For the inference-based reasoning tasks, comparison of the mean score revealed that the inference scores remained almost similar across the age groups, F (2,60)=0.206, p=0.82, irrespective of the gender (t [61]=0.167, p=0.868). However, the scores pertaining to the justification for inference were found to increase with age, F (2,60)=6.04, p<0.05, without gender as a factor (t [61]=1.767, p=0.082). Post hoc pairwise comparison revealed significant differences in the inference justification performance between older groups (Groups II and III) and younger group (Group I) (p<0.05). There was no statistically significant difference between the older groups (Group II and III) (p=0.99). ROC analysis determined the cut-off score as 0.5, discriminating between the younger (Group I) and older groups (Groups II and III) for inference justification scores with 62% sensitivity, 86% specificity, and 0.7 statistically significant AUC (p<0.05).

Qualitative analysis showed that most of the responses belong to the ‘No reason’ category across the age groups and a smaller percentage of responses under ‘appropriate reasoning’ categories. Among the ‘appropriate categories,’ majority of the preschoolers reasoned under the category of ‘appropriate reasoning with story content.’ The codes identified under the smaller percentage of appropriate reasoning categories and their frequency across the age groups under the justification of inference task are presented in Table 5.

Table 5. Codes with its frequencies under appropriate reasoning categories for justification of inference task.

Question Group I Codes (Frequency) Group II Codes (Frequency) Group III Codes (Frequency)
Justification for Inference Q1 - - -
Justification for Inference Q2 - Hugged (1)
When dog came to bite (1)
Granny locked the dog (1)
-
I love (1) I love (1) Saw love (1)
Justification for Inference Q3 Kept umbrella (2)
Umbrella (1)
Kept umbrella (9) Kept umbrella (7)
- Helped by keeping umbrella (1) Boy couldn't afford dog getting wet so kept umbrella (1)
Justification for Inference Q4 Went outside while raining (1) - Told not to go outside (1)
Told to take care when going outside (1)
- Alone (2)
Mother takes care of me (1)
Grandpa tried not to bite (1)
Justification for Inference Q5 Licking hands (1) Opened dog (1)
Licking hands (1)
Robber caught and Dog barked (2)
Went to the cage (1)
Licked hands (2)
Dog made robber ran (2)
When barked (2)
Touched (1)
- Dog helped (1)
Loved dog (1)
Dog helped (2)
Saved him (1)
Justification for Inference Q6 Robber ran (1) Ran away (1) Robber ran (3)
- Robber ran fast (1) Grandparents at house (1)
Robber ran and should have called police (1)

Note: The writing on white background represents codes identified under the category of ‘appropriate reasoning with story content.’ The writings under grey background represent the codes identified under ‘appropriate reasoning integrating story content with prior knowledge’.

Discussion

The evaluation of the psychometric properties of the story-based reasoning tasks revealed excellent inter-rater reliability and moderate test-retest reliability. The moderate test-retest reliability of executive function-based cognitive tasks during the preschool period, as observed in this study, is consistent with existing literature, which reports similar reliability ranges (0.52–0.66) for this stage of development ( Espy & Cwik, 2004; Isquith et al., 2005; Na et al., 2024; Willoughby & Blair, 2011). Studies have attributed moderate test-retest reliabilities in preschoolers to several key factors. First, the rapid developmental changes typical of this age group ( Na et al., 2024) can significantly impact performance consistency. Additionally, preschoolers' fluctuating effort and concentration contribute to variations in task engagement ( Espy & Cwik, 2004). Moreover, cognitive constructs such as executive functions are less differentiated in early childhood, which leads to higher reliability when assessed as broad constructs, rather than in specific domains like reasoning, as seen in the present study ( Willoughby & Blair, 2011). Nevertheless, the small sample size in the present study, along with factors such as the interval between assessments and the challenge of maintaining a consistent level of attention among preschoolers during both sessions, suggests potential for further refinement. Optimizing these elements may help achieve higher test-retest reliability for the reasoning tasks.

The tool utilized that consisted of a story and reasoning tasks in a question format aligned with the theoretical framework proposed for exploring reasoning skills among preschoolers ( Kendeou et al., 2019). A recent survey among teachers in Indian context also highlights the potential of stories among preschoolers for assessments ( Prasanna et al., 2024a). The use of story as the assessment material is also consistent with the other research that supports the efficacy of story-based approaches among preschoolers ( Dawes et al., 2019; O’Reilly et al., 2022). Besides adding culturally and linguistically relevant data on reasoning skills to the existing literature in this domain among preschoolers ( Muller et al., 2007; Shtulman & Carey, 2007), the current study has offered additional depth by exploring the development of reasoning skills across explanation, prediction and inference domains using a single story. Moreover, the present format of these tasks, wherein domains of the reasoning abilities could be assessed on spontaneously emerged output of typically developing preschoolers, is aligned with the earlier perspectives in this regard ( Reed et al., 2015). Further, the components of the story of the tool, such as the characters, sequences, objects, dress, pictures, vocabulary, and task formats, were designed to reflect the real-life Indian context, which could offer appropriate familiarity and suitability to Indian preschoolers and aid in accurate representation of their abilities ( Rao et al., 2021; Sternberg, 2004).

The story-based tasks assessed the reasoning abilities of typically developing Indian preschool children. The comparison of mean reasoning scores across the age groups in the explanation, prediction, and justification of inference task revealed that the preschooler’s reasoning skills improved significantly with age. In most tasks (explanation and justification of inference), a significant change in performance was observed from Group I (36–48 months) to Group II (49–60 months) and III (61–72 months) than between Group II (49–60 months) and III (61–72 months), indicating the rapid development of reasoning skills during the initial phase of the preschool period. The results agree with the previous literature that younger preschoolers show a significant improvement in reasoning skills with age than older preschoolers ( Hong et al., 2005). The results also imitate the age bands of thinking differences (Symbolic function substage: 2–4 years, Intuitive thought substage: 4 to 7 years) in Piaget’s preoperational stage of cognitive development ( Piaget, 1952). However, the results obtained in the prediction task showed evidence for age-wise developmental changes than in age bands, with significant improvement in performance with age in years. The improved performance in reasoning skills with age across the tasks was not surprising as the preschool period is known to be critical for most of the developmental domains, including executive function skills ( Garon et al., 2008).

