Abstract
Background: Despite a pervasive cortico-centric view in cognitive neuroscience, subcortical structures including the thalamus have been shown to be increasingly involved in higher cognitive functions. Previous structural and functional imaging studies demonstrated cortico-thalamo-cortical loops which may support various cognitive functions including language. However, large-scale functional connectivity of the thalamus during language tasks has not been examined before. Methods: The present study employed meta-analytic connectivity modeling to identify language-related coactivation patterns of the left and right thalami. The left and right thalami were used as regions of interest to search the BrainMap functional database for neuroimaging experiments with healthy participants reporting language-related activations in each region of interest. Activation likelihood estimation analyses were then carried out on the foci extracted from the identified studies to estimate functional convergence for each thalamus. A functional decoding analysis based on the same database was conducted to characterize thalamic contributions to different language functions. Results: The results revealed bilateral frontotemporal and bilateral subcortical (basal ganglia) coactivation patterns for both the left and right thalami, and also right cerebellar coactivations for the left thalamus, during language processing. In light of previous empirical studies and theoretical frameworks, the present connectivity and functional decoding findings suggest that cortico-subcortical-cerebellar-cortical loops modulate and fine-tune information transfer within the bilateral frontotemporal cortices during language processing, especially during production and semantic operations, but also other language (e.g., syntax, phonology) and cognitive operations (e.g., attention, cognitive control). Conclusion: The current findings show that the language-relevant network extends beyond the classical left perisylvian cortices and spans bilateral cortical, bilateral subcortical (bilateral thalamus, bilateral basal ganglia) and right cerebellar regions.
Supplementary Information
The online version contains supplementary material available at 10.1007/s00429-024-02795-3.
Keywords: Thalamus, Language, Functional connectivity, Meta-analytic connectivity modeling, Brain networks
Introduction
Thalamus and language
Cortical-subcortical loops have been associated mainly with non-cognitive or lower cognitive functions such as sensorimotor and affective functions. This lack of association between cognition and subcortex could be due, at least partially, to a cortico-centric view or cortical bias pervasive in cognitive neuroscience literature. Potential contributors to this cortical bias include a priori hypotheses about language and other cognitive functions being mostly cortical, which guides region of interest definition; proliferation of surface-based imaging, recording and stimulation techniques such as magnetoencephalography, electroencephalography, functional near-infrared spectroscopy, and transcranial magnetic stimulation; relatively small size of subcortical nuclei and their high person-to-person variability; hemodynamic response function optimized for cortex not being sensitive enough to subcortical time course of activation; and cortex-based preprocessing and analytical procedures (e.g., coregistration, smoothing) not being ideal for subcortical nuclei (Janacsek et al. 2022; Llano 2013, 2016). Indeed, there is increasing evidence that subcortical structures contribute to cognitive functions such as working memory, cognitive control and language (Copland and Angwin 2019; Copland et al. 2021; Crosson 2021; Janacsek et al. 2022; Murphy et al. 2022; Shine et al. 2023; Theofanopoulou and Boeckx 2016; Ullman 2016).
One of the prominent subcortical structures involved in cognitive functions is the thalamus. The thalamus is a small diencephalic structure located bilaterally near the center of the brain in close proximity to the basal ganglia (Fig. 1A). As illustrated in Fig. 1B, each thalamus consists of multiple nuclei, in total 15 in the automated anatomic labelling atlas 3 (AAL3) (Rolls et al. 2020), although there are discrepancies between specific atlases due to parcellation differences (histological, structural or functional MRI, etc.) and inclusion or exclusion of very small or difficult-to-parcellate nuclei. The thalamic nuclei can be grouped anatomically into four major groups: anterior, medial, lateral and posterior (Kumar et al. 2023). These thalamic nuclei have long been known to act as relay stations between the body and the cerebral cortex, transferring and filtering sensorimotor information, and regulating sleep and wakefulness (Cassel and de Vasconcelos 2015; Torrico and Munakomi 2019). However, this structure has been increasingly associated with higher cognitive functions including language, as well.
Lesions in the thalamus, mostly in the left hemisphere, have long been linked to what is known as thalamic aphasia with symptoms primarily including naming deficits (problems of word finding and retrieval such as semantic paraphasia) with relatively preserved repetition, fluency and grammar (Crosson 2013; De Witte et al. 2011; Nadeau and Crosson 1997), while these language impairments frequently co-occur with symptoms in other domains such as amnestic problems, executive dysfunctions and behavior and/or mood alterations (De Witte et al. 2011). This involvement of the thalamus with language processing is consistent with the literature on functional imaging of the thalamus, a review of which correlated thalamic activation mostly with language production tasks (e.g., word or sentence production) and naming, with stronger activation in the left than the right thalamus (Llano 2013). Consistently, an extensive review of neuroscience research also associated the thalamus with lexical, semantic, and prosodic processing; category-specific naming; speech production; linguistic integration, among other cognitive functions (Janacsek et al. 2022). Still other language functions attributed to the thalamus include a role in syntactic structure building (syntactic unification) and syntactic and semantic analysis (detection of syntactic and semantic violations) (David et al. 2011; Friederici et al. 2009; Liu et al. 2023; Wahl et al. 2008), and serving as a central monitor for language-related cortical activations during comprehension and production (Klostermann et al. 2013).
Structural and functional connectivity between thalamus and cortex
Given the aforementioned associations between the thalamus and cognitive functions including language, several studies sought to reveal structural and functional connections between the thalamus and cortical sites that have been more traditionally linked to those cognitive functions. Structural connections have been typically investigated in vivo using diffusion-weighted tractography (DWT), which involves a special use of MRI to identify white matter fibers by measuring diffusion of water molecules (Alexander et al. 2007; van den Heuvel and Hulshoff Pol 2010), while functional connectivity has been frequently studied using resting-state fMRI (rsfMRI), which estimates spontaneous low-frequency fluctuations in the BOLD signal (Lee et al. 2013; van den Heuvel and Hulshoff Pol 2010). It is generally assumed that functional connections strongly correlate with structural white matter connections (Greicius et al. 2009; Huang and Ding 2016; van den Heuvel and Hulshoff Pol 2010). This section briefly reviews previous DWT and rsfMRI studies that examined structural and functional connectivity patterns of the thalamus.
DWT studies revealed structural connections between the anterior, medial, posterior and lateral nuclei groups of the thalamus and various subcortical and cortical sites including the prefrontal, parietal and temporal cortices (Bohsali et al. 2015; Cunningham et al. 2017; Ford et al. 2013; Grodd et al. 2020; Jeon et al. 2014; Kumar et al. 2023). Of these studies, some have particularly associated the anterior nuclei of the thalamus with the limbic and memory systems through their connections with other diencephalic structures (hypothalamus), medial temporal lobe structures (amygdala, hippocampus) and prefrontal cortex (Grodd et al. 2020), while others have linked the thalamus with the default mode network through its connections with the hippocampus, medial prefrontal cortex, superior frontal gyrus and the precuneus (Cunningham et al. 2017). In studies more specifically examining tracts connecting language-related regions, Broca’s area (pars opercularis, pars triangularis) as well as other prefrontal regions were found to structurally connect especially to the ventral anterior nucleus of the thalamus (Barbas et al. 2013; Bohsali et al. 2015; Ford et al. 2013; Jeon et al. 2014). Other thalamic nuclei that exhibited structural connections with Broca’s area and other prefrontal regions include the pulvinar in the posterior nuclei group (Bohsali et al. 2015) and the medial dorsal nucleus (Barbas et al. 2013; Jeon et al. 2014). It has been suggested that the connections between Broca’s area and the thalamus (ventral anterior nucleus, pulvinar) constitute a cortico-thalamo-cortical loop that plays a role in lexical-semantic processing (Bohsali et al. 2015; Crosson 2021). In particular, the thalamus is argued to mediate transfer of semantic (pars triangularis/orbitalis) and phonological (pars opercularis) information between these anterior/posterior subdivisions of Broca’s area during lexical-semantic processing, given also previous research associating thalamic aphasia with word finding problems and semantic paraphasias (Bohsali et al. 2015; Crosson 2021). In some of those DWT studies, prefrontal regions including Broca’s area which are structurally connected to the thalamus were found to connect also to the basal ganglia structures including the putamen (Ford et al. 2013) and the caudate body/head (Jeon et al. 2014). This cortico-striatal-thalamo-cortical loop was also associated with lexical-semantic processing, with the cortico-striatal part of the network serving to enhance the activation of contextually most suitable semantic/phonemic representations while inhibiting competitors (Ford et al. 2013).
Investigations of the thalamus using rsfMRI identified functional connectivity patterns across the cortex largely overlapping with its structural connections (Cunningham et al. 2017; Hale et al. 2015; O’Muircheartaigh et al. 2015), but also with some differences mainly due to certain medial group nuclei projecting to the cortex indirectly via subcortical connections while their functional connectivity involves all cortical lobes (Kumar et al. 2017). Furthermore, clinical imaging studies also revealed altered structural and functional thalamocortical connections in various neurological conditions. For instance, decreased prefrontal-thalamic structural and functional connectivity was associated with cognitive impairments in Schizophrenia (Giraldo-Chica et al. 2018; Giraldo-Chica and Woodward 2017). Likewise, altered resting-state functional connectivity between the thalamus and cortex has been observed in mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s disease (Wang et al. 2012; Zhou et al. 2013). These alterations mainly include reduced connections of the thalamus with precentral and parietal cortices, as well as increased connections in a host of cortical sites, interpreted as thalamocortical network disconnection and compensatory mechanisms, respectively (Wang et al. 2012; Zhou et al. 2013). Other conditions with altered structural and/or functional thalamo-cortical connections include autism (Tomasi and Volkow 2019), developmental dyslexia (Müller-Axt et al. 2017; Tschentscher et al. 2019), and age-related cognitive decline (Fama and Sullivan 2015). Finally, altered resting-state functional and effective connectivity as well as task-based effective connectivity was observed in the basal ganglia (putamen, caudate nucleus) - thalamo - cortical [(pre) supplementary motor cortex, primary motor cortex, inferior frontal gyrus, middle and superior temporal gyri] circuit in developmental stuttering, pointing to dysfunctional circuits that support speech planning and timing cues necessary to initiate and execute motor sequences (Lu et al. 2010; Qiao et al. 2017). These associations highlight the various functions that the thalamus assumes in various cognitive domains.
As briefly reviewed here, previous explorations of structural and functional connectivity of the thalamus demonstrated cortico-thalamic as well as cortico-striatal-thalamic networks which may underlie various functions likely including linguistic ones. However, these techniques are not without limitations, which may limit the conclusions drawn. For example, the limited resolution of DWT based on the currently available models make it difficult to link white matter fibers to specific grey matter sites (Ford et al. 2013; Kumar et al. 2017; Xiang et al. 2010). Indeed, a comparison of DWT and rsfMRI of the thalamus suggests that structural connections identified with DWT do not align well with rsfMRI connections and that due to its methodological limitations (spatial resolution, fiber crossings, etc.), DWT is of limited use in reflecting functional variability in corticothalamic connections (Kumar et al. 2017). Moreover, the extent to which functional and structural connections identified in these studies are language-relevant is not clear as DWT and rsfMRI detect task-independent connectivity patterns which may not apply, to the same degree, to different cognitive domains or functions. In other words, language tasks may engage certain thalamocortical connections more than others. Finally, although effective connectivity can be used in a task-dependent manner to reveal causal associations among brain regions, it is usually carried out with small sample sizes and for specific tasks, limiting generalizability of findings across populations and tasks within a functional domain.
A recent development in neuroimaging research which can potentially overcome the aforementioned limitations of structural, resting-state and effective functional connectivity techniques is meta-analytic connectivity modeling (MACM). Using a database of functional neuroimaging experiments (BrainMap), MACM can compute functional convergence of studies that report activation in a predetermined region of interest (ROI) in a task-independent manner; i.e., across all behavioral domains available in the database, (Erickson et al. 2017; Robinson et al. 2010) or task-dependent manner; i.e., for a specific behavioral domain such as language (Ardila et al. 2016; Bernal et al. 2015; Viñas-Guasch and Wu 2017). Because a broad database of experiments with various tasks and experimental designs are pooled, MACM can yield highly generalizable findings (Samartsidis et al. 2020).
MACM has been used to study language-related coactivation patterns of cortical and subcortical sites potentially relevant for language, including the left IFG (Bernal et al. 2015; Bulut 2022), right IFG (Bulut 2022), Wernicke’s area (Ardila et al. 2016) and putamen (Viñas-Guasch and Wu 2017). Recent MACM investigations of IFG revealed a language network spanning largely left-lateralized frontal, temporal and parietal regions as well as several subcortical structures (Bernal et al. 2015; Bulut 2022). These subcortical structures were the thalamus, putamen and the cerebellum, which coactivated with the left pars opercularis (BA44) (Bernal et al. 2015; Bulut 2022). When the coactivation patterns of the IFG subdivisions (pars opercularis, pars triangularis, pars orbitalis) were compared in the left hemisphere, only the left pars opercularis was found to significantly coactivate with the left basal ganglia (putamen), left thalamus (medial dorsal nucleus) and the right cerebellum (culmen) (Bulut 2022). Likewise, in the right IFG, significant coactivation with the left thalamus (anterior nucleus) was found only for the right pars opercularis (Bulut 2022). Despite these studies examining language-related coactivation patterns of mainly cortical regions, no MACM of the thalamus was conducted before and no large-scale language-related functional connectivity of the thalamus was carried out. Indeed, the thalamus, which has been shown to influence whole-brain activity, has not been sufficiently explored in the human neuroimaging literature, making it hard to attribute specific functions to it (Shine et al. 2023).
Against this background, the present study utilizes MACM to investigate language-related coactivation patterns of the left and right thalami using a large-scale dataset. To accomplish this, we searched the BrainMap functional database to identify neuroimaging experiments reporting language-related activations in the left and right thalami, separately. The identified activation foci were then subjected to activation likelihood estimation (ALE) analyses to determine which areas significantly coactivated with the left and right thalami. In addition to MACM, a functional decoding analysis was performed for the same areas using the same database in order to characterize linguistic functions of the areas, which could then help interpret the functional profiles of the networks identified. Identification of language-related coactivation patterns of the thalamus can provide insights into the cortico-subcortical organization of language and inform neurocognitive models of language processing, which are largely cortico-centric, thereby helping alleviate cortical bias in cognition in general and language in particular. In addition, delineating the thalamocortical network of language processing in health may provide a baseline against which to compare the language network in thalamic aphasia.
Materials and methods
Database search
The database searches aimed to identify language-related functional imaging (fMRI or PET) experiments which report whole-brain activations within the left or right thalamus. The searches were carried out within the BrainMap functional database on January 4, 2023 (and repeated on April 18, 2023 with the same number of hits) using Sleuth Version 3.0.4 (Fox et al. 2005; Fox and Lancaster 2002; Laird et al. 2005). At the time when the searches were repeated, the functional database comprised 4173 papers, 21,336 experiments, and 106,520 subjects. The following search criteria were used: “experimental context: normal mapping”, “experimental activation: activations only”, “behavioral domain: cognition-language”. In addition to these experimental criteria, the left and right thalami were separately included among the search terms as regions of interest (ROIs). These ROIs were specified using a combination of Talairach Daemon (TD) labels (Lancaster et al. 1997, 2000) embedded in Sleuth. That is, the left thalamus was specified by the combination of “TD Label - Hemisphere - Left Cerebrum” AND “TD Label - Gyrus - Thalamus”, and the right thalamus was specified by the combination of “TD Label - Hemisphere - Right Cerebrum” AND “TD Label - Gyrus - Thalamus”. Thus, two searches were performed, one for the left thalamus and another one for the right thalamus. The search term “normal mapping” ensured inclusion of only the experiments conducted with healthy subjects.
As the purpose of the searches was to identify language-related coactivations of the ROIs, the searches were restricted to the “cognition-language” behavioral domain only, which comprises all linguistic levels (phonology, orthography, semantics, syntax, speech) available on BrainMap. However, we did not include “action-execution-speech” among the search terms to exclude purely action-related processes of articulation given the involvement of the thalamus in motor planning, execution, and action selection (Fisher and Reynolds 2014; Kumar et al. 2023). Note that the term “action-execution-speech” was not included as an exclusionary criterion, either, meaning that experiments which involved both action-execution-speech and a language subdomain were also identified by the searches. Also note that an experiment may relate to more than one behavioral domain/subdomain (e.g., both cognition-language and perception-audition).
The database searches yielded 129 papers reporting 186 experiments from a total of 1961 participants for the left thalamus, and 88 papers reporting 118 experiments from a total of 1317 participants for the right thalamus. The distribution of the identified experiments across BrainMap language subcategories are summarized in Table 1 (for details on the BrainMap taxonomy, please refer to Fox et al. 2005; Lancaster et al. 2012). Note that for the left and right thalami, there were 4 and 5 experiments, respectively, which were categorized as language but were not assigned to a specific language subcategory. For both ROIs, the language subcategory with the highest number of hits was semantics followed by speech. It should be cautioned, however, that the sheer number of identified experiments per language subcategory or paradigm class (explained below) does not indicate functional specialization within the ROIs for that subcategory or paradigm, as these numbers may have been driven simply by larger representation in the database (e.g., more semantic hits for the ROIs may be due to higher number of experiments in the database that pertain to semantics compared to other subcategories). Therefore, the total number of experiments in the database as well as percentages of ROI hits for each subcategory or paradigm are also provided in the tables below. Also, inferential statistics based on observed and expected fractions are carried out below (Functional decoding) to examine functional specialization for language subcategories. In terms of percentages of hits per subcategory, speech had the highest representation for both ROIs among the subcategories.
Table 1.
Domain | Category (Subcategory) | Database total | L thalamus hits | R thalamus hits | ||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
# | % | # | % | |||
Cognition | Language (Orthography) | 355 | 18 | 5.1 | 14 | 3.9 |
Cognition | Language (Phonology) | 462 | 21 | 4.5 | 12 | 2.6 |
Cognition | Language (Semantics) | 1519 | 96 | 6.3 | 56 | 3.7 |
Cognition | Language (Speech) | 1229 | 92 | 7.5 | 53 | 4.3 |
Cognition | Language (Syntax) | 251 | 13 | 5.2 | 6 | 2.4 |
Note: In this and the following table, the # columns indicate the raw number of experiments identified for the relevant ROI for each category, while the % columns indicate the percentage of all the experiments in the database for each category which were identified for the relevant ROI.
The distribution of the identified experiments across BrainMap paradigm classes related to language are summarized in Table 2 for each ROI. In parallel with the language subcategory distributions, the paradigm with the highest number of hits for both ROIs was semantic monitor/discrimination, which is defined on the BrainMap website as “Discriminate between the meanings of individual lexical items or to indicate if target word is semantically related to the probe word.” (http://www.brainmap.org/taxonomy/paradigms/). The paradigm with the second highest number of hits for both ROIs was overt reading. In terms of percentages of hits per paradigm, if we put aside the paradigms with minimal contribution (1–3 hits) to the samples, figurative language had the highest percentage of hits followed by naming (covert) and word generation (covert) for the left thalamus, and, for the right thalamus, the highest was reading (overt) followed by semantic monitor/discrimination and reading (covert).
Table 2.
Paradigm | Database total | L thalamus hits | R thalamus hits | ||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
# | % | # | % | ||
Affective words | 284 | 3 | 1.1 | 1 | 0.4 |
Figurative language | 39 | 6 | 15.4 | 1 | 2.6 |
Imagined objects/scenes | 268 | 1 | 0.4 | 1 | 0.4 |
Lexical decision | 106 | 3 | 2.8 | 3 | 2.8 |
Naming (covert) | 109 | 7 | 6.4 | 2 | 1.8 |
Naming (overt) | 334 | 13 | 3.9 | 8 | 2.4 |
Orthographic discrimination | 303 | 8 | 2.6 | 8 | 2.6 |
Passive listening | 598 | 4 | 0.7 | 3 | 0.5 |
Phonological discrimination | 426 | 14 | 3.3 | 10 | 2.3 |
Reading (covert) | 374 | 13 | 3.5 | 11 | 2.9 |
Reading (overt) | 347 | 20 | 5.8 | 21 | 6.1 |
Recitation/repetition (covert) | 60 | 2 | 3.3 | 2 | 3.3 |
Recitation/repetition (overt) | 139 | 8 | 5.8 | 2 | 1.4 |
Semantic monitor/discrimination | 974 | 53 | 5.4 | 31 | 3.2 |
Syntactic discrimination | 87 | 5 | 5.7 | 3 | 3.4 |
Visual object identification | 673 | 2 | 0.3 | 1 | 0.1 |
Word generation (covert) | 304 | 19 | 6.3 | 8 | 2.6 |
Word generation (overt) | 346 | 19 | 5.5 | 8 | 2.3 |
Word stem completion (covert) | 10 | 1 | 10.0 | 1 | 10.0 |
Word stem completion (overt) | 35 | 1 | 2.9 | 1 | 2.9 |
Writing | 27 | 2 | 7.4 | 1 | 3.7 |
Some of the tasks used by the identified experiments involved a button press or a hand movement including a lexical decision task, orthographic, semantic, and syntactic discrimination tasks, and writing. In order to minimize motoric confounds due to hand movement, which typically involves the right hand, we identified experiments which did not control for such movement in the baseline task for both the left and right thalami and performed robustness analyses without these confounded experiments in addition to the main analyses. In other words, we excluded experiments where the main task involves hand movement such as a button press and the control task involves a low-level baseline without a corresponding hand movement such as fixation or rest. We thus identified 23 experiments (16 papers) for the left thalamus, and 20 experiments (11 papers) for the right thalamus as confounding hand movement. Hence, the robustness analysis of the left thalamus included 163 experiments (113 papers), and that of the right thalamus included 98 experiments (77 papers) which had appropriate baselines to control for hand movement.
The foci identified in each search were grouped using the most conservative approach (Turkeltaub et al. 2012); i.e., foci reported in multiple experiments from a single study were combined and entered into the meta-analyses as a single experiment to avoid overinfluence of a single experiment on the results. Coordinates reported in Talairach space were automatically converted into MNI space using the icbm2tal transform (Laird et al. 2010; Lancaster et al. 2007).
MACM analyses
The activation coordinates identified for each ROI were entered in the ALE analyses, which were run on GingerALE 3.0.2 (Eickhoff et al. 2009, 2012) to compute convergence of coactivations for each ROI. These analyses were run separately with all the experiments identified in the database (main analyses) and with the experiments that had appropriate baselines to control for hand movement (robustness analyses) as explained in the preceding section. The ALE analyses were performed in accordance with the standard procedures reported in previous research (Cieslik et al. 2015; Müller et al. 2017; Wojtasik et al. 2020). Specifically, using a full-width half-maximum based on the sample size in each experiment, 3D Gaussian probability distributions centered at each foci group were generated (Eickhoff et al. 2009). Then, the union of modeled activation maps was obtained to calculate voxel-wise ALE scores. Next, the union of these activation probabilities were compared against the null hypothesis of random spatial association between the experiments. Finally, the p-value distributions derived from these probabilities were thresholded at a voxel-level uncorrected cluster-forming threshold of p < 0.001 and a cluster-level corrected threshold of p < 0.05 (family-wise error-corrected for multiple comparisons), with 10,000 thresholding permutations.
Following the coactivation analyses separately for each ROI, the conjunction/intersection of coactivation (L thalamus ∩ R thalamus) was visualized using the “Overlay Logicals” utility of the Mango software (Lancaster et al. 2010). The Talairach Daemon embedded in GingerALE was used to generate anatomical labels as the nearest gray matter within 5 mm for the activation peaks (Lancaster et al. 1997, 2000). The Mango software (Lancaster et al. 2010) was used to visualize the ALE results, which were overlaid on the MNI template Colin27_T1_seg_MNI.nii (Kochunov et al. 2002) downloaded from the GingerALE website. The Sleuth files (workspace files including metadata of the experiments identified in each search, and text files containing the foci obtained from the identified experiments and entered in the meta-analyses) as well as the GingerALE output files along with coactivation peaks and cluster analyses for each ROI are available at the online data repository.
Functional decoding
Functional decoding of the ROIs was carried out using the Mango Behavioral Analysis Plugin (Lancaster et al. 2012), which was used in previous research to examine functional specialization in various brain regions (Erickson et al. 2017; Sundermann and Pfleiderer 2012). The plugin utilizes the metadata of articles in the BrainMap database to calculate the observed fraction of activation coordinates for a given behavioral subdomain (e.g., cognition.attention, or cognition.language.phonology) that fall within a prespecified ROI and compares it to the fraction that would be expected if the distribution was random. If the difference between the observed and the expected fraction is high, then the ROI is associated with this behavior. In the original description of the Mango Plugin, the statistical threshold for significance is determined as a Z-score ≥ 3.0, which corresponds to a one-tailed (testing only positive association) p-value of 0.05, Bonferroni corrected for 51 behavioral subdomains in BrainMap (Lancaster et al. 2012). However, since the purpose of the functional decoding procedure carried out in the present study was to characterize linguistic functions of each ROI, rather than testing their domain-specificity (e.g., language vs. working memory), the analysis was restricted to the five language subdomains available on BrainMap (orthography, phonology, semantics, speech and syntax). Hence, in the present study, the statistical threshold was set as Z > 2.33 (corresponding to a one-tailed p-value of 0.05, Bonferroni corrected for the five language subdomains examined). The functional decoding analysis was performed on the same date as the database search, again using the left and right thalami as ROIs. For functional decoding, the ROIs were defined based on the Automated Anatomical Labeling (AAL) atlas (Tzourio-Mazoyer et al. 2002) and using MRIcron (https://www.nitrc.org/projects/mricron) (Rorden and Brett 2000), as illustrated in Fig. 2 (the ROIs can be downloaded from the online data repository). The sizes of the left and right thalamic ROIs were 7699mm3 and 7762mm3, respectively. The ROIs were visually inspected using the Talairach Daemon in Mango (Lancaster et al. 1997, 2000) and using different brain templates in MRIcron, including AAL3.1 (Rolls et al. 2020), AICHA (Joliot et al. 2015), and the Harvard-Oxford Subcortical Atlas (Desikan et al. 2006; Frazier et al. 2005) to ensure that the intended brain regions were captured and that the parcellation was consistent across other subcortical atlases, which was the case.
Results
MACM main results
Chi-square tests were carried out to examine whether the number of papers and experiments identified for the left and right thalami differed significantly. It was found that significantly more papers (X2 > 7.747, p = 0.005) and experiments (X2 > 15.211, p < 0.001) were identified for the left than right thalamus. This finding shows that more studies in the BrainMap database reported activations in the left than right thalamus for language tasks.
The coactivation and intersection results for each ROI are visualized in Fig. 3 and the coactivation results are summarized in Table 3. As can be seen from comparison of the coactivation maps (Fig. 3A and B), the left thalamus showed more extensive coactivation (total cluster size = 122,232mm3) than the right thalamus (total cluster size = 84,456mm3). Despite this difference, the ROIs coactivated with a generally overlapping network of regions (Fig. 3C). Both ROIs significantly coactivated with mainly left-lateralized cortical clusters spanning the frontal, temporal and, to a limited extent, parietal lobes, as well as largely bilateral subcortical and right cerebellar clusters.
Table 3.
Cluster | Anatomical Label (Nearest Gray Matter within 5 mm) |
BA | MNI Coordinates | ALE | Z | Cluster Size (mm3) | ||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
x | y | z | ||||||
L thalamus | ||||||||
1 | L Insula | 13 | -34 | 22 | 0 | 0.113 | 10.04 | 39,160 |
L IFG | 9 | -44 | 10 | 28 | 0.092 | 8.46 | ||
L IFG | 9 | -50 | 10 | 20 | 0.086 | 8.02 | ||
L IFG | 44 | -52 | 16 | 12 | 0.081 | 7.60 | ||
L MFG | 46 | -48 | 26 | 16 | 0.080 | 7.55 | ||
L Precentral gyrus | 6 | -46 | -2 | 44 | 0.073 | 6.96 | ||
L Precentral gyrus | 6 | -50 | -10 | 34 | 0.051 | 5.01 | ||
L Precentral gyrus | 6 | -54 | -6 | 24 | 0.045 | 4.37 | ||
L Postcentral gyrus | 43 | -58 | -16 | 22 | 0.035 | 3.33 | ||
2 | L Medial dorsal nucleus (Thalamus) | -8 | -16 | 6 | 0.183 | 14.83 | 29,008 | |
R Medial dorsal nucleus (Thalamus) | 10 | -18 | 6 | 0.073 | 6.96 | |||
R Ventral lateral nucleus (Thalamus) | 14 | -8 | 10 | 0.064 | 6.15 | |||
R Thalamus | 22 | -28 | 0 | 0.048 | 4.69 | |||
R Lateral globus pallidus | 18 | 0 | 2 | 0.040 | 3.86 | |||
3 | L Fusiform gyrus | 37 | -42 | -60 | -18 | 0.077 | 7.29 | 14,728 |
L MTG | 22 | -54 | -42 | 6 | 0.065 | 6.25 | ||
L STG | 41 | -58 | -16 | 6 | 0.053 | 5.14 | ||
L IOG | 19 | -38 | -80 | -6 | 0.043 | 4.17 | ||
L MTG | 20 | -54 | -46 | -12 | 0.037 | 3.52 | ||
4 | L FGmed | 32 | -2 | 14 | 46 | 0.116 | 10.25 | 14,576 |
5 | R Insula | 13 | 36 | 26 | -8 | 0.087 | 8.09 | 9488 |
R Claustrum | 36 | 20 | -2 | 0.077 | 7.27 | |||
R IFG | 50 | 18 | -4 | 0.055 | 5.33 | |||
6 | L SPL | 7 | -26 | -62 | 48 | 0.062 | 5.96 | 3896 |
7 | R STG | 22 | 54 | -32 | 4 | 0.068 | 6.48 | 3624 |
8 | R Culmen (Cerebellum, anterior lobe) | 36 | -66 | -26 | 0.063 | 6.05 | 2464 | |
R Culmen (Cerebellum, anterior lobe) | 34 | -58 | -26 | 0.050 | 4.85 | |||
R Culmen (Cerebellum, anterior lobe) | 26 | -54 | -22 | 0.039 | 3.74 | |||
9 | R STG | 22 | 58 | -10 | -2 | 0.053 | 5.18 | 2000 |
R Postcentral gyrus | 43 | 62 | -6 | 12 | 0.039 | 3.74 | ||
10 | R IFG | 9 | 48 | 10 | 28 | 0.062 | 6.01 | 1936 |
11 | R Precentral gyrus | 4 | 54 | -8 | 38 | 0.050 | 4.87 | 1352 |
R thalamus | ||||||||
1 | L Thalamus | -12 | -18 | 6 | 0.105 | 10.32 | 35,008 | |
R Medial dorsal nucleus (Thalamus) | 12 | -18 | 4 | 0.105 | 10.31 | |||
R Claustrum | 34 | 22 | -4 | 0.073 | 7.69 | |||
R Thalamus | 22 | -28 | 0 | 0.062 | 6.70 | |||
R Putamen | 22 | 4 | 2 | 0.047 | 5.24 | |||
L Thalamus | -6 | -26 | -4 | 0.045 | 5.08 | |||
R Insula | 13 | 40 | 20 | 8 | 0.044 | 4.93 | ||
L Thalamus | -20 | -30 | -2 | 0.040 | 4.54 | |||
R Insula | 13 | 50 | 14 | -4 | 0.035 | 3.96 | ||
R Parahippocampal gyrus | 30 | 18 | -38 | 8 | 0.029 | 3.32 | ||
2 | L Claustrum | -32 | 20 | -2 | 0.104 | 10.27 | 25,736 | |
L IFG | 9 | -42 | 10 | 26 | 0.069 | 7.36 | ||
L IFG | 47 | -46 | 20 | -2 | 0.059 | 6.38 | ||
L MFG | 46 | -44 | 26 | 22 | 0.053 | 5.83 | ||
L Precentral gyrus | 4 | -46 | -4 | 46 | 0.051 | 5.66 | ||
L Precentral gyrus | 6 | -50 | 2 | 36 | 0.050 | 5.60 | ||
L IFG | 9 | -52 | 12 | 28 | 0.048 | 5.39 | ||
L STG | 41 | -60 | -20 | 6 | 0.045 | 5.08 | ||
L Insula | 13 | -50 | 10 | -2 | 0.044 | 4.94 | ||
L MTG | -58 | -32 | 2 | 0.039 | 4.38 | |||
L STG | 22 | -56 | -8 | -2 | 0.033 | 3.77 | ||
L IFG | 44 | -54 | 18 | 12 | 0.033 | 3.76 | ||
L Precentral gyrus | 6 | -56 | -4 | 24 | 0.032 | 3.65 | ||
L TTG | 42 | -60 | -10 | 14 | 0.032 | 3.60 | ||
3 | L FGmed | 6 | -2 | 10 | 50 | 0.079 | 8.20 | 10,376 |
L Cingulate gyrus | 32 | 0 | 20 | 38 | 0.070 | 7.44 | ||
4 | L Fusiform gyrus | 37 | -42 | -60 | -18 | 0.064 | 6.91 | 4800 |
5 | R STG | 22 | 58 | -28 | 4 | 0.052 | 5.77 | 3608 |
R STG | 22 | 60 | -10 | 2 | 0.033 | 3.72 | ||
6 | L SPL | 7 | -26 | -62 | 48 | 0.046 | 5.18 | 2792 |
L SPL | 7 | -30 | -52 | 48 | 0.043 | 4.81 | ||
7 | R Precentral gyrus | 4 | 54 | -8 | 38 | 0.043 | 4.85 | 1120 |
R Precentral gyrus | 6 | 48 | 2 | 42 | 0.031 | 3.50 | ||
8 | R Declive (Cerebellum, posterior lobe) | 12 | -66 | -14 | 0.039 | 4.41 | 1016 |
Note: MNI Coordinates correspond to cluster peaks, and anatomical labels indicate gray matter nearest to the cluster peaks. Please refer to the online data repository for cluster analyses with full reports of structures included in each cluster. L: Left, R: Right, FGmed: Medial frontal gyrus, IFG: Inferior frontal gyrus, IOG: Inferior Occipital Gyrus, MFG: Middle frontal gyrus, MTG: Middle temporal gyrus, SPL: Superior parietal lobule, STG: Superior temporal gyrus, TTG: Transverse temporal gyrus
For both ROIs, the left frontal clusters included the insula, inferior frontal gyrus peaking at BA44 and 9, but also spanning parts of BA45 and 47, middle frontal gyrus, precentral gyrus and medial frontal regions. The right frontal coactivations were limited compared to the left-hemispheric ones, however, involving the right insula and precentral gyrus for both ROIs, and the right inferior frontal gyrus (peaking at BA9, but also spanning parts of BA44, 45 and 47) for the left thalamus.
As for the temporal coactivations, both ROIs coactivated with parts of the primary auditory cortex including BA41 and 42. The ROIs also coactivated with the association cortex of the middle and superior temporal lobe bilaterally, including BA22, and with the left fusiform gyrus. As can be seen in the conjunction analysis (Fig. 3C), particularly the left thalamus coactivated with the posterior superior/middle temporal cortex bilaterally with greater involvement of the left hemispheric regions overlapping with Wernicke’s area. Within the parietal lobe, both ROIs coactivated mainly with the left superior parietal lobule including the precuneus, while this cluster also spanned parts of the left inferior parietal lobule for the left thalamic ROI.
Subcortically, both ROIs coactivated with themselves as well as the homotopic thalamus in the contralateral hemisphere, including the medial dorsal nucleus and the ventral dorsal nucleus. Other subcortical peaks included the claustrum and the right basal ganglia (the left thalamus coactivating with the right lateral globus pallidus, and the right thalamus coactivating with the right putamen). In addition, the subcortical clusters spanned parts of the bilateral striatum (putamen and caudate body) and bilateral pallidum (lateral globus pallidus) of the basal ganglia for both the left and right thalami. Finally, both ROIs exhibited significant coactivations with the right cerebellum. The left thalamus coactivated mostly with the anterior lobe (culmen) (56.5% of the cerebellar cluster), but also with the posterior lobe (declive, tuber, uvula, pyramis) (43.5%) of the right cerebellum, while the right thalamus coactivated mostly with the posterior lobe (declive) (97.6%), but, to a limited extent, also with the anterior lobe (culmen) (2.4%) of the right cerebellum.
MACM robustness results
This section summarizes the results of the analyses conducted with the experiments that had appropriate baselines to control for hand movement. The robustness coactivation and intersection results for each ROI are visualized in Figure S1 and the coactivation results are summarized in Table S1 in the Supplementary Information. The pattern of coactivation results in the robustness analyses was largely similar to the pattern of results in the main analyses. In particular, both thalami coactivated mainly with bilateral frontotemporal regions and bilateral subcortical regions (i.e., basal ganglia and thalamus). The primary difference between the main results and the robustness results involved the coactivations of the right thalamus, which were reduced in the robustness analyses (a total cluster size reduction of 45.0% compared to the main results) particularly in the left hemisphere and which did not extend to the cerebellum contrary to the main results. This suggests that at least part of the left cortical and subcortical, as well as all right cerebellar, coactivations of the right thalamus in the main results may have been driven by hand movement during task performance. The coactivations of the left thalamus, however, were not reduced to such an extent in the robustness analyses (a total cluster size reduction of 8.9% compared to the main results) and, despite being bilateral, still exhibited left dominance in the cortex and subcortex, and spanned the right cerebellum, as in the main results. Taken together, the robustness results largely support the main results that the language-relevant coactivation network of the thalamus involves bilateral frontotemporal and bilateral subcortical regions for both the left and right thalami, and also the right cerebellum for the left thalamus, even after controlling for potential confounds due to hand movement.
Functional decoding results
The functional decoding results for the left and right thalami are illustrated in Fig. 4. The results showed that the left thalamus was significantly associated with speech (Z = 4.91), semantics (Z = 4.02) and syntax (Z = 2.70). The right thalamus, on the other hand, was not significantly associated with any language subdomains (Z < 0.62).
Discussion
The meta-analytic connectivity modeling of the left and right thalami identified bilateral frontotemporal and bilateral subcortical coactivations for both regions of interest, and right cerebellar coactivations for the left thalamus, during language tasks. The distribution of experiments included in the analyses across language subcategories and paradigm classes and the functional decoding analysis showed that thalamic activation was associated mostly with language production/speech, semantics and syntax. Taken together with previous empirical studies and theoretical frameworks, the findings suggest that cortico-subcortical-cerebellar-cortical loops modulate and fine-tune information transfer within the bilateral frontotemporal cortices during language processing.
Cortically, the language-related coactivation patterns of both the left and right thalami spanned frontotemporal regions, distributed bilaterally but stronger in the left hemisphere for the left thalamus, which were previously associated with language functions. Although both ROIs showed left parietal coactivations mainly within the superior parietal lobule, the parietal coactivations of only the left thalamic ROI survived the robustness analyses. These findings are consistent with previous DWT and rsfMRI investigations of the thalamus which revealed structural and functional connectivity between the thalamus and mainly frontotemporal cortical sites (Bohsali et al. 2015; Cunningham et al. 2017; Ford et al. 2013; Grodd et al. 2020; Hale et al. 2015; Jeon et al. 2014; Kumar et al. 2023; O’Muircheartaigh et al. 2015). In the present study, the frontal coactivations spanned bilaterally the precentral gyrus, insula and the inferior frontal gyrus for both ROIs. The IFG coactivations spanned subdivisions of the Broca’s complex including BA44, 45 and 47 bilaterally but greater in the left hemisphere and for the left thalamus. In parallel with these findings, previous research on structural connectivity of the thalamus with language-related regions linked the thalamus (ventral anterior nucleus, medial dorsal nucleus, pulvinar) with Broca’s area (BA44, 45) as well as other prefrontal regions (Barbas et al. 2013; Bohsali et al. 2015; Ford et al. 2013; Jeon et al. 2014). These connections constitute a cortico-thalamo-cortical loop, which is argued to play a role in lexical-semantic processing (Bohsali et al. 2015; Crosson 2021). In this framework, the thalamus mediates transfer of semantic and phonological information between BA45 and 44 during lexical-semantic processing (Bohsali et al. 2015; Crosson 2021). The present findings suggest that this loop may extend to BA47 as well, given also previous research implicating this area with semantic processes (Binder et al. 2009; Hagoort and Indefrey 2014; Rodd et al. 2015) and with interface functions between semantic and limbic networks (Belyk et al. 2017).
In the temporal lobe, the coactivation networks of both ROIs included the primary auditory cortex as well as the association cortex of the middle and superior temporal lobe including BA22 bilaterally. In particular, coactivations were seen in the left posterior superior and middle temporal gyri, overlapping with regions traditionally considered as Wernicke’s area, especially for the left thalamus. These posterior superior/middle temporal cortices were previously associated with various linguistic functions, primarily including phonological processing (particularly the superior temporal gyrus) (Turkeltaub and Branch Coslett 2010; Vigneau et al. 2006), syntactic processing (Friederici 2012; Hagoort and Indefrey 2014; Heard and Lee 2020; Rodd et al. 2015; Vigneau et al. 2006; Walenski et al. 2019) and semantic processing (Friederici 2012; Hagoort and Indefrey 2014; Rodd et al. 2015; Vigneau et al. 2006). Predominance of the semantic and speech subcategories and semantic and production tasks in the distribution of the pooled experiments (see Tables 1 and 2) and the significant involvement of the left thalamus with speech, semantics and syntax in the functional decoding analysis suggest that the observed coactivation patterns underlie these language functions in particular.
Further, the frontotemporal coactivation patterns of the thalamus found here are compatible with the Memory, Unification and Control (MUC) Model, which associates the left temporal lobe with memory processes and the left IFG with unification processes underlying language (Hagoort 2005, 2013, 2016). According to this view, the memory areas within the left temporal lobe embody the mental lexicon by storing lexical information, while the unification areas within the left IFG are responsible for combinatorial processing of linguistic information. Executive control processes such as attention, selection, turn-taking in conversation and code-switching engaged in language processing are subserved by the control network spanning the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex; i.e., the middle frontal gyrus (BA46, 9, 8, 6), the anterior cingulate cortex and parts of the parietal cortex (Hagoort 2005, 2013, 2016; Xiang et al. 2010). The MUC Model emphasizes dynamic interaction between the memory, unification and control processes. Importantly, gradients within the frontotemporal regions are proposed that map phonological processes dorsally, semantic processes ventrally and syntactic processes in between these ventral and dorsal sites (Hagoort 2013, 2016; Xiang et al. 2010). Specifically, the model associates left BA44/6 and the left posterior superior temporal gyrus/sulcus with phonological processing, left BA45/44 and the left posterior middle temporal gyrus with syntactic processing, and left BA47/45 and the left inferior temporal cortices with semantic processing. As these cortical sites largely overlap with the frontotemporal coactivations of the thalamus observed here, it is possible that indirect cortico-thalamo-cortical loops regulate transfer of information via the direct cortico-cortical circuits within the frontotemporal system.
In particular, cortico-thalamo-cortical, or transthalamic, loops with driver inputs from cortical layer 5 to higher-order thalamus (e.g., the pulvinar and the ventral anterior nucleus) have been suggested to regulate functional connectivity within and across cortical regions and, thus, to be critical for communication amongst cerebral networks underlying various functions including language (Antunes and Malmierca 2021; Crosson 2021; Shepherd and Yamawaki 2021; Sherman 2016; Usrey and Sherman 2019). For instance, in Crosson’s recurrent circuit model, cortico-thalamo-cortical loops support the direct cortico-cortical circuits during word retrieval and other language functions (Crosson 2021). According to the model, during word retrieval, the cortico-thalamo-cortical loop helps maintain stable representations of semantic information at the semantic-lexical interface, which are then compared with emerging lexical alternatives that represent the semantic concept. Thus, the cortico-thalamo-cortical loop provides an error signal for cortico-cortical mechanisms regarding semantic-lexical pairings until a good lexical match has been found for the semantic concept (Crosson 2021). When this error signal is compromised through lesion of the relevant left thalamic nucleus, this results in visual-semantic errors during word retrieval, which is typical of thalamic aphasia (Crosson 2021). Hence, the model proposes a division of labor between direct cortico-cortical and indirect cortico-thalamo-cortical loops, such that the former allows for change necessary to progress to higher-order stages of information processing, while the latter tends to maintain current activity patterns necessary to generate error feedback to the lower-order cortex (Crosson 2021; Kawaguchi 2017). It is further claimed that oscillatory synchrony between the thalamic relays and the cortex in the gamma band is a neural signature for information processing in these two circuits (Crosson 2021). The functional role of these cortico-thalamo-cortical loops during language processing may extend beyond lexical-semantic processes to include unification and control operations at multiple language levels (e.g., syntax, morphology, phonology) which may be shared with other cognitive domains/networks (e.g., theory of mind, attention) through recruitment of bilateral cortical, bilateral subcortical and right cerebellar regions not classically associated with language functions (Hagoort 2016, 2019).
The coactivation patterns of the left and right thalami did not only entail cortical regions, but also subcortical structures including the bilateral basal ganglia and, for the right thalamic ROI, the right cerebellum. In particular, the coactivations of both ROIs spanned parts of the bilateral striatum (putamen and caudate body) and bilateral pallidum (lateral globus pallidus) of the basal ganglia. Previous functional imaging, animal model and clinical studies linked the basal ganglia (usually the putamen and the caudate nucleus) with diverse emotional, motor and cognitive processes (Albin et al. 1989; Chakravarthy et al. 2010; Groenewegen 2003; Lanciego et al. 2012; Packard and Knowlton 2002). Along these lines, the basal ganglia-thalamo-cortical circuitry has been implicated in a range of sensorimotor, cognitive, emotional and motivational brain functions (Alexander and Crutcher 1990; Copland and Angwin 2019; Groenewegen 2003; Murphy et al. 2022). In the domain of language, the left putamen was found to coactivate with areas closely linked to language primarily including frontotemporal regions and these coactivations were associated mainly with semantic processes (Viñas-Guasch and Wu 2017). The left caudate, too, was involved in language processing (Crinion et al. 2006; Tan et al. 2011; Zou et al. 2012), and lesions in the left caudate nucleus correlated with speech and language impairments following stroke, associating this region with control processes involving speech and language (Grönholm et al. 2016). Previous structural and functional connectivity studies, too, showed that prefrontal regions including Broca’s area which were connected to the thalamus were also linked to the basal ganglia structures including the putamen (Bulut 2022; Ford et al. 2013) and the caudate body/head (Jeon et al. 2014). This cortico-striatal-thalamo-cortical loop was also associated with lexical-semantic processing, with the cortico-striatal part of the circuitry increasing the activation of contextually appropriate semantic/phonemic representations while inhibiting competitors (Ford et al. 2013).
The subcortical coactivations of both the left and right thalami identified here included parts of the right cerebellum in the main results, while the right cerebellar coactivations of only the left thalamus survived the robustness analyses. In particular, the left thalamus coactivated mostly with the anterior lobe (culmen) of the right cerebellum. Traditionally associated with coordination of motor control, the cerebellum, especially its right hemisphere, has more recently been linked to mediation of cognitive functions including language (Murdoch 2010). The right cerebellum has been involved in speech and language processing in neuroimaging research (Booth et al. 2007; Wildgruber et al. 2001). In addition, morphological differences in the cerebellum were observed in children with primary language impairments (specific language impairment) and secondary language impairments (comorbid with autism spectrum disorder) compared to their typically developing peers (Hodge et al. 2010). In another study, people with lesions in the left pars opercularis showed greater activation in the right cerebellum during speech production compared to neurologically intact controls and patient controls with lesions somewhere else in the left hemisphere, which was interpreted as recruitment of a compensatory cognitive control mechanism during speech production (Lorca-Puls et al. 2021). In a further fMRI study comparing language production and comprehension, both cortical (bilateral frontal and temporal sites) and subcortical-cerebellar (left thalamus, bilateral caudate, left putamen and bilateral cerebellum) structures were associated with increasing constituent size of the produced or comprehended items (Giglio et al. 2022). Importantly, these subcortical and cerebellar regions showed greater activation for production than comprehension, which was interpreted as reflecting articulatory requirements in production (Giglio et al. 2022). Furthermore, previous resting-state fMRI and meta-analytic connectivity modeling studies showed that Broca’s and Wernicke’s areas were functionally connected to the basal ganglia, the thalamus and the right cerebellum, hence suggesting a language-related function for these subcortical structures (Bulut 2022; Tomasi and Volkow 2012). In particular, investigation of language-related meta-analytic connectivity of the left and right pars opercularis, triangularis and orbitalis using the same database as the present study revealed subcortical (left thalamus, left putamen) and right cerebellar coactivations only for the left pars opercularis (BA44), suggesting that it may be a major hub in the language network with connections to diverse cortical, subcortical and cerebellar structures (Bulut 2022). Relatedly, previous research emphasized the roles of the basal ganglia and the cerebellum that provide output to the thalamic nuclei including the ventral lateral, ventral anterior, intralaminar and mediodorsal nuclei, which are in turn connected to the premotor and prefrontal cortices, and suggested that this cerebellar-basal ganglia-thalamo-cortical system engages in motor and cognitive aspects of language (Barbas et al. 2013). In this framework, the thalamo-cortical circuitry is gated by the inhibitory thalamic reticular nucleus and modulated by dopamine, a specialization in primates (Barbas et al. 2013).
The functional decoding analysis carried out in the present study significantly associated the left, but not the right, thalamus with speech, semantics and syntax, respectively, but not with phonology or orthography. This functional characterization is in accord with the distribution of paradigm classes (Table 2), which shows greater contribution of experiments from semantic (semantic monitoring/discrimination, naming) and production (word generation, reading, naming) tasks to the coactivation analysis. The association of the left thalamus with semantics is consistent with the literature on thalamic aphasia which occurs usually after left thalamic lesion and involves naming deficits (problems of word finding and retrieval such as semantic paraphasia) with relatively preserved repetition (Crosson 2013; De Witte et al. 2011; Nadeau and Crosson 1997). Consistently, the thalamus (particularly ventral anterior nucleus and the pulvinar) and its connections with the prefrontal structures including Broca’s area have been associated with lexical-semantic processing, with the thalamus mediating transfer of semantic (pars triangularis/orbitalis) and phonological (pars opercularis) information between these anterior/posterior subdivisions of Broca’s area during lexical-semantic processing (Bohsali et al. 2015; Crosson 2021). Also, the association of the left thalamus with speech aligns well with studies showing more activation in the thalamus for language production than comprehension tasks (Giglio et al. 2022). Indeed, a review of literature on functional imaging of the thalamus during language tasks correlated thalamic activation mostly with language production tasks (e.g., word or sentence production) and naming, with stronger activation in the left than the right thalamus (Llano 2013). In this review, thalamic activity was found especially for perceptually challenging tasks, suggesting an attentional role to the thalamus (Johnson and Ojemann 2000; Llano 2013). Other speech-related functions attributed to the thalamus, in concert with the cerebellum and the basal ganglia, include the fine-tuning of timing and sequencing in speaking (Hagoort 2019). Finally, the significant association of the left thalamus with syntax is consistent with studies linking this structure to syntactic structure building and syntactic and semantic analysis (detection of syntactic and semantic violations) (David et al. 2011; Friederici et al. 2009; Liu et al. 2023; Murphy et al. 2022; Wahl et al. 2008). In particular, it has been suggested that the thalamus mediates between early syntactic processes contributing to phrase structure building and late integration processes through its connectivity with the anterior frontotemporal and parietotemporal cortices, respectively (Friederici et al. 2009; Wahl et al. 2008).
Limitations
Several limitations should be addressed. First, the experiments included in the MACM analyses were pooled from all language subcategories and tasks that reported activation in the ROIs, without specifically testing or contrasting involvement with particular language components (e.g., syntax, semantics, phonology, speech) or tasks/modalities (e.g., comprehension, production), which can influence the coactivation patterns. The reason for this is twofold. First, in this first large-scale language-related functional connectivity investigation of the thalamus, our purpose was to delineate the coactivation patterns of the left and right thalami for the language function in general. Second, restricting the analyses on individual language subcategories would have decreased the number of experiments included in each analysis, thereby substantially decreasing the statistical power for most of those subcategories particularly for the right thalamus, but also for the left. This would aggravate the subcortical bias that may already misrepresent the number of experiments linking the thalamus to language functions. Nevertheless, the present findings should be interpreted with caution as there may be differences in the engagement of the connectivity network of the thalamus for different language components or tasks/modalities. When more language-related studies involving the thalamus become available through expansion of the database used here or inclusion/creation of others, future MACM studies can examine coactivation patterns of the thalamus for different language components and/or tasks.
The second limitation concerns inclusion of the left and right thalami as a whole in the analyses, without subdivision into their constituent nuclei. Given that the thalamus consists of many small thalamic nuclei with potentially different functions, pooling all thalamic activations into single analyses may have influenced the results. Indeed, certain thalamic nuclei such as the pulvinar, ventral anterior, ventrolateral and centromedian nuclei have been highlighted as particularly relevant for higher-order functions such as language (Antunes and Malmierca 2021; Crosson 2021; Llano 2016; Shepherd and Yamawaki 2021; Sherman 2016; Usrey and Sherman 2019). Nevertheless, the clinical and functional imaging literature has not consistently implicated a specific thalamic nucleus with language (Llano 2016), with clinical studies reporting thalamic aphasia following lesions to different nuclei (Crosson 2013; De Witte et al. 2011; Nadeau and Crosson 1997), and functional imaging studies with language tasks reporting peaks of activation in almost all thalamic subregions (Llano 2013). Moreover, the relatively small size of thalamic nuclei in view of the limitations of commonly available MRI scanners (e.g., spatial resolution) and analytical techniques (e.g., smoothing windows) coupled with greater susceptibility of subcortical structures to movement artifacts make it difficult to correlate language/cognition with activation in individual thalamic nuclei (Janacsek et al. 2022; Llano 2016). Finally, we chose not to analyze coactivation patterns of individual thalamic nuclei also for the same reasons mentioned in the previous paragraph; namely, our purpose being delineation of the coactivation patterns of the left and right thalami for the language function in general and increasing statistical power by keeping the number of included experiments at maximum.
Finally, given that the thalamus and other subcortical and cerebellar structures that were found to coactivate with the thalamus in the present study have been associated with motor planning, action execution and selection (Brownsett et al. 2014; Fisher and Reynolds 2014; Giglio et al. 2022; Kumar et al. 2023), the likelihood that the observed coactivations may underlie such motor-related activity should be discussed. We have already addressed motoric activation due to hand movement (e.g., button press) by running robustness analyses which included only the experiments that had appropriate baselines to control for hand movement and which revealed a similar pattern of results to the main analyses, particularly for the left thalamus (see the MACM robustness results above and the Supplementary Information). However, articulation-related motoric activity may still have contributed to the present results, given that we did not exclude experiments that did not control articulation-related motoric activity (e.g., overt naming versus a low-level baseline such as fixation). Although we did not include “action-execution-speech” among the search terms to exclude purely action-related processes of articulation, this term was not included as an exclusionary criterion, either, meaning that experiments which involved both action-execution-speech and a language subdomain were also identified by the searches. As above, the main reason for this was to increase statistical power by pooling a high number of language-relevant experiments for both the left and right thalami. In particular, exclusion of such experiments; i.e., language-related experiments that did not control articulation-related motoric activity, would have resulted in removal of experiments on language production. These studies are much less represented in the literature than comprehension studies. Moreover, we consider speech articulation to be an integral part of the language production system. That is why we decided to keep these studies in our meta-analysis.
Conclusion
The present study demonstrated bilateral frontotemporal and bilateral subcortical (basal ganglia) coactivation patterns for both the left and right thalami, and also right cerebellar coactivation patterns for the left thalamus, during language processing. When considered together with previous empirical studies and theoretical frameworks, the meta-analytic connectivity and functional decoding findings suggest that cortico-subcortical-cerebellar-cortical loops modulate and fine-tune information transfer within the bilateral frontotemporal cortices during language processing, especially during production and semantic operations, but also other language (e.g., syntax, morphology, phonology) and cognitive operations (e.g., attention, cognitive control). Overall, the current findings show that the language-relevant network extends beyond the classical left perisylvian cortices to include bilateral cortical, bilateral subcortical (bilateral thalamus, bilateral basal ganglia) and right cerebellar regions. This extended language-relevant network for the thalamus in health can be used as a benchmark to compare the language network in thalamic aphasia.
Electronic supplementary material
Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.
Author contributions
TB and PH conceptualized the study; TB carried out the analyses and visualized the results; TB and PH wrote and reviewed the manuscript.
Funding
Open Access funding enabled and organized by Projekt DEAL. The authors declare that no funds, grants, or other support were received during the preparation of this manuscript.
Open Access funding enabled and organized by Projekt DEAL.
Data availability
All data and analysis files associated with the study are available at the online data repository: 10.17605/OSF.IO/JQNFK
Declarations
Competing interests
The authors have no relevant financial or non-financial interests to disclose.
Footnotes
Publisher’s Note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
References
- Albin RL, Young AB, Penney JB (1989) The functional anatomy of basal ganglia disorders. Trends Neurosci 12(10). 10.1016/0166-2236(89)90074-X [DOI] [PubMed]
- Alexander GE, Crutcher MD (1990) Functional architecture of basal ganglia circuits: neural substrates of parallel processing. Trends Neurosci 13(7):266–271. 10.1016/0166-2236(90)90107-L [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Alexander AL, Lee JE, Lazar M, Field AS (2007) Diffusion Tensor Imaging of the brain. Neurotherapeutics 4(3). 10.1016/j.nurt.2007.05.011 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- Antunes FM, Malmierca MS (2021) Corticothalamic pathways in Auditory Processing: recent advances and insights from other sensory systems. Front Neural Circuits 15(August):1–25. 10.3389/fncir.2021.721186 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ardila A, Bernal B, Rosselli M (2016) How Extended Is Wernicke’s Area? Meta-Analytic Connectivity Study of BA20 and Integrative Proposal. Neuroscience Journal, 2016, 1–6. 10.1155/2016/4962562 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- Barbas H, García-Cabezas MÁ, Zikopoulos B (2013) Frontal-thalamic circuits associated with language. Brain Lang 126(1):49–61. 10.1016/j.bandl.2012.10.001 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Belyk M, Brown S, Lim J, Kotz SA (2017) Convergence of semantics and emotional expression within the IFG pars orbitalis. NeuroImage 156(January):240–248. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.04.020 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bernal B, Ardila A, Rosselli M (2015) Broca’s area network in language function: a pooling-data connectivity study. Front Psychol 6(May):1–8. 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00687 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Binder JR, Desai RH, Graves WW, Conant LL (2009) Where is the semantic system? A critical review and meta-analysis of 120 functional neuroimaging studies. Cereb Cortex 19(12). 10.1093/cercor/bhp055 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- Bohsali AA, Triplett W, Sudhyadhom A, Gullett JM, McGregor K, FitzGerald DB, Crosson B (2015) Broca’s area – thalamic connectivity. Brain Lang 141:80–88. 10.1016/j.bandl.2014.12.001 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Booth JR, Wood L, Lu D, Houk JC, Bitan T (2007) The role of the basal ganglia and cerebellum in language processing. Brain Res 1133(1). 10.1016/j.brainres.2006.11.074 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- Brownsett SLE, Warren JE, Geranmayeh F, Woodhead Z, Leech R, Wise RJS (2014) Cognitive control and its impact on recovery from aphasic stroke. Brain 137(1):242–254. 10.1093/brain/awt289 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bulut T (2022) Meta-analytic connectivity modeling of the left and right inferior frontal gyri. Cortex 155:107–131. 10.1016/j.cortex.2022.07.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cassel J-C, de Vasconcelos AP (2015) The cognitive thalamus: a bridal chamber not to forget. Neurosci Biobehavioral Reviews 54:1–2. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2015.01.017 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chakravarthy VS, Joseph D, Bapi RS (2010) What do the basal ganglia do? A modeling perspective. Biol Cybern. 10.1007/s00422-010-0401-y [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cieslik EC, Mueller VI, Eickhoff CR, Langner R, Eickhoff SB (2015) Three key regions for supervisory attentional control: evidence from neuroimaging meta-analyses. Neurosci Biobehavioral Reviews 48:22–34. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.11.003 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Copland DA, Angwin AJ (2019) Subcortical contributions to Language. In: de Zubicaray GI, Schiller NO (eds) The Oxford Handbook of Neurolinguistics. Oxford University Press, pp 850–876. 10.1093/oxfordhb/9780190672027.013.33
- Copland DA, Brownsett S, Iyer K, Angwin AJ (2021) Corticostriatal regulation of Language functions. Neuropsychol Rev 31(3):472–494. 10.1007/s11065-021-09481-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Crinion J, Turner R, Grogan A, Hanakawa T, Noppeney U, Devlin JT, Price CJ (2006) Language control in the bilingual brain. Science 312(5779). 10.1126/science.1127761 [DOI] [PubMed]
- Crosson B (2013) Thalamic mechanisms in language: a reconsideration based on recent findings and concepts. Brain Lang 126(1):73–88. 10.1016/j.bandl.2012.06.011 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Crosson B (2021) The role of Cortico-Thalamo-Cortical Circuits in Language: recurrent circuits revisited. Neuropsychol Rev 31(3):516–533. 10.1007/s11065-019-09421-8 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cunningham SI, Tomasi D, Volkow ND (2017) Structural and functional connectivity of the precuneus and thalamus to the default mode network. Hum Brain Mapp 38(2):938–956. 10.1002/hbm.23429 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- David O, Maess B, Eckstein K, Friederici AD (2011) Dynamic causal modeling of Subcortical Connectivity of Language. J Neurosci 31(7):2712–2717. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3433-10.2011 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- De Witte L, Brouns R, Kavadias D, Engelborghs S, De Deyn PP, Mariën P (2011) Cognitive, affective and behavioural disturbances following vascular thalamic lesions: a review. Cortex 47(3):273–319. 10.1016/j.cortex.2010.09.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Desikan RS, Ségonne F, Fischl B, Quinn BT, Dickerson BC, Blacker D, Killiany RJ (2006) An automated labeling system for subdividing the human cerebral cortex on MRI scans into gyral based regions of interest. NeuroImage 31(3). 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.01.021 [DOI] [PubMed]
- Eickhoff SB, Laird AR, Grefkes C, Wang LE, Zilles K, Fox PT (2009) Coordinate-based activation likelihood estimation meta-analysis of neuroimaging data: a random-effects approach based on empirical estimates of spatial uncertainty. Hum Brain Mapp 30(9):2907–2926. 10.1002/hbm.20718 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Eickhoff SB, Bzdok D, Laird AR, Kurth F, Fox PT (2012) Activation likelihood estimation meta-analysis revisited. NeuroImage 59(3):2349–2361. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.09.017 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Erickson LC, Rauschecker JP, Turkeltaub PE (2017) Meta-analytic connectivity modeling of the human superior temporal sulcus. Brain Struct Function 222(1):267–285. 10.1007/s00429-016-1215-z [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fama R, Sullivan EV (2015) Thalamic structures and associated cognitive functions: relations with age and aging. Neurosci Biobehavioral Reviews 54:29–37. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2015.03.008 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fisher SD, Reynolds JNJ (2014) The intralaminar thalamus-an expressway linking visual stimuli to circuits determining agency and action selection. Front Behav Neurosci. 10.3389/fnbeh.2014.00115 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ford AA, Triplett W, Sudhyadhom A, Gullett J, McGregor K, FitzGerald DB, Crosson B (2013) Broca’s area and its striatal and thalamic connections: a diffusion-MRI tractography study. Front Neuroanat 7(May). 10.3389/fnana.2013.00008 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- Fox PT, Lancaster JL (2002) Mapping context and content: the BrainMap model. Nat Rev Neurosci 3(4):319–321. 10.1038/nrn789 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fox PT, Laird AR, Fox SP, Fox PM, Uecker AM, Crank M, Lancaster JL (2005) Brainmap taxonomy of experimental design: description and evaluation. Hum Brain Mapp 25(1):185–198. 10.1002/hbm.20141 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Frazier JA, Chiu S, Breeze JL, Makris N, Lange N, Kennedy DN, Biederman J (2005) Structural brain magnetic resonance imaging of limbic and thalamic volumes in pediatric bipolar disorder. Am J Psychiatry 162(7). 10.1176/appi.ajp.162.7.1256 [DOI] [PubMed]
- Friederici AD (2012) The cortical language circuit: from auditory perception to sentence comprehension. Trends Cogn Sci 16(5):262–268. 10.1016/j.tics.2012.04.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Friederici AD, Kotz SA, Scott SK, Obleser J (2009) Disentangling syntax and intelligibility in auditory language comprehension. Hum Brain Mapp 31(3):NA–NA. 10.1002/hbm.20878 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Giglio L, Ostarek M, Weber K, Hagoort P (2022) Commonalities and asymmetries in the neurobiological infrastructure for Language Production and Comprehension. Cereb Cortex 32(7):1405–1418. 10.1093/cercor/bhab287 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Giraldo-Chica M, Woodward ND (2017) Review of thalamocortical resting-state fMRI studies in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res 180:58–63. 10.1016/j.schres.2016.08.005 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Giraldo-Chica M, Rogers BP, Damon SM, Landman BA, Woodward ND (2018) Prefrontal-Thalamic Anatomical Connectivity and executive cognitive function in Schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry 83(6):509–517. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2017.09.022 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Greicius MD, Supekar K, Menon V, Dougherty RF (2009) Resting-state functional connectivity reflects structural connectivity in the default Mode Network. Cereb Cortex 19(1):72–78. 10.1093/cercor/bhn059 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grodd W, Kumar VJ, Schüz A, Lindig T, Scheffler K (2020) The anterior and medial thalamic nuclei and the human limbic system: tracing the structural connectivity using diffusion-weighted imaging. Sci Rep 10(1):10957. 10.1038/s41598-020-67770-4 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Groenewegen HJ (2003) The basal ganglia and Motor Control. Neural Plast 10(1–2):107–120. 10.1155/NP.2003.107 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grönholm EO, Roll MC, Horne MA, Sundgren PC, Lindgren AG (2016) Predominance of caudate nucleus lesions in acute ischaemic stroke patients with impairment in language and speech. Eur J Neurol 23(1). 10.1111/ene.12822 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- Hagoort P (2005) On Broca, brain, and binding: a new framework. Trends Cogn Sci 9(9):416–423. 10.1016/j.tics.2005.07.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hagoort P (2013) MUC (Memory, Unification, Control) and beyond. Front Psychol 4(JUL):1–13. 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00416 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hagoort P (2016) MUC (Memory, Unification, Control). In Neurobiology of Language (pp. 339–347). Elsevier. 10.1016/B978-0-12-407794-2.00028-6
- Hagoort P (2019) The neurobiology of language beyond single-word processing. Science 366(6461):55–58. 10.1126/science.aax0289 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hagoort P, Indefrey P (2014) The Neurobiology of Language Beyond single words. Annu Rev Neurosci 37(1):347–362. 10.1146/annurev-neuro-071013-013847 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hale JR, Mayhew SD, Mullinger KJ, Wilson RS, Arvanitis TN, Francis ST, Bagshaw AP (2015) Comparison of functional thalamic segmentation from seed-based analysis and ICA. NeuroImage 114:448–465. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.04.027 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Heard M, Lee YS (2020) Shared neural resources of rhythm and syntax: An ALE meta-analysis. Neuropsychologia, 137(November 2019), 107284. 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2019.107284 [DOI] [PubMed]
- Hodge SM, Makris N, Kennedy DN, Caviness VS, Howard J, McGrath L, Harris GJ (2010) Cerebellum, language, and cognition in autism and specific language impairment. J Autism Dev Disord 40(3). 10.1007/s10803-009-0872-7 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- Huang H, Ding M (2016) Linking functional connectivity and structural connectivity quantitatively: a comparison of methods. Brain Connect 6(2):99–108. 10.1089/brain.2015.0382 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Janacsek K, Evans TM, Kiss M, Shah L, Blumenfeld H, Ullman MT (2022) Subcortical cognition: the Fruit below the rind. Annu Rev Neurosci 45:361–386. 10.1146/annurev-neuro-110920-013544 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jeon HA, Anwander A, Friederici AD (2014) Functional network mirrored in the prefrontal cortex, caudate nucleus, and thalamus: high-resolution functional imaging and structural connectivity. J Neurosci 34(28):9202–9212. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0228-14.2014 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Johnson MD, Ojemann GA (2000) The role of the human thalamus in language and memory: evidence from electrophysiological studies. Brain Cogn 42(2). 10.1006/brcg.1999.1101 [DOI] [PubMed]
- Joliot M, Jobard G, Naveau M, Delcroix N, Petit L, Zago L, Tzourio-Mazoyer N (2015) AICHA: An atlas of intrinsic connectivity of homotopic areas. Journal of Neuroscience Methods, 254. 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2015.07.013 [DOI] [PubMed]
- Kawaguchi Y (2017) Pyramidal cell subtypes and their synaptic connections in layer 5 of rat frontal cortex. Cereb Cortex 27(12). 10.1093/cercor/bhx252 [DOI] [PubMed]
- Klostermann F, Krugel LK, Ehlen F (2013) Functional roles of the thalamus for language capacities. Front Syst Neurosci 7(JUNE):1–8. 10.3389/fnsys.2013.00032 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kochunov P, Lancaster J, Thompson P, Toga AW, Brewer P, Hardies J, Fox P (2002) An optimized individual target brain in the Talairach Coordinate System. NeuroImage 17(2):922–927. 10.1006/nimg.2002.1084 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kumar VJ, van Oort E, Scheffler K, Beckmann CF, Grodd W (2017) Functional anatomy of the human thalamus at rest. NeuroImage 147(December 2016):678–691. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.12.071 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kumar VJ, Scheffler K, Grodd W (2023) The structural connectivity mapping of the intralaminar thalamic nuclei. Sci Rep 13(1):11938. 10.1038/s41598-023-38967-0 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Laird AR, Lancaster JL, Fox PT (2005) BrainMap: the Social Evolution of a human brain mapping database. Neuroinformatics 3(1):065–078. 10.1385/NI:3:1:065 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Laird AR, Robinson JL, McMillan KM, Tordesillas-Gutiérrez D, Moran ST, Gonzales SM, Lancaster JL (2010) Comparison of the disparity between Talairach and MNI coordinates in functional neuroimaging data: validation of the Lancaster transform. NeuroImage 51(2):677–683. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.02.048 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lancaster JL, Rainey LH, Summerlin JL, Freitas CS, Fox PT, Evans AC, Mazziotta JC (1997) Automated labeling of the human brain: a preliminary report on the development and evaluation of a forward-transform method. Hum Brain Mapp 5(4):238–242. 10.1002/(SICI)1097-0193(1997)5:4<238::AID-HBM6>3.0.CO;2-4 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lancaster JL, Woldorff MG, Parsons LM, Liotti M, Freitas CS, Rainey L, Fox PT (2000) Automated Talairach Atlas labels for functional brain mapping. Hum Brain Mapp 10(3):120–131. 10.1002/1097-0193(200007)10:3<120::AID-HBM30>3.0.CO;2-8 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lancaster JL, Tordesillas-Gutiérrez D, Martinez M, Salinas F, Evans A, Zilles K, Fox PT (2007) Bias between MNI and Talairach coordinates analyzed using the ICBM-152 brain template. Hum Brain Mapp 28(11):1194–1205. 10.1002/hbm.20345 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lancaster JL, Cykowski MD, McKay DR, Kochunov PV, Fox PT, Rogers W, Mazziotta J (2010) Anatomical global spatial normalization. Neuroinformatics 8(3):171–182. 10.1007/s12021-010-9074-x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lancaster JL, Laird AR, Eickhoff SB, Martinez MJ, Fox PM, Fox PT (2012) Automated regional behavioral analysis for human brain images. Front Neuroinformatics 6(JULY 2012):1–12. 10.3389/fninf.2012.00023 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lanciego JL, Luquin N, Obeso JA (2012) Functional neuroanatomy of the basal ganglia. Cold Spring Harbor Perspect Med 2(12). 10.1101/cshperspect.a009621 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- Lee MH, Smyser CD, Shimony JS (2013) Resting-state fMRI: a review of methods and clinical applications. Am J Neuroradiol 34(10). 10.3174/ajnr.A3263 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- Liu Y, Gao C, Wang P, Friederici AD, Zaccarella E, Chen L (2023) Exploring the neurobiology of Merge at a basic level: insights from a novel artificial grammar paradigm. Front Psychol 14(May):1–19. 10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1151518 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Llano DA (2013) Functional imaging of the thalamus in language. Brain Lang 126(1):62–72. 10.1016/j.bandl.2012.06.004 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Llano DA (2016) The Thalamus and Language. In Neurobiology of Language (pp. 95–114). Elsevier. 10.1016/B978-0-12-407794-2.00009-2
- Lorca-Puls DL, Gajardo-Vidal A, Oberhuber M, Prejawa S, Hope TMH, Leff AP, Price CJ (2021) Brain regions that support accurate speech production after damage to Broca’s area. Brain Commun 3(4):1–19. 10.1093/braincomms/fcab230 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lu C, Peng D, Chen C, Ning N, Ding G, Li K, Lin C (2010) Altered effective connectivity and anomalous anatomy in the basal ganglia-thalamocortical circuit of stuttering speakers. Cortex 46(1):49–67. 10.1016/j.cortex.2009.02.017 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Müller VI, Cieslik EC, Serbanescu I, Laird AR, Fox PT, Eickhoff SB (2017) Altered brain activity in unipolar depression revisited: Meta-analyses of neuroimaging studies. JAMA Psychiatry 74(1):47–55. 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2016.2783 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Müller-Axt C, Anwander A, von Kriegstein K (2017) Altered structural connectivity of the Left Visual Thalamus in Developmental Dyslexia. Curr Biol 27(23):3692–3698e4. 10.1016/j.cub.2017.10.034 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Murdoch BE (2010) The cerebellum and language: historical perspective and review. Cortex 46(7):858–868. 10.1016/j.cortex.2009.07.018 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Murphy E, Hoshi K, Benítez-Burraco A (2022) Subcortical syntax: reconsidering the neural dynamics of language. J Neurolinguistics 62(March 2021):101062. 10.1016/j.jneuroling.2022.101062 [Google Scholar]
- Nadeau SE, Crosson B (1997) Subcortical aphasia. Brain Lang 58(3):355–402. 10.1006/brln.1997.1707 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- O’Muircheartaigh J, Keller SS, Barker GJ, Richardson MP (2015) White Matter Connectivity of the Thalamus delineates the Functional Architecture of competing Thalamocortical systems. Cereb Cortex 25(11):4477–4489. 10.1093/cercor/bhv063 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Packard MG, Knowlton BJ (2002) Learning and memory functions of the basal ganglia. Annu Rev Neurosci 25(1):563–593. 10.1146/annurev.neuro.25.112701.142937 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Qiao J, Wang Z, Zhao G, Huo Y, Herder CL, Sikora CO, Peterson BS (2017) Functional neural circuits that underlie developmental stuttering. PLoS ONE 12(7):e0179255. 10.1371/journal.pone.0179255 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Robinson JL, Laird AR, Glahn DC, Lovallo WR, Fox PT (2010) Metaanalytic connectivity modeling: delineating the functional connectivity of the human amygdala. Hum Brain Mapp 31(2):173–184. 10.1002/hbm.20854 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rodd JM, Vitello S, Woollams AM, Adank P (2015) Localising semantic and syntactic processing in spoken and written language comprehension: an activation likelihood estimation meta-analysis. Brain Lang 141:89–102. 10.1016/j.bandl.2014.11.012 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rolls ET, Huang CC, Lin CP, Feng J, Joliot M (2020) Automated anatomical labelling atlas 3. NeuroImage 206. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.116189 [DOI] [PubMed]
- Rorden C, Brett M (2000) Stereotaxic display of brain lesions. Behav Neurol 12(4). 10.1155/2000/421719 [DOI] [PubMed]
- Samartsidis P, Montagna S, Laird AR, Fox PT, Johnson TD, Nichols TE (2020) Estimating the prevalence of missing experiments in a neuroimaging meta-analysis. Res Synthesis Methods 11(6):866–883. 10.1002/jrsm.1448 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shepherd GMG, Yamawaki N (2021) Untangling the cortico-thalamo-cortical loop: cellular pieces of a knotty circuit puzzle. Nat Rev Neurosci 22(7):389–406. 10.1038/s41583-021-00459-3 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sherman SM (2016) Thalamus plays a central role in ongoing cortical functioning. Nat Neurosci 19(4):533–541. 10.1038/nn.4269 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shine JM, Lewis LD, Garrett DD, Hwang K (2023) The impact of the human thalamus on brain-wide information processing. Nat Rev Neurosci 24(7):416–430. 10.1038/s41583-023-00701-0 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Statements & Declarations
- Sundermann B, Pfleiderer B (2012) Functional connectivity profile of the human inferior frontal junction: involvement in a cognitive control network. BMC Neurosci 13(1):119. 10.1186/1471-2202-13-119 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tan LH, Chen L, Yip V, Chan AHD, Yang J, Gao JH, Siok WT (2011) Activity levels in the left hemisphere caudate-fusiform circuit predict how well a second language will be learned. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 108(6). 10.1073/pnas.0909623108 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- Theofanopoulou C, Boeckx C (2016) The central role of the thalamus in language and cognition. In K. Fujita & C. Boeckx (Eds.), Advances in Biolinguistics: The human language faculty and its biological basis (pp. 230–255). Routledge. 10.4324/9781315709529-20
- Tomasi D, Volkow ND (2012) Resting functional connectivity of language networks: characterization and reproducibility. Mol Psychiatry 17(8):841–854. 10.1038/mp.2011.177 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tomasi D, Volkow ND (2019) Reduced local and increased long-range functional connectivity of the Thalamus in Autism Spectrum Disorder. Cereb Cortex 29(2):573–585. 10.1093/cercor/bhx340 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Torrico TJ, Munakomi S (2019) Neuroanatomy, Thalamus. StatPearls [PubMed]
- Tschentscher N, Ruisinger A, Blank H, Díaz B, von Kriegstein K (2019) Reduced structural connectivity between left auditory thalamus and the motion-sensitive planum temporale in developmental dyslexia. J Neurosci 39(9):1435–1418. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1435-18.2018 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Turkeltaub PE, Branch Coslett H (2010) Localization of sublexical speech perception components. Brain Lang 114(1). 10.1016/j.bandl.2010.03.008 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- Turkeltaub PE, Eickhoff SB, Laird AR, Fox M, Wiener M, Fox P (2012) Minimizing within-experiment and within-group effects in activation likelihood estimation meta-analyses. Hum Brain Mapp 33(1):1–13. 10.1002/hbm.21186 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tzourio-Mazoyer N, Landeau B, Papathanassiou D, Crivello F, Etard O, Delcroix N, Joliot M (2002) Automated anatomical labeling of activations in SPM using a macroscopic anatomical parcellation of the MNI MRI single-subject brain. NeuroImage 15(1). 10.1006/nimg.2001.0978 [DOI] [PubMed]
- Ullman MT (2016) The Declarative/Procedural Model: A Neurobiological Model of Language Learning, Knowledge, and Use. In Neurobiology of Language (pp. 953–968). Elsevier. 10.1016/B978-0-12-407794-2.00076-6
- Usrey WM, Sherman SM (2019) Corticofugal circuits: communication lines from the cortex to the rest of the brain. J Comp Neurol 527(3):640–650. 10.1002/cne.24423 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- van den Heuvel MP, Hulshoff Pol HE (2010) Exploring the brain network: a review on resting-state fMRI functional connectivity. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol 20(8):519–534. 10.1016/j.euroneuro.2010.03.008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vigneau M, Beaucousin V, Hervé PY, Duffau H, Crivello F, Houdé O, Tzourio-Mazoyer N (2006) Meta-analyzing left hemisphere language areas: Phonology, semantics, and sentence processing. NeuroImage 30(4):1414–1432. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.11.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Viñas-Guasch N, Wu YJ (2017) The role of the putamen in language: a meta-analytic connectivity modeling study. Brain Struct Function 222(9):3991–4004. 10.1007/s00429-017-1450-y [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wahl M, Marzinzik F, Friederici AD, Hahne A, Kupsch A, Schneider G-H, Klostermann F (2008) The human thalamus processes Syntactic and Semantic Language violations. Neuron 59(5):695–707. 10.1016/j.neuron.2008.07.011 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Walenski M, Europa E, Caplan D, Thompson CK (2019) Neural networks for sentence comprehension and production: an ALE-based meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies. Hum Brain Mapp 40(8):2275–2304. 10.1002/hbm.24523 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wang Z, Jia X, Liang P, Qi Z, Yang Y, Zhou W, Li K (2012) Changes in thalamus connectivity in mild cognitive impairment: evidence from resting state fMRI. Eur J Radiol 81(2):277–285. 10.1016/j.ejrad.2010.12.044 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wildgruber D, Ackermann H, Grodd W (2001) Differential contributions of motor cortex, basal ganglia, and cerebellum to speech motor control: effects of syllable repetition rate evaluated by fMRI. NeuroImage 13(1). 10.1006/nimg.2000.0672 [DOI] [PubMed]
- Wojtasik M, Bludau S, Eickhoff SB, Mohlberg H, Gerboga F, Caspers S, Amunts K (2020) Cytoarchitectonic characterization and functional decoding of Four New areas in the human lateral Orbitofrontal cortex. Front Neuroanat 14(February):1–18. 10.3389/fnana.2020.00002 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Xiang H-D, Fonteijn HM, Norris DG, Hagoort P (2010) Topographical functional connectivity pattern in the Perisylvian Language Networks. Cereb Cortex 20(3):549–560. 10.1093/cercor/bhp119 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zhou B, Liu Y, Zhang Z, An N, Yao H, Wang P, Jiang T (2013) Impaired functional connectivity of the Thalamus in Alzheimer’s disease and mild cognitive impairment: a resting-state fMRI study. Curr Alzheimer Res 10(7):754–766. 10.2174/15672050113109990146 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zou L, Ding G, Abutalebi J, Shu H, Peng D (2012) Structural plasticity of the left caudate in bimodal bilinguals. Cortex 48(9). 10.1016/j.cortex.2011.05.022 [DOI] [PubMed]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
All data and analysis files associated with the study are available at the online data repository: 10.17605/OSF.IO/JQNFK