Summary
Experimental inversion of circadian and behavioral rhythms by 12 h adversely affects markers of metabolic health. We investigated the effects of a more modest 5-h delay in behavioral cycles. Fourteen participants completed an 8-day in-patient laboratory protocol, with controlled sleep-wake opportunities, light-dark cycles, and diet. The 5-h delay in behavioral cycles was induced by delaying sleep opportunity. We measured melatonin to confirm central circadian phase, fasting markers and postprandial metabolism, energy expenditure, subjective sleepiness, and appetite, throughout the waking period. After the phase delay, there was slower gastric emptying at breakfast, lower fasting plasma glucose, higher postprandial plasma glucose and triglycerides, and lower thermic effect of feeding. Any changes were abolished or attenuated within 48–72 h. These data extend our previous findings, which showed no time-of-day effect in healthy adults on daytime energy expenditure or thermic effect of feeding when accounting for circadian variation in resting metabolic rate.
Subject areas: biological sciences, physiology, human metabolism
Graphical abstract
Highlights
-
•
5-h circadian desynchrony disrupted metabolic physiology and subjective appetite
-
•
Resting energy expenditure did not change across the waking period
-
•
Gastric emptying and postprandial responses were all disrupted
-
•
Metabolic disturbance was largely reversed within 48–72 h of the desynchrony
Biological sciences; Physiology; Human metabolism.
Introduction
Circadian rhythms have a period of approximately 24 h and are important for multiple aspects of physiology, including cardio-metabolic health.1,2 They are generated by endogenous clocks, and synchronized to external environmental time cues, called zeitgebers.3 In mammals, the “central” clock is located within the suprachiasmatic nuclei (SCN) of the hypothalamus and is synchronized by the environmental light-dark cycle.4,5 Physiological outputs from the SCN synchronize “peripheral” clocks throughout the rest of the body. Peripheral clocks regulate metabolism and other key tissue functions and can also entrain independently of the SCN to other environmental inputs, in particular meal timing.6,7,8
The evolving field of chrono-nutrition combines traditional circadian research with mealtime and diet composition, to explore the impact of feeding and fasting on body weight control and biomarkers of health.2,9,10 Delaying meals by 5 h within a fixed light-dark schedule induced a delay of 5–6 h in rhythmic glucose metabolism6 and increased appetite while reducing energy expenditure.11 Postprandial metabolism, in particular glucose and insulin responses, follows a 24-h rhythm12,13,14 with a well-defined endogenous circadian component.15,16,17 The rate of gastric emptying may be lower in the evening compared to the morning, although evidence is limited.18 Human resting metabolic rate (RMR) has been shown to exhibit a circadian rhythm.19 Postprandial energy expenditure, in the thermic effect of food (TEF), has also been proposed to vary according to time of day,20,21,22,23 although our work indicates that rhythms in postprandial energy expenditure do not exist per se, and previous findings are a consequence of circadian RMR.24
Another aspect of chrono-nutrition that has received interest is morning-loaded calorie intake (large breakfast and small dinner) compared to evening-loaded intake (small breakfast and large dinner). In a highly controlled 4-week cross-over weight loss trial, morning-loaded calorie intake reduced appetite compared to evening-loaded calorie intake, suggesting that calories ingested in the morning can help control appetite during energy deficit.25 Despite this effect on appetite, the distribution of calorie intake across the day did not impact energy metabolism.25 However, this study only applied morning measurement of resting energy metabolism and TEF, and monitoring diurnal changes is preferable. Nonetheless, previous studies conducted using a parallel design over 12 or more weeks suggested morning-loaded calorie intake may increase weight loss.26,27
Together, these studies indicate the importance of time to the short- and long-term response to calorie intake. There remains, however, a clear requirement for additional controlled studies in humans to explore mechanisms of daily metabolism. This is especially true for protocols that model real-life scenarios such as circadian desynchrony and the misalignment between internal biological rhythms and the external environment. Most studies of circadian desynchrony to date have employed a 12-h change in environmental/behavioral cycles, but the effect of more moderate desynchrony, as commonly experienced in, e.g., trans-meridian flights, is poorly understood. Within a controlled 8-day laboratory protocol, we therefore exposed participants to an acute 5-h delay in light-dark, sleep-wake, and feed-fast cycles. We hypothesized that circadian desynchrony the day after the 5-h delay would impair daytime energy metabolism, specifically that thermic response to food would be lower after the 5-h phase delay, and only partially return to baseline over the following 5 days.
Results and discussion
Participants arrived at the laboratory on Day 0 and were admitted following confirmation of adherence to the pre-lab routine by actigraphy. Using a phase-shift protocol, the behavioral cycles of participants including sleep-wake timing, light exposure, and meal timing were delayed by 5 h, beginning on the evening of Day 2 into Day 3 (Figure 1A).10 To maintain a consistent fasting period between last energy intake and the test breakfast, a supper was provided 5 h after dinner on the evening of Day 2 (∼23 h). Participants remained on the delayed behavioral/environmental cycle for 5 days to monitor adaptation of biological rhythms to the new schedule. The phase delay had the effect of acutely misaligning the timing of delayed behavioral and environmental cycles from the endogenous circadian rhythms that would initially remain aligned to the sleep-wake timing and meal timing of the participants before the phase delay. It also preserved a nightly 8-h sleep opportunity, to minimize any acute effect of sleep curtailment. The protocol compared energy expenditure, postprandial metabolic responses, and melatonin onset timing throughout the experiment, as follows: (a) Circadian rhythms aligned to behavior cycles (Day 2); (b) acute misalignment of circadian rhythms following 5-h delay in behavioral cycles (Day 3); (c) adaptation of circadian rhythms to the delayed behavioral cycles (Day 5 and Day 7) (Figures 1A and 1B).
Fourteen participants (8 male, 6 female) completed the study. Participant characteristics according to sex can be found in Table 1. Mean age of participants was 45.6years (±9.6), and mean BMI was 31.7 kg/m2 (±3.0). The study was a fixed-feeding protocol designed to maintain participants at a stable weight. Energy intake tailored to individual requirements, calculated as basal metabolic rate using the Mifflin-St. Jeor equation,28 multiplied by a physical activity factor of 1.1–1.3. Breakfast, lunch, and dinner were isocaloric (33.3% of total daily energy intake), and the macronutrient composition of the diets was 20% protein, 35% fat, and 45% carbohydrate (Table S1). Supper on Day 2 contained the same macronutrient composition. Each meal contained a minimum energy content of 2,301 kJ, with individual participant energy requirements being met through the addition of prepared milkshakes in quantities specific to requirements of the individual participant. Meals were initiated 1 h after waking, with lunch and dinner separated by 5 h, respectively. Compliance was assessed by weighing back any leftover foods and changes in bodyweight during the study. Mean weight change during the in-patient study period was −0.6kg (±0.5), and BMI at the end of the study was 31.5 kg/m2 (±3.0). Compliance with the diet, assessed by weighing back any leftover foods, was excellent, supported by the <1% change in bodyweight during the in-patient laboratory period.
Table 1.
Male (n = 8) | Female (n = 6) | |
---|---|---|
Age [yrs] | 48.1 [±7.5] | 42.3 [±11.8] |
Height [cm] | 178.9 [±4.9] | 165.7 [±8.3] |
Baseline weight [kg] | 100.6 [±6.8] | 88.2 [±15.7] |
Baseline BMI [kg/m2] | 31.5 [±3.3] | 31.9 [±2.9] |
Mean energy intake [kcal] | 2373 [±220] | 2000 [±270] |
Weight change [kg] | −0.8 [±0.4] | −0.4 [±0.4] |
BMI change [kg/m2] | −0.2 [±3.3] | −0.1 [±2.9] |
Note: data are presented as mean and standard deviation (SD).
Rhythms of melatonin, sleepiness, and alertness exhibit gradual realignment to the new behavioral cycle
The average pre-shift wake-time and bedtime of participants were 07:21 h (range: 06:30–08:40 h) and 23:10 h (range: 22:30–00:40 h), respectively. Dim-light melatonin onset (DLMO) is the gold-standard marker of the central SCN clock.29,30 We observed a significant effect of study day on measured DLMO (p < 0.001). Compared to the pre-phase delay on Day 2, DLMO on Day 3 was 1 h 26 min (p < 0.001) later, 3 h 6 min (p < 0.001) later on Day 5, and 3 h 38 min (p < 0.001) later on Day 7 (Figure 2A; Table S2). Previous research investigating the effects of 6-h westbound flight showed that DLMO delayed by 1.3 h per day on average.31 Thus, our DLMO data demonstrated a similar rate of resynchrony as described in previous research.6,31 This change in DLMO occurred despite the participants wearing blue-light-blocking glasses in the second half of the wake period and so receiving less blue light during the phase delay portion of the photic phase-response curve. The change in DLMO, and thus SCN phase, is therefore likely driven by a lack of morning (phase advancing) light, incomplete blockade of light by the glasses, and non-photic signals. The relative contribution of these influences is unclear, but this does not alter the primary findings of the study.
We next considered the time interval between waking and DLMO, in which there was also a significant effect of study day (p < 0.001). DLMO occurred at an average of 15 h 18 min after wake time on Day 2. However, compared to Day 2, the DLMO on Day 3 occurred 3 h 33 min (p < 0.001) closer to wake time, on Day 5 occurred 1 h 53 min (p < 0.001) closer to wake time, and on Day 7 occurred 1 h 12 min (p = 0.005) closer to wake time (Figure 2B; Table S2).
The effect of the 5-h phase delay was also confirmed by the significant changes in subjective sleepiness and decreased subjective alertness that were observed immediately following the phase delay (Figures 3A and 3B). Higher scores on the Karolinska Sleepiness Scale (KSS) indicated higher sleepiness, whereas higher scores on the Alertness scale indicated lower alertness levels. For sleepiness and alertness, there was a significant day-by-time-point interaction, (p < 0.001), and a significant main effect of time point (p < 0.0001), but no significant main effect of study day (p = 0.412). Between 14 and 17 h after waking on Day 3, participants exhibited significantly greater sleepiness (p < 0.001) and significantly lower alertness levels (p < 0.001) at levels similar to those experienced after 20–22 h of wakefulness on Day 2. Between Day 3 to Day 7, temporal profiles of sleepiness and alertness gradually reverted toward levels similar to those observed on Day 2 (Figures 3A and 3B). The actigraphy data indicated no significant difference in sleep efficiency, actual wake time (as a percentage of total sleep time), or actual sleep time (as a percentage of total sleep time) (Figure S1).
Energy expenditure is unaffected, whereas postprandial thermic effect of feeding adapts with the central clock, following a 5-h phase delay
We have recently reported findings from a controlled hypocaloric dietary intervention demonstrating that morning versus evening energy load does not influence energy metabolism in humans, when measuring daily RMR or TEF in response to varying meal energy content in the morning period.25 In our previous study, we measured basal energy expenditure (RMR) and post-prandial energy metabolism using indirect calorimetry only at breakfast. In the present study, we also utilized indirect calorimetry to measure energy metabolism in a laboratory setting throughout the whole waking period, to examine time-of-day effects. Our present study also has the advantage of extended 5-h postprandial TEF measures, which, factoring in the energy content of meals, likely captures the majority of the TEF response.32 There was no difference in whole-day waking energy expenditure (EE) (p = 0.368) or fasting RMR (p = 0.145) between days (Figure 4A; Table S2). Thus, the present study corroborates and extends our previous findings,25 by demonstrating no differences in waking energy expenditure measured robustly measured over 15.5 h of the 16-h waking period. This finding is also consistent with Ravussin et al.,33 who found no differences in 24-h energy expenditure measured in a respiratory chamber comparing front-loaded energy intake within a time-restricted 6-h period to isoenergetic meals consumed over 12 h.
Previous studies using indirect calorimetry to measure postprandial TEF responses have suggested ∼2-fold higher TEF following breakfast compared to dinner.20,21,22 Our recent model proposed in Ruddick-Collins et al.24 suggests that underlying circadian variance in RMR likely explains any apparent differences in postprandial TEF responses between meals at different times of day. The present study showed no day-by-meal interaction (p = 0.964) or main effect of meal for TEF responses (p = 0.208). However, there was a main effect of study day (p < 0.001), i.e., the total daily (sum of all meals) overall TEF response. Total daily TEF immediately following the phase delay (Day 3) was 83.3 kJ (p = 0.001) lower compared to Day 2, before a gradual increase such that by Day 7, TEF responses were near-identical to the baseline pre-phase shift Day 2, i.e., total TEF on Day 7 was 82.2 kJ (p = 0.001) higher compared to Day 3 (Figure 4B; Table S2). These data are in agreement with our proposed model of circadian RMR.24 The change in total daily TEF observed in this study, without any significant difference in TEF responses to individual meals, likely reflects central circadian control of RMR rather than altered TEF per se, given circadian rhythmicity in RMR is tied to the peak and nadir of core body temperature (CBT),19 and would be expected to shift gradually in line with the SCN clock.34 Consequently, TEF is likely to be of limited importance for influencing energy expenditure in favor of enhanced metabolic capacity at any time of day and in the context of weight management.
Gastric emptying after breakfast is delayed in response to a 5-h phase delay but exhibits rapid adaptation to the new behavioral cycle
With the use of stable isotope tracers, gastric emptying (GE) rate may be quantified as the half-time, i.e., the time taken for 50% of the meal to empty the stomach, and the time-lag, i.e., the time point at which the rate of excretion of the tracer reaches its maximum.35,36,37 Here, we assessed gastric emptying after breakfast each day by the 13C octanoic acid breath test.38,39 We show a significant effect of the 5-h phase delay on the GE half-time (T-½) (p = 0.038), with post-hoc tests revealing that the T-½ immediately following the phase delay (Day 3) was delayed by 90.3 min (p = 0.037) compared to the pre-shift (Day 2) (Figure 5A). There was no significant effect of the phase delay on the gastric emptying time-lag (p = 0.155) (Figure 5B). Our data reveal that 5-h misalignment causes an acute effect on gastric emptying in response to the breakfast meal, before adaptation to the new behavioral cycle. A circadian rhythm in gastric emptying has previously been shown, with more rapid emptying in response to identical meals at 8 a.m. versus 8 p.m..18 It is possible that the lower gastric emptying half-time following breakfast at 1 p.m. on Day 3 compared to breakfast at 8 a.m. on Day 2 reflects the acute misalignment of the first daily meal relative to this underlying circadian variance in gastric emptying rate. However, as we did not measure gastric emptying in response to lunch, it is not possible from our data to determine whether the difference also may relate to other factors, such as extended wakefulness between Day 2 and Day 3.
Fasting and postprandial glucose, insulin, and triglyceride concentrations exhibit limited responses to a 5-h phase delay
Higher morning fasting glucose levels compared to the afternoon have been shown in healthy, non-diabetic individuals similar to the participants in our study.40,41 Our data showed a significant effect of day on fasting plasma glucose levels (p = 0.003), with post-hoc comparisons showing a 0.31 mmol/L (p = 0.005) lower fasting glucose on Day 3 and 0.29 mmol/L (p = 0.011) lower fasting glucose on Day 5 when measured at ∼13.00 h following the phase delay, compared to when measured at ∼08.00 h before the phase delay (Figure 6A; Table S2).
There were no significant day-by-meal interactions for postprandial glucose iAUC (p = 0.905), but a significant main effect of study day (p = 0.005) and a significant main effect of meal (p = 0.006). For the main effect of day, total daily postprandial glucose iAUC between days was 57.4 mmol/L⋅min higher on Day 3 compared to Day 2 (p = 0.003) (Figure 6B; Table S2). For the main effect of meals, postprandial glucose iAUC was higher on average following breakfast compared to dinner (p = 0.004) (Table S2; Figure S2). This is likely due to differences in meal composition as, although our meals were matched for energy and macronutrient content and meals between study days (i.e., breakfast on each study day) was the same, the specific foods at each meal (i.e., breakfast, lunch, and dinner) differed and the glycemic index (GI) and/or glycemic load (GL) were not considered. Nevertheless, the fact that the between-day postprandial glucose iAUC differences showed glucose remaining elevated above the pre-phase delay baseline levels is consistent with the overall effects of later meal timing, relative to SCN phase, on plasma glucose levels.12,14,42 There was no significant day-by-meal interaction effect for plasma insulin iAUC (p = 0.796), but a significant main effect of meal (p = 0.008) was evident. For the main effect of meal, post-hoc comparisons indicated that insulin iAUC was 2591.4 pmol/L⋅min (p = 0.007) lower after dinner compared to breakfast (Table S1; Figure S2), reflecting the higher postprandial glucose iAUC.
Glucose was also measured with continuous glucose monitoring (CGM), which recorded interstitial glucose levels in 5-min intervals across the day and was used to calculate the Mean Amplitude of Glycaemic Excursions (MAGE; the average amplitude of glucose fluctuations with a magnitude of greater than 1 SD) and the Continuous Overall Net Glycemic Action (CONGA; glucose variability within 1 h segments of a continuous time-series). Data were available for test Days 2, 3, and 5. There was no significant effect of the 5-h phase delay on the MAGE (p = 0.196) or on the CONGA (p = 0.409) (Table S2).
Circadian variation in circulating levels of triglycerides (TGs) has also been demonstrated under constant routine conditions, characterized by lower basal levels throughout the daytime before increasing and peaking during the biological night.42 In response to meal timing, diurnal variation in postprandial TG responses has also been shown with greater elevations in postprandial TG in response to a meal during the biological night compared to the same isocaloric meal during the biological day.43 We observed no effect of the intervention on fasting TGs (p = 0.262) (Figure 6E, Table S2). For postprandial TG levels, although there was no significant day-by-meal interaction (p = 0.242), there were significant main effects of study day (p = 0.003) and meals (p < 0.001). For the main effect of day, compared to the pre-shift alignment condition, the total daily postprandial TG iAUC on Day 3 was 24.9 mmol/L⋅min (p = 0.003) higher and was 20.4 mmol/L⋅min (p = 0.026) higher on Day 5 (Figure 6F; Table S2). For the main effect of meal, compared to breakfast, TG iAUC following lunch was 89.4 mmol/L⋅min (p < 0.001) higher and 55.5 mmol/L⋅min (p < 0.001) higher following dinner (Table S2; Figure S2). Taken together, our data are consistent with previous research demonstrating that mean postprandial TG concentrations are higher at night than during the day.43
Subjective hunger, fullness, and appetite are unaffected by a 5-h phase delay
We assessed subjective measures of hunger, appetite, and fullness using visual analogue scale (VAS), every hour from waking for 16 h during the test days (Figure S3). We found no significant differences in subjective hunger (p = 0.992), fullness (p = 0.982), or appetite (p = 0.990), between days. Previous laboratory studies have indicated that rhythms in subjective hunger and appetite have an endogenous circadian component, peaking in the biological evening, independent of the behavioral cycle and food intake.6,44 We have recently shown that earlier, morning-loaded temporal distribution of energy improves appetite and lowers appetite hormones during hypocaloric dieting.25 In a recent laboratory study that delayed meals by 5 h (similar to our present study), such that breakfast, lunch, and dinner occurred 5 h, 9h, and 13 h, respectively, after wake time, the later temporal meal timing increased waking subjective appetite and dysregulated hunger and energy balance hormones.11 Using a similar design, Wehrens et al.6 did not detect significant effects of mealtimes delayed by 5 h but did show lower morning hunger levels under constant routine conditions. The overall weight of data suggests some temporal variation in subjective hunger and appetite, and the lack of any observed effect in our present study may be due to design and analytical differences between our study and previous research.
Limitations of the study
The findings from this metabolically healthy sample may not be generalizable to population groups with elevated cardio-metabolic risk factors, which could potentially exhibit more impaired metabolic responses to a 5-h phase delay. Although our participants were metabolically healthy, they represent a different demographic to most human physiology studies. However, this move away from studying healthy young adults is important for the field of human physiology. We have not analyzed our data separately for male versus female participants as the study was not powered to detect sex effects. Although our meals were matched for energy content, it is also possible that the differences in the GI of the meals, particularly breakfast, may have influenced the postprandial glucose and insulin responses observed in our study. Due to the existing complexity and therefore participant burden within the protocol, we did not include objective measures of sleep (e.g., polysomnography) or measure energy metabolism during the sleep period. As in any circadian desynchrony study, altered sleep timing may alter sleep quality. Nonetheless, we provided participants with an 8-h sleep opportunity each day to minimize such effects, so it is unlikely that there was a substantive influence of sleep alteration on our data.
Conclusions
In response to our 5-h phase delay protocol, some markers of daily metabolism, in particular gastric emptying and total postprandial glucose levels, exhibited acute change following the 5-h phase delay. However, these markers reverted to pre-misalignment values as DLMO, the gold-standard marker of the SCN clock, synchronized to the new behavioral cycle. We confirm and extend our recent research challenging assumptions regarding the time-of-day effect on energy metabolism.24 Together, this body of work suggests limited importance of meal timing on TEF. This study represents an important refinement of previous proof-of-concept studies to model physiological challenges experienced in the real world.
Resource availability
Lead contact
Further information and requests for resources and reagents should be directed to and will be fulfilled by the lead contact by Professor Jonathan D Johnston, Section of Chronobiology, School of Biosciences, Faculty of Health and Medical Sciences, University of Surrey, Guildford, Surrey GU2 7XH, UK (j.johnston@surrey.ac.uk).
Materials availability
This study did not generate new unique reagents.
Data and code availability
-
•
All data reported in this paper will be shared by the lead contact upon request.
-
•
This paper does not report any original code.
-
•
Any additional information required to reanalyze the data reported in this paper is available from the lead contact upon request.
Acknowledgments
This work was funded by MRC grant (MR/P012205/1 to A.M.J., J.D.J., and P.J.M.). We thank staff of the Surrey Clinical Research Facility; L. Bamford for measurement of metabolite concentrations on the Pentra C400 Clinical Chemistry Analyzer; N. Jackson for assistance with the isotopic enrichments at the Stable Isotope Mass Spectrometry Unit, University of Surrey; F. Shojaee-Moradie for assistance with ELISA; A. Collins, H. Biyikoglu, and R Manders for assistance with operating GEMs; and P. Jefcoate and A. Brealy for assistance with meal preparation. Finally, we thank D van der Veen for helpful discussions during manuscript preparation.
Author contributions
Conceptualization, A.M.J., J.D.J., P.J.M., and L.R.C.; methodology, A.M.J., J.D.J., P.J.M., L.R.C., B.F., J.v.G., M.S., J.M., and C.D.M.; formal analysis, J.M., A.F., J.v.G., M.S., and C.D.M.; investigation, A.F., L.R.C., and V.R.; writing—original draft, A.F. and L.R.C.; writing—review & editing, all authors; funding acquisition, A.M.J., J.D.J., and P.J.M.; supervision, J.D.J., B.F., A.M.J., P.J.M., and L.R.C.
Declaration of interests
J.D.J. has collaborated with Nestle ´and has undertaken consultancy work for Kellogg’s and International Flavors and Fragrances (IFF). A.M.J holds a voluntary position at British Nutrition Foundation, London, as an Advisory Committee member.
STAR★Methods
Key resources table
REAGENT or RESOURCE | SOURCE | IDENTIFIER |
---|---|---|
Biological samples | ||
Human Blood and Saliva Samples | This study | N/A |
Chemicals, peptides, and recombinant proteins | ||
13c Octonoic Acid | CK Isotopes Limited | DLM-619 |
Critical commercial assays | ||
Insulin ELISA Assays | DRG International GmbH | Cat# EIA-2935 |
ABX Pentra Glucose Assays | Horiba Instruments Ltd. | Cat# A11A01668 |
ABX Pentra Triglyceride Assays | Horiba Instruments Ltd. | Cat# A11A01640 |
Direct Saliva Melatonin RIA | NovoLytiX GmbH | Cat# RK-DSM2 |
Software and algorithms | ||
IBM SPSS, Version 27.0 | Armonk, NY, USA: IBM Corp | https://www.ibm.com/products/spss-statistics |
SAS, Version 9.4 | NC, USA: SAS Institute Inc | https://www.sas.com/en_us/software/stat.html |
GraphPad Prism, Version 9.1 | GraphPad Software 2021, La Jolla California, USA | https://www.graphpad.com/ |
WinDiets software | Professional Version, Robert Gordon University, Aberdeen, UK, 2017 | https://www.rgu.ac.uk/windiets |
CareLink iPro Therapy Management Software (CGMS; web-based system) | Medtronic Limited, Hertfordshire UK | https://carelink.minimed.eu/ipro/ |
Other | ||
RMR and ventilated hood | GEM Nutrition | http://www.gemnutrition.co.uk/ |
Continuous Glucose Monitors (iPro2 system) | Medtronic Limited, Hertfordshire UK | https://www.medtronic.com/uk-en/index.html |
Experimental model and study participant details
Participants
All study procedures were reviewed and given a favourable ethical opinion by the University of Surrey Ethics Committee [UEC/2018/041/FHMS (CRC382)]. Fourteen participants (8 male, 6 female) completed the study in a single experimental group [Table 1]. All participants were of White British ethnicity. All participants provided written informed consent prior to participation in the study. Eligibility criteria included adult males and females aged 20–60 years, body mass index (BMI) of 27–40 kg/m2 inclusive, habitual 6–9 h sleep per night, and otherwise healthy. Exclusion criteria included prescription or nutritional supplement use known to affect sleep, extreme chronotype (Horne-Östberg score <30 or >70; habitual bedtime outside 22:00-01:00), any history of sleep or circadian disorders, history of metabolic disease, or current smokers. As a condition of entry to the laboratory study, participants were required to follow a 1-week baseline run-in period, maintaining a consistent and habitual sleep-wake timing and consistent spacing between breakfast, lunch, and dinner, to be consumed 1 h, 6 h, and 11 h after waking, respectively. Participants were also required to obtain 15 min of natural, outdoor light exposure within 90 min of waking.
To confirm adherence, participants were required to call the University of Surrey Clinical Research Facility [CRF] voicemail within 10 min of going to bed and waking up. In addition, participants were also provided with two actiwatches (Cambridge Neurotechnology, Cambridge, UK), which recorded activity levels and environmental light levels; one actiwatch was worn on the non-dominant wrist to record activity, while the other was worn around the neck to monitor light exposure near eye level. The actigraphy data was downloaded on Day 0 when participants presented to the CRF for admission to the study, to ensure compliance with the baseline sleep-wake schedule. Compliance confirmed by actigraphy was a condition of admission to the laboratory phase of the study.
Method details
Laboratory study design
We evaluated the effect of a 5 h phase delay on energy expenditure and related endocrine and metabolic pathways in humans. This objective was achieved by conducting a 7-day residential, phase-shift protocol, in which the behavioral cycles of sleep-wake timing, light-dark, and meal timing, were delayed by 5 h between days 2–3 [Figures 1A and 1B].10 Throughout the laboratory study, light levels were maintained at 500 lux in the direction of gaze and blue light-blocking glasses were worn for the second half of the lights-on/wake period. The sleep-wake timing for all participants during the in-patient stay were standardised to within 30mins of their habitual sleep-wake timing from the run-in period.
The extension of the light cycle by 5 h between Day 2 and Day 3, coupled with the related delay in the sleep-wake cycle and timing of meals the following day by 5 h, created misaligned timing of cues in light, sleep-wake, and meals, for the circadian system. This “phase delay” or “phase shift” would have the effect of altering the timing of external behaviors [light-dark, sleep-wake, and meals], while internal circadian rhythms would initially remain aligned with the previous timing of behaviors on Day 2. The phase delay also preserved a nightly 8 h sleep opportunity, to minimise any acute effect of sleep curtailment.
Participants arrived at the laboratory on Day 0 and were admitted following confirmation of adherence to the pre-lab routine by actigraphy [Figure 1A]. Participants continued to wear the actiwatch on their non-dominant wrist to provide estimates of sleep-wake parameters derived from the actigraphy data. Day 1 of the protocol served as a habituation day to the study schedule and laboratory conditions maintained at the pre-laboratory sleep-wake and feed/fast cycles, and no testing was conducted on this day. Day 2 of the protocol followed the exact same timing of the feeding/fasting and sleep/wake cycle as Day 1, but included baseline testing for experimental measures.
A 5-h delay in sleep onset and feed-fast/light-dark was induced on the evening of Day 2 into Day 3. To maintain a consistent fasting period between last energy intake and the test breakfast, a supper was provided 5 h after dinner on the evening of Day 2 [∼23.00 h]. Participants remained on the delayed behavioral/environmental cycle for 5 days to monitor adaptation of biological rhythms to the new schedule. The protocol compared energy expenditure, metabolic responses, and melatonin onset timing throughout the experiment, as follows: a) Daily rhythms aligned to behavior cycles [Day 2]; b) Acute misalignment of daily rhythms following 5 h delay in behavioral cycles [Day 3]; c) Adaptation of daily rhythms to the delayed behavioral cycles [Day 5 and Day 7] [Figure 1A].
The study was a fixed-feeding protocol designed to maintain participants at a stable weight. Energy intake tailored to individual requirements, calculated as basal metabolic rate using the Mifflin-St. Joer equation,28 multiplied by a physical activity factor of 1.1–1.3. Breakfast, lunch, and dinner were isocaloric [33.3% of total daily energy intake], and the macronutrient composition of the diets was 20% protein, 35% fat, and 45% carbohydrate [Table S2]. Supper on Day 2 was matched to the energy content of other meals and was on average ∼3,123kJ additional energy, with the same macronutrient composition of other meals. Each meal contained a minimum energy content of 2301kJ, with individual participant energy requirements being met through the addition of prepared milkshakes in quantities specific to requirements of the individual participant. Meals were initiated 1 h after waking, with lunch and dinner separated by 5 h, respectively.
Basal energy expenditure
Basal energy expenditure (resting metabolic rate, RMR) and the thermic effect of feeding [TEF] were measured using indirect calorimetry [GEM Nutrition, Bath, UK]. RMR was measured for 30-min, within 5-min of waking, to obtain a fasted baseline RMR measure. Thereafter, participants’ RMR was measured for 10-min every half-hour, for 5 h after breakfast, lunch, and dinner respectively. Thus, measures of energy expenditure covered 15.5 h of the 16 h waking period [Figure 1B]. Participants lay in a semi-supine position for the duration of each measurement. RMR was calculated from VO2 and VCO2 using the Elia and Livesey45 equation and measured on a minute-by-minute basis. For the baseline fasted RMR measure, the initial 5-min of the data was excluded, and the RMR calculated from a 15-min moving average with the lowest coefficient of variation [CV]. Postprandial energy expenditure from each subsequent 10-min measurement was determined by calculating consecutive data from a minimum of 5-min with the lowest CV. The TEF was calculated using incremental area under the curve [iAUC] based on a number of methods, reflecting different methodological approaches used in the literature, as described by Ruddick-Collins et al.24
Blood and saliva collection
Blood samples for glucose, lipids, and insulin, were collected immediately prior to each meal via a cannula inserted into the forearm, and then at 15, 30, 60, 90, and 120 min time points in the postprandial period [Figure 1B]. 4.5mL was collected at each time point, and separated, respectively, into 2mL lithium heparin and 2mL potassium EDTA vacutainers. In addition, to assess circadian phase, blood samples for melatonin were collected every hour for 13 consecutive samples, e.g., between 16.00 h and 04.00 h [Figure 1B]. 4mL blood was collected into lithium heparin vacutainers for this. In the event that there were difficulties obtaining a melatonin blood sample, participants provided a 2mL saliva sample collected at the scheduled collection time point. Upon collection of a blood sample, sample tubes were inverted ten times and cooled until centrifugation [1260 x g at 4°C for 10-min, within 30-min of collection]. Plasma samples were then stored at −20°C [melatonin] or −80°C [glucose, lipids, insulin], pending analysis. Saliva samples were sealed and stored immediately at −80°C.
Endocrine and metabolic measures
Plasma melatonin was analyzed by 3H melatonin radioimmunoassay, which has been validated by gas chromatography-mass spectrometry [GCMS], as previously described.46 The inter-assay CV was 23.5% at 9.7 pg/mL, 8.4% at 35.5 pg/mL, 9.8% at 73.5 pg/mL, and 10.1% at 136.5 pg/mL. Salivary melatonin was analyzed by the 125I melatonin radioimmunoassay.46 The inter-assay CV for salivary melatonin was 24.4% at 2 pg/mL and 5.3% at 16.4 pg/mL. Melatonin results were analyzed by calculating the timing of melatonin signal onset, known as dim light melatonin onset [DLMO], an established marker of the endogenous phase of the central SCN clock, as previously described.29 3 pg/mL of salivary melatonin is considered comparable to 10 pg/mL.29 Thus, for the present study, DLMO was determined as the time from which melatonin levels rose above of pre-specified absolute threshold of ≥3 and ≥10 pg/mL for salivary and plasma samples, respectively.29
Plasma insulin was analyzed by enzyme-linked immunosorbent assay [ELISA] (DRG International, Germany). A standard curve of 0–100uIU/mL was derived from the human insulin standards provided with the ELISA kits. The inter-assay CV was 4.85% at 20.79uIU/mL and 7.28% at 65.98uIU/mL.
Plasma glucose and triglyceride [TGs] concentrations were analyzed by enzyme assays [Pentra C400 Clinical Chemistry Analyzer] (Horiba Medical, UK).47,48 For plasma glucose, the inter-assay CV was 1.61% at 5.23 mmol/L and 1.40% at 14.22 mmol/L For plasma triglycerides, the inter-assay CV was 3.43% at 1.42 mmol/L and 2.75% at 2.57 mmol/L. For plasma glucose, insulin, and plasma TGs, postprandial area under the curve [AUC] and incremental area under the curve [iAUC] were calculated using the trapezoidal rule for the 120min postprandial period, as well as fasting levels. Homeostatic model of insulin resistance [HOMA-IR] was also calculated from fasting insulin and glucose measures using the HOMA-2 calculator.49
Continuous glucose monitoring
Glucose was also measured using continuous glucose monitoring [CGM]. CGMs (Medtronic, USA) recorded interstitial glucose levels through a probe inserted into subcutaneous abdominal fat, and glucose data was recorded in 5-min intervals and calibrated against 3–4 finger-prick blood glucose samples taken across the day. For the CGM data, the Mean Amplitude of Glycemic Excursions [MAGE]50 and the Continuous Overall Net Glycemic Action [CONGA]51 were calculated using the EasyGV V9.0.R2 macro in Microsoft Excel.52 The MAGE is defined as the average amplitude of glucose fluctuations with a magnitude of greater than 1 SD (42). The CONGA is defined as the glucose variability within short defined segments of a continuous time-series, e.g., 1 h.51
Gastric emptying
Gastric emptying was assessed by the 13C octanoic acid breath test.37,38,39 Participants were provided with a standardised test day breakfast enriched with 100mg of 13C octanoic acid stable isotope tracer, mixed into eggs as octanoic acid is readily solubilised in egg yolk.35,36 The ratio of 13C–12C in the breath was measured immediately before breakfast [-5mins] and at 15, 30, 45, 60, 75, 90, 120, 150, 180, 240, and 300 min after breakfast, and expressed as a percentage of dose per hour. Values for the cumulative percentage of 13CO2 recovered were fitted by the following model:
Where y is the cumulative percentage dose of 13CO2 recovered at time-point t (mins), and m, k, and ß are the estimated parameters. From this model, the gastric emptying half-time [T-½] and solid lag phase [T-lag] were calculated by non-linear regression.
Subjective measures
Participants also completed subjective measures of sleepiness, alertness, hunger, appetite, and fullness, every hour from waking for 16 h each test day [Figure 1B]. Participants completed the Karolinska Sleepiness Scale [KSS], which has been validated against electroencephalographic [EEG] activity measures.53 Participants also completed a subjective alertness scale, which has previously been shown to correlate with internal biological [i.e., circadian] time and with the duration of prior wakefulness.54 For subjective hunger, appetite, and satiety, participants completed a subjective visual analogue scale [VAS], which have been shown to have good reproducibility and validity for use in controlled feeding studies.55,56
Quantification and statistical analysis
Power calculation and statistical analysis
Sample size estimation was based on previously published data by Morris et al.,22 which investigated the effects of 12 h circadian misalignment on early (∼2 h) thermic effect of feeding [TEF] and postprandial energy expenditure in a controlled laboratory intervention. Based on an expected difference of >1SD between TEF aligned to circadian vs. behavioral patterns, and for an alpha level of 5% and 90% power, a sample size of 13 was estimated. In anticipation of a 15% non-completion rate, the study aimed to run 15 participants through the experimental protocol.
Data were analyzed in IBM Statistical Package for the Social Sciences (IBM SPSS Statistics for Windows, Version 27.0. Armonk, NY, USA: IBM Corp) using a linear mixed model [LMM] with an compound symmetry covariance structure, with participants entered as random effects to account for within-person correlations on repeated measures over study days. Use of LMM prevented listwise deletion due to missing data. Where study day was the only dependent variable, this was entered as fixed effects [without interaction term] into the model, with participants entered as random effects. For outcome measures which had both study day and meal as dependent variables, these were entered as fixed effects with an interaction term, and participants as random effects. The accepted significance for all post-hoc comparisons following a statistically significant main effect were adjusted for multiple comparisons using the Bonferroni correction. Data from the KSS, alertness scale, and VAS, were analyzed using LMM in SAS (Version 9.4, NC, USA: SAS Institute Inc.). Data were assessed for normality by visual inspection of Q-Q normality plots and histograms of the residuals in SPSS, and calculation of z-scores for skewness and kurtosis (within ±1.96) where necessary. Data not following a normal distribution were log-transformed. The assumptions of linearity and homoscedasticity were checked against residuals plots of the residual and predicted values from the model. GraphPad Prism v9.1 (GraphPad Software 2021, La Jolla California, USA) was used for graphing of outcome measures.
Published: October 11, 2024
Footnotes
Supplemental information can be found online at https://doi.org/10.1016/j.isci.2024.111161.
Supplemental information
References
- 1.Johnston J.D., Ordovás J.M., Scheer F.A., Turek F.W. Circadian Rhythms, Metabolism, and Chrononutrition in Rodents and Humans. Adv. Nutr. 2016;7:399–406. doi: 10.3945/an.115.010777. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Flanagan A., Bechtold D.A., Pot G.K., Johnston J.D. Chrono-nutrition: From molecular and neuronal mechanisms to human epidemiology and timed feeding patterns. J. Neurochem. 2021;157:53–72. doi: 10.1111/jnc.15246. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Aschoff J. Circadian Rhythms in Man. Science (1979) 1965;148:1427–1432. doi: 10.1126/science.148.3676.1427. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Stephan F.K., Zucker I. Circadian Rhythms in Drinking Behavior and Locomotor Activity of Rats Are Eliminated by Hypothalamic Lesions. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 1972;69:1583–1586. doi: 10.1073/pnas.69.6.1583. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Reppert S.M., Weaver D.R. Coordination of circadian timing in mammals. Nature. 2002;418:935–941. doi: 10.1038/nature00965. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Wehrens S.M.T., Christou S., Isherwood C., Middleton B., Gibbs M.A., Archer S.N., Skene D.J., Johnston J.D. Meal Timing Regulates the Human Circadian System. Curr. Biol. 2017;27:1768–1775.e3. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2017.04.059. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Eckel-mahan K.L., Patel V.R., de Mateo S., Orozco-solis R., Ceglia N.J., Sahar S., Dilag-Penilla S.A., Dyar K.A., Baldi P., Sassone-Corsi P. Reprogramming of the Circadian Clock by Nutritional Challenge. Cell. 2013;155:1464–1478. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2013.11.034. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Isherwood C.M., van der Veen D.R., Hassanin H., Skene D.J., Johnston J.D. Human glucose rhythms and subjective hunger anticipate meal timing. Curr. Biol. 2023;33:1321–1326.e3. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2023.02.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Ruddick-Collins L.C., Morgan P.J., Johnstone A.M. Mealtime: A circadian disruptor and determinant of energy balance? J. Neuroendocrinol. 2020;32 doi: 10.1111/jne.12886. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Ruddick-Collins L.C., Johnston J.D., Morgan P.J., Johnstone A.M. The Big Breakfast Study: Chrono-nutrition influence on energy expenditure and bodyweight. Nutr. Bull. 2018;43:174–183. doi: 10.1111/nbu.12323. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Vujović N., Piron M.J., Qian J., Chellappa S.L., Nedeltcheva A., Barr D., Heng S.W., Kerlin K., Srivastav S., Wang W., et al. Late isocaloric eating increases hunger, decreases energy expenditure, and modifies metabolic pathways in adults with overweight and obesity. Cell Metab. 2022;34:1486–1498.e7. doi: 10.1016/j.cmet.2022.09.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Van Cauter E., Désir D., Decoster C., Féry F., Balasse E.O. Nocturnal Decrease in Glucose Tolerance During Constant Glucose Infusion. J. Clin. Endocrinol. Metab. 1989;69:604–611. doi: 10.1210/jcem-69-3-604. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Morgan L.M., Aspostolakou F., Wright J., Gama R. Diurnal variations in peripheral insulin resistance and plasma non-esterified fatty acid concentrations: a possible link. Ann. Clin. Biochem. 1999;36:447–450. doi: 10.1177/000456329903600407. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Morgan L.M., Shi J.W., Hampton S.M., Frost G. Effect of meal timing and glycaemic index on glucose control and insulin secretion in healthy volunteers. Br. J. Nutr. 2012;108:1286–1291. doi: 10.1017/S0007114511006507. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Qian J., Dalla Man C., Morris C.J., Cobelli C., Scheer F.A.J.L. Differential effects of the circadian system and circadian misalignment on insulin sensitivity and insulin secretion in humans. Diabetes Obes. Metab. 2018;20:2481–2485. doi: 10.1111/dom.13391. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Morris C.J., Yang J.N., Garcia J.I., Myers S., Bozzi I., Wang W., Buxton O.M., Shea S.A., Scheer F.A.J.L. Endogenous circadian system and circadian misalignment impact glucose tolerance via separate mechanisms in humans. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 2015;112:E2225–E2234. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1418955112. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Scheer F.A.J.L., Hilton M.F., Mantzoros C.S., Shea S.A. Adverse metabolic and cardiovascular consequences of circadian misalignment. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 2009;106:4453–4458. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0808180106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Goo R.H., Moore J.G., Greenberg E., Alazraki N.P. Circadian variation in gastric emptying of meals in humans. Gastroenterology. 1987;93:515–518. doi: 10.1016/0016-5085(87)90913-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Zitting K.M., Vujovic N., Yuan R.K., Isherwood C.M., Medina J.E., Wang W., Buxton O.M., Williams J.S., Czeisler C.A., Duffy J.F. Human Resting Energy Expenditure Varies with Circadian Phase. Curr. Biol. 2018;28:3685–3690.e3. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2018.10.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Bo S., Fadda M., Castiglione A., Ciccone G., De Francesco A., Fedele D., Guggino A., Parasiliti Caprino M., Ferrara S., Vezio Boggio M., et al. Is the timing of caloric intake associated with variation in diet-induced thermogenesis and in the metabolic pattern? A randomized cross-over study. Int. J. Obes. 2015;39:1689–1695. doi: 10.1038/ijo.2015.138. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Richter J., Herzog N., Janka S., Baumann T., Kistenmacher A., Oltmanns K.M. Twice as High Diet-Induced Thermogenesis After Breakfast vs Dinner On High-Calorie as Well as Low- Calorie Meals. J. Clin. Endocrinol. Metab. 2020;105:e211–e221. doi: 10.1210/clinem/dgz311. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Morris C.J., Garcia J.I., Myers S., Yang J.N., Trienekens N., Scheer F.A.J.L. The human circadian system has a dominating role in causing the morning/evening difference in early diet-induced thermogenesis. Obesity. 2015;23:2053–2058. doi: 10.1002/oby.21189. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Romon M., Edme J., Boulenguez C., Lescroart J., Frimat P. Circadian variation of diet-induced thermogenesis. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 1993;57:476–480. doi: 10.1093/ajcn/57.4.476. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Ruddick-Collins L.C., Flanagan A., Johnston J.D., Morgan P.J., Johnstone A.M. Circadian Rhythms in Resting Metabolic Rate Account for Apparent Daily Rhythms in the Thermic Effect of Food. J. Clin. Endocrinol. Metab. 2022;107:e708–e715. doi: 10.1210/clinem/dgab654. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Ruddick-Collins L.C., Morgan P.J., Fyfe C.L., Filipe J.A.N., Horgan G.W., Westerterp K.R., Johnston J.D., Johnstone A.M. Timing of daily calorie loading affects appetite and hunger responses without changes in energy metabolism in healthy subjects with obesity. Cell Metab. 2022;34:1472–1485.e6. doi: 10.1016/j.cmet.2022.08.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Garaulet M., Gómez-Abellán P., Alburquerque-Béjar J.J., Lee Y.C., Ordovás J.M., Scheer F.A.J.L. Timing of food intake predicts weight loss effectiveness Prof. Int. J. Obes. 2013;37:604–611. doi: 10.1038/ijo.2012.229. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Jakubowicz D., Barnea M., Wainstein J., Froy O. High Caloric intake at breakfast vs. dinner differentially influences weight loss of overweight and obese women. Obesity. 2013;21:2504–2512. doi: 10.1002/oby.20460. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Mifflin M.D., St Jeor S.T., Hill L.A., Scott B.J., Daugherty S.A., Koh Y.O. A new predictive equation for resting energy expenditure in healthy individuals. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 1990;51:241–247. doi: 10.1093/ajcn/51.2.241. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Benloucif S., Burgess H.J., Klerman E.B., Lewy A.J., Middleton B., Murphy P.J., Parry B.L., Revell V.L. Measuring melatonin in humans. J. Clin. Sleep Med. 2008;4:66–69. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Reiter R.J. Pineal Melatonin: Cell Biology of Its Synthesis and of Its Physiological Interactions. Endocr. Rev. 1991;12:151–180. doi: 10.1210/edrv-12-2-151. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Boulos Z., Macchi M.M., Stürchler M.P., Stewart K.T., Brainard G.C., Suhner A., Wallace G., Steffen R. Light visor treatment for jet lag after westward travel across six time zones. Aviat Space Environ. Med. 2002;73:953–963. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Reed G.W., Hill J.O. Measuring the thermic effect of food. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 1996;63:164–169. doi: 10.1093/ajcn/63.2.164. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Ravussin E., Beyl R.A., Poggiogalle E., Hsia D.S., Peterson C.M. Early Time-Restricted Feeding Reduces Appetite and Increases Fat Oxidation But Does Not Affect Energy Expenditure in Humans. Obesity. 2019;27:1244–1254. doi: 10.1002/oby.22518. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Dibner C., Schibler U., Albrecht U. The mammalian circadian timing system: Organization and coordination of central and peripheral clocks. Ann Rev Physiol. 2010;72:517–549. doi: 10.1146/annurev-physiol-021909-135821. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Ghoos Y.F., Maes B.D., Geypens B.J., Mys G., Hiele M.I., Rutgeerts P.J., Vantrappen G. Measurement of gastric emptying rate of solids by means of a carbon-labeled octanoic acid breath test. Gastroenterology. 1993;104:1640–1647. doi: 10.1016/0016-5085(93)90640-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Maes B.D., Ghoos Y.F., Geypens B.J., Mys G., Hiele M.I., Rutgeerts P.J., Vantrappen G. Combined carbon-13-glycine/carbon-14-octanoic acid breath test to monitor gastric emptying rates of liquids and solids. J. Nucl. Med. 1994;35:824–831. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.von Gerichten J., Elnesr M.H., Prollins J.E., de Mel I.A., Flanagan A., Johnston J.D., Fielding B.A., Short M. The [13C]octanoic acid breath test for gastric emptying quantification: A focus on nutrition and modeling. Lipids. 2022;57:205–219. doi: 10.1002/lipd.12352. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Umpleby M., Fielding B.A. In: Nutrition Research Methodologies. 1st ed. Lovegrove A., Hodson L., Sharma S., Lanham-New S.A., editors. John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.; 2015. Stable Isotopes in Nutrition Research. [Google Scholar]
- 39.Perri F., Pastore M.R., Annese V. C-octanoic acid breath test for measuring gastric emptying of solids. Eur. Rev. Med. Pharmacol. Sci. 2006;9:3–8. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Bolli G.B., De Feo P., de Cosmo S., Perriello G., Ventura M.M., Calcinaro F., Lolli C., Campbell P., Brunetti P., Gerich J.E. Demonstration of a Dawn Phenomenon in Normal Human Volunteers. Diabetes. 1984;33:1150–1153. doi: 10.2337/diab.33.12.1150. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Troisi R.J., Cowie C.C., Harris M.I. Diurnal Variation in Fasting Plasma Glucose Implications for Diagnosis of Diabetes in Patients Examined in the Afternoon. JAMA. 2000;284:3157–3159. doi: 10.1001/jama.284.24.3157. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Morgan L., Arendt J., Owens D., Folkard S., Hampton S., Deacon S., English J., Ribeiro D., Taylor K. Effects of the endogenous clock and sleep time on melatonin, insulin, glucose and lipid metabolism. J. Endocrinol. 1998;157:443–451. doi: 10.1677/joe.0.1570443. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Romon M., Le Fur C., Lebel P., Edmé J.L., Fruchart J.C., Dallongeville J. Circadian variation of postprandial lipemia. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 1997;65:934–940. doi: 10.1093/ajcn/65.4.934. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Scheer F.A.J.L. The Internal Circadian Clock Increases Hunger and Appetite in the Evening Independent of Food Intake and Other Behaviors. Obesity. 2013;16:3–7. doi: 10.1002/oby.20351. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Elia M., Livesey G. Energy expenditure and fuel selection in biological systems: the theory and practice of calculations based on indirect calorimetry and tracer methods. World Rev. Nutr. Diet. 1992;70:68–131. doi: 10.1159/000421672. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Middleton B. Measurement of melatonin and 6-sulphatoxymelatonin. Methods Mol. Biol. 2013;1065:171–199. doi: 10.1007/978-1-62703-616-0_11. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Lott J.A., Turner K. Evaluation of Trinder’s glucose oxidase method for measuring glucose in serum and urine. Clin. Chem. 1975;21:1754–1760. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Fossati P., Prencipe L. Serum triglycerides determined colorimetrically with an enzyme that produces hydrogen peroxide. Clin. Chem. 1982;28:2077–2080. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.University of Oxford DTUnit HOMA2 Calculator. 2019. https://www.dtu.ox.ac.uk/homacalculator/
- 50.Service F.J., Molnar G.D., Rosevear J.W., Ackerman E., Gatewood L.C., Taylor W.F. Mean amplitude of glycemic excursions, a measure of diabetic instability. Diabetes. 1970;19:644–655. doi: 10.2337/diab.19.9.644. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Mcdonnell C.M., Donath S.M., Vidmar S.I., Werther G.A., Cameron F.J. A novel approach to continuous glucose analysis utilizing glycemic variation. Diabetes Technol. Ther. 2005;7:253–263. doi: 10.1089/dia.2005.7.253. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Hill N.R. University of Oxford; 2010. Easy GV.https://www.phc.ox.ac.uk/research/resources/easygv [Google Scholar]
- 53.Åkerstedt T., Gillberg M. Subjective and objective sleepiness in the active individual. Int. J. Neurosci. 1990;52:29–37. doi: 10.3109/00207459008994241. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Lockley S.W., Dijk D.J., Kosti O., Skene D.J., Arendt J. Alertness, mood and performance rhythm disturbances associated with circadian sleep disorders in the blind. J. Sleep Res. 2008;17:207–216. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2869.2008.00656.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Flint A., Raben A., Blundell J.E., Astrup A. Reproducibility, power and validity of visual analogue scales in assessment of appetite sensation in single test meal. Int J Obesity. 2000;24:38–48. doi: 10.1038/sj.ijo.0801083. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Douglas S.M., Leidy H.J. Novel Methodological Considerations Regarding the Use of Visual Analog Scale (VAS) Appetite Questionnaires in Tightly Controlled Feeding Trials. Curr. Dev. Nutr. 2019;3:nzz061. doi: 10.1093/cdn/nzz061. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
-
•
All data reported in this paper will be shared by the lead contact upon request.
-
•
This paper does not report any original code.
-
•
Any additional information required to reanalyze the data reported in this paper is available from the lead contact upon request.