Contrary to the expectation, the age-related difference in reasoning performance was not found in the inference task. More specifically, the findings from the inference task suggested that Group I (36–48 months) children performed like the Group II (49–60 months) and III (61–72 months) children. These results indicate that children as young as 36–48 months of age (Group I) can make inferences. Previous literature supports the findings attesting that preschoolers make logical inferences easily and earlier in development ( Moshman, 2004). However, we could not exclude the fact that the inference task’s yes/no question nature might also have influenced the extent of this lack of age-related differences. In yes/no questions, researchers opine that younger preschoolers might show a yes/no bias (all yes responses, i.e., yes bias and vice versa) due to social pressure or difficulty inhibiting yes responses ( Okanda & Itakura, 2010). Therefore, there is a chance that the younger participants in this study might have also undergone such yes/no bias in the inference task influencing the lack of age-related differences. Hence, it indicates that, though yes/no questions have been recommended for reasoning skills assessment ( Säre et al., 2017), we need to be cautious regarding the chance of yes/no bias in younger preschoolers. However, despite the probability of yes/no bias in this study, the preschooler’s early inferential ability observed in the previous literature adds substantial weight to the conclusion of inferential ability in younger preschoolers.

Another interesting finding comparing mean reasoning scores was that the preschoolers performed poorly on the justification of inference task than on the other tasks. Though preschoolers could make inferences, the findings contend that they find it difficult to justify their inferences. This difficulty could be because the ability to justify the inferences is a higher quality of thinking requiring metacognition ( Moshman, 2004). Though metacognition was not explicitly studied in this research, previous literature findings indicate that children require metacognitive awareness to justify their inferences ( Whittaker & McMullen, 2014), which is beyond the preschool age in development ( Veenman et al., 2006), lend support for the poorer performance in the current study. However, it is also noteworthy that a smaller percentage of older preschoolers (Group II, 49–60 months and Group III, 61–72 months) could provide appropriate responses (20%) in the justification of inference tasks using story content. Though it could be due to individual differences, it might also indicate the traces of metalogical development for reasoning from 4 years of age using the linguistic content, suggesting the scope of improvement in justification if provided with support.

On qualitative analysis, results from the explanation and prediction task revealed that among the appropriate reasons made by the preschoolers, younger preschoolers (Group I, 36–48 months) predominantly reason under the category of ‘reasoning with story content,’ and the older preschoolers (Group II, 49–60 months and Group III, 61–72 months) reason in the category of ‘reasoning integrating story content with prior knowledge.’ The results indicate that while younger preschoolers reason with reference to the inference based on the story content premise, older preschoolers undergo a higher thinking process of coordinating the logical inferences from the story and prior knowledge to conclude and reason. The results support the previous literature findings that preschoolers integrate inferences from linguistic context ( Florit et al., 2011) and access prior knowledge while reasoning ( Gopnik et al., 2004). The findings also align with the comprehension development process, which activates and integrates linguistic knowledge with background knowledge to connect the story contents and form a mental representation to comprehend ( Kendeou et al., 2019).

Another interesting finding in qualitative analysis was with respect to the justification of the inference task. The results from the justification of inference task revealed that preschoolers depend on the source of knowledge from the linguistic contents to justify their inference. We have observed that older preschoolers tried to justify inference with respect to the source of knowledge from the story contents rather than the prior knowledge premise. It was also noted that most preschoolers provided empty reasons such as ‘I know’ for justification questions, claiming they had always known the inference. The findings are supported by the previous literature ( Kuhn et al., 2004; Taylor et al., 1994) that children tend to give such responses when unsure or unaware of the answer. This could be because the metacognitive awareness of reasoning is in the emerging stage during the preschool period ( Veenman et al., 2006). Based on the pattern of development observed in other tasks of reasoning, it could be expected that with increase in age over the preschool period, with the development of metacognitive awareness of reasoning, children might justify their inference majorly by integrating the story content with their prior knowledge and own interpretation, thereby following the similar pattern of reasoning development.

All the developmental changes in reasoning obtained in this study could be attributed to the biological maturational change of the brain ( Andrews, 2020). While the reasoning skills are linked with specific brain regions such as the rostro lateral prefrontal cortex, inferior parietal lobe, cingulate cortex, corpus callosum among adolescents and adults ( Feng et al., 2020; Wendelken et al., 2016; Willbrand et al., 2023; Wright et al., 2008), in the case of younger children, as the brain structures are less mature, rather than a specific region, the improvement in processing speed with the increased neural connection formations has been associated ( Andrews, 2020; Kail et al., 2016; Tooley et al., 2022; Wendelken et al., 2016, 2017). Literature indicates that the improvement in processing speed with structural connectivity changes such as neural network formations, global white matter integrity, changes in the brain volume, and reduction in cortical thickness in the fronto parietal regions correspond with reasoning development ( Baron & Leonberger, 2012; Botdorf & Riggins, 2018; Bruchhage et al., 2020; Chen et al., 2017; Ferrer et al., 2014; Wendelken et al., 2016). The preschool period which blossoms through biological maturation process of neural network formations ( Brown & Jernigan, 2012), thereby could have contributed to the improvement in reasoning skills observed in the current research. These biological maturational changes could also have been facilitated by the everyday interactional context ( Niklas et al., 2018; Säre, Luik, & Tulviste, 2016b), working memory ( Andrews, 2020), language ( Richland & Burchinal, 2013) and theory of mind development ( Taggart et al., 2005; Whittaker & McMullen, 2014). Consistent with existing literature gender wise differences were not found in all the reasoning tasks ( Ardila et al., 2011). The study’s findings need to be cautiously generalized, as reasoning responses are dynamic.

In conclusion, the study describes the general reasoning skill development in typically developing Indian preschool children between 36 to 72 months of age using story-based tasks. The study utilized a valid and reliable story-based task for the purpose, and the findings concluded that reasoning skills increase with age during preschool in a pattern of moving from reasoning based on story content to reasoning integrating the story content with prior knowledge. The study findings provide clinical and educational implications with insights on general reasoning development in preschoolers and the utility of age-appropriate story-based explanation, prediction, and inference question tasks to assess and foster reasoning responses in preschoolers. The findings also guide educators to scaffold reasoning in preschoolers, and the tool aid in early identification of reasoning deficits and guide planning intervention for Indian preschoolers with reasoning deficits. Though the study findings provide insight into general reasoning development in preschoolers, the sampler size was smaller and the role of contributing factors such as working memory, theory of mind, language, bilingualism/multilingualism, and social interaction on reasoning was not explored, and it remains a limitation of the present study. Therefore, future studies considering such factors could provide an in-depth understanding of reasoning development to assess and foster reasoning during preschool years. Additionally, the test-retest reliability of the given tasks could be further investigated by optimizing the sample size, time interval, and the attentiveness of participants between the two assessments.

Acknowledgments

We acknowledge all the participants in the study.

Funding Statement

This work was supported by Educational Research and Innovations Committee (ERIC) grant under the National Council of Educational Research and Training (NCERT) under grant number F. No. 4-51(764)/2019-DER/426 dated 24/10/2019.

The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.

[version 3; peer review: 3 approved]

Data availability

Underlying data

Figshare: ‘An Insight into developmental changes in reasoning skills among Indian Preschoolers - A Cross-Sectional Study using a story-based approach’, https://doi.org/10.6084/m9.figshare.22140617.v1 ( Prasanna et al., 2023a).

This project contains the following underlying data:

  • Data file 1. (Quantitative and Qualitative data of Reasoning responses)

Reporting guidelines

Figshare: STROBE checklist for ‘An Insight into developmental changes in reasoning skills among Indian Preschoolers - A Cross-Sectional Study using a story-based approach’, https://doi.org/10.6084/m9.figshare.22140689.v2 ( Prasanna et al., 2023b).

Data are available under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International license (CC-BY 4.0).

