Abstract
How biological neural networks reliably process information in the presence of spontaneous activity remains controversial. In mouse primary visual cortex (V1), stimulus-evoked and spontaneous activity show orthogonal (dissimilar) patterns, which is advantageous for separating sensory signals from internal noise. However, studies in carnivore and primate V1, which have functional columns, have reported high similarity between stimulus-evoked and spontaneous activity. Thus, the mechanism of signal-noise separation in the columnar visual cortex may be different from that in rodents. To address this issue, we compared spontaneous and stimulus-evoked activity in marmoset V1 and higher visual areas. In marmoset V1, spontaneous and stimulus-evoked activity showed similar patterns as expected. However, in marmoset higher visual areas, spontaneous and stimulus-evoked activity were progressively orthogonalized along the cortical hierarchy, eventually reaching levels comparable to those in mouse V1. These results suggest that orthogonalization of spontaneous and stimulus-evoked activity is a general principle of cortical computation.
Subject terms: Extrastriate cortex, Dynamical systems
How the brain accurately processes sensory information in the presence of internal noise remains unclear. Here the authors report that, in primate visual cortex, spontaneous and visually-evoked activity are progressively orthogonalized along the cortical hierarchy.
Introduction
Spontaneous activity is one of the most characteristic features of the biological brain that distinguishes it from current artificial neural networks. Studies on global spontaneous activity, which is often observed with functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), collectively revealed the precise similarity between spatial patterns of spontaneous and task-evoked brain networks1–7. Resting-state activity can be used to predict individual task-evoked fMRI activity8. Such similarity provides important clues for understanding how the intrinsic dynamics of neural circuits may gauge cognitive functions1,8–12. However, at the cellular and mesoscales, the presence of similarities between spontaneous and evoked neural activity remains controversial. Recent studies using cellular resolution calcium imaging reported that, in the primary visual cortex (V1) of mice, patterns of spontaneous activity are orthogonal to those of visually evoked activity13–15. Theoretical considerations indicate that the orthogonal relationship separates stimulus-related signals from internally generated noise, which is advantageous for perceptual stability14,16. The fact that much of the spontaneous activity in the mouse V1 encodes behavioural information13,15 further supports the idea that the orthogonal relationship is advantageous for separating stimulus-related and behavioural information.
In contrast to these recent studies, several previous studies that used voltage-sensitive dye imaging and electrode-array recordings showed high similarity between the spatial patterns of spontaneous and stimulus-evoked activity3,4,17,18. Notably, seminal works by Grinvald et al. reported that, in V1 of cats and macaques, neuronal activity patterns resembling iso-orientation columns spontaneously appeared without visual stimulation3,6.
The apparent differences between the findings of the two sets of studies may stem from several key differences. First, there is a difference in experimental techniques: mesoscale functional recording of neural population activity in cats and primates versus two-photon calcium imaging at a cellular resolution in mice. Second, a difference in analytic techniques: the former set of studies mostly used simple spatial correlation, whereas the latter set of studies devised a geometric analysis of neural activity space. Third, there is a potential species-related difference. At the level of local circuits, carnivores and primates possess neocortex with functional columns19,20, whereas rodents possess non-columnar neocortex21,22. In addition to these differences, the fact that both sets of studies were conducted only on V1 makes it difficult to extend these results to general cortical computations. Most importantly, if spontaneous and evoked activities share similar patterns in all the visual areas of carnivores/primates, it would be of great interest to determine how the cortical network could distinguish between stimulus-related information from internally generated noise.
Therefore, we aimed to distinguish between these possibilities and to gain insights into the principles of cortical computations by investigating the relationship between spontaneous and stimulus-evoked activity in multiple cortical areas comprising the hierarchical visual network of marmoset monkeys. Marmoset monkeys have a visual network with a well-defined hierarchy23,24. Unlike macaques, most visual areas in marmosets are exposed on the cortical surface and are thus accessible using optical approaches25–30. To systematically characterize the spatiotemporal patterns of spontaneous activity across the marmoset visual areas, we used a newly developed primate-optimized expression system of genetically encoded calcium indicators (Hashimoto et al., in preparation).
Results
To observe spontaneous and stimulus-evoked activity across the cortical network, we used marmoset monkeys whose flat neocortex allows easy optical access to many cortical areas23. Grid-based injection of adeno-associated virus carrying primate-optimized GCaMP (Tandem-GcaMP31; Hashimoto et al., in preparation) (Fig. 1a), a genetically encoded calcium indicator, successfully transduced a large contiguous cortical volume spanning the occipital and parietal lobes (Fig. 1b). Wide-field imaging with visual stimulation revealed patchy functional maps in multiple visual areas (Fig. 1c), demonstrating columnar-scale functional resolution throughout the large field of view (FOV).
Previous mesoscale functional imaging studies on carnivore and primate V1s reported patchy spatial patterns of spontaneous activity that corresponded with the patchy patterns of iso-orientation columns3,6,9. Using widefield calcium imaging, we first examined spontaneous activity across cortical areas and determined whether patchy spatial patterns of spontaneous activity exist beyond V1. The large-sized FOV allowed us to reveal large-scale spatial patterns of spontaneous activity that extend across the cortical areas32,33. Importantly, the high spatiotemporal resolution of calcium imaging revealed patchy spatial patterns embedded within the large-scale patterns (Supplementary Movies 1, 2). Notably the patchy pattern was detectable throughout the FOV well beyond V1 including regions near parietal association areas (Fig. 1d), suggesting that the patchy spatial pattern was homogeneously present throughout the primate neocortex. Consistent with this idea, imaging in the temporal cortex revealed the presence of wave-like spontaneous activity with patchy texture (Fig. 1e–g; Supplementary Movie 3), and even in the parietal and frontal areas where the presence of columnar functional map is unknown (Supplementary Fig. 1). Collectively, these results suggest that the patchy activity pattern is a canonical mode of spontaneous activity in the primate neocortex.
We performed principal component analysis (PCA) on the widefield spontaneous activity and then obtained a principal component (PC) that best correlated with the visually evoked activity to examine potential differences in spontaneous patches across areas, similar to the previous studies34, using data covering V1 and the secondary visual cortex (V2) in the same FOV. In all three examined datasets, we found that the half-width-half-maximum of the autocorrelation profile was larger for V2 than for V1 (mean difference, 19.9%). Thus, the spontaneous patches in V2 were larger than those in V1 and similar in size to the orientation columns in each area (Supplementary Fig. 2), suggesting a potential correspondence between the spontaneous patches and functional columns. Cellular-scale imaging has also revealed differences in the spatial clustering of spontaneously coactive neurons across areas. We found that the proportion of coactive neurons in spatial proximity was significantly lower in the MT than in the V1 and V2 (p < 0.0001, two-sample t test corrected by Bonferroni’s method; Supplementary Fig. 3), suggesting that spontaneously co-active neurons were less clustered in the MT than in the V1 and V2. These results suggest that spontaneous activity was heterogeneous across areas, based on the size of the meso-scale patchy organization and the degree of clustering of co-active neurons within the patches.
We next conducted two-photon imaging to compare spontaneous and stimulus-evoked activity at the cellular scale (Fig. 2a), and investigated whether the trial-averaged evoked responses were similar to the spontaneous patterns. Further, to address the question of across-areal generality and potential species-related differences, we experimented on V1, V2, and the middle temporal visual area (MT) in the dorsal visual pathway of marmosets35 and on mouse V1.
Consistent with previous reports on cat and macaque V1s3,15,39, snapshots of spontaneous activity often appeared similar to visually evoked orientation responses (Fig. 2b–d). A similar correspondence between spontaneous activity and the functional map was also observed in V2 (Fig. 2e). However, in the downstream visual areas MT, we found much lower similarities of spontaneous activity and visually evoked orientation and direction responses, respectively (Fig. 2f; Supplementary Fig. 4).
Population data showed that, in all three areas, spatial correlation between single frames of spontaneous activity and evoked responses was high in V1 and V2, and became smaller in MT (Fig. 2g; Supplementary Fig. 5a). Widefield imaging also revealed that the similarity between spontaneous PCs and visually evoked maps was significantly higher in V1 and V2 than in the MT (p < 0.02, t test; Supplementary Fig. 6). These results confirmed the previous finding of close correspondence between spontaneous and evoked activity in V13,6,9,17,18,34, and Supplementary it to V2. However, the correspondence did not apply to downstream area MT, suggesting that the relationship between spontaneous and evoked activity varies across cortical areas.
Subsequently, to obtain a better picture of the relationship between spontaneous and evoked activities across the three cortical areas, we next conducted a geometrical analysis introduced in the recent mouse studies13. We applied the geometrical analysis to both the marmoset data and also to the mouse data acquired in-house using the same experimental techniques. Briefly, spontaneous and evoked neural population activities were expressed as vectors that were then projected onto three orthogonal subspaces within the activity space (Fig. 3a). The first subspace is shared between evoked and spontaneous activity (“shared space”). The second and third subspaces are respectively specific to spontaneous and evoked activity (“spontaneous-only space” and “stim-only space”). After projection to three orthogonal subspaces, the principal components (PCs) of each activity space were calculated to visualize the dominant activity patterns (Fig. 3b; Supplementary Fig. 7). As expected from the frame-by-frame correlation analysis, spatial similarity between PCs of shared space and visually evoked responses were high in V1 and V2 but low in MT (Fig. 3c, middle; see also Supplementary Fig. 5b). In contrast, the similarity between PCs of stim-only space and evoked responses were low in V1 and V2 but high in MT (Fig. 3c, top). PCs in spontaneous-only space were dissimilar to the evoked maps in all areas, as expected (Fig. 3c, bottom).
To quantify the contribution of the averaged signals to the total variance in single-trial neural activity, we performed a cross-validated regression of neural activity. In the marmosets V1, V2, MT and mouse V1, the percent of the total variance of single-trial activity explained by the averaged signals ranged between 30 and 50% (Supplementary Fig. 8). Thus, the trial-averaged neural activity explained a substantial fraction of the single-trial stimulus-evoked responses in all tested visual areas.
Finally, we focused on single-trial activities to quantify the fraction of variance of the visually evoked activity projected to the shared activity space (Fig. 4a, b). A previous study on mouse V1 reported that the fraction of the projected variance was approximately 20%, which led to the conclusion that stimulus-evoked activity and spontaneous activity are nearly orthogonal13. In contrast to mouse V1, the fraction of stimulus-related variance explained by Shared Space was approximately 50% in the marmoset V1 (50 ± 2.7%, mean ± SEM). The projected variance slightly decreased in V2, but remained high (44 ± 2.3%). The projected variance decreased further in MT (31 ± 1.4%). The fraction of stimulus-related variance in the shared space for mouse V1 recorded using the same experimental setup yielded the smallest value (18 ± 2.0%), which is comparable to a previously reported value13. Each PC in marmoset V1 and V2 had larger projected variances compared to marmoset MT and mouse V1. This was consistent with the relatively large, shared subspace in marmoset V1 and V2 compared to the other two areas (Supplementary Fig. 9). The same tendency was observed when shared space was limited to one dimension (Supplementary Fig. 10). The projected variance of the mouse V1 was similar after controlling the potential SNR differences between marmoset data and mouse data (Supplementary Fig. 11). The projected variance in marmoset V1 was significantly larger than that in mouse V1 after controlling for the number of cells and trials included in the analysis (Supplementary Fig. 12b, c). The same tendency across the three marmoset visual areas and mouse V1 was also observed when the data were summarized by animal (Supplementary Fig. 5c) and when non-trial averaged visual responses were used (Fig. 4c).
The estimated dimensionality of shared space, as quantified by the number of PCs with large projections of stimulus-related variance, showed a similar tendency to the amount of projected variance; the dimensionality of shared space was larger for marmosets V1 and V2 than for marmoset MT and mouse V1 (Supplementary Fig. 12a). Collectively, these results suggest that, in the marmoset, the overlap between spontaneous and evoked activity progressively decreased along the cortical hierarchy.
Discussion
In this study, we systematically investigated the patterns of spontaneous neocortical activity and its relation to stimulus-evoked activity across multiple hierarchically organized areas in the marmoset neocortex using mesoscale and cellular scale functional imaging with a newly developed expression system of genetically encoded calcium indicators in the primate brain. We found that spontaneous activity in the marmoset neocortex appeared as propagating waves with patchy spatial patterns on top of the waves. Notably, patchy spontaneous activity was not a unique feature of V1, but rather a general characteristic of primate neocortex, which includes the occipital, parietal, temporal, and frontal areas. However, the similarities between spontaneous and stimulus-evoked activity patterns varied across areas. In V1, the similarity between spontaneous and evoked activity was high, which is consistent with the findings of previous studies on cats and macaques. However, cellular imaging revealed lesser patchiness in the higher visual areas. Accordingly, stimulus-evoked and spontaneous activity were dissimilar in higher visual areas of the marmoset, as in mouse V1. The analysis of neural activity space further revealed that the stimulus-related variance projected onto the shared space progressively decreased along the cortical hierarchy (Fig. 4d). Thus, these results suggest that the marmoset visual network achieves the separation of stimulus-related signal information and internally generated noise through processing in the hierarchical cortical areal network.
Consistent with the findings of previous studies on primates, carnivores, and rodents31,33,36, we found that spontaneous activity appeared as propagating waves. Previous studies, particularly seminal works by Grinvald et al., have also reported that spontaneous cortical activity in the primary visual areas of carnivores and primates show characteristic patchy spatial patterns3,9,34. In the present study, we therefore examined the generalizability of the previous observations beyond the level of V1. The high signal-to-noise ratio of the primate-optimized calcium imaging allowed us to further reveal that patchy spatial patterns were superimposed on top of these waves. Notably, we revealed that patchy spontaneous activity was not a unique feature of V1, but rather a general characteristic of primate neocortex. In the cat and primate V1, iso-orientation columns are connected by horizontal connections37–39. In the higher cortical areas of primates, both long-range and short-range cortico-cortical connections are known to have patchy spatial patterns in primates40,41. Thus, the presence of patchy spatial patterns may indicate that spontaneous activity propagates along cortico-cortical connections.
The present study confirms previously reported similarities between spontaneous and visually evoked activity in V1 of cats and macaques3,6,34. Using the same recording method in mice, we also confirmed that state spaces spanned by stimulus-evoked responses and by internally generated noise were already orthogonal in V113,14. These findings showed that the contradictory results in rodents and non-columnar mammals are not attributable to methodological differences42 but rather to species-related difference in V143. The species-related difference may reflect the difference in the synaptic integration by visual cortical neurons (Supplementary Discussion 1). It has been reported that synaptic integration by visual cortical neurons differs across columnar and non-columnar mammals: Synaptic inputs to a single neuron are more likely to share similar function in the columnar visual cortex44 than in the non-columnar visual cortex45. Because neurons with similar functional properties tend to have high noise correlation46 as well as spontaneous correlation47, marmoset V1 neurons are more likely to be activated by correlated inputs from presynaptic neurons that share similar functional properties than mouse neurons. This difference in synaptic integration maybe reflected in the presence/absence of columnar architecture. Recent studies on recurrent neural networks suggest that such differences in local connectivity patterns are related to the dimensionality of spontaneous and evoked neural network activity48,49. Avitan et al. reported that, in the zebrafish optic tectum, spontaneous and visually evoked activity patterns become less similar and geometrically diverse during development50. It is of great interest for future studies to examine whether the relationship between the spontaneous and evoked activity in the mouse V1 and the marmoset MT follows a similar developmental pattern.
Of note, the use of anaesthesia can affect spontaneous and stimulus-driven activity patterns. Okun et al. recorded spontaneous and evoked activities in the primary auditory cortex (A1) of anesthetized rats and, unlike the Stringer et al. study conducted in awake V1, showed considerable similarity between spontaneous and evoked neuronal activity patterns51. Thus, the use of anaesthesia can affect the degree of similarity between spontaneous and evoked neuronal activities. In the present study, to account for this possibility and minimize the effect of anaesthesia on the comparison between mouse V1 and marmoset V1, we conducted mouse experiments under anaesthetized conditions comparable to those used for marmosets. Hence, the difference between marmoset V1 and mouse V1 found in the present study is unlikely to be explained solely by anaesthesia. However, some differences in the study by Stringer et al. and the present study in mouse V1 (e.g. larger dimensionality of the shared space) may be attributable to the use of anaesthesia. Stringer et al. reported that the one-dimensional behaviours-stimulus shared subspace can be interpreted as a multiplicative gain modulation of cortical stimulus responses. Such multiplicative gain modulation has been linked to attentional modulation of cortical activity52,53. It is likely that the anaesthetic condition used in the present study strongly attenuated this component of neuronal activity (in fact the variance projected to the 1st PC in our data was 8% whereas that in Stringer et al. was 15%).
Previous studies on information processing in the hierarchical cortical network reported increasingly larger spatial scales (i.e., receptive fields)54 and temporal scales along the cortical hierarchy55. In this study, we revealed a computational role for the hierarchical cortical network in progressively orthogonalizing spontaneous and stimulus-evoked activity. Such orthogonalization likely continues to the frontal cortex, where neurons form distinct assemblies in spontaneous and task-related periods56. A potential functional role of orthogonalization may be the separation of stimulus-related signals and internal noise in the neural activity space14,16,57–64. Importantly, our study showed that, unlike orthogonalization in mice, which is already completed in V1, marmosets take place at the multiple steps of interareal processing. One possible explanation for the progressive orthogonalization is that the activity patterns of stim-only PCs are more effective in activating neurons in the downstream area than those of shared PCs. Indeed, Semedo et al. reported that V2 activity reflects specific patterns of V1 activity but not necessarily the largest activity65. Such across areal activity transmission filters out shared PCs and hence, enables segregation of sensory signals and internal noise (Supplementary Discussion 2). Because almost all of the visual areas are accessible for optical recording and manipulation, the marmoset monkey is ideally suited for testing these hypotheses.
The similarity of spontaneous and evoked neuronal activity has been interpreted as evidence Supplementary the functional significance of spontaneous cortical activity, such as the Bayesian prior18, recent history of experience66 and prediction of incoming inputs67. In marmoset V1, the estimated number of dimensions in the shared subspace was three, which was larger than the dimensionality of mouse V1, but not by a large margin (Supplementary Fig. 12a). These results suggest that even if spontaneous activity in marmoset V1 serves as Bayesian prior, it may only provide rudimentary information about the visual scene (e.g. orientation or colour). Nevertheless, the overlap of spontaneous and evoked activity patterns found in marmoset V1 is consistent with the hypotheses about the functions of spontaneous activity listed above. In contrast, in mouse V1, spontaneous and sensory-evoked activities are nearly orthogonal and represent behavioural information such as facial expression13. The present study shows that the marmoset visual areas downstream to V1 achieve an orthogonal relationship of spontaneous and sensory-evoked activities, suggesting that spontaneous activity in these areas may represent behavioural information68, similar to mouse V1. Interestingly, a recent study suggested that neural coding in V1 and V2 was optimized for sensory representation, whereas that in the MT was optimized for visual discrimination69. Ruff and Cohen have reported that visual attention further optimizes stimulus representation by MT neurons by aligning it to the axis of the activity space, which is important for guiding behaviour70. It is tempting to speculate that the difference in the site of orthogonalization in the marmoset and the mouse is related to the difference in the depth of the hierarchical cortical network in the two species: the marmoset with a deep cortical network may benefit initially parallel spontaneous activity, whereas the mouse with a shallow cortical network benefit more by having orthogonalized spontaneous activity already at V1. Nevertheless, despite the species difference in circuit-level implementations, the orthogonalization of activity patterns is likely a common computational goal for separating sensory signals and internal noise in biological neural circuits.
Methods
Animals
A total of 12 adult common marmosets (Callithrix jacchus; 7 males and 5 females; body weight, 280–350 g; age, 1–2 years) obtained from Nihon Clea and four Thy1-GCaMP6 mice (4 males; GP 4.3, JAX#024275; body weight, 28–35 g; age, P50–60) obtained from Jackson Laboratory were used. All animals were housed in a 12:12 h light-dark cycle, had access to water and food ad libitum and were not used for other experiments before the present study. All animal experiments were carried out following the institutional welfare guidelines laid down by the Animal Care and Use Committee of the University of Tokyo, and approved by the Ethical Committee of the University of Tokyo. Of the 12 marmosets, 10 were used to obtain widefield imaging data [Large FOV covering the occipital-parietal cortex, two animals; V1/V2, three animals; MT, four animals; Frontal cortex, one animal], and nine were used to obtain two-photon imaging data [V1, 12 FOVs in three animals; V2, 23 FOVs in four animals; MT, 11 FOVs in four animals]. Four mice were used to obtain the two-photon imaging data (12 FOVs in V1).
Plasmid construction
pAAV-Thy1S-tTA and pAAV-TRE-GCaMP6f-WPRE were kindly provided by Dr. T. Yamamori25. All newly designed GCaMP expression plasmids were constructed using pAAV-TRE-GCaMP6f-WPRE as a template. For the generation of pAAV-TRE-GCaMP6s-P2A-GCaMP6s-WPRE (tandem GCaMP6s), gene fragments of GCaMP6s and P2A were obtained from pGP-CMV-GCaMP6s-WPRE (which was a gift from Douglas Kim & GENIE Project [Addgene plasmid # 40753; http://n2t.net/addgene:40753; RRID: Addgene_40753]), and pAAV-hSyn1-GCaMP6s-P2A-nls-dTomato (which was a gift from Jonathan Ting [Addgene plasmid # 51084; http://n2t.net/addgene:51084; RRID: Addgene_51084]), respectively. pAAV-TRE-GCaMP6f-WPRE was used as a template for this plasmid.
Virus Production
AAV plasmids were packaged into AAV serotype 9 using the AAV Helper-Free system (Agilent Technologies). In brief, pAAV vector, pRC9, and pHelper plasmids were transfected into HEK293 cells. Seventy-two hours after transfection, AAV2/9 particles were purified using the AAV Purification kit (Takara, Japan). The AAV solution was concentrated to the optimal volume by centrifugation using an Amicon Ultra-4 100k centrifugal filter unit (Millipore). The number of genomic copies was quantified with intercalating dyes (Thermo Fisher Scientific, MA, USA) and two sets of primers for WPRE or hGHpA genes, using LightCycler 480 (Roche, Basel, Switzerland). The final titration of the AAV was estimated as relative quantitation according to a calibration curve calculated from the known numbers of copies of AAV plasmids.
Virus injection
All surgical procedures were performed under aseptic conditions. Marmosets were anaesthetized with isoflurane (4.0–5.0% for induction and 1.5–3.0% for maintenance in a mixture of 20–50% O2 and air). Throughout surgery, percutaneous oxygen saturation (SpO2) and heart rate were monitored and maintained at >96% and <200 bpm, respectively. The rectal temperature was maintained at 37 °C using an electric blanket and a feedback-controlled heating pad. Before surgery, cefovecin (14 mg/kg, i.m.) or ampicillin (15–20 mg/kg, s.c.) was administered as an antibiotic prophylaxis. Meloxicam (0.1–0.2 mg/kg, s.c.) was administered to reduce pain and inflammation. The scalp was sterilized with povidone-iodine. All incision sites were pre-treated with local injections of lidocaine HCl (2%) or lidocaine jelly. After the scalp incision, a custom-made metal head post was attached to the skull using dental acrylic (Shofu Inc.). Marmosets were then head-fixed on a custom-made metal stage using a head post.
For AAV injections, small craniotomies (1–2 mm in diameter, 4–12 sites, 1–2 mm apart) were made around the targeted cortical regions, and the dura mater was cut to expose the cortical surface. The brain was washed with gentamycin-mixed artificial cerebrospinal fluid (ACSF). An AAV cocktail with a 1:1 mixture of AAV2/9-Thy1S-tTA (3.11 × 1013 vg/ml) and AAV2/9-TRE-tandem-GCaMP6s (2.65 × 1013 vg/ml) was loaded into a glass pipette and injected using a Nanoject III (Drummond Scientific Company). The pipette was inserted 300–500 µm below the cortical surface, and 0.5–1.0 µl AAV was injected at 0.12 µl/min in a single injection site. After the injection, the craniotomies were covered with Kwik-SilTM (World Precision Instruments), the head post was detached, and the scalp was sutured. All wound sites were treated with gentamycin ointment. After the surgery, warmed physiological saline (5 ml) was administered subcutaneously to prevent dehydration, and the animals were returned to their home cage for recovery. The next day, meloxicam and/or ampicillin were/was administered for postoperative management. The animals were maintained for 4–5 weeks before the imaging experiments to ensure sufficient GCaMP expression.
Preparation for imaging experiments
The marmosets were treated with atropine sulphate (0.2 mg/kg, i.m.) and anesthetized with a mixture of ketamine (45 mg/kg, i.m.) and xylazine (3.0 mg/kg, i.m.). SpO2, heart rate, and rectal temperature were monitored as previously described71 (Hashimoto et al., in preparation). Meloxicam (0.1–0.2 mg/kg, s.c.) was administered as an anti-inflammatory drug. A tracheotomy was performed, and the animal was mechanically ventilated. End-tidal CO2 was monitored and kept at 3.4–4.0% throughout the experiment. An intravenous catheter was placed in the femoral vein, and anaesthesia was maintained by constant infusion of remifentanil (6.0–15.0 μg/kg/h, i.v.) mixed in lactated Ringer’s solution (2.0 ml/kg/h) and dexamethasone (0.4 mg/kg/h). Muscle relaxation was induced by vecuronium bromide (0.1 mg/kg/h, i.v.). Additional doses of isoflurane (2.0%–3.0%) and N2O (50% in O2 and air) were administered intraoperatively. A custom-made metal head post was attached to the skull, and the animal was mounted on a stereotaxic apparatus. A large cranial window was made over the targeted brain regions (12 mm diameter circle or 8 mm × 16 mm square, depending on the targeted brain regions), and the dura mater was opened. A glass coverslip (10 mm in diameter or 8 mm × 16 mm square) attached to a custom-made metal rim was placed on the exposed cortical surface, and the brain was sealed with Kwik-SilTM (World Precision Instruments) and dental cement (Sun Medical). During imaging, anaesthesia was maintained by remifentanil (6.0–15.0 μg/kg/h, i.v.), and doses of isoflurane and N2O were decreased to 0–0.5% and 10–50%, respectively. The eyes were covered with contact lenses and frequently moistened with an ophthalmic solution to ensure a clear view throughout the experiment.
Widefield Ca2+ imaging
In vivo wide-field imaging was performed using a macrozoom fluorescence microscope (MVX10, Olympus) equipped with a 2× objective (2x MVX Plan Apochromat Lens, NA 0.25, Olympus). GCaMP was excited by a mercury lamp through a GFP mirror unit (U-MGFPHQ/XL, Olympus, excitation peak: 488 nm, emission peak: 507 nm). Fluorescence images were acquired using an sCMOS camera (Zyla 4.2 sCMOS, Andor Technology) controlled by NIS Elements BR (Nikon). A square region of the cortex (6 × 6 mm to 15 × 15 mm, 512 × 512 or 256 × 256 pixels) was imaged at 5 Hz.
Two-photon Ca2+ imaging
After widefield imaging, the animals were moved under a two-photon microscope (A1RMP, Nikon, Tokyo, Japan) equipped with a water immersion objective (16× or 25× with NA 0.8 or NA 1.1, respectively, Nikon) to perform two-photon calcium imaging. GCaMP was excited at a 920-nm wavelength using a Ti:sapphire laser (Mai Tai HP DeepSee, Spectra-Physics). A square region of the cortex (800 μm × 800 μm in 512 × 512 pixels with a 16× objective for marmosets; 500 μm × 500 μm in 512 × 512 pixels with a 25× objective for mice) was imaged at 2 Hz. The position of the FOV was selected from the functional map obtained using widefield imaging. The spatial pattern of blood vessels on the cortical surface was used to guide the FOV. The depth of the imaged plane was carefully adjusted manually between the scans. Image planes from the same cortical location were separated by at least 30 μm in depth to avoid imaging the same neurons twice.
Visual stimulation
Visual stimuli were generated using custom-written programs in PsychoPy72. A 32-inch LCD monitor with a 60-Hz refresh rate (ME32B, Samsung) was positioned 30 cm in front of the marmosets. The stimulus screen spanned −35.6° to 35.6° horizontal and −50.7° to 50.7° vertical of the animal’s visual field. The stimuli were presented to both eyes. For mapping orientation preferences, a drifting square-wave grating [100% contrast; 0.5–1 cycle per degree (cpd); 2 Hz] tilted at one of four to eight orientations in equal steps, moving in one of two directions orthogonal to the orientation was presented. To map orientation preferences, we presented a drifting square-wave grating [100% contrast; 0.5–1 cpd; 2 Hz] tilting at one of four to eight orientations in equal steps and moving in one of two directions orthogonal to the orientation. For mapping direction preferences, random dots 100% coherence; dot size, 0.5 °; speed, [20 °/s] moving in one of eight or twelve directions in equal steps were used. Each stimulus started with a blank period of uniform grey (4 s) followed by the same period of visual stimulation. Each condition was repeated 20–100 times in pseudorandom orders.
In both the widefield and two-photon imaging experiments, spontaneous activity was recorded separately from the visually evoked responses in the dedicated scans. During scans for spontaneous activity recordings, the LCD monitor used for visual stimulation was turned off. For each FOV, we conducted a spontaneous activity scan either before or after the visual stimulus scans. The interval between the two scans was no longer than 30 min.
Widefield imaging data analysis
All analyses were performed using custom-written programs in MATLAB (MathWorks, Natick, MA, USA). For widefield imaging data, small drifts between the imaging frames were realigned by maximizing the correlation between the images. The relative change in fluorescence (ΔF/F) was computed using the following equation: ΔF/F = (F − F0)/F0. For data with visual stimulation, F0 is the average fluorescence during the pre-stimulus period (baseline), and F is the fluorescence during stimulus presentation. For spontaneous activity data, F0 is the average across the entire scan, and F is the fluorescence during spontaneous activity. To compute trial-averaged responses, images or time courses were sorted by stimulus conditions and averaged across all repetitions.
The method for calculating orientation preference maps and direction preference maps has been described elsewhere71. Briefly, single-condition maps to the tested orientation (or directions of motion) stimuli were obtained by calculating the relative change in fluorescence (ΔF/F) between the baseline (1 s before stimulus onset) and the stimulus period (4 s). Each map was high-pass filtered by subtracting the low spatial frequency background, which was obtained by applying 2D median filters three times (kernel size: 0.45 mm by side) to the original map. Direction preference maps (HLS maps) were obtained from single-condition maps. In the HLS map, hue (H) represents the preferred orientation or direction calculated by vector averaging, saturation (S) represents the global measure of orientation or direction tuning, which corresponds to 1 − CV (circular variance in orientation or direction preference), and lightness (L) represents the response to the best direction (ΔF/F).
Movies of spontaneous activity are shown in ΔF/F, calculated as described above. For our analysis of patchy spontaneous activity in widefield imaging, we followed procedures commonly employed in previous studies, namely spatial filtering and PCA9,34. We first down-sampled the images from 512 × 512 pixels to 256 × 256 pixels, followed by spatial high-pass filtering (σ = 180 μm for occipital-parietal FOV, and σ = 280 μm for V1/V2 and MT FOV). We consider that the use of high-pass filtering did not artificially create patchy spatial patterns; first, we used high-pass filters instead of bandpass filters. Whereas bandpass filters may artificially bias patchy patterns by selectively suppressing both low and high spatial frequency components, high-pass filters are less likely to have such a bias toward a particular frequency band. Second, patchy spatial patterns that resemble the spatial patterns of the orientation columns are readily visible in the data without high-pass filtering (Supplementary Movies 1–4). To obtain underlying spatial patterns from the data in an unbiased manner, PCA was then applied to the filtered images. PCs clearly showing the spatial patterns of blood vessels were discarded (typically three to five PCs in the first 20 PCs; see Supplementary Fig. 13 for the top 20 PCs for example data). To estimate the spontaneous patch size (Supplementary Fig. 2) for each FOV, we first searched for a spontaneous PC that best correlated with the visually evoked activity. We chose to analyse widefield imaging data covering V1 and V2 in the same FOV because these data allowed us to compare the two cortical areas without being affected by differences in animals and/or other experimental conditions. Autocorrelation analysis was performed separately for V1 and V2, and the sizes of the two autocorrelation profiles were compared. Because this analysis used widefield imaging data obtained from one FOV per animal, we did not have sufficient data to perform statistical testing.
Two-photon imaging data analysis
For the cell-level analysis, cell bodies were automatically identified by template matching with a circular template using high-pass filtered images of the temporal variance of fluorescence signals (Fig. 2b). Time courses of individual cells were extracted by summing pixel values within the contours of cell bodies and then converted to ΔF/F using a percentile filter with a sliding window (baseline, lower 30 percentile; window size, 24.5 s), removing the slow drift of signals. For the data obtained from transgenic mice, we additionally subtracted background signals to minimize contamination of neuropil signals73. For each cell, the background signal time course was obtained by averaging the pixels surrounding the cell body, and then, the background signal time course was subtracted from the cell’s time course after multiplying by a scaling factor (0.7). In a control analysis that accounted for a potential SNR difference between mice and marmosets due to the difference in magnification (Supplementary Fig. 11), we set the number of pixels allocated to each cell to 100 for both mice and marmosets.
Visual responses were calculated using the time-course averaged across trials (repetitions). The response to each orientation condition was defined as the mean of the responses to two drifting gratings moving in opposing directions orthogonal to the orientation. The response to each direction condition was defined as the mean of the responses to gratings or dots moving in the direction specified by the condition. For the analysis of the similarity of spatial patterns of spontaneous activity and visually evoked activities (Fig. 2g), single-condition response maps to each orientation and direction were calculated as described previously21 and then correlated with each frames of spontaneous activity. The cellular orientation map (Fig. 2b) was obtained by calculating the preferred orientation for each neuron using vector averaging of the orientation responses71. To compare visually evoked and spontaneous activity patterns, we selected FOVs (1) with stable recordings of both visual stimulation scan and spontaneous activity scan and (2) with sufficiently large number of active cells (>100).
To analyse the clustering of spontaneously active neurons (Supplementary Fig. 3), we adopted the clustering index used in previous studies71,74. For each spontaneous frame, we calculated the median of absolute pairwise difference of spontaneous activity between pairs of neurons across all the pairs that were located within 400 μm (orientation column spacing in the marmoset V130). Then, we did the same calculation for the position-shuffled control. The clustering index for the plane was defined as the value obtained for the position-shuffled control divided by the value obtained for the real data.
Neuronal activity was decomposed into orthogonal subspaces as described by Stringer et al.13. We specifically used the procedure described by Stringer et al. to analyse spontaneous activity, which did not use any behavioural information. Visual stimulation and spontaneous activity scans were obtained separately for each group of neurons within the same FOV. We projected the visually evoked and spontaneous activities into three orthogonal subspaces: stimulus-only space, shared space, and spontaneous-only space, using the following procedures (Fig. 3a). The fraction of single-trial stimulus-evoked response explained by the trial-averaged neural activity was estimated using cross-validation. Half of the trials were used to make the predictor (i.e., trial-averaged neural activity), and the other half were used to estimate the trial-to-trial variance. In the analysis in which we controlled for the number of cells and trials (Supplementary Fig. 12b–c), we set the number of cells and trials to be equal across all FOVs by randomly selecting 400 cells and 20 trials in each FOV.
For each visual stimulation scan, neural activity was averaged across trials. We treated each frame of the trial-averaged visual evoked response as an individual visual response pattern (frames corresponding to the stimulus-off periods were discarded). Depending on the number of orientations/directions tested, the total number of trial-averaged visual responses for each set of stimuli ranged from 32 to 64. The frames in which visual stimuli were presented were then collected and denoted as trial-averaged visual responses, producing a matrix of trial-averaged visually evoked activity with dimension . For cross-validation, we used half of the trials to obtain , and spared the other half of the trials for later calculation of the projected variance of the visually evoked activity. Spontaneous activity , which is a matrix was subjected to PCA, and the first 50 PCs were retained (). Note that, for each FOV, was the same for the visually evoked activity and spontaneous activity. The stimulus-only activity was obtained by regressing out 50 spontaneous PCs from each column of the trial-averaged visual response, . Thus, each column in is orthogonal to the spontaneous PCs.
1 |
We set the number of spontaneous PCs to 50 for the following reasons. In the spontaneous activity scans, the number of frames (>1000) exceeded the number of cells (<1000). Therefore, the full dimensions of the spontaneous space are equal to the number of cells. Because the visual responses were recorded in the same set of cells, if we used whole spontaneous space, the entire variance of the visual responses would be projected onto the spontaneous space. Thus, we limited the spontaneous space to smaller dimensions. To set the number of PCs to use, we referred to a previous study by Stringer et al., where they used the first 128 dimensions of spontaneous PCs for the recording of ~10000 cells. Because the number of cells recorded in our study was approximately 1000 at most (~500 on average), we reasoned that 50 PCs would be sufficient to construct a shared space. To confirm that the present results did not depend on the exact number of PCs used, we conducted the same analyses with 100 spontaneous PCs (Supplementary Fig. 14). The matrix of shared activities was obtained by projecting each column of the trial-averaged visual responses onto 50 spontaneous PCs. Thus, each column of the shared activity is contained in the space spanned by 50 spontaneous PCs:
2 |
The matrix of spontaneous-only activity was obtained by regressing out each column of the trial-averaged visual responses from each column (i.e., frame) of spontaneous activity . Thus, each column of the spontaneous-only activity was orthogonal to each column of the visually evoked activity :
3 |
Because the number of trial-averaged visual responses was smaller than the number of recorded cells in each FOV (>100), visual response space did not encompass full neural activity space which has the number of dimensions equal to the number of recorded neurons. Thus, we projected out all the trial-averaged visual responses instead of projecting out some PCs of the trial-averaged responses. The PCs of each subspace (Fig. 3b) were obtained by applying singular value decomposition or PCA, and the PCs of the shared space were obtained by finding a sequence of orthogonal directions that maximized the sum of squares of the trial averaged visual response :
4 |
such that
5 |
and
6 |
and
7 |
This was calculated as the left singular vector of the shared activity . PCs of stim-only space were similarly obtained as the left singular vector of the stimulus-only activity . The PCs of the spont-only space were obtained as the principal components of the spont-only activity . For the estimation of the dimensionality of Shared Space, we counted the number of PCs that explained larger than 3% of the variance. The same threshold (3% of variance) was used to obtain PCs of the three subspaces for projecting single-trial stimulus-evoked activities (Fig. 4c). Note that the choice of 50 PCs at the initial step for constructing Shared Space could have resulted in underestimation of the number of remaining PCs in each subspace.
The similarity between each PC and the visual responses (i.e. single-condition responses) was calculated using a cell-wise correlation. The amount of stimulus-related variance projected onto the shared space was calculated as the stimulus-related variance projected to the space spanned by the first 50 spontaneous PCs that were used to calculate the shared space. Trial-averaged visual responses were projected onto the first 50 spontaneous PCs , and the sum of the squared projection lengths was calculated (Fig. 4a). To quantify the amount of variance in the stimulus-related activity projected onto each shared PC, we used the held-out half of the visual stimulation trials, which were not used to calculate the shared PC. The trial-averaged responses calculated using the held-out trials were projected onto each shared PC, and the squared projection length was used as the projected variance on the shared PC (Fig. 4b).
For the projection of single trial activity to subspaces (Fig. 4c), we used PCs of the three subspaces obtained as described above (note that PCs with greater than 3 % explained variance were used). The mean number of remaining PCs for Evoked-Only and Sponta-Only spaces were as follows: V1: 1.1, 2.4; V2 1.6, 2.4; MT, 3.2, 2.1; Mouse V1: 4.7, 2.5 [Area Name: mean number of remaining Evoked-Only PCs, mean number of remaining Sponta-Only PCs]. Neural activity in each frame of the held-out trials of the visual stimulation scans (with the blank period discarded) and spontaneous activity scans were projected onto the PCs of the three subspaces. For each subspace, the sum of the squared projected lengths was calculated and regarded as the amount of variance projected onto the subspace. The variances projected onto the three subspaces were calculated for each scan and normalized to the total variance, which was the sum across frames. Note that the projected variance on sponta-only PC and stim-only PC was non-zero in the visual stimulation block and the spontaneous block, respectively (Fig. 4c). Because single-trial visually evoked response not only contains visually evoked components but also non-visual (“spontaneous”) components in an additive manner13,15, trial-to-trial variability of single-trial visually evoked responses in our data likely contained both visually related variability and spontaneous activity-related components. We believe that some of the latter components were projected onto the sponta-only PC. Regarding the non-zero projected variance on stim-only PCs in the spontaneous block, we believe that this was because we projected out the top 50 PCs of spontaneous activity, instead of the full PCs, to define stim-only space. The residual PCs of spontaneous activity that were not used to define stim-only space could have contributed to the projection to the stim-only space.
Statistics
Statistical tests were conducted using Statistics Toolbox in Matlab. Independent group comparisons were performed using two-sample Kolmogorov-Smirnov tests, Wilcoxon rank-sum test, and two-sample t tests. No statistical methods were used to pre-determine sample sizes, but our sample sizes were similar to those used in previous studies. Allocation in the experimental groups was not randomized. Data collection and analysis were not blinded to experimental conditions.
Reporting summary
Further information on research design is available in the Nature Portfolio Reporting Summary linked to this article.
Supplementary information
Source data
Acknowledgements
We thank members of the Ohki laboratory and Dr. M. Kaschube for helpful discussions; A. Hayashi, Y. Kato, M. Taki, T. Inoue, Y. Sono, T. Honda for animal care; N. Yamamoto and T. Ohmine for lab administration. Drs. T. Yamamori, A. Watakabe for providing pAAV-Thy1S-tTA and pAAV-TRE-GCaMP6f; Drs. T. Ebina, Y. Masamizu, M. Matsuizaki for technical advice; The Genetically-Encoded Neuronal Indicator and Effector (GENIE) Project for providing GCaMP6f/s. This work was supported by Brain/MINDS and Brain/MINDS2.0 from AMED (14533320, JP16dm0207034, JP20dm0207048, JP21dm0207014, JP23wm0625001, JP24wm0625203 to K.O., JP21dm0207111 to Dr. H. Hirai and the Brain/MINDS AAV vector core); Institute for AI and Beyond (to K.O.); CREST-JST (JPMJCR22P1 to K.O.), JSPS KAKENHI (25221001, 19H05642, 20H05917, to K.O., 17K14931, 20H05052, 21H0516513, 24H02331 to T. Matsui, 19K21207, 21K15181, 23K27279, 23H04663 to T. Murakami); JSPS Research Fellowship for Young Scientist (20J12796 to T.H.), Brain/MINDS-beyond from AMED (JP20dm0307031 to T. Matsui); JST PRESTO (JPMJPR19M9 to T. Matsui); JST FOREST (JPMJFR224H to T. Murakami).
Author contributions
T. Matsui., T.H., T. Murakami and K.O. conceived the project. M.U. developed and provided GCaMP expression systems optimized for primates. T. Matsui. T.H., T. Murakami, M.U., K.K., and T.K. performed in vivo experiments. T. Matsui, T.H., T. Murakami, and K.O. analyzed the data. T.H., K.O and T. Matsui performed simulations. All authors discussed the results. T. Matsui, and K.O. wrote the manuscript with contributions from all authors. T.Matsui, T.H., T. Murakami, and M.U. contributed equally to this work.
Peer review
Peer review information
Nature Communications thanks Jorrit Montijn and the other, anonymous, reviewer(s) for their contribution to the peer review of this work. A peer review file is available.
Data availability
Minimum dataset that are necessary to interpret, verify and extend the research in the article are available from the corresponding authors. Example data can be downloaded in FigShare (doi:10.6084/m9.figshare.25448167). Source data are provided with this paper.
Code availability
The codes used in this study are available from the corresponding authors. Example codes can be downloaded in GitHub (https://github.com/teppei-matsui/NN_Simulation).
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Footnotes
Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
These authors contributed equally: Teppei Matsui, Takayuki Hashimoto, Tomonari Murakami, Masato Uemura.
Contributor Information
Teppei Matsui, Email: tematsui@mail.doshisha.ac.jp.
Takayuki Hashimoto, Email: hashimoto@m.u-tokyo.ac.jp.
Kenichi Ohki, Email: kohki@m.u-tokyo.ac.jp.
Supplementary information
The online version contains supplementary material available at 10.1038/s41467-024-54322-x.
References
- 1.Fox, M. D. & Raichle, M. E. Spontaneous fluctuations in brain activity observed with functional magnetic resonance imaging. Nat. Rev. Neurosci.8, 700–711 (2007). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Arieli, A., Sterkin, A., Grinvald, A. & Aertsen, A. Dynamics of ongoing activity: Explanation of the large variability in evoked cortical responses. Science273, 1868–1871 (1996). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Kenet, T., Bibitchkov, D., Tsodyks, M., Grinvald, A. & Arieli, A. Spontaneously emerging cortical representations of visual attributes. Nature425, 954–956 (2003). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Tsodyks, M., Kenet, T., Grinvald, A. & Arieli, A. Linking spontaneous activity of single cortical neurons and the underlying functional architecture. Science286, 1943–1946 (1999). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Vincent, J. L. et al. Intrinsic functional architecture in the anaesthetized monkey brain. Nature447, 83–86 (2007). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Omer, D. B., Fekete, T., Ulchin, Y., Hildesheim, R. & Grinvald, A. Dynamic Patterns of Spontaneous Ongoing Activity in the Visual Cortex of Anesthetized and Awake Monkeys are Different. Cereb. Cortex29, 1291–1304 (2019). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Smith, S. M. et al. Correspondence of the brain’s functional architecture during activation and rest. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA106, 13040–13045 (2009). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Tavor, I. et al. Task-free MRI predicts individual differences in brain activity during task performance. Science352, 216–220 (2016). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Smith, G. B., Hein, B., Whitney, D. E., Fitzpatrick, D. & Kaschube, M. Distributed network interactions and their emergence in developing neocortex. Nat. Neurosci.21, 1600–1608 (2018). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Luczak, A., Barthó, P. & Harris, K. D. Spontaneous events outline the realm of possible sensory responses in neocortical populations. Neuron62, 413–425 (2009). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Carrillo-Reid, L. & Yuste, R. Playing the piano with the cortex: Role of neuronal ensembles and pattern completion in perception and behavior. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol.64, 89–95 (2020). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Leopold, D. A. & Maier, A. Ongoing physiological processes in the cerebral cortex. Neuroimage62, 2190–2200 (2012). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Stringer, C. et al. Spontaneous behaviors drive multidimensional, brainwide activity. Science364, 255 (2019). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Rumyantsev, O. I. et al. Fundamental bounds on the fidelity of sensory cortical coding. Nature580, 100–105 (2020). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Musall, S., Kaufman, M. T., Juavinett, A. L., Gluf, S. & Churchland, A. K. Single-trial neural dynamics are dominated by richly varied movements. Nat. Neurosci.22, 1677–1686 (2019). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Moreno-Bote, R. et al. Information-limiting correlations. Nat. Neurosci.17, 1410–1417 (2014). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Fiser, J., Chiu, C. & Weliky, M. Small modulation of ongoing cortical dynamics by sensory input during natural vision. Nature431, 573–578 (2004). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Berkes, P., Orbán, G., Lengyel, M. & Fiser, J. Spontaneous cortical activity reveals hallmarks of an optimal internal model of the environment. Science331, 83–87 (2011). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Hubel, D. H. & Wiesel, T. N. Shape and arrangement of columns in cat’s striate cortex. J. Physiol.165, 559–568 (1963). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Hubel, D. H. & Wiesel, T. N. Receptive fields and functional architecture of monkey striate cortex. J. Physiol.195, 215–243 (1968). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Ohki, K., Chung, S., Ch’ng, Y. H., Kara, P. & Reid, R. C. Functional imaging with cellular resolution reveals precise micro-architecture in visual cortex. Nature433, 597–603 (2005). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Mrsic-Flogel, T. D. et al. Homeostatic regulation of eye-specific responses in visual cortex during ocular dominance plasticity. Neuron54, 961–972 (2007). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Mitchell, J. F. & Leopold, D. A. The marmoset monkey as a model for visual neuroscience. Neurosci. Res93, 20–46 (2015). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Solomon, S. G. & Rosa, M. G. A simpler primate brain: The visual system of the marmoset monkey. Front Neural Circuits8, 96 (2014). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Sadakane, O. et al. Long-term two-photon calcium imaging of neuronal populations with subcellular resolution in adult non-human primates. Cell Rep.13, 1989–1999 (2015). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Ebina, T. et al. Arm movements induced by noninvasive optogenetic stimulation of the motor cortex in the common marmoset. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA116, 22844–22850 (2019). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Yamada, Y., Matsumoto, Y., Okahara, N. & Mikoshiba, K. Chronic multiscale imaging of neuronal activity in the awake common marmoset. Sci. Rep.6, 35722 (2016). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Park, J. E. et al. Generation of transgenic marmosets expressing genetically encoded calcium indicators. Sci. Rep.6, 34931 (2016). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Zeng, H. H. et al. Local homogeneity of tonotopic organization in the primary auditory cortex of marmosets. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA116, 3239–3244 (2019). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Roe, A. W., Fritsches, K. & Pettigrew, J. D. Optical imaging of functional organization of V1 and V2 in marmoset visual cortex. Anat. Rec. A Discov. Mol. Cell Evol. Biol.287, 1213–1225 (2005). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Uemura M. et al. Paper presented at the Society for Neuroscience, San Diego, CA (2018).
- 32.Sato, T. K., Nauhaus, I. & Carandini, M. Traveling waves in visual cortex. Neuron75, 218–229 (2012). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Matsui, T., Murakami, T. & Ohki, K. Transient neuronal coactivations embedded in globally propagating waves underlie resting-state functional connectivity. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA113, 6556–6561 (2016). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.O’Hashi, K. et al. Interhemispheric synchrony of spontaneous cortical states at the cortical column level. Cereb. Cortex28, 1794–1807 (2018). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Jeffs, J., Federer, F. & Angelucci, A. Corticocortical connection patterns reveal two distinct visual cortical areas bordering dorsal V2 in marmoset monkey. Vis. Neurosci.32, E012 (2015). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Mitra, A. et al. Spontaneous infra-slow brain activity has unique spatiotemporal dynamics and laminar structure. Neuron98, e296 (2018). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Gilbert, C. D. & Wiesel, T. N. Columnar specificity of intrinsic horizontal and corticocortical connections in cat visual cortex. J. Neurosci.9, 2432–2442 (1989). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Malach, R., Amir, Y., Harel, M. & Grinvald, A. Relationship between intrinsic connections and functional architecture revealed by optical imaging and in vivo targeted biocytin injections in primate striate cortex. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA90, 10469–10473 (1993). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Bosking, W. H., Zhang, Y., Schofield, B. & Fitzpatrick, D. Orientation selectivity and the arrangement of horizontal connections in tree shrew striate cortex. J. Neurosci.17, 2112–2127 (1997). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Tanigawa, H., Wang, Q. & Fujita, I. Organization of horizontal axons in the inferior temporal cortex and primary visual cortex of the macaque monkey. Cereb. Cortex15, 1887–1899 (2005). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Goldman-Rakic, P. S. & Schwartz, M. L. Interdigitation of contralateral and ipsilateral columnar projections to frontal association cortex in primates. Science216, 755–757 (1982). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Cohen, M. R. & Kohn, A. Measuring and interpreting neuronal correlations. Nat. Neurosci.14, 811–819 (2011). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Kondo, S., Yoshida, T. & Ohki, K. Mixed functional microarchitectures for orientation selectivity in the mouse primary visual cortex. Nat. Commun.7, 13210 (2016). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Wilson, D. E., Whitney, D. E., Scholl, B. & Fitzpatrick, D. Orientation selectivity and the functional clustering of synaptic inputs in primary visual cortex. Nat. Neurosci.19, 1003–1009 (2016). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Chen, T. W. et al. Ultrasensitive fluorescent proteins for imaging neuronal activity. Nature499, 295–300 (2013). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Ko, H. et al. Functional specificity of local synaptic connections in neocortical networks. Nature473, 87–91 (2011). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Nauhaus, I., Busse, L., Carandini, M. & Ringach, D. L. Stimulus contrast modulates functional connectivity in visual cortex. Nat. Neurosci.12, 70–76 (2009). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Recanatesi, S., Ocker, G. K., Buice, M. A. & Shea-Brown, E. Dimensionality in recurrent spiking networks: Global trends in activity and local origins in connectivity. PLoS Comput Biol.15, e1006446 (2019). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Dubreuil, A., Valente, A., Beiran, M., Mastrogiuseppe, F. & Ostojic, S. The role of population structure in computations through neural dynamics. Nat. Neurosci.25, 783–794 (2022). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Avitan, L. et al. Spontaneous and evoked activity patterns diverge over development. Elife10:e61942 (2021). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 51.Okun, M. et al. Population rate dynamics and multineuron firing patterns in sensory cortex. J. Neurosci.32, 17108–17119 (2012). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.McAdams, C. J. & Maunsell, J. H. Effects of attention on orientation-tuning functions of single neurons in macaque cortical area V4. J. Neurosci.19, 431–441 (1999). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Reynolds, J. H. & Heeger, D. J. The normalization model of attention. Neuron61, 168–185 (2009). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Freeman, J. & Simoncelli, E. P. Metamers of the ventral stream. Nat. Neurosci.14, 1195–1201 (2011). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Murray, J. D. et al. A hierarchy of intrinsic timescales across primate cortex. Nat. Neurosci.17, 1661–1663 (2014). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Kiani, R. et al. Natural grouping of neural responses reveals spatially segregated clusters in prearcuate cortex. Neuron85, 1359–1373 (2015). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Averbeck, B. B., Latham, P. E. & Pouget, A. Neural correlations, population coding and computation. Nat. Rev. Neurosci.7, 358–366 (2006). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Shamir, M. & Sompolinsky, H. Implications of neuronal diversity on population coding. Neural Comput18, 1951–1986 (2006). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Zohary, E., Shadlen, M. N. & Newsome, W. T. Correlated neuronal discharge rate and its implications for psychophysical performance. Nature370, 140–143 (1994). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Abbott, L. F. & Dayan, P. The effect of correlated variability on the accuracy of a population code. Neural Comput11, 91–101 (1999). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Oram, M. W., Földiák, P., Perrett, D. I. & Sengpiel, F. The ‘Ideal Homunculus’: decoding neural population signals. Trends Neurosci.21, 259–265 (1998). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Ecker, A. S., Berens, P., Tolias, A. S. & Bethge, M. The effect of noise correlations in populations of diversely tuned neurons. J. Neurosci.31, 14272–14283 (2011). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Sompolinsky, H., Yoon, H., Kang, K. & Shamir, M. Population coding in neuronal systems with correlated noise. Phys. Rev. E Stat. Nonlin Soft Matter Phys.64, 051904 (2001). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Montijn, J. S., Meijer, G. T., Lansink, C. S. & Pennartz, C. M. Population-level neural codes are robust to single-neuron variability from a multidimensional coding perspective. Cell Rep.16, 2486–2498 (2016). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Semedo, J. D., Zandvakili, A., Machens, C. K., Yu, B. M. & Kohn, A. Cortical areas interact through a communication subspace. Neuron102, 249–259.e244 (2019). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Han, F., Caporale, N. & Dan, Y. Reverberation of recent visual experience in spontaneous cortical waves. Neuron60, 321–327 (2008). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Koren, V. & Denève, S. Computational account of spontaneous activity as a signature of predictive coding. PLoS Comput Biol.13, e1005355 (2017). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Snyder, L. H., Grieve, K. L., Brotchie, P. & Andersen, R. A. Separate body- and world-referenced representations of visual space in parietal cortex. Nature394, 887–891 (1998). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Manning, T. S. et al. Transformations of sensory information in the brain reflect a changing definition of optimality. PLoS Comput Biol.20, e1011783 (2024). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 70.Ruff, D. A. & Cohen, M. R. Simultaneous multi-area recordings suggest that attention improves performance by reshaping stimulus representations. Nat. Neurosci.22, 1669–1676 (2019). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71.Nishiyama, M., Matsui, T., Murakami, T., Hagihara, K. M. & Ohki, K. Cell-type-specific thalamocortical inputs constrain direction map formation in visual cortex. Cell Rep.26, 1082–1088.e1083 (2019). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72.Peirce, J. W. Generating stimuli for neuroscience using PsychoPy. Front Neuroinform2, 10 (2008). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73.Ohtsuki, G. et al. Similarity of visual selectivity among clonally related neurons in visual cortex. Neuron75, 65–72 (2012). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74.Martin, K. A. & Schröder, S. Functional heterogeneity in neighboring neurons of cat primary visual cortex in response to both artificial and natural stimuli. J. Neurosci.33, 7325–7344 (2013). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 75.Paxinos, G., Watson, C., Petrides, M., Rosa, M. & Tokuno, H. The marmoset brain in stereotaxic coordinates, pp. 324 (Academic Publisher, Cambridge, Massachusetts, 2011).
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
Minimum dataset that are necessary to interpret, verify and extend the research in the article are available from the corresponding authors. Example data can be downloaded in FigShare (doi:10.6084/m9.figshare.25448167). Source data are provided with this paper.
The codes used in this study are available from the corresponding authors. Example codes can be downloaded in GitHub (https://github.com/teppei-matsui/NN_Simulation).