Skip to main content
Wiley Open Access Collection logoLink to Wiley Open Access Collection
. 2024 Feb 28;33(6):e14180. doi: 10.1111/jsr.14180

The effect of single‐component sleep restriction therapy on depressive symptoms: A systematic review and meta‐analysis

Katrina Yan Kei Tse 1, Leonie Franziska Maurer 2, Colin Alexander Espie 1,3,4, Simon David Kyle 1,
PMCID: PMC11596993  PMID: 38419123

Summary

Sleep restriction therapy is a behavioural component within cognitive behavioural therapy for insomnia and is an effective standalone treatment for insomnia, but its effect on depressive symptoms remains unclear. This review aimed to synthesise and evaluate the impact of single‐component sleep restriction therapy on depressive symptoms relative to a control intervention. We searched electronic databases and sleep‐related journals for randomised controlled trials and uncontrolled clinical trials, published from 1 January 1986 until 19 August 2023, that delivered sleep restriction therapy to adults with insomnia. Random‐effects meta‐analysis of standardised mean differences and Cochrane risk of bias assessment were performed on randomised controlled trials, while uncontrolled clinical trials were discussed narratively. The meta‐analysis was pre‐registered on PROSPERO (ID: CRD42020191803). We identified seven randomised controlled trials (N = 1102) and two uncontrolled clinical trials (N = 22). Findings suggest that sleep restriction therapy is associated with a medium effect for improvement in depressive symptoms at post‐treatment (N c  = 6, g = −0.45 [95% confidence interval = −0.70 to −0.21], p < 0.001) and a small effect at follow‐up (N c  = 4, g = −0.31 [95% confidence interval = −0.45 to −0.16], p < 0.001). Five of the seven included randomised controlled trials were judged to have a high risk of bias. Standalone sleep restriction therapy appears to be efficacious for improving depressive symptoms at post‐treatment and follow‐up. However, conclusions are tentative due to the small number of trials and because none of the trials was performed in a population with clinically defined depression. Large‐scale trials are needed to test the effect of sleep restriction therapy in patients experiencing depression and insomnia. Findings also highlight the need to improve the standardisation and reporting of sleep restriction therapy procedures, and to design studies with more rigorous control arms to reduce potential bias.

Keywords: cognitive behavioural therapy, depression, insomnia, meta‐analysis, psychological intervention, sleep restriction therapy

1. INTRODUCTION

Insomnia disorder is a chronic sleep disorder characterised by difficulties initiating and/or maintaining night‐time sleep, which in turn causes distress and impairments during the day (American Psychiatric Association, 2013). Despite being under‐recognised and underdiagnosed (Grandner & Chakravorty, 2017; Grandner & Malhotra, 2015; Saleem et al., 2017), insomnia is highly prevalent, with about 10% experiencing symptoms severe enough to meet Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorder, Fifth Edition (DSM‐5) criteria for insomnia disorder (Davidson et al., 2019; Ree et al., 2017; Sateia et al., 2017; Wilson et al., 2019). Insomnia is associated with reduced quality of life (Kyle et al., 2010; LeBlanc et al., 2007) and confers marked risk for medical disorders (Sofi et al., 2014; Spiegelhalder et al., 2010) as well as mental disorders, such as major depressive disorder (Baglioni et al., 2011; Hertenstein et al., 2019).

Insomnia and depression are closely linked, and there is high comorbidity between the two (Staner, 2010). Individuals with insomnia are likely to experience more negative mood and less positive mood as a consequence of poor sleep (Buysse et al., 2007; Riedel & Lichstein, 2000). Additionally, sleep impairment is one of the key diagnostic criteria for depression (American Psychiatric Association, 2013). Of those individuals with a current depressive episode, more than 60% report having insomnia‐related sleep disturbances (Ohayon et al., 2000; Tsuno et al., 2005; Weissman et al., 1996; Yates et al., 2007). Furthermore, epidemiological studies reveal that insomnia is a key predictor for the onset of depression (Baglioni et al., 2011; Hertenstein et al., 2019) and increases the risk of depression twofold (Baglioni et al., 2011; Li et al., 2016). Complaints of disrupted sleep are one of the most common residual symptoms following depression remission (Carney et al., 2007), and confer an increased risk of relapse and recurrence (Baglioni et al., 2011; Li et al., 2016). Given the strong associations between insomnia and depression, treatments that alleviate both insomnia and depressive symptoms are important to provide optimal treatment outcome.

The recommended treatment of choice for insomnia disorder is cognitive behavioural therapy for insomnia (CBT‐I; Brasure et al., 2016; Qaseem et al., 2016; Riemann et al., 2023; Sateia et al., 2017; Schutte‐Rodin et al., 2008; World Health Organization, 2021). As a multicomponent psychological therapy, CBT‐I typically consists of sleep restriction therapy (SRT), stimulus control therapy, and cognitive therapies, supplemented by sleep hygiene education (SHE) and relaxation training (Baglioni et al., 2022; Morin & Benca, 2012). There is a large body of evidence demonstrating its efficacy in improving night‐time insomnia symptoms (Geiger‐Brown et al., 2015; Ho et al., 2015; Irwin et al., 2006; Koffel et al., 2015; Montgomery & Dennis, 2004; Murtagh & Greenwood, 1995; Okajima et al., 2010; Pallesen et al., 1998; Trauer et al., 2015), daytime insomnia symptoms (Benz et al., 2020) as well as depressive symptoms (Ballesio et al., 2018; Benz et al., 2020; Cunningham & Shapiro, 2018). In particular, multicomponent CBT‐I is associated with a medium effect (0.34–0.37) for improvement in depressive symptoms at post‐treatment (Ballesio et al., 2018; Benz et al., 2020). Furthermore, a recent review revealed that CBT‐I may be effective in preventing the development of depression in individuals without diagnoses of depression (Boland et al., 2023). These findings suggest sleep improvement may be a mechanism through which depressive symptoms improved and depression onset could be prevented.

Sleep restriction therapy (Spielman et al., 1987) is often considered the most active component of CBT‐I (Riemann et al., 2023), given its strong association with treatment outcomes (Maurer et al., 2021; Miller et al., 2014). It involves reducing excessive time in bed and reducing variability in bedtime and risetime, both of which are considered perpetuating factors in the maintenance of insomnia. According to the Triple R model (Maurer et al., 2018), the three key pathways through which SRT exerts its effect are: (1) restricting time spent in bed over successive nights directly strengthens homeostatic sleep drive and dampens pre‐sleep hyperarousal; (2) regularising sleep schedule tightens the circadian control of sleep and wakefulness, improving the stability and consolidation of sleep; (3) reconditioning the association between bedroom factors and sleep increases the likelihood that bedroom‐related stimuli will trigger sleep response.

Compared with multicomponent CBT‐I, single‐component SRT has a similar order of magnitude in improving self‐reported sleep continuity parameters and insomnia symptoms (Maurer et al., 2021; Okajima et al., 2010). In particular, Maurer et al.'s (2021) meta‐analysis revealed that SRT has a medium‐to‐large effect size for improving self‐reported sleep continuity parameters (i.e. sleep‐onset latency, wake‐time after sleep onset and sleep efficiency [SE]) and a large effect size for improving insomnia severity (measured by the Insomnia Severity Index) compared with control at post‐treatment. Furthermore, the American Academy of Sleep Medicine (AASM) clinical practice guidelines in 2021 (Edinger et al., 2021) gave a conditional recommendation in favour of SRT as treatment of chronic insomnia in adults. As a protocol‐based, single‐component treatment, SRT may have the potential to be integrated with depression treatment to improve sleep outcomes in patients experiencing depression. However, it is currently unclear to what extent SRT is effective in improving depressive symptoms. Understanding the effect of SRT on depressive symptoms could also offer valuable insights into the role of sleep regularity and sleep continuity in improving depression.

1.1. Objectives

Previous meta‐analyses have primarily focused on the effects of multi‐component CBT‐I on depressive symptoms, but little is known about the effectiveness of SRT on depressive symptoms. Thus, the current study aimed to appraise the evidence for single‐component SRT on depressive symptoms in individuals with insomnia relative to a control intervention.

2. METHODS

The systematic review and meta‐analysis were undertaken in compliance with PRISMA guidelines for the reporting of systematic reviews (Page et al., 2021). We included multiple study designs in our search, including randomised controlled trials (RCTs) and uncontrolled clinical trials (UCTs), to provide a comprehensive overview of the literature available. Meta‐analysis was conducted on RCTs, while UCTs were discussed narratively. The protocol for the meta‐analysis was pre‐registered on PROSPERO (ID: CRD42020191803). We initially planned, as a secondary objective, to also extract and analyse mood‐related outcomes beyond depression; however, our search only yielded one study, precluding any meaningful syntheses. We do however include a table describing this study in the supplement (Appendix 5).

2.1. Search strategy

Published and unpublished studies were identified by searching electronic databases (PubMed, Web of Knowledge, Scopus, CENTRAL, and PsychINFO), clinical trial registries (ClinicalTrials.gov, ISRCTN Registry and WHO International Clinical Trials Registry Platform) and journals focused on sleep research (Journal of Sleep Research, Sleep, Sleep Medicine, Behavioural Sleep Medicine). The database coverage included studies published in English between 1 January 1986 (1 year before the publication of the SRT guidelines by Spielman et al., 1987) and 19 August 2023. The search strategy used for all database platforms was the same as the previous meta‐analyses of SRT (Maurer et al., 2018; Miller et al., 2014): “insomnia”, “sleep disorder” or “sleep disturbance”, AND “sleep restrict*”, “sleep compress*”, “bedtime restriction” or “time in bed restriction” in title, abstract or keyword (see Appendix 6 for full line by line search strategy in each database). Additionally, reference lists of the included studies were searched for potentially relevant studies.

All citations produced by the search strategy were exported to a reference management software (Mendeley, Elsevier, London, UK) where duplicates were removed. Search results were screened for relevance based on title and abstract by the first author (KT). Records that did not include individuals with insomnia and did not implement SRT were excluded based on title and/or abstract. Full‐text articles were then obtained for the screened citations, which were subsequently screened further by KT using the eligibility criteria below. Study selection was discussed between authors (KT, LM and SK) where there was uncertainty until consensus was reached.

2.2. Eligibility criteria

Inclusion criteria were as follows. (1) Adults (≥ 18 years old). (2) Sample fulfils the criteria for insomnia disorder, as verified by diagnostic criteria; or self‐report symptoms of insomnia, assessed by validated questionnaires, clinical interviews or sleep diary criteria. Individuals with insomnia comorbid with other medical or mental health conditions were included as long as the focus was on insomnia. (3) Implemented a standalone SRT intervention (or in combination with sleep hygiene). Studies that delivered a modification of SRT were included as long as the treatment involved systematic reduction of time in bed for the treatment of insomnia, consistent with the approach of Spielman et al. (1987), whereby an initial, curtailed sleep window was prescribed with the aim of extending over subsequent weeks based on pre‐specified SE criteria. (4) Outcomes included mean scores of depressive symptoms using validated depression questionnaires. (5) RCTs and UCTs published in English between 1986 and 2023. (6) For RCTS, permitted control conditions included receiving no treatment, wait list control (WLC), treatment as usual, or control interventions that have no evidence for improving sleep or insomnia (i.e. sleep hygiene; Edinger et al., 2021).

Exclusion criteria were: (1) conference abstract, dissertation, letter, case study or review; (2) applied sleep compression as the intervention, which involved using a more incremental and progressive approach to decreasing time in bed; or (3) implemented SRT as a package treatment (e.g. brief behavioural intervention for insomnia, which combines SRT with stimulus control).

2.3. Data extraction

The following characteristics were extracted from the RCTs and UCTs: (1) publication year; (2) geographic location; (3) study design; (4) total number of participants randomised, and the number of female participants; (5) mean age and standard deviation; (6) method of recruitment; (7) hypnotic or psychiatric medication status; (8) comorbidity; (9) diagnostic criteria for insomnia; (10) intervention format; (11) control group; (12) measure of depressive symptoms. We also extracted information on SRT characteristics for each study: (1) sleep window generation; (2) minimum time in bed; (3) SE criteria and change to sleep window; (4) position of the sleep window. The first author (KT) extracted the study characteristics.

Two researchers (KT, LM) independently extracted summary data from RCTs (means and standard deviations of depressive symptoms, and number of participants providing outcome data) from each intervention group (SRT and control) at all available timepoints, and cross‐checked their findings. Where there was uncertainty, the authors (KT, LM and SK) discussed until consensus was reached and reached out to corresponding authors where necessary.

2.4. Risk of bias in individual studies

All RCTs were appraised using the Revised Cochrane risk‐of‐bias tool for randomised trials (RoB2; Sterne et al., 2019). Studies were rated against the following criteria: (1) randomisation process; (2) deviations from intended interventions; (3) missing outcome data; (4) measurement of the outcome; (5) selection of the reported result. The overall rating varies between low risk, some concerns, or high risk. Two researchers (KT, LM) independently evaluated the risk of bias in each study and cross‐checked their findings. Where there was a discrepancy in the appraisal of the risk of bias, the authors (KT, LM, SK) discussed until a consensus was reached.

2.5. Data synthesis and analysis

The primary outcome for this review was self‐reported depressive symptoms at post‐treatment for RCTs. Exploratory analysis was conducted on self‐reported measures of depressive symptoms at follow‐up. Effect sizes and 95% confidence intervals (CIs) were calculated for the between‐group differences at post‐treatment and follow‐up. Post‐treatment was defined as the assessment point after treatment completion; follow‐up was defined as the single next assessment point after post‐treatment.

Since studies assessed depressive symptoms using different psychometric scales, standardised mean difference was used to standardise the findings of the studies to a uniform scale before they were combined. This was conducted by calculating the size of the treatment effect (difference in mean outcome between groups) relative to the variability of the study (standard deviation of outcome among participants).

Hedges' g (Hedges, 1981) was the effect size statistic for this meta‐analysis. Hedges' g was expressed as the difference between the mean of the treatment group against the mean of the control group, relative to the pooled weighted standard deviation:

g=μtxμctrls,

where μtx and μctrl denote the mean of the treatment group and control group, respectively, and s denotes the pooled weighted standard deviation (SD):

s=ntx1SDtx2+nctrl1SDctrl2ntx+nctrl2,

where ntx and nctrl denote the number of participants in treatment and control groups, respectively, and SDtx and SDctrl denote the standard deviation of treatment and control groups, respectively. Effect sizes were interpreted as either small (0–0.32), medium (0.33–0.55) or large (≥ 0.56; Lipsey & Wilson, 1993). Effect sizes were calculated using published means and standard deviation. Where studies reported other measures of dispersion, such as standard errors (SE), the following formula was used to transform to standard deviation:

SD=SE×N,

where N denotes the sample size.

Meta‐analyses were performed using the meta 6.2‐1 package (Balduzzi et al., 2019) in R (version 4.2.2). The inverse‐variance method was used to conduct the meta‐analysis. The random effects model was employed as we expected heterogeneity between the studies (e.g. different SRT components, different control groups). The Wald‐type CI was presented. Restricted maximum likelihood estimator was used to estimate between‐study variance (Veroniki et al., 2016). Heterogeneity was inspected using I 2 statistics, which describes the percentage of variation across studies (Higgins et al., 2003; Higgins & Thompson, 2002). We defined I 2 values as either low (0%–30%), moderate (31%–50%), substantial (51%–75%) or considerable heterogeneity (76%–100%). The 95% CI around I 2 were calculated. In line with the recommendations from the Cochrane Handbook for Systematic Reviews of Interventions (Higgins & Green, 2022), for meta‐analysis with less than 10 studies, publication bias was not assessed. Forest plots were created to provide graphical overview of the meta‐analysis.

3. RESULTS

3.1. Study selection

We identified 1222 records from the search. After removing duplicates, we screened 962 titles and abstracts, of which 916 records were excluded for not delivering SRT as a standalone intervention, not including measures of depressive symptoms, or not having exclusively adults as participants. We then assessed 46 full‐text studies for eligibility, of which 37 studies were excluded because: (1) it was a conference abstract (n = 15); (2) standalone SRT was not implemented (n = 5); and (3) measures of depressive symptoms were not included (n = 17). The list of excluded studies from full‐text screening can be found in Table S6 in Appendix 7. In total, nine studies were eligible for inclusion in the review, of which seven studies were RCTs and were included in the meta‐analysis, while two studies were UCTs and were discussed narratively (Figure 1). One study (Falloon et al., 2015) allowed participants to be as young as 16 years to participate. We nevertheless included this study because their baseline characteristics showed that participants were clearly from the adult population (mean age, SRT = 55.4 ± 12.7; control = 51.8 ± 13.4).

FIGURE 1.

FIGURE 1

PRISMA flowchart showing the process of study selection of the systematic review. RCT, randomised controlled trials; SRT, sleep restriction therapy; UCT, uncontrolled clinical trials.

3.2. Study characteristics

Table 1 summarises the characteristics of studies included in the review. The earliest study was published in 2012 (Epstein et al., 2012), and the latest in 2023 (Kyle et al., 2023). RCTs recruited more participants (n range: 49–642) than UCTs (n range: 7–15). The number of participants from Kyle et al. (2023) represented 62% of the total number of participants in the meta‐analysis at post‐treatment (total N p = 847, Kyle N p = 528) and 70% at follow‐up (total N p = 735, Kyle N p = 512). Three studies recruited participants from the community (Epstein et al., 2012; Lancee et al., 2019; Maurer et al., 2020), two studies recruited participants from primary care (Falloon et al., 2015; Kyle et al., 2023), and four studies recruited from both community and primary care settings (Aji et al., 2020; Gieselmann & Pietrowsky, 2019; Kalmbach et al., 2019; Krieger et al., 2019). Six studies recruited participants who were free of medication (Aji et al., 2020; Epstein et al., 2012; Falloon et al., 2015; Kalmbach et al., 2019; Lancee et al., 2019; Maurer et al., 2020), and six studies excluded psychiatric comorbidity (Aji et al., 2020; Epstein et al., 2012; Falloon et al., 2015; Kalmbach et al., 2019; Krieger et al., 2019; Maurer et al., 2020). Depression severity was not a primary outcome in any of the included studies, and none of the studies specifically recruited patients with a diagnosis for depression (see Appendix 2 for baseline score of depression measure); though Lancee 2019 was the only included study to have depression as part of their inclusion criteria. Five studies (Falloon et al., 2015; Gieselmann & Pietrowsky, 2019; Kalmbach et al., 2019; Krieger et al., 2019; Maurer et al., 2020) recruited participants with depression scores classified as “normal”/“mild depression”, and two studies (Epstein et al., 2012; Kyle et al., 2023) recruited participants with depression scores classified as “moderate depression” (Appendix 2). In all studies, the diagnostic criteria for insomnia were verified using validated questionnaires, clinical interviews and/or sleep diary. Hypnotic medication was allowed in three studies (Gieselmann & Pietrowsky, 2019; Krieger et al., 2019; Kyle et al., 2023) and psychiatric comorbidity was allowed in three studies (Gieselmann & Pietrowsky, 2019; Kyle et al., 2023; Lancee et al., 2019).

TABLE 1.

Characteristics of studies included in the review.

Author, year, location Study design Sample size randomised (female), mean age (SD), recruitment Hypnotic medication status, comorbidity Diagnostic criteria of insomnia (determined by) Intervention & its format Control Depression measure (PT, FU) Risk of bias
Kyle et al. (2023), UK RCT

N = 642 (489 f)

SRT (n = 321): 55.7 years (15.3)

SHE (n = 321): 55.2 years (16.5)

Adults from medical practices

Medication allowed, psychiatry comorbidity allowed DSM‐5 Insomnia Disorder (SCI)

4‐session SRT

Individual, FtF (S1, S3), Phone (S2, S4)

SHE

PHQ‐9

(3 m, 6 m, 12 m)

Some risk of bias
Maurer et al. (2020), UK RCT

N = 56 (39 f)

SRT (n = 27): 40.63 years (9.13)

TBR (n = 29): 40.93 years (9.24)

Adults from community

Medication free, psychiatric comorbidity excluded DSM‐5 Insomnia Disorder (SCI ≤ 16 and telephone interview)

4‐week SRT

Individual, FtF (1, 2 week), Phone (3, 4 week)

TBR

HADS‐D

(4 wk, 3 m)

Some risk of bias
Krieger et al. (2019), Switzerland RCT

N = 62 (45 f)

SRT (n = 41): 46.59 years (17.52)

CAU (n = 21): 45.24 years (12.40)

Adults from physician referrals and community

Medication allowed, psychiatric comorbidity excluded ICSD‐3 Insomnia Disorder (Telephone interview)

8‐week SRT

Internet‐based, one session per week

CAU

ADS‐K

(8 week, 6 m)

High risk of bias
Kalmbach (2019), USA RCT

N = 102 (102 f)

SRT (n = 52): 56.65 years (4.95)

SHE (n = 50): 57.34 years (5.97)

Postmenopausal women from primary care, sleep clinic and community

Medication free, depression comorbidity excluded DSM‐5 Insomnia Disorder Self‐reported WASO of ≥ 1 hr on ≥ 3 nights per week; objective WASO of ≥ 45 min (Clinical interview & 2 nights of PSG)

2‐week SRT

Individual, FtF (initial & ending session), phone (every 3–4 days between 1 and 2 weeks)

SHE

BDI‐II

(4 week, 6 m)

High risk of bias

Gieselmann (2019),

Germany

RCT

N = 49 (25 f)

SRT (n = 27): 39.3 years (14.47)

WLC (n = 22): 42.74 years (11.73)

Adults from medical practice, university and community

Medication allowed, psychiatric comorbidity allowed Insomnia disorder SOL of > 30 min for ≥ 3 nights per week over the last 6 months (Clinical interview) 3‐week SRT (combined with imagination exercise and sleep hygiene advise), individual, FtF (1, 3 weeks) WLC

CES‐D

(4 week, 2 m)

High risk of bias
Falloon (2015), New Zealand RCT

N = 97 (75 f)

SSR (n = 46): 55.4 years (12.7)

SHE (n = 51): 51.8 years (13.4)

Adults from general practices

Medication free, psychiatric comorbidity excluded

Insomnia disorder for ≥ 6 months

Sleep difficulties on ≥ 3 nights per week (Questionnaires, physical examination, sleep diary at baseline)

3‐week SSR

Individual, FtF (1, 3 weeks)

SHE

PHQ‐9

(/, 6 m)

High risk of bias

Epstein (2012),

Canada

RCT

N = 94 (57 f)

SRT (n = 44): 68.00 years (8.25)

WLC (n = 50): 69.50 years (8.34)

Older adults from community

Medication free, psychiatric comorbidity excluded Insomnia Disorder for ≥ 6 months sleep onset or maintenance of ≥ 45 min per night for ≥ 3 nights per week; Complained of impaired daytime functioning (2‐week sleep diary at baseline) 6‐week SRT (with sleep hygiene instructions), Group (4–6 individuals), FtF (1, 2, 4 weeks), Phone (5–6 weeks) WLC

GDS

(6 week, /)

High risk of bias

Aji (2020),

Australia

UCT

N = 15 (15 f)

40.0 years (14.2)

Adults from clinician referrals and community

Medication free, severe psychiatric comorbidity excluded DSM‐5 Insomnia Disorder (ISI ≥ 15, PSQI > 5, clinical interview) 3‐week SRT Mobile app /

HADS‐D

(6 week, /)

/

Lancee (2019),

Netherlands

UCT

N = 7 (4 f)

45.7 years (range 28–69)

Adults from community

Medication free, insomnia with depressive symptoms, psychiatric comorbidity allowed Insomnia disorder (SCID‐5‐RV, ISI ≥ 10, SE ≤ 85, clinical interview) 6‐week SRT Individual, FtF (1, 3 weeks), phone (2, 4, 5, 6 weeks) /

PHQ‐9 & BDI‐II

(6 week, 3 & 6 m)

/

Abbreviations: ADS‐K, Allgemeine Depressions‐Skala – Kurzform (German version of CES‐D); BDI‐II, Beck Depression Inventory II; CAU, care as usual; CES‐D, Center for Epidemiological Studies Depression Scale; DSM‐5, Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorder, Fifth Edition; FtF, face to face; FU, follow‐up; GDS, Geriatric Depression Scale; HADS‐D, Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale – Depression Subscale; ICSD‐3, International Classification of Sleep Disorders, Third Edition; ISI, Insomnia Severity Index; PHQ‐9‐D, Patient Health Questionnaire – Depression Scale; PSG, polysomnography; PSQI, Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index; PT, post‐treatment; RCT, randomised controlled trial; SCI, sleep condition indicator; SCID‐RV, Structured Clinical Interview for DSM‐5 – Research Version; SE, sleep efficiency; SHE, sleep hygiene education; SOL, sleep‐onset latency; SRT, sleep restriction therapy; SSR, simplified sleep restriction; TBR, time in bed regularisation; UCT, uncontrolled clinical trial; WASO, wake after sleep onset; WLC, wait list control; S, session; m, month.

Of the nine studies, eight (Aji et al., 2020; Epstein et al., 2012; Gieselmann & Pietrowsky, 2019; Kalmbach et al., 2019; Krieger et al., 2019; Kyle et al., 2023; Lancee et al., 2019; Maurer et al., 2020) delivered SRT, while one (Falloon et al., 2015) delivered simplified sleep restriction (SSR), a modified version of SRT where initial sleep window is generated from self‐reported total sleep time plus 50% of the time spent awake in bed. We have created a breakdown of SRT procedures from included studies using guidelines as recommended by Kyle et al. (2015; Appendix 1). Four studies (Epstein et al., 2012; Falloon et al., 2015; Kyle et al., 2023; Maurer et al., 2020) provided information on all SRT parameters. All studies that reported the information set the sleep window based on diary‐reported total sleep time and positioned the sleep window based on patient preference. There was variability in minimum sleep window time, SE criteria and sleep window change. The duration of the SRT intervention ranged from 2 to 8 weeks. Six studies delivered individual intervention (Falloon et al., 2015; Gieselmann & Pietrowsky, 2019; Kalmbach et al., 2019; Kyle et al., 2023; Lancee et al., 2019; Maurer et al., 2020), one delivered group intervention (Epstein et al., 2012) and two delivered the intervention digitally (Aji et al., 2020; Krieger et al., 2019). Five studies delivered the intervention using both face‐to‐face sessions and phone calls (Epstein et al., 2012; Kalmbach et al., 2019; Kyle et al., 2023; Lancee et al., 2019; Maurer et al., 2020), while two used face‐to‐face sessions only (Falloon et al., 2015; Gieselmann & Pietrowsky, 2019). As for the control group, three RCTs used passive control groups, including WLC (Epstein et al., 2012; Gieselmann & Pietrowsky, 2019) and care as usual (CAU; Krieger et al., 2019), while three studies used SHE as minimally active control (Falloon et al., 2015; Kalmbach et al., 2019; Kyle et al., 2023) and one study used time in bed regularisation (TBR) as an active control (Maurer et al., 2020). The treatment rationale of TBR was bedtime consistency/regularity of bedtime and rise time, which is a common theme within SHE.

Measures of depressive symptoms were self‐reported questionnaires that assessed state‐level symptoms within a specific time frame, these included Beck Depression Inventory II (BDI‐II; Beck et al., 1996), Center for Epidemiological Studies Depression Scale (CES‐D; Radloff, 1977), Patient Health Questionnaire (PHQ‐9; Kroenke et al., 2001), Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale (HADS; Zigmond & Snaith, 1983) and the Geriatric Depression Scale (GDS; Yesavage et al., 1982). The post‐treatment period was between 4 and 12 weeks, while the follow‐up period was between 2 and 6 months. Kyle et al. (2023) also had a 12‐month follow‐up assessment in addition to their 6‐month follow‐up. Only the 3‐month post‐treatment and 6‐month follow‐up from Kyle et al. (2023) were included in the meta‐analysis given it was closest to timepoints of other RCTs.

3.3. Risk of bias within RCT studies

Figure 2 presents the quality assessment of the included RCT studies. Overall, we judged five studies (Epstein et al., 2012; Falloon et al., 2015; Gieselmann & Pietrowsky, 2019; Kalmbach et al., 2019; Krieger et al., 2019) to have a high risk of bias, and we raised some concerns of bias for two studies (Kyle et al., 2023; Maurer et al., 2020). All studies had an adequate randomisation procedure. Six studies (Epstein et al., 2012; Falloon et al., 2015; Kalmbach et al., 2019; Krieger et al., 2019; Kyle et al., 2023; Maurer et al., 2020) had an adequate allocation concealment, whereas one (Gieselmann & Pietrowsky, 2019) did not. Six studies (Epstein et al., 2012; Falloon et al., 2015; Gieselmann & Pietrowsky, 2019; Krieger et al., 2019; Kyle et al., 2023; Maurer et al., 2020) used treatment protocols to avoid deviations from intended interventions and used intention to treat analysis, while one (Kalmbach et al., 2019) did not. Six studies (Epstein et al., 2012; Falloon et al., 2015; Gieselmann & Pietrowsky, 2019; Krieger et al., 2019; Kyle et al., 2023; Maurer et al., 2020) reported completeness of outcome data and provided details on attrition rate, while one study (Kalmbach et al., 2019) did not.

FIGURE 2.

FIGURE 2

Risk of bias summary.

Being aware of the assigned intervention could create bias when measuring self‐reported depressive symptoms, which were assessed using participant‐reported questionnaires in all studies. Three studies (Epstein et al., 2012; Gieselmann & Pietrowsky, 2019; Krieger et al., 2019) used WLC or CAU as a minimally active control arm, which did not require any engagement with the treatment, making knowledge of the assigned treatment likely to bias the outcome assessment. Two studies (Kalmbach et al., 2019; Kyle et al., 2023) delivered SHE as a minimally active control arm through weekly emails (Kalmbach et al., 2019), a booklet (Kyle et al., 2023) or through verbal advice from the study general practitioner (Falloon et al., 2015). Although SHE did not match for time with the therapist and treatment structure with the intervention group (i.e. regular sessions), it required active engagement with the treatment (i.e. following SHE advice), making knowledge of the assigned treatment less likely to bias the outcome assessment. The control treatment from Maurer et al. (2020), TBR, matched well with SRT for time with the therapist, treatment engagement, and treatment structure, so knowledge of the assigned treatment was less likely to affect outcome assessment. Finally, only one study (Kyle et al., 2023) was pre‐registered, five studies (Epstein et al., 2012; Gieselmann & Pietrowsky, 2019; Kalmbach et al., 2019; Krieger et al., 2019; Maurer et al., 2020) registered the aims of the trial retrospectively, while one study (Falloon et al., 2015) had no registration for the trial. Appendix 3 presents the breakdown of each RoB2 item for each study.

4. META‐ANALYSIS

4.1. At post‐treatment

Six studies measured depressive symptoms at post‐treatment (Epstein et al., 2012; Gieselmann & Pietrowsky, 2019; Kalmbach et al., 2019; Krieger et al., 2019; Kyle et al., 2023; Maurer et al., 2020; Figure 3a). A significant medium effect was found for reduction of depressive symptoms favouring SRT arm versus control (N c = 6; g = −0.45 [95% CI = −0.70 to −0.21], p < 0.001). The I 2 test revealed moderate heterogeneity between studies (I 2 = 48% [95% CI = 0 to 79], p = 0.09).

FIGURE 3.

FIGURE 3

Forest plot: effects of sleep restriction therapy (SRT) on measures of depressive symptoms at post‐treatment and follow‐up.

4.2. At follow‐up

Four studies measured depressive symptoms at follow‐up (Falloon et al., 2015; Kalmbach et al., 2019; Kyle et al., 2023; Maurer et al., 2020; Figure 3b). A significant small effect was found for reduction of depressive symptoms favouring SRT versus control (N c = 4; g = −0.31 [95% CI = −0.45 to −0.16], p < 0.001). The I 2 test revealed small heterogeneity between studies (I 2 = 0% [95% CI = 0 to 85], p = 0.83).

4.3. Narrative synthesis of uncontrolled studies

Aji et al. (2020) delivered a 3‐week SRT protocol via mobile application to 15 women with insomnia. Baseline score of depressive symptoms (HADS‐D) was 7.3 (SD = 4.9), which is classified as “normal”. Aji reported no significant difference between baseline and 6‐week post‐treatment, although no means and standard deviation of post‐treatment HADS score were provided in the paper.

Lancee et al. (2019) delivered 6‐week SRT to seven individuals with insomnia and depressive symptoms; six individuals completed assessments at all timepoints. Baseline scores of depressive symptoms (PHQ‐9) were 12.2 (SD = 2.3), which equates to “moderate” depression, though none of the participants were experiencing a current depressive episode at baseline (as defined by Structured Clinical Interview for DSM‐5, the Research Version). Depression severity was measured by PHQ‐9 and BDI‐II. Compared with pre‐treatment, Lancee reported in the supplementary materials that PHQ‐9 scores were significantly lower at post‐treatment (g = 1.66). Lancee reported no significant differences for BDI‐II at post‐treatment though a large effect was observed (g = 1.30). Both depression measures (PHQ‐9 and BDI‐II) were significantly lowered at 3‐month and 6‐month follow‐up compared with pre‐treatment, with large effect sizes (3 m FU: PHQ‐9, g = 5.40; BDI‐II, g = 2.80; 6 m FU: PHQ‐9, g = 3.17; BDI‐II, g = 1.95). Appendix 4 presents the summary findings of uncontrolled trials.

5. DISCUSSION

5.1. Summary

The primary objective of this review was to evaluate the impact of single‐component SRT on depressive symptoms. Meta‐analysis of RCTs revealed a medium treatment effect for SRT versus control on depressive symptoms at post‐treatment (g = −0.45) and a small treatment effect at follow‐up of up to 6 months (g = −0.31). While the number of included studies was small (post‐treatment N c = 6; follow‐ups N c = 4) and variability between studies (heterogeneity and prediction interval) was moderate, our findings indicate that SRT may be efficacious in improving depressive symptoms.

Our current findings are comparable in magnitude, if not slightly larger, than effects observed for full CBT‐I (Ballesio et al., 2018; Benz et al., 2020) at post‐treatment (CBT‐I individual face‐to‐face, d range: 0.34–0.37), although we cannot draw firm conclusions given the differences in number of trials and populations (e.g. SRT N c  = 7, CBT‐I N c  = 30; Benz et al., 2020). Related to this, Kyle et al. (2023) had the largest sample size of the meta‐analysis, representing 62% of the total number of participants at post‐treatment (total N p = 830, Kyle N p = 528) and 70% at follow‐up (total N p = 735, Kyle N p = 512). Findings of this meta‐analysis are therefore heavily weighted on this study.

Maurer et al. (2020) was the only RCT in the meta‐analysis that did not show an effect size in favour of SRT for measures of depressive symptoms at post‐treatment. One possible explanation is that their control group, TBR, matched well with SRT for therapist time, structure of treatment, and implementation and review of a weekly sleep schedule. Controlling for these non‐specific treatment effects may have attenuated group effects for depression.

5.2. Methodological considerations

There are several key limitations of our meta‐analysis that may affect interpretation of our findings. While we appraised study risk of bias, we did not perform a formal assessment of the certainty of evidence (through, e.g., the GRADE system). We found wide prediction intervals that stretch to the possibility of a null effect and moderate heterogeneity between studies. Having a small number of studies meant we were not able to assess publication bias or conduct subgroup analysis to explore possible causes of heterogeneity. Heterogeneity is likely due to clinical diversity, including variability in characteristics of the sample population, SRT implementation, and the different types of depression outcome measures.

Less than half of the included studies provided information on all SRT parameters. We found variability in minimum sleep window time, SE criteria and sleep window change, which may affect the mechanisms of SRT, such as homeostatic sleep pressure. Variation in SRT parameters may reflect attempts to minimise the side‐effects of SRT from excessive sleep loss, to improve treatment experience/engagement, and/or minimise drop‐out. Consistent with previous work on SRT (Kyle et al., 2015; Maurer et al., 2021), we recommend that future studies report the full SRT procedures to increase transparency.

Five different depression measures were used from the included studies, two of which did not include a sleep item (i.e. HADS, GDS). Studies where the depression measure had a sleep item may have led to larger effects, given that SRT is an effective treatment for relieving insomnia symptoms (Maurer et al., 2021). However, it should be noted that Epstein et al. (2012) used a depression measure without a sleep item (GDS) and reported one of the largest treatment effects (g = −0.81) in our meta‐analysis.

Five out of seven included studies were classified as high risk of bias, only two studies raised some concerns on the risk of bias. The two most affected domains in the risk of bias were trial pre‐registration and blinding outcome assessment. Only Kyle et al. (2023) pre‐registered the trial hypotheses, methods and analyses; five studies registered the trial after data collection. Registering the trial retrospectively makes it difficult to distinguish between confirmatory analyses and exploratory analyses, and creates scope for biased reporting.

The nature of behavioural sleep treatment made it difficult to blind participants and researchers from the treatment and control conditions. Non‐specific treatment effects are important to control for, particularly when self‐reported outcome measures are used (Furukawa et al., 2021). However, it is generally quite difficult to control for all non‐specific treatment effects in behavioural therapies, especially given the constraints of resource personnel, funding and time. One should therefore be aware of the possible consequences from different types of control groups when interpreting and comparing findings from behavioural therapies.

5.3. Broader considerations

No study examined depression as a primary outcome or specifically recruited individuals with a diagnosis of depression. Five out of seven included studies were conducted in individuals with no‐to‐mild levels of depression. Although participants in the other two studies (Epstein et al., 2012; Kyle et al., 2023) had moderate‐to‐severe level of depression at baseline, neither study had depression severity cut‐offs as their inclusion criteria; participants were simply not excluded based on depression level. The observed effects in our current findings may be underestimated, given depression severity of the sample population is not in the clinical range, creating little room for improvement. Future studies may therefore wish to recruit individuals with a diagnosis of depression to allow findings to be more translatable to practice. Consistent with previous recommendations (Edinger et al., 2021), future studies may also wish to focus on elucidating possible side‐effects of time in bed restriction during SRT in a sample with clinical depression.

There is a need for future mechanistic work to examine how insomnia reduction through SRT drives improvement in depression. Individuals with insomnia report high levels of maladaptive cognitive patterns linked to depression, including worry (Harvey, 2000; Jansson & Linton, 2006; Jansson‐Fröjmark et al., 2012; Mitchell et al., 2012), rumination (Carney et al., 2006; Mitchell et al., 2012; Thomsen et al., 2003) and emotional dysregulation (Galbiati et al., 2020; Palagini et al., 2017). Being awake at night, where there is typically a lack of visual and auditory stimuli, may be conducive to intrusive and ruminative thoughts (Lovato & Gradisar, 2014) and fuel negatively toned cognitive activity, thus further disrupting sleep. These bidirectional interactions are likely self‐sustaining and may develop into or exacerbate depressive symptoms overtime (McLaughlin et al., 2007). During SRT, the sleep window is restricted, which directly strengthens homeostatic sleep drive (Maurer et al., 2018) and reduces wake‐time in bed (Maurer et al., 2021; Simon et al., 2023). Improvement in sleep continuity and sleep depth may also have direct effects on daytime mood, worry (Harvey, 2000; Jansson & Linton, 2006; Jansson‐Fröjmark et al., 2012; Mitchell et al., 2012), rumination (Carney et al., 2006; Mitchell et al., 2012; Thomsen et al., 2003) and emotional processing (Galbiati et al., 2020; Palagini et al., 2017). Regularising bedtime and rise times likely engenders more regular light exposure, social rhythm and potentially behavioural activation, which may feedback to reinforce good sleep quality, daytime functioning and reduce depressive symptoms. These pathways require dedicated evaluation in future studies.

AUTHOR CONTRIBUTIONS

Katrina Yan Kei Tse: Conceptualization; investigation; writing – original draft; writing – review and editing; visualization; methodology; formal analysis; software; data curation; project administration; validation. Leonie Franziska Maurer: Supervision; conceptualization; writing – review and editing; investigation. Colin Alexander Espie: Supervision; writing – review and editing; conceptualization; funding acquisition. Simon David Kyle: Supervision; conceptualization; writing – review and editing; funding acquisition.

FUNDING INFORMATION

KYT, CAE and SDK are supported by the NIHR Oxford Health Biomedical Research Centre (NIHR203316).

CONFLICT OF INTEREST STATEMENT

The authors declare no conflicts of interest.

Supporting information

DATA S1. Supporting Information.

JSR-33-e14180-s001.docx (944.5KB, docx)

Tse, K. Y. K. , Maurer, L. F. , Espie, C. A. , & Kyle, S. D. (2024). The effect of single‐component sleep restriction therapy on depressive symptoms: A systematic review and meta‐analysis. Journal of Sleep Research, 33(6), e14180. 10.1111/jsr.14180

DATA AVAILABILITY STATEMENT

Data sharing is not applicable to this article as no new data were created or analyzed in this study.

REFERENCES

  1. Aji, M. , Glozier, N. , Bartlett, D. , Peters, D. , Calvo, R. A. , Zheng, Y. , Grunstein, R. , & Gordon, C. (2020). A feasibility study of a mobile app to treat insomnia. Translational Behavioral Medicine, 11, 604–612. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. American Psychiatric Association . (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.). American Psychiatric Association. [Google Scholar]
  3. Baglioni, C. , Battagliese, G. , Feige, B. , Spiegelhalder, K. , Nissen, C. , Voderholzer, U. , Lombardo, C. , & Riemann, D. (2011). Insomnia as a predictor of depression: A meta‐analytic evaluation of longitudinal epidemiological studies. Journal of Affective Disorders, 135, 10–19. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Baglioni, C. , Espie, C. A. , Spiegelhalder, K. , Gavriloff, D. , & Riemann, D. (2022). Recommendation of the European academy for cognitive‐Behavioural therapy for insomnia (CBT‐I) for high quality training for health professionals. Cognitive‐Behavioural Therapy for Insomnia (CBT‐I) across the Life Span, 243–250. 10.1002/9781119891192.CH24 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  5. Balduzzi, S. , Rücker, G. , & Schwarzer, G. (2019). How to perform a meta‐analysis with R: A practical tutorial. Evidence‐Based Mental Health, 22, 153–160. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Ballesio, A. , Aquino, M. R. J. V. , Feige, B. , Johann, A. F. , Kyle, S. D. , Spiegelhalder, K. , Lombardo, C. , Rücker, G. , Riemann, D. , & Baglioni, C. (2018). The effectiveness of behavioural and cognitive behavioural therapies for insomnia on depressive and fatigue symptoms: A systematic review and network meta‐analysis. Sleep Medicine Reviews, 37, 114–129. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Beck, A. T. , Steer, R. A. , & Brown, G. K. (1996). Manual for the Beck depression inventory‐II. Psychological Corporation. [Google Scholar]
  8. Benz, F. , Knoop, T. , Ballesio, A. , Bacaro, V. , Johann, A. F. , Rücker, G. , Feige, B. , Riemann, D. , & Baglioni, C. (2020). The efficacy of cognitive and behavior therapies for insomnia on daytime symptoms: A systematic review and network meta‐analysis. Clinical Psychology Review, 80, 101873. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Boland, E. M. , Goldschmied, J. R. , & Gehrman, P. R. (2023). Does insomnia treatment prevent depression? Sleep, 46, 1–8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Brasure, M. , Fuchs, E. , MacDonald, R. , Nelson, V. A. , Koffel, E. , Olson, C. M. , Khawaja, I. S. , Diem, S. , Carlyle, M. , Wilt, T. J. , Ouellette, J. , Butler, M. , & Kane, R. L. (2016). Psychological and behavioral interventions for managing insomnia disorder: An evidence report for a clinical practice guideline by the American College of Physicians. Annals of Internal Medicine, 165, 113–124. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Buysse, D. J. , Thompson, W. , Scott, J. , Franzen, P. L. , Germain, A. , Hall, M. , Moul, D. E. , Nofzinger, E. A. , & Kupfer, D. J. (2007). Daytime symptoms in primary insomnia: A prospective analysis using ecological momentary assessment. Sleep Medicine, 8, 198–208. 10.1016/j.sleep.2006.10.006 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Carney, C. E. , Edinger, J. D. , Meyer, B. , Lindman, L. , & Istre, T. (2006). Symptom‐focused rumination and sleep disturbance. Behavioral Sleep Medicine, 4, 228–241. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Carney, C. E. , Segal, Z. V. , Edinger, J. D. , & Krystal, A. D. (2007). A comparison of rates of residual insomnia symptoms following pharmacotherapy or cognitive‐behavioral therapy for major depressive disorder. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 68, 254–260. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Cunningham, J. E. A. , & Shapiro, C. M. (2018). Cognitive Behavioural therapy for insomnia (CBT‐I) to treat depression: A systematic review. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 106, 1–12. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Davidson, J. R. , Dickson, C. , & Han, H. (2019). Cognitive behavioural treatment for insomnia in primary care: A systematic review of sleep outcomes. The British Journal of General Practice, 69, E657–E664. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Edinger, J. D. , Arnedt, J. T. , Bertisch, S. M. , Carney, C. E. , Harrington, J. J. , Lichstein, K. L. , Sateia, M. J. , Troxel, W. M. , Zhou, E. S. , Kazmi, U. , Heald, J. L. , & Martin, J. L. (2021). Behavioral and psychological treatments for chronic insomnia disorder in adults: An American Academy of sleep medicine clinical practice guideline. Journal of Clinical Sleep Medicine, 17, 255–262. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Epstein, D. R. , Sidani, S. , Bootzin, R. R. , & Belyea, M. J. (2012). Dismantling multicomponent behavioral treatment for insomnia in older adults: A randomized controlled trial. Sleep, 35, 797–805. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Falloon, K. , Raina Elley, C. , Fernando, A. , Lee, A. C. , & Arroll, B. (2015). Simplified sleep restriction for insomnia in general practice: A randomised controlled trial. British Journal of General Practice, 65, e508–e515. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Furukawa, T. A. , Suganuma, A. , Ostinelli, E. G. , Andersson, G. , Beevers, C. G. , Shumake, J. , Berger, T. , Boele, F. W. , Buntrock, C. , Carlbring, P. , Choi, I. , Christensen, H. , Mackinnon, A. , Dahne, J. , Huibers, M. J. H. , Ebert, D. D. , Farrer, L. , Forand, N. R. , Strunk, D. R. , … Cuijpers, P. (2021). Dismantling, optimising, and personalising internet cognitive behavioural therapy for depression: A systematic review and component network meta‐analysis using individual participant data. Lancet Psychiatry, 8, 500–511. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Galbiati, A. , Sforza, M. , Fasiello, E. , Casoni, F. , Marrella, N. , Leitner, C. , Zucconi, M. , & Ferini‐Strambi, L. (2020). The association between emotional dysregulation and REM sleep features in insomnia disorder. Brain and Cognition, 146, 105642. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Geiger‐Brown, J. M. , Rogers, V. E. , Liu, W. , Ludeman, E. M. , Downton, K. D. , & Diaz‐Abad, M. (2015). Cognitive behavioral therapy in persons with comorbid insomnia: A meta‐analysis. Sleep Medicine Reviews, 23, 54–67. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Gieselmann, A. , & Pietrowsky, R. (2019). The effects of brief chat‐based and face‐to‐face psychotherapy for insomnia: A randomized waiting list controlled trial. Sleep Medicine, 61, 63–72. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Grandner, M. A. , & Chakravorty, S. (2017). Insomnia in primary care: Misreported, mishandled, and just plain missed. Journal of Clinical Sleep Medicine, 13, 937–939. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Grandner, M. A. , & Malhotra, A. (2015). Sleep as a vital sign: Why medical practitioners need to routinely ask their patients about sleep. Sleep Health, 1, 11–12. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Harvey, A. G. (2000). Pre‐sleep cognitive activity: A comparison of sleep‐onset insomniacs and good sleepers. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 39, 275–286. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Hedges, L. V. (1981). Distribution theory for Glass's estimator of effect size and related estimators. Journal of Educational Statistics, 6, 107–128. [Google Scholar]
  27. Hertenstein, E. , Feige, B. , Gmeiner, T. , Kienzler, C. , Spiegelhalder, K. , Johann, A. , Jansson‐Fröjmark, M. , Palagini, L. , Rücker, G. , Riemann, D. , & Baglioni, C. (2019). Insomnia as a predictor of mental disorders: A systematic review and meta‐analysis. Sleep Medicine Reviews, 43, 96–105. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Higgins, J. P. T. , & Thompson, S. G. (2002). Quantifying heterogeneity in a meta‐analysis. Statistics in Medicine, 21, 1539–1558. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Higgins, J. P. T. , Thompson, S. G. , Deeks, J. J. , & Altman, D. G. (2003). Measuring inconsistency in meta‐analyses. British Medical Journal, 327, 557–560. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Higgins, J. P. T. , & Green, S. (2022). Handbook for Systematic Reviews of Interventions. Cochrane. [Google Scholar]
  31. Ho, F. Y. Y. , Chung, K. F. , Yeung, W. F. , Ng, T. H. , Kwan, K. S. , Yung, K. P. , & Cheng, S. K. (2015). Self‐help cognitive‐behavioral therapy for insomnia: A meta‐analysis of randomized controlled trials. Sleep Medicine Reviews, 19, 17–28. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Irwin, M. , Cole, J. , & Nicassio, P. M. (2006). Comparative meta‐analysis of behavioral interventions for insomnia and their efficacy in middle‐aged adults and in older adults 55+ years of age. Health Psychology, 25, 3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Jansson, M. , & Linton, S. J. (2006). The development of insomnia within the first year: A focus on worry. British Journal of Health Psychology, 11, 501–511. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Jansson‐Fröjmark, M. , Harvey, A. G. , Norell‐Clarke, A. , & Linton, S. J. (2012). Associations between psychological factors and nighttime/daytime symptomatology in insomnia. Cognitive Behaviour Therapy, 41, 273–287. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Kalmbach, D. A. , Cheng, P. , Arnedt, J. T. , Anderson, J. R. , Roth, T. , Fellman‐Couture, C. , Williams, R. A. , & Drake, C. L. (2019). Treating insomnia improves depression, maladaptive thinking, and hyperarousal in postmenopausal women: Comparing cognitive‐behavioral therapy for insomnia (CBTI), sleep restriction therapy, and sleep hygiene education. Sleep Medicine, 55, 124–134. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Koffel, E. A. , Koffel, J. B. , & Gehrman, P. R. (2015). A meta‐analysis of group cognitive behavioral therapy for insomnia. Sleep Medicine Reviews, 19, 6–16. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Krieger, T. , Urech, A. , Duss, S. B. , Blättler, L. , Schmitt, W. , Gast, H. , Bassetti, C. , & Berger, T. (2019). A randomized controlled trial comparing guided internet‐based multi‐component treatment and internet‐based guided sleep restriction treatment to care as usual in insomnia. Sleep Medicine, 62, 43–52. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Kroenke, K. , Spitzer, R. L. , & Williams, J. B. W. (2001). The PHQ‐9: Validity of a brief depression severity measure. Journal of General Internal Medicine, 16, 606–613. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Kyle, S. D. , Aquino, M. R. J. , Miller, C. B. , Henry, A. L. , Crawford, M. R. , Espie, C. A. , & Spielman, A. J. (2015). Towards standardisation and improved understanding of sleep restriction therapy for insomnia disorder: A systematic examination of CBT‐I trial content. Sleep Medicine Reviews, 23, 83–88. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Kyle, S. D. , Espie, C. A. , & Morgan, K. (2010). Not just a minor thing, it is something major, which stops you from functioning daily’: Quality of life and daytime functioning in insomnia. Behavioral Sleep Medicine, 8, 123–140. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Kyle, S. D. , Siriwardena, A. N. , Espie, C. A. , Yang, Y. , Petrou, S. , Ogburn, E. , Begum, N. , Maurer, L. F. , Robinson, B. , Gardner, C. , Lee, V. , Armstrong, S. , Pattinson, J. , Mort, S. , Temple, E. , Harris, V. , Yu, L. M. , Bower, P. , & Aveyard, P. (2023). Clinical and cost‐effectiveness of nurse‐delivered sleep restriction therapy for insomnia in primary care (HABIT): A pragmatic, superiority, open‐label, randomised controlled trial. The Lancet, 402, 975–987. 10.1016/S0140-6736(23)00683-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Lancee, J. , Maric, M. , & Kamphuis, J. H. (2019). Sleep restriction therapy may be effective for people with insomnia and depressive complaints : Evidence from a case series. Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapy, 48, 1–6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. LeBlanc, M. , Beaulieu‐Bonneau, S. , Mérette, C. , Savard, J. , Ivers, H. , & Morin, C. M. (2007). Psychological and health‐related quality of life factors associated with insomnia in a population‐based sample. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 63, 157–166. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Li, L. , Wu, C. , Gan, Y. , Qu, X. , & Lu, Z. (2016). Insomnia and the risk of depression: A meta‐analysis of prospective cohort studies. BMC Psychiatry, 16, 375. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Lipsey, M. W. , & Wilson, D. B. (1993). The efficacy of psychological, educational, and behavioral treatment: Confirmation from meta‐analysis. American Psychologist, 48, 1181–1209. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Lovato, N. , & Gradisar, M. (2014). A meta‐analysis and model of the relationship between sleep and depression in adolescents: Recommendations for future research and clinical practice. Sleep Medicine Reviews, 18, 521–529. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Maurer, L. F. , Espie, C. A. , & Kyle, S. D. (2018). How does sleep restriction therapy for insomnia work? A systematic review of mechanistic evidence and the introduction of the triple‐R model. Sleep Medicine Reviews, 42, 127–138. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Maurer, L. F. , Espie, C. A. , Omlin, X. , Reid, M. J. , Sharman, R. , Gavriloff, D. , Emsley, R. , & Kyle, S. D. (2020). Isolating the role of time in bed restriction in the treatment of insomnia: A randomised, controlled, dismantling trial comparing sleep restriction therapy with time in bed regularisation. Sleep, 43, 1–12. 10.1093/sleep/zsaa096 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Maurer, L. F. , Schneider, J. , Miller, C. B. , Espie, C. A. , & Kyle, S. D. (2021). The clinical effects of sleep restriction therapy for insomnia: A meta‐analysis of randomised controlled trials. Sleep Medicine Reviews, 58, 101493. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. McLaughlin, K. A. , Borkovec, T. D. , & Sibrava, N. J. (2007). The effects of worry and rumination on affect states and cognitive activity. Behavior Therapy, 38, 23–28. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Miller, C. B. , Espie, C. A. , Epstein, D. R. , Friedman, L. , Morin, C. M. , Pigeon, W. R. , Spielman, A. J. , & Kyle, S. D. (2014). The evidence base of sleep restriction therapy for treating insomnia disorder. Sleep Medicine Reviews, 18, 415–424. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Mitchell, L. , Mogg, K. , & Bradley, B. P. (2012). Relationships between insomnia, negative emotionality and attention control. Sleep and Biological Rhythms, 10, 237–243. [Google Scholar]
  53. Montgomery, P. , & Dennis, J. (2004). A systematic review of non‐pharmacological therapies for sleep problems in later life. Sleep Medicine Reviews, 8, 47–62. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Morin, C. M. , & Benca, R. (2012). Chronic insomnia. The Lancet, 379, 1129–1141. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Murtagh, D. , & Greenwood, K. (1995). Identifying effective psychological treatments for insomnia: A meta‐analysis. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 63, 79–89. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Ohayon, M. M. , Shapiro, C. M. , & Kennedy, S. H. (2000). Differentiating DSM‐IV anxiety and depressive disorders in the general population: Comorbidity and treatment consequences. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 45, 166–172. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Okajima, I. , Komada, Y. , & Inoue, Y. (2010). A meta‐analysis on the treatment effectiveness of cognitive behavioral therapy for primary insomnia. Sleep and Biological Rhythms, 9, 24–34. [Google Scholar]
  58. Page, M. J. , et al. (2021). The PRISMA 2020 statement: An updated guideline for reporting systematic reviews. British Medical Journal, 372, 105906. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Palagini, L. , Moretto, U. , Dell'Osso, L. , & Carney, C. (2017). Sleep‐related cognitive processes, arousal, and emotion dysregulation in insomnia disorder: The role of insomnia‐specific rumination. Sleep Medicine, 30, 97–104. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Pallesen, S. , Nordhus, I. H. , & Kvale, G. (1998). Nonpharmacological interventions for insomnia in older adults: A meta‐analysis of treatment efficacy. Psychotherapy, 35, 472–482. [Google Scholar]
  61. Qaseem, A. , Kansagara, D. , Forciea, M. A. , Cooke, M. , Denberg, T. D. , & for the Clinical Guidelines Committee of the American College of Physicians . (2016). Management of chronic insomnia disorder in adults: A clinical practice guideline from the American college of physicians. Annals of Internal Medicine, 165, 125–133. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Radloff, L. S. (1977). The CES‐D scale: A self‐report depression scale for research in the general population. Applied Psychological Measurement, 1, 385–401. [Google Scholar]
  63. Ree, M. , Junge, M. , & Cunnington, D. (2017). Australasian Sleep Association position statement regarding the use of psychological/behavioral treatments in the management of insomnia in adults. Sleep Medicine, 36, S43–S47. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Riedel, B. W. , & Lichstein, K. L. (2000). Insomnia and daytime functioning. Sleep Medicine Reviews, 4, 277–298. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Riemann, D. , Espie, C. A. , Altena, E. , Arnardottir, E. S. , Baglioni, C. , Bassetti, C. L. A. , Bastien, C. , Berzina, N. , Bjorvatn, B. , Dikeos, D. , Dolenc Groselj, L. , Ellis, J. G. , Garcia‐Borreguero, D. , Geoffroy, P. A. , Gjerstad, M. , Gonçalves, M. , Hertenstein, E. , Hoedlmoser, K. , Hion, T. , … Spiegelhalder, K. (2023). The European insomnia guideline: An update on the diagnosis and treatment of insomnia 2023. Journal of Sleep Research, 32, e14035. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Saleem, A. H. , al Rashed, F. A. , Alkharboush, G. A. , Almazyed, O. M. , Olaish, A. H. , Almeneessier, A. S. , & BaHammam, A. (2017). Primary care physicians' knowledge of sleep medicine and barriers to transfer of patients with sleep disorders. A cross‐sectional study. Saudi Med J, 38, 553–559. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Sateia, M. J. , Buysse, D. J. , Krystal, A. D. , Neubauer, D. N. , & Heald, J. L. (2017). Clinical practice guideline for the pharmacologic treatment of chronic insomnia in adults: An American Academy of sleep medicine clinical practice guideline. Journal of Clinical Sleep Medicine, 13, 307–349. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Schutte‐Rodin, S. , Broch, L. , Buysse, D. , Dorsey, C. , & Sateia, M. (2008). Clinical guideline for the evaluation and Management of Chronic Insomnia in adults. Journal of Clinical Sleep Medicine, 04, 487–504. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Simon, L. , Steinmetz, L. , Feige, B. , Benz, F. , Spiegelhalder, K. , & Baumeister, H. (2023). Comparative efficacy of onsite, digital, and other settings for cognitive behavioral therapy for insomnia: A systematic review and network meta‐analysis. Scientific Reports, 13, 1–11. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Sofi, F. , Cesari, F. , Casini, A. , Macchi, C. , Abbate, R. , & Gensini, G. F. (2014). Insomnia and risk of cardiovascular disease: A meta‐analysis. European Journal of Preventive Cardiology, 21, 57–64. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Spiegelhalder, K. , Scholtes, C. , & Riemann, D. (2010). The association between insomnia and cardiovascular diseases. Nature and Science of Sleep, 2, 71–78. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Spielman, A. , Saskin, P. , & Thorpy, M. J. (1987). Treatment of chronic insomnia by restriction of time in bed. Sleep, 10, 45–56. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Staner, L. (2010). Comorbidity of insomnia and depression. Sleep Medicine Reviews, 14, 35–46. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Sterne, J. A. C. , et al. (2019). RoB 2: A revised tool for assessing risk of bias in randomised trials. BMJ, 366. 10.1136/bmj.l4898 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Thomsen, D. K. , Mehlsen, M. Y. , Christensen, S. , & Zachariae, R. (2003). Rumination—relationship with negative mood and sleep quality. Personality and Individual Differences, 34, 1293–1301. [Google Scholar]
  76. Trauer, J. M. , Qian, M. Y. , Doyle, J. S. , Rajaratnam, S. M. W. , & Cunnington, D. (2015). Cognitive behavioral therapy for chronic insomnia: A systematic review and meta‐analysis. Annals of Internal Medicine, 163, 191–204. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Tsuno, N. , Besset, A. , & Ritchie, K. (2005). Sleep and depression. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 66, 1254–1269. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Veroniki, A. A. , Jackson, D. , Viechtbauer, W. , Bender, R. , Bowden, J. , Knapp, G. , Kuss, O. , Higgins, J. P. T. , Langan, D. , & Salanti, G. (2016). Methods to estimate the between‐study variance and its uncertainty in meta‐analysis. Research Synthesis Methods, 7, 55–79. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Weissman, M. M. , Bland, R. C. , Canino, G. J. , Faravelli, C. , Greenwald, S. , Hwu, H. G. , Joyce, P. R. , Karam, E. G. , Lee, C. K. , Lellouch, J. , Lépine, J. P. , Newman, S. C. , Rubio‐Stipec, M. , Wells, J. E. , Wickramaratne, P. J. , Wittchen, H. , & Yeh, E. K. (1996). Cross‐national epidemiology of major depression and bipolar disorder. Journal of the American Medical Association, 276, 293–299. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. Wilson, S. , Anderson, K. , Baldwin, D. , Dijk, D. J. , Espie, A. , Espie, C. , Gringras, P. , Krystal, A. , Nutt, D. , Selsick, H. , & Sharpley, A. (2019). British Association for Psychopharmacology consensus statement on evidence‐based treatment of insomnia, parasomnias and circadian rhythm disorders: An update. Journal of Psychopharmacology, 33, 923–947. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. World Health Organization . (2021). International statistical classification of diseases and related health problems (11th ed.). World Health Organisation. [Google Scholar]
  82. Yates, W. R. , Mitchell, J. , Rush, A. J. , Trivedi, M. , Wisniewski, S. R. , Warden, D. , Bryan, C. , Fava, M. , Husain, M. M. , & Gaynes, B. N. (2007). Clinical features of depression in outpatients with and without co‐occurring general medical conditions in STAR*D: Confirmatory analysis. Primary Care Companion to the Journal Of Clinical Psychiatry, 9, 7–15. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  83. Yesavage, J. A. , Brink, T. L. , Rose, T. L. , Lum, O. , Huang, V. , Adey, M. , & Leirer, V. O. (1982). Development and validation of a geriatric depression screening scale: A preliminary report. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 17, 37–49. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  84. Zigmond, A. S. , & Snaith, R. P. (1983). The hospital anxiety and depression scale. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 67, 361–370. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

DATA S1. Supporting Information.

JSR-33-e14180-s001.docx (944.5KB, docx)

Data Availability Statement

Data sharing is not applicable to this article as no new data were created or analyzed in this study.


Articles from Journal of Sleep Research are provided here courtesy of Wiley

RESOURCES