References

  1. Almanasreh E, Moles R, Chen TF: Evaluation of methods used for estimating content validity. Res. Soc. Adm. Pharm. 2019;15(2):214–221. 10.1016/j.sapharm.2018.03.066 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Alvarez AL, Booth AE: Motivated by Meaning: Testing the Effect of Knowledge-Infused Rewards on Preschoolers’ Persistence. Child Dev. 2014;85(2):783–791. 10.1111/cdev.12151 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Amsterlaw J: Children’s beliefs about everyday reasoning. Child Dev. 2006;77(2):443–464. 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2006.00881.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Andrews G: Reasoning and Problem Solving. Hupp S, Jewell JD, editors. The encyclopedia of child and adolescent development. John Wiley & Sons, Inc.;2020; pp.1–11. 10.1002/9781119171492.wecad143 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  5. Ardila A, Rosselli M, Matute E, et al. : Gender Differences in Cognitive Development. Dev. Psychol. 2011;47(4):984–990. 10.1037/a0023819 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Baron IS, Leonberger KA: Assessment of intelligence in the preschool period. Neuropsychol. Rev. 2012;22(4):334–344. 10.1007/s11065-012-9215-0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Bauer JR, Booth AE: Exploring potential cognitive foundations of scientific literacy in preschoolers: Causal reasoning and executive function. Early Child. Res. Q. 2019;46(1):275–284. 10.1016/j.ecresq.2018.09.007 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  8. Blank M, Rose SA, Berlin LJ: Reasoning and Problem Solving in Young Children. Friedman MP, Das JP, O’Connor N, editors. Intelligence and Learning (NATO) Confe. Boston, MA: Springer;1981; pp.231–236. 10.1007/978-1-4684-1083-9_21 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  9. Bonawitz BE, Ferranti D, Saxe R, et al. : Just do it? Investigating the gap between prediction and action in toddlers’ causal inferences. Cognition. 2010;115(1):104–117. 10.1016/j.cognition.2009.12.001 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Botdorf M, Riggins T: When less is more: Thinner fronto-parietal cortices are associated with better forward digit span performance during early childhood. Neuropsychologia. 2018;121(May):11–18. 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2018.10.020 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Bronkhorst H, Roorda G, Suhre C, et al. : Logical Reasoning in Formal and Everyday Reasoning Tasks. Int. J. Sci. Math. Educ. 2020;18:1673–1694. 10.1007/s10763-019-10039-8 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  12. Brown TT, Jernigan TL: Brain development during the preschool years. Neuropsychol. Rev. 2012;22(4):313–333. 10.1007/s11065-012-9214-1 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Bruchhage MMK, Ngo GC, Schneider N, et al. : Functional connectivity correlates of infant and early childhood cognitive development. Brain Struct. Funct. 2020;225(2):669–681. 10.1007/s00429-020-02027-4 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Chen Z, De Beuckelaer A, Wang X, et al. : Distinct neural substrates of visuospatial and verbal-analytic reasoning as assessed by Raven’s Advanced Progressive Matrices. Sci. Rep. 2017;7(1):16230. 10.1038/s41598-017-16437-8 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Conti-Ramsden G, Durkin K: Language development and assessment in the preschool period. Neuropsychol. Rev. 2012;22(4):384–401. 10.1007/s11065-012-9208-z [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Dawes E, Leitão S, Claessen M, et al. : Oral literal and inferential narrative comprehension in young typically developing children and children with developmental language disorder. Int. J. Speech Lang. Pathol. 2019;21(3):275–285. 10.1080/17549507.2019.1604803 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Durkin MS, Wang W, Shrout PE, et al. : Evaluating a ten questions screen for childhood disability: Reliability and internal structure in different cultures. J. Clin. Epidemiol. 1995;48(5):657–666. 10.1016/0895-4356(94)00163-K [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Espy KA, Cwik MF: The development of a Trial Making Test in young children: The TRAILS-P. Clin. Neuropsychol. 2004;18(3):411–422. 10.1080/138540409052416 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Feng L, Bi X, Zhang H: Brain Regions Identified as Being Associated With Verbal Reasoning Through the Use of Imaging Regression via Internal Variation. J. Am. Stat. Assoc. 2020;116(0):144–158. 10.1080/01621459.2020.1766468 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Ferrer E, Whitaker KJ, Steele J, et al. : White Matter Maturation Supports the Development of Reasoning Ability Through its Influence on Processing Speed. Dev. Sci. 2014;16(6):941–951. 10.1111/desc.12088 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Florit E, Roch M, Levorato MC: Listening Text Comprehension of Explicit and Implicit Information in Preschoolers: The Role of Verbal and Inferential Skills. Discourse Process. 2011;48(2):119–138. 10.1080/0163853X.2010.494244 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  22. Garon N, Bryson SE, Smith IM: Executive Function in Preschoolers: A Review Using an Integrative Framework. Psychol. Bull. 2008;134(1):31–60. 10.1037/0033-2909.134.1.31 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Gopnik A, Sobel DM, Danks D, et al. : A Theory of Causal Learning in Children: Causal Maps and Bayes Nets. Psychol. Rev. 2004;111(1):3–32. 10.1037/0033-295X.111.1.3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Greenfield EA, Moorman SM: Childhood Socioeconomic Status and Later Life Cognition: Evidence From the Wisconsin Longitudinal Study. J. Aging Health. 2019;31(9):1589–1615. 10.1177/0898264318783489 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Heyman GD, Gee CL, Giles JW: Preschool Children’s Reasoning about Ability. Child Dev. 2003;74(2):516–534. 10.1111/1467-8624.7402013 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Hong L, Chijun Z, Xuemei G, et al. : The influence of complexity and reasoning direction on children’s causal reasoning. Cogn. Dev. 2005;20(1):87–101. 10.1016/j.cogdev.2004.11.001 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  27. Isquith PK, Crawford JS, Espy KA, et al. : Assessment of executive function in preschool-aged children. Ment. Retard. Dev. Disabil. Res. Rev. 2005;11(3):209–215. 10.1002/mrdd.20075 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Kail RV, Lervag A, Hulme C: Longitudinal Evidence Linking Processing Speed to the Development of Reasoning. Dev. Sci. 2016;19(6):1067–1074. 10.1111/desc.12352 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Kelemen D, Widdowson D, Posner T, et al. : Teleo-functional constraints on preschool children’s reasoning about living things. Dev. Sci. 2003;6(3):329–345. 10.1111/1467-7687.00288 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  30. Kendeou P, McMaster KL, Butterfuss R, et al. : The Inferential Language Comprehension (iLC) Framework: Supporting Children’s Comprehension of Visual Narratives. Top. Cogn. Sci. 2019;12(1):256–273. 10.1111/tops.12457 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Khemlani SS: Reasoning. Wixted JT, editor. Stevens’ Handbook of Experimental Psychology and Cognitive Neuroscience. 4th ed. John Wiley & Sons, Inc.;2018; pp.1–45. 10.1002/9781119170174.epcn311 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  32. Killick S, Boffey M: Building Relationships through Storytelling A foster carer’s guide to attachment and stories. The Fostering Network. 2012;2012. [Google Scholar]
  33. Koliopoulos D, Christidou V, Symidala I, et al. : Pre-energy reasoning in preschool children. Rev. Sci. Math. ICT Educ. 2009;3(1):123–140. Reference Source [Google Scholar]
  34. Kuhn D, Katz JB, Dean D: Developing reason. Think. Reason. 2004;10(2):197–219. 10.1080/13546780442000015 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  35. Lakkanna S, Venkatesh K, Bhat JS, et al. : Assessment of Language Development A Manipal Manual. 1st ed. Manipal Universiy Press;2021. [Google Scholar]
  36. Legare CH, Gelman SA, Wellman HM: Inconsistency with prior knowledge triggers children’s causal explanatory reasoning. Child Dev. 2010;81(3):929–944. 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010.01443.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Marian V, Blumenfeld HK, Kaushanskaya M: The Language Experience and Proficiency Questionnaire (LEAP-Q): Assessing Language Profiles in Bilinguals and Multilinguals. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 2007;50(August):940–967. 10.1044/1092-4388(2007/067) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. McCormack T, Hoerl C: Children’s reasoning about the causal significance of the temporal order of events. Dev. Psychol. 2005;41(1):54–63. 10.1037/0012-1649.41.1.54 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Moshman D: From inference to reasoning: The construction of rationality. Think. Reason. 2004;10(2):221–239. 10.1080/13546780442000024 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  40. Muller UM, Miller M, Michalczyk K, et al. : False belief understanding The influence of person, grammatical mood, counterfactual reasoning and working memory. Br. J. Dev. Psychol. 2007;25(4):615–632. 10.1348/026151007X182962 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  41. Na W, Shuqing Z, Xiaojie G, et al. : Development and test of reliability and validity of the Visual Cognitive Ability Assessment Scale for Preschool Children. Chin. J. Child Health Care. 2024;32(2):138–141. 10.11852/zgetbjzz2023-0790 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  42. Niklas F, Cohrssen C, Tayler C: Making a difference to children’s reasoning skills before school-entry: The contribution of the home learning environment. Contemp. Educ. Psychol. 2018;54(July):79–88. 10.1016/j.cedpsych.2018.06.001 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  43. Nussipzhanova B, Berdibayeva S, Garber A, et al. : Cognitive development of pre-school children with language and speech disorders. The European Journal of Social and Behavioural Sciences. 2018;21(1):13–27. 10.15405/ejsbs.227 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  44. O’Reilly C, Devitt A, Hayes N: Critical thinking in the preschool classroom - A systematic literature review. Think. Skills Creat. 2022;46(December):101110. 10.1016/j.tsc.2022.101110 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  45. Okanda M, Itakura S: When do children exhibit a “yes” bias? Child Dev. 2010;81(2):568–580. 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01416.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Paley V: In their own words. Teacher Preparation as an Inspirational Practice: Building Capacities for Responsiveness. 1st ed. Routledge;2013; p.138. [Google Scholar]
  47. Paris AH, Paris SG: Assessing Narrative compehension in young children. Read. Res. Q. 2003;38(1):36–76. 10.1598/RRQ.38.1.3 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  48. Pasnak R, Kidd JK, Gadzichowski KM, et al. : Abstracting sequences: Reasoning that is a key to academic achievement. J. Genet. Psychol. 2015;176(3):171–193. 10.1080/00221325.2015.1024198 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Piaget J: The origins of intelligence in children. 2nd ed. W W Norton & Co.;1952. 10.1037/11494-000 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  50. Polit DF, Beck CT: The Content Validity Index: Are You Sure You Know What’s Being Reported? Critique and Recommendations. Res. Nurs. Health. 2006;29(5):489–497. 10.1002/nur.20147 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Prasanna A, Anakkathil Anil M, et al. : Teacher’s practice and perception of the influence of stories during preschool child development: A cross-sectional study from ethnically diverse South Indian city. Child Lang. Teach. Ther. 2024a;40(1):5–23. 10.1177/02656590241228419 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  52. Prasanna A, Anakkathil Anil M, Bajaj G, et al. : Do preschool children have modality specific recall abilities? - A cross-sectional pilot study. Cogent Educ. 2022;9(1). 10.1080/2331186X.2022.2083519 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  53. Prasanna A, Bajaj G, Anil MA, et al. : Reasoning Research Data among Indian Preschoolers.[Dataset]. figshare. 2023a. 10.6084/m9.figshare.22140617.v1 [DOI]
  54. Prasanna A, Bajaj G, Anil MA, et al. : STROBE checklist for the article.[Dataset]. figshare. 2023b. 10.6084/m9.figshare.22140689.v2 [DOI]
  55. Prasanna A, Bajaj G, Anil MA, et al. : A Delphi Survey-based Development and Validation of Cognitive-Communication Assessment Tool for Indian Preschoolers. CoDAS. 2024b;36(1):e20220309. 10.1590/2317-1782/20232022309 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Prasanna A, Bhat JS, Anil MA, et al. : A DAY AT GRANDPARENTS HOUSE- STORY PICTURES (Patent No. L-99255/2021). Copyright office, Government of India. 2021a.
  57. Prasanna A, Bhat JS, Anil MA, et al. : A DAY AT GRANDPARENTS HOUSE (Patent No. L-98307/2021). Copyright office, Government of India. 2021b.
  58. Rao N, Chan SWY, Su Y, et al. : Measuring Being “Developmentally on Track”: Comparing Direct Assessment and Caregiver Report of Early Childhood Development in Bangladesh, China, India and Myanmar. Early Educ. Dev. 2021;33(6):1013–1035. 10.1080/10409289.2021.1928446 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  59. Reed HC, Hurks PPM, Kirschner PA, et al. : Preschoolers causal reasoning during shared picture book storytelling: A cross-case comparison descriptive study. J. Res. Child. Educ. 2015;29(3):367–389. 10.1080/02568543.2015.1042126 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  60. Richland LE, Burchinal MR: Early Executive Function Predicts Reasoning Development. Psychol. Sci. 2013;24(1):87–92. 10.1177/0956797612450883 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Rochanavibhata S, Kaushanskaya M, Marian V: Child Language Experience and Proficiency Questionnaire. 2019.
  62. Säre E, Luik P, Fisher R: Supporting educational researchers and practitioners in their work in education: Assessing the verbal reasoning skills of five- to six-year-old children. Eur. Early Child. Educ. Res. J. 2016a;24(5):638–651. 10.1080/1350293X.2016.1213564 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  63. Säre E, Luik P, Tulviste T: Improving pre-schoolers’ reasoning skills using the philosophy for children programme. Trames. 2016b;20(3):273–295. 10.3176/tr.2016.3.03 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  64. Säre E, Tulviste T, Luik P: The function of questions in developing a preschooler’s verbal reasoning skills during philosophical group discussions. Early Child Dev. Care. 2017;189(4):555–568. 10.1080/03004430.2017.1331221 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  65. Saleem SM: Modified Kuppuswamy socioeconomic scale updated for the year 2019. Indian Journal of Forensic and Community Medicine. 2019;6(1):1–3. 10.18231/2394-6776.2019.0001 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  66. Shtulman A, Carey S: Improbable or Impossible ? How Children Reason About the Possibility of Extraordinary Events. Child Dev. 2007;78(3):1015–1032. 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01047.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Simms NK, Frausel RR, Richland LE: Working memory predicts children’s analogical reasoning. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 2018;166:160–177. 10.1016/j.jecp.2017.08.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Socher M, Ingebrand E, Wass M, et al. : The relationship between reasoning and language ability: comparing children with cochlear implants and children with typical hearing. Logoped. Phoniatr. Vocol. 2022;47(2):73–83. 10.1080/14015439.2020.1834613 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Sternberg RJ: Culture and intelligence. Am. Psychol. 2004;59(5):325–338. 10.1037/0003-066X.59.5.325 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Taggart G, Ridley K, Rudd P, et al. : Thinking skills in the early years a literature review. The Mere, Upton Park Slough, Berkshire: National Foundation for Educational Research;2005. Reference Source [Google Scholar]
  71. Tanveer S, Croucher MJ, Porter R: Cultural modification of neuropsychiatric assessment: complexities to consider. BJPsych Open. 2022;8(2):e68–e63. 10.1192/bjo.2022.33 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Taylor M, Esbensen BM, Bennett RT: Children’s Understanding of Knowledge Acquisition: The Tendency for Children to Report That They Have Always Known What They Have Just Learned. Child Dev. 1994;65(6):1581–1604. 10.2307/1131282 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Thomas DR: A General Inductive Approach for Analyzing Qualitative Evaluation Data. Am. J. Eval. 2006;27(2):237–246. 10.1177/1098214005283748 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  74. Tooley UA, Park AT, Leonard JA, et al. : The Age of Reason: Functional Brain Network Development during Childhood. J. Neurosci. 2022;42(44):8237–8251. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0511-22.2022 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Van de Sande E, Kleemans T, Verhoeven L, et al. : The linguistic nature of Children’s scientific reasoning. Learn. Instr. 2019;62(December 2017):20–26. 10.1016/j.learninstruc.2019.02.002 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  76. Veenman MVJ, Van Hout-Wolters BHAM, Afflerbach P: Metacognition and learning: Conceptual and methodological considerations. Metacogn. Learn. 2006;1(1):3–14. 10.1007/s11409-006-6893-0 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  77. Visser-Bochane MI, Reijneveld SA, Krijnen WP, et al. : Identifying Milestones in Language Development for Young Children Ages 1 to 6 Years. Acad. Pediatr. 2020;20(3):421–429. 10.1016/j.acap.2019.07.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Wendelken C, Ferrer E, Ghetti S, et al. : Frontoparietal structural connectivity in childhood predicts development of functional connectivity and reasoning ability: A large-scale longitudinal investigation. J. Neurosci. 2017;37(35):8549–8558. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3726-16.2017 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Wendelken C, Ferrer E, Whitaker KJ, et al. : Fronto-Parietal Network Reconfiguration Supports the Development of Reasoning Ability. Cereb. Cortex. 2016;26(5):2178–2190. 10.1093/cercor/bhv050 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. Whittaker JV, McMullen MB: Preschool: Good Thinking! Fostering Children’s Reasoning and Problem Solving. YC Young Children. JSTOR;2014; (Vol.69, Issue3). [Google Scholar]
  81. Willbrand EH, Ferrer E, Bunge SA, et al. : Development of Human Lateral Prefrontal Sulcal Morphology and Its Relation to Reasoning Performance. J. Neurosci. 2023;43(14):2552–2567. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1745-22.2023 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  82. Willinger U, Deckert M, Schmöger M, et al. : Developmental Steps in Metaphorical Language Abilities: The Influence of Age, Gender, Cognitive Flexibility, Information Processing Speed, and Analogical Reasoning. Lang. Speech. 2019;62(2):207–228. 10.1177/0023830917746552 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  83. Willoughby M, Blair C: Test-retest reliability of a new executive function battery for use in early childhood. Child Neuropsychology. 2011;17(6):564–579. 10.1080/09297049.2011.554390 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  84. Wright SB, Matlen BJ, Baym CL, et al. : Neural correlates of fluid reasoning in children and adults. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2008;1(MAR):8. 10.3389/neuro.09.008.2007 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
F1000Res. 2025 Feb 4. doi: 10.5256/f1000research.172849.r357827

Reviewer response for version 3

Tulika Borah 1

This study offers an insightful exploration of developmental changes in reasoning skills among Indian preschoolers, utilizing a story-based approach. The cross-sectional design is appropriate for capturing key age-related differences in reasoning abilities at different stages of early childhood. The use of stories as a tool to assess cognitive development is particularly engaging, as it reflects how young children interact with and process information in a naturalistic setting.

The findings contribute valuable knowledge to the growing body of literature on early cognitive development, specifically in the Indian context. However, there is room for further discussion on the cultural relevance of the stories used and how they may influence reasoning patterns across different regions or socioeconomic backgrounds in India. A deeper consideration of these cultural nuances could strengthen the generalizability of the results.

The methodology is robust, but a longitudinal design could provide a more comprehensive understanding of how reasoning skills evolve over time. Additionally, a more detailed exploration of individual differences, such as socio-economic status, family environment, or educational exposure, level and type of intelligence could offer a fuller picture of the factors that shape cognitive development in Indian preschoolers.

Overall, the article is well-written and offers important insights into early childhood reasoning skills. It would benefit from further contextual analysis and exploration of broader factors influencing cognitive development.

Suggested editing

Section- Discussion:

Para 2, 1 st line- ‘The tool utilized that consisted of a story and reasoning tasks in a question format aligned with the theoretical framework proposed for exploring reasoning skills among preschoolers’

Para 9, 9 th line- ‘Though the study findings provide insight into general reasoning development in preschoolers, the sampler size was smaller and the role of contributing factors such as working memory, theory of mind, language, bilingualism/multilingualism, and social interaction on reasoning was not explored, and it remains a limitation of the present study’.

Is the work clearly and accurately presented and does it cite the current literature?

Yes

If applicable, is the statistical analysis and its interpretation appropriate?

Yes

Are all the source data underlying the results available to ensure full reproducibility?

Yes

Is the study design appropriate and is the work technically sound?

Yes

Are the conclusions drawn adequately supported by the results?

Yes

Are sufficient details of methods and analysis provided to allow replication by others?

Yes

Reviewer Expertise:

Early childhood Care and Education, Spatial intelligence

I confirm that I have read this submission and believe that I have an appropriate level of expertise to confirm that it is of an acceptable scientific standard.

F1000Res. 2024 Nov 21. doi: 10.5256/f1000research.172849.r330866

Reviewer response for version 3

Ramesh Kaipa 1

I enjoyed reading the resubmission. I recommend this article for approval.

Is the work clearly and accurately presented and does it cite the current literature?

Partly

If applicable, is the statistical analysis and its interpretation appropriate?

Yes

Are all the source data underlying the results available to ensure full reproducibility?

Yes

Is the study design appropriate and is the work technically sound?

Yes

Are the conclusions drawn adequately supported by the results?

Partly

Are sufficient details of methods and analysis provided to allow replication by others?

Yes

Reviewer Expertise:

Speech and Language Therapy

I confirm that I have read this submission and believe that I have an appropriate level of expertise to confirm that it is of an acceptable scientific standard.

F1000Res. 2024 Aug 27. doi: 10.5256/f1000research.168597.r300709

Reviewer response for version 2

Ramesh Kaipa 1

I thank the authors for addressing my comments. My only comment is that considering a moderate test-retest reliability (0.66), how can authors explain the reliability of the findings? Otherwise, this paper is a good addition to the existing literature.

Is the work clearly and accurately presented and does it cite the current literature?

Partly

If applicable, is the statistical analysis and its interpretation appropriate?

Yes

Are all the source data underlying the results available to ensure full reproducibility?

Yes

Is the study design appropriate and is the work technically sound?

Yes

Are the conclusions drawn adequately supported by the results?

Partly

Are sufficient details of methods and analysis provided to allow replication by others?

Yes

Reviewer Expertise:

Speech and Language Therapy

I confirm that I have read this submission and believe that I have an appropriate level of expertise to confirm that it is of an acceptable scientific standard, however I have significant reservations, as outlined above.

F1000Res. 2024 Oct 1.
Gagan Bajaj 1

1.  I thank the authors for addressing my comments. My only comment is that considering a moderate test-retest reliability (0.66), how can authors explain the reliability of the findings? Otherwise, this paper is a good addition to the existing literature.

Reply: We thank the reviewer for highlighting this aspect and giving us the opportunity to clarify. We have discussed the moderate test-retest reliability of our task in relation to other similar cognitive tasks. Furthermore, we have provided a potential explanation for these findings and outlined future directions for further optimization in the discussion and future directions sections of the manuscript, respectively. We hope the reviewer finds our efforts satisfactory.

F1000Res. 2024 Aug 23. doi: 10.5256/f1000research.168597.r300710

Reviewer response for version 2

Andreas Demetriou 1

The paper is sufficiently improved so that it can be accepted for indexing.

Is the work clearly and accurately presented and does it cite the current literature?

Yes

If applicable, is the statistical analysis and its interpretation appropriate?

Yes

Are all the source data underlying the results available to ensure full reproducibility?

Yes

Is the study design appropriate and is the work technically sound?

Partly

Are the conclusions drawn adequately supported by the results?

No

Are sufficient details of methods and analysis provided to allow replication by others?

Yes

Reviewer Expertise:

Cognitive development and individual differences in intelligence. Integration of cognitive developmental and psychometric theory.

I confirm that I have read this submission and believe that I have an appropriate level of expertise to confirm that it is of an acceptable scientific standard.

F1000Res. 2024 Oct 1.
Gagan Bajaj 1

1. The paper is sufficiently improved so that it can be accepted for indexing.

Reply: We are grateful to the reviewer for accepting the paper for indexing.

F1000Res. 2023 Nov 21. doi: 10.5256/f1000research.144794.r206526

Reviewer response for version 1

Andreas Demetriou 1

The study is interesting and the paper well written. It has several limitations:

  1. The number of children in each group is small. Power must be reported.

  2. No specification of logical schemes is made.

  3. No differentiation between changes in reasoning and language. No association with brain is justified.

Is the work clearly and accurately presented and does it cite the current literature?

Yes

If applicable, is the statistical analysis and its interpretation appropriate?

Yes

Are all the source data underlying the results available to ensure full reproducibility?

Yes

Is the study design appropriate and is the work technically sound?

Partly

Are the conclusions drawn adequately supported by the results?

No

Are sufficient details of methods and analysis provided to allow replication by others?

Yes

Reviewer Expertise:

Cognitive development and individual differences in intelligence. Integration of cognitive developmental and psychometric theory.

I confirm that I have read this submission and believe that I have an appropriate level of expertise to confirm that it is of an acceptable scientific standard, however I have significant reservations, as outlined above.

F1000Res. 2024 Jun 28.
Gagan Bajaj 1

1. The study is interesting and the paper well written. It has several limitations.

Reply: We are grateful to the reviewer for the constructive feedback and suggestions. The authors have endeavoured to revise the manuscript according to these valuable suggestions and hope that the reviewer will find our efforts satisfactory.

2. The number of children in each group is small. Power must be reported.

Reply: The sample size of the study was determined using the formula n = Z 1-α/2 2 SD 2 /d 2 at 95% confidence level, 20% absolute error, 80% power with an anticipated standard deviation of 0.789 based on pilot testing. The same has been reported in the method section.  The present study was initiated as a preliminary investigation of a larger project aimed at exploring cognitive-communication skills within this population. Undoubtedly, given its preliminary nature, the study was constrained by a smaller sample size, a limitation that has been transparently acknowledged and addressed within the study's limitations section.

3. No specification of logical schemes is made.

Reply: This research serves as a continuation of our previous work, which developed a tool specifically designed for assessing cognitive-communication skills among preschoolers in the Indian context, based on a story (Prasanna, Bajaj, et al., 2024). The current study represents the evaluation phase of this tool, focusing on the reasoning development among Indian preschoolers. The manuscript has been updated to clearly emphasize this aspect. Additionally, we have provided sub-headings in the method section to enhance the logical flow of the information.

4.  No differentiation between changes in reasoning and language.

Reply:  Thank you for the kind feedback. We acknowledge that the present study exclusively included children with age-appropriate speech and language abilities, with their responses analysed for cognitive accuracy. While we recognize the potential for analysing responses in terms of various linguistic elements, such as syntactic and semantic depth, this aspect was beyond the scope of the present research. For example, in a task involving a reasoning question like "Why did Virat get scared?" responses such as "Dog," "When saw dog," and "Because dog was there" were all deemed correct reasoning responses, despite their linguistic differences in Mean Length of Utterance (MLU).

We value the reviewer's suggestion to distinguish between cognitive and linguistic aspects, and we look forward to incorporating this consideration into our future research, particularly in the study of reasoning abilities among children with language disorders. Same has been updated under the future directions of the present study.

5.  No association with brain is justified.

Reply: Thank you for the kind suggestion. The neurophysiological correlates of reasoning abilities among preschoolers, discussed in the present work, have been updated in the discussion section.

F1000Res. 2023 Jul 5. doi: 10.5256/f1000research.144794.r170971

Reviewer response for version 1

Ramesh Kaipa 1

Summary: This is a well-developed study. The authors have attempted to develop a stimulus that is culturally relevant to Indian children. The authors need to be appreciated for this line of work. I do have some comments that are mentioned below.

Introduction:

  1. The rationale for this study is not developed appropriately. The authors need to justify how will the current line of research advance the empirical knowledge.

  2. It would help if the authors could review prior literature pertaining to reasoning skills and language development in children.

Methods:

  1. “What did the authors mean by “Children who qualified across all the ten items of the TQS”. Is there a cut-off score in TQS? The authors expand on this.

  2. Can authors present data on the participants’ language usage? Were they bilingual or multilingual? I believe this variable could skew the findings.

  3. The authors mentioned that 13 children were excluded. Was it from the cohort of 63 children?

  4. Can I please have the authors present data on the reliability and validity of the assessment tool “A Day at the Grandparents House”?

  5. If the story was designed in English, how did the authors make sure that the translated version was faithful to the original version? It would be helpful to compare the psychometric properties of the original version to the translated version.

  6. Can the authors present the findings of test-retest reliability for the pilot test on 15 children?

Results

  1. The results are well presented, along with visual illustrations.

Discussion

  1. The authors make a case that the findings can be attributed to biological maturation. The authors need to discuss the specific neurophysiological substrates that are correlated with higher-order tasks such as the one used in the current study. 

Is the work clearly and accurately presented and does it cite the current literature?

Partly

If applicable, is the statistical analysis and its interpretation appropriate?

Yes

Are all the source data underlying the results available to ensure full reproducibility?

Yes

Is the study design appropriate and is the work technically sound?

Yes

Are the conclusions drawn adequately supported by the results?

Partly

Are sufficient details of methods and analysis provided to allow replication by others?

Yes

Reviewer Expertise:

Speech and Language Therapy

I confirm that I have read this submission and believe that I have an appropriate level of expertise to confirm that it is of an acceptable scientific standard, however I have significant reservations, as outlined above.

F1000Res. 2024 Jun 28.
Gagan Bajaj 1

1. Summary: This is a well-developed study. The authors have attempted to develop a stimulus that is culturally relevant to Indian children. The authors need to be appreciated for this line of work. I do have some comments that are mentioned below.

Reply: We are grateful to the reviewer for the constructive feedback and suggestions. The authors have endeavoured to revise the manuscript according to these valuable suggestions and hope that the reviewer will find our efforts satisfactory.

2. Introduction

2a. The rationale for this study is not developed appropriately. The authors need to justify how will the current line of research advance the empirical knowledge.

Reply: The rationale of the study has been revisited to ensure that it aligns with the research gaps illustrated in the introduction and justifies the potential of the present work in advancing empirical knowledge.

2b. It would help if the authors could review prior literature pertaining to reasoning skills and language development in children.

Reply: The literature on reasoning skills and their significance in children's language development has been reviewed and updated within the manuscript. Consequently, the importance of the association between the two has been underscored in the rationale and discussion sections of the manuscript, respectively.

3. Methods

3a. What did the authors mean by “Children who qualified across all the ten items of the TQS.” Is there a cut-off score in TQS? The authors expand on this.

Reply: Thank you for providing the opportunity to clarify. The TQS does not have any cut-off scoring; instead, it functions as a screening tool for the presence or absence of childhood disabilities. In the present study, the TQS was utilized to ensure that only participants who were found to have no disability across all ten items of the questionnaire were included. This statement has been rephrased in the manuscript to enhance clarity.

3b. Can authors present data on the participants’ language usage? Were they bilingual or multilingual? I believe this variable could skew the findings.

Reply: The information regarding the number of languages known by the participants in the study has been updated in the manuscript. Although 89% of the participants were bilingual and the remaining 11% multilingual, all of them had English as their L2, with a linguistic proficiency score in English greater than 7 on LEAP-Q (child version). However, since the present study focused solely on the English language, we did not perform an analysis exploring performance differences in the children based on the nature of their L1. This has been noted as a limitation and a future direction in the manuscript.

3c. The authors mentioned that 13 children were excluded. Was it from the cohort of 63 children?

Reply:  Thank you for providing the opportunity to clarify. A total of 76 children were considered for the study, out of which 13 children were excluded based on the study selection criteria. This left us with 63 children, who were further subdivided into three age groups, each comprising 21 children.

3d. Can I please have the authors present data on the reliability and validity of the assessment tool “A Day at the Grandparents House”?

Reply:  Thank you for the opportunity to clarify. The tool used in the present research was developed in our previous work, where its content validity was established (Prasanna, Bajaj, et al., 2024). This information is referenced in the method section of the current study. Further evaluation of the tool's psychometric properties, specifically in terms of test-retest and inter-rater reliability, was conducted in the present research through a pilot study. The findings are detailed in the results section. The content has been updated accordingly to enhance clarity.

3e. If the story was designed in English, how did the authors make sure that the translated version was faithful to the original version? It would be helpful to compare the psychometric properties of the original version to the translated version.

Reply:   We apologize for the lack of clarity in our write-up, which led to this confusion. The present research employed the original English version of the tool, without any translations. Our study explored reasoning development among preschoolers using a story-based cognitive-communication assessment tool developed in our previous work (Prasanna, Bajaj, et al., 2024). The content validity of this tool was established in that prior research. In the current study, reliability measures for the reasoning tasks were established through a pilot study before assessing the reasoning abilities of the participants. The method section has been revised to avoid any confusion.

3f. Can the authors present the findings of test-retest reliability for the pilot test on 15 children?

Reply:  The test-retest reliability of the tool was established through the pilot testing on 15 children and the findings are presented in the results section.

  1. Results: The results are well presented, along with visual illustrations.

Reply: Thank you for the kind feedback.

  1. Discussion

5a. The authors make a case that the findings can be attributed to biological maturation. The authors need to discuss the specific neurophysiological substrates that are correlated with higher-order tasks such as the one used in the current study.

Reply: Thank you for the kind suggestion. The neurophysiological correlates of reasoning abilities among preschoolers, discussed in the present work, have been updated in the discussion section.

Associated Data

    This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

    Data Citations

    1. Prasanna A, Bajaj G, Anil MA, et al. : Reasoning Research Data among Indian Preschoolers.[Dataset]. figshare. 2023a. 10.6084/m9.figshare.22140617.v1 [DOI]
    2. Prasanna A, Bajaj G, Anil MA, et al. : STROBE checklist for the article.[Dataset]. figshare. 2023b. 10.6084/m9.figshare.22140689.v2 [DOI]

    Data Availability Statement

    Underlying data

    Figshare: ‘An Insight into developmental changes in reasoning skills among Indian Preschoolers - A Cross-Sectional Study using a story-based approach’, https://doi.org/10.6084/m9.figshare.22140617.v1 ( Prasanna et al., 2023a).

    This project contains the following underlying data:

    • Data file 1. (Quantitative and Qualitative data of Reasoning responses)

    Reporting guidelines

    Figshare: STROBE checklist for ‘An Insight into developmental changes in reasoning skills among Indian Preschoolers - A Cross-Sectional Study using a story-based approach’, https://doi.org/10.6084/m9.figshare.22140689.v2 ( Prasanna et al., 2023b).

    Data are available under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International license (CC-BY 4.0).


    Articles from F1000Research are provided here courtesy of F1000 Research Ltd

    RESOURCES