Skip to main content
PLOS ONE logoLink to PLOS ONE
. 2024 Dec 5;19(12):e0311794. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0311794

Micronutrients absorbed via the oral mucosa reduce emotion dysregulation in 5-10-year-old children: A three-phased randomized wait-list-controlled trial

Nurina M Katta 1, Neville M Blampied 1, Matt Eggleston 2, Julia J Rucklidge 1,*
Editor: Ebenezer Wiafe3
PMCID: PMC11620378  PMID: 39636814

Abstract

Objective

Previous evidence has established that micronutrient capsules can improve emotion regulation in children. This three-phased randomized open-label waitlist-controlled study investigated the safety of a micronutrient powder absorbed by the oral mucosa and its effects on emotion dysregulation in 5-to-10-year-old children. The primary outcome measures were the Revised Clinician-rated Temper and Irritability Scale (CL-ARI) and the Clinical Global Impressions-Improvement Scale (CGI-I).

Method

Forty-eight children with moderate-to-severe symptoms of irritability were randomized to an initial treatment group (ITG) or waitlist control group (IWLG) (four-week delayed start), followed by the two groups alternating between taking the micronutrients for four weeks or having a four-week break. For the last three months of the trial, both groups took the micronutrients continuously.

Results

Overall adherence rates were high (93%). At the end of RCT phase, there were large group differences (CL-ARI; d = 1.25, p < .001), and 67% in the ITG and 8% in the IWLG were ‘much’ or ‘very much’ improved (CGI-I). Further, the ITG displayed a clinically meaningful reduction in Attention Deficit/Hyperactivity Impulsivity Disorder (ADHD) and Oppositional Defiant Disorder (ODD) symptoms as measured with the Child Swanson, Nolan, and Pelham-IV Questionnaire 26 (SNAP-IV) compared to IWLG. The treatment effect regressed when participants stopped taking the micronutrients and was reinstated when participants were taking the micronutrients. The observed benefits were maintained over a sustained time period. The IWLG reported significantly more headaches (p = .040) and sweating (p = .037) at the end of RCT. By the end of the study, seven participants (14.5%) dropped out non-differentially by group (p = .22).

Conclusion

The findings showed that micronutrients absorbed by the oral mucosa are a safe intervention that can effectively improve emotion dysregulation in children. Future double-blinded, randomized, placebo-controlled trials are needed to support these findings.

Introduction

Emotion dysregulation is the maladaptive management of emotional responses that is expressed in rapid and uncontrolled shifts in emotion inappropriate to the situation, age, and developmental level [13]. This can interfere with fostering empathy, knowing the difference between internal emotional experiences and the external expression of emotions, and behaving socially appropriately [4, 5].

Clinically, emotion dysregulation is expressed as irritability [1], which is a response to frustration characterised by sustained grumpiness, touchiness, and resentfulness, as well as temper outbursts, intense anger, and occasional aggressive behavior [2, 6, 7]. Chronic emotion dysregulation is associated with a range of adverse psychosocial outcomes, including anxiety and depression, reduced wellbeing and self-esteem, damaged relationships, increased crime, and poor occupational and academic accomplishments [6, 811]. For example, the longitudinal Dunedin cohort study showed that children with undercontrolled temperament at age three were twice as likely to develop a gambling disorder at 21 and 32 relative to children classified as having a well-adjusted temperament [12]. In another study, better self-control skills in children predicted improved physical health as well as reduced substance use disorders and criminal offending in later life [13].

Typically, children make substantial progress in developing effective emotion regulation skills during the first five years of life; hence, emotion dysregulation is most predominant at preschool age and reduces over time [14, 15]. However, around 3% of youth suffer from severe irritability and clinically significant symptoms, with prevalence rates being higher in psychiatric samples [2, 6, 16].

A diagnosis where emotion dysregulation plays an important role is Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD), and for children with ADHD, psychiatric stimulant medication is often prescribed as the main form of treatment [1, 17, 18]. However, more than half of children with ADHD do not report improved symptoms of emotion dysregulation in response to stimulants [19, 20], and overall, the evidence for stimulants improving irritability is limited. Additionally, stimulants have been shown to sometimes increase irritability as an adverse side- or withdrawal effect [19, 2123]. Alternatively, behavioral interventions have effectively treated emotion dysregulation [24]; however, the lack of guidelines on how to apply intervention strategies and barriers to treatment, such as transport issues, stigma, and the shortage of trained clinicians and specialist services can interfere with effective treatment delivery [2527].

Potentially circumventing the challenges of current treatment options, recent evidence has established the role of vitamins and minerals (micronutrients) on improving emotion dysregulation. For instance, micronutrients led to a large and significant reduction in problematic social behavior and conduct problems (symptoms frequently associated with emotion dysregulation) in 14 children with ADHD [28]. Further, an RCT found that micronutrients reduced aggression, emotionally dysregulated mood and behavior, conduct problems, and hyperactivity/impulsivity and inattention to a greater degree than a placebo [29]. Another RCT found 54% of youth with both ADHD and emotion dysregulation issues who took micronutrients for eight weeks improved ‘much’ or ‘very much’, while only 18% did so in the placebo group [30]. However, the product tested in these studies was micronutrient pills, and past evidence has shown that children in particular might find swallowing pills challenging [31, 32]. For example, in one study, medication was rejected in two-thirds of babies and children, or its delivery was unsuccessful [32], interfering with effective treatment.

Recently, a double-blinded RCT trial with 72 stressed university students found that micronutrients improved symptoms of irritability and anger compared to a placebo [33]. The novelty of this study was that it used a dissolvable powder absorbed in the mouth rather than capsules. The high observed adherence to treatment (95.3 to 97.3%) indicated that the powder may be a more convenient way to take micronutrients than capsules. Further, the powder is absorbed by the oral mucosa, a highly vascularized mucous membrane, which allows the micronutrients to enter the blood flow immediately. Compared to absorbing micronutrients via the gut, this mode of delivery avoids first-pass liver metabolism of the micronutrients and potentially impacts the brain more directly, potentially enhancing therapeutic effects [34, 35].

Given irritability is a prevalent issue among children that can have detrimental effects on psychosocial wellbeing, and children can struggle with swallowing capsules, further research into alternative treatment options is warranted. As emotion dysregulation typically represents normative behavior during preschool years, this study aimed at investigating the safety and effectiveness of orally-absorbed micronutrients as an intervention for 5-10-year-old children with moderate-severe symptoms of irritability.

Methods

Study design

This study was designed as a three-phased randomized open-label waitlist-controlled trial. All study procedures were approved by the Northern Health and Disability Ethics Committee (21/NTA/169) and prospectively registered with the Australian New Zealand Clinical Trials Registry: ACTRN12622000162718.

Participants and entry criteria

Participants were recruited nationally across Aotearoa New Zealand through advertisement posts on the Te Puna Toiora Mental Health and Nutrition Facebook page from March 2022 to June 2022. Recruitment ended when the intended sample size was reached. To be eligible, participants had to be between 5 and 10 years of age, live in New Zealand, be fluent in English, and show moderate to severe symptoms of irritability (≥6 on the Affective-Reactivity Index) [36]. Children taking any form of psychiatric medication at the time of or four weeks prior to the study were excluded from participation due to potential interactions with the micronutrients. Caregivers of participants not eligible for the trial were emailed to inform them that they were ineligible and sent information regarding local services they could approach for psychological support. See Fig 1 (CONSORT diagram) for participant flow.

Fig 1. CONSORT flow diagram.

Fig 1

Sample size

The number of participants was based on the What Works Clearinghouse Standards for single-case designs [37, 38], being considered a sufficient sample size to determine whether the treatment is successful in reducing emotion dysregulation with sufficient power at a 5% level of significance. The researchers anticipated a moderate effect size (~d = 0.5) based on previous studies investigating the same intervention (i.e. True Hope Ultimate Sticks), establishing moderate effect sizes on emotion dysregulation [33].

Intervention and dosing

The product tested is called EMPowerplus Truehope Ultimate Sticks (Sticks), which are pouches containing 36 vitamins, minerals, and amino acids in form of a powder and available in banana, sour berry, and tropical punch flavor. Participants were asked to take one Stick per day, putting the micronutrient powder in their mouth and waiting until it was dissolved. It was suggested to not eat or drink anything 10 minutes before and after taking the intervention. The ingredients of the Sticks are shown in S1 Table in the supplemental material.

Study phases

Screening and consent procedure. Potential participants were screened for eligibility by obtaining information regarding their demographics as well as completing measures assessing their children’s emotion dysregulation via an online questionnaire. If children were eligible, participants and their caregivers were met face-to-face or via Zoom to explain the study procedure and, if participants wanted to take part, obtain written informed consent from the caregivers and assent from the children.

Baseline phase. Caregivers completed a baseline assessment online and met with the study coordinator in person at the University of Canterbury or online via Zoom together with their child to complete a variety of clinician-administered measures (see measures below).

RCT phase. Once the baseline assessment had been completed and the participants had been randomized and allocated to either the initial treatment group (ITG) or initial waitlist group (IWLG), they were asked to take the intervention for four weeks or told that they had a four-week delayed start to taking the intervention, respectively.

Immediate Replication Phase (IRP). The IRP used a parallel groups design to detect if potential treatment effects observed in the RCT phase could be replicated and whether accumulation of therapy effects could be detected. During the IRP phases (week 8 and 12), the groups took turns at alternately taking and then stopping the micronutrients every four weeks.

Treatment maintenance phase. The treatment maintenance phase examined whether the potential treatment effect was maintained throughout a further two-month period. At the end of the maintenance phase, participants in both groups had taken the micronutrients for three months continuously. Study phases are shown in S1 Fig in the supplemental material.

Outcome measures

Clinician-rated measures were completed by the study coordinator who met with the participants and their caregivers face-to-face or by Zoom. All other questionnaires were completed by the caregiver online.

Primary outcome measures

Revised Clinician-Rated Temper and Irritability Scale (CL-ARI). The CL-ARI is a clinician-administered measure that assesses irritability in children over the past week, considering temper outbursts and irritable mood, their frequency, severity, and duration, as well as potential impairment in the family, in school, or with peers over three subscales: Temper Outbursts (e.g. “Did your child show any of these moderate verbal or physical behaviors over the past week?”), Irritable Mood (e.g. “Thinking now about irritable mood in particular, over the past week how many days was your child’s mood negative, cranky, grumpy, or crabby?”), and Impairment (“To what extent did your child’s temper and/or irritable mood cause problems for the family?”). The items are scored on a 5-point Likert scale (e.g. 0 = none, 4 = more than one outburst every day). Total CL-ARI scores range from 0–100, with higher scores indicating more severe emotion dysregulation. The scale has good internal consistency (α = .89) and adequate test-retest reliability (ICC = .67) [39]. Caregivers were interviewed to collect data every four weeks on the Total CL-ARI and all subscales. The CL-ARI was administered by the study coordinator, a postgraduate PhD student in Psychology and supervised by a clinical psychologist. As part of the supervision, the supervisor conducted reliability checks and assessed whether the CL-ARI procedure was followed correctly.

Clinical Global Impressions—Improvement Scale (CGI-I) [40]. The CGI-I is a clinician-administered scale that rates potential changes in the participants’ functioning compared to baseline based on all information obtained from the caregivers. Each item has seven responses, ranging from ‘very much improved’ (1) to ‘very much worse’ (7) and helps to provide a brief, overall assessment of the participant. The CGI-I has demonstrated high external validity (r = 0.74) [41]. Aside from baseline, the CGI-I was completed every four weeks. At the end of each treatment phase, participants were classified as ‘Responders’ if they were identified as having ‘much’ or ‘very much’ improved, based on all information that was available to the study coordinator, including all parent-report and clinician-administered measures. The CGI-I was administered every four weeks by the study coordinator and supervised by a clinical psychologist.

Secondary outcome measures

Parent-Target-Problem Rating Scale (PTP). The PTP is a clinician-administered measure that asks the caregiver to identify two main issues the child struggles with. Once identified, the clinician asked follow-up questions regarding some examples of the problem, as well as its frequency, duration, and intensity. Every four weeks, the study coordinator assessed changes in the identified problem behaviors, rating each on a 9-point Likert scale (1 = problem resolved or extremely improved, 9 = extremely worse).

Clinical Global Impressions—Severity Scale (CGI-S) [40]. The CGI-S is a clinician-administered measure that rates the participant’s severity of symptoms, functioning, and behavior over the past seven days. The scale asks “Considering your total clinical experience with this particular population, how impaired is the patient at this time?” The item is scored on a 7-point Likert scale from 1 (normal, not impaired) to 7 (markedly impaired) [40]. The CGI-S was administered by the study coordinator and supervised by a clinical psychologist.

Children’s Global Assessment Scale (CGAS) [42]. The CGAS is a clinician-reported measure that assesses children’s overall functioning based on the observed and reported information at each visit (every four weeks). The scale ranges from 0 to 100, with higher scores representing better functioning, for example: “60–51: Variable functioning with sporadic difficulties or symptoms in several but not all social areas; disturbance would be apparent to those who encounter the child in a dysfunctional setting or time but not to those who see the child in other settings.”

Affective Reactivity Index (ARI) [36]. The ARI is a concise, caregiver-rated scale assessing the child’s irritability, incorporating seven items asking about feelings of irritability during the past seven days (e.g., “My child is easily annoyed by others”, “My child gets angry frequently”, “Overall, irritability causes my child problems”). Statements can be scored from 0 (not true) to 2 (certainly true). The ARI has been shown to have good internal consistency (α = .84 -.92) [43] and is considered an appropriate measure to assess irritability [36]. The total score is the sum of the first six items and ranges from 0–12. The ARI was completed online every two weeks by the caregiver.

Emotional Outburst Inventory (EMO-1) [44]. The EMO-I is a caregiver-rated irritability measure that assesses phasic irritability in youth in clinical settings, assessing outburst severity, frequency, and duration. It has adequate internal consistency (α = 0.82) and validity. It has nine questions including multiple-choice items, Likert-scale items, and one open question (“What helps your child to calm down?”). The EMO-1 was used to describe the sample at baseline and at the end of the trial in terms of outburst behaviors, frequency, and duration.

Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ) [45]. The SDQ has 25 items, each scored on a 3-point Likert scale (‘not true’, ‘somewhat true’, and ‘certainly true’) across five subscales (emotional symptoms, conduct problems, hyperactivity/inattention, peer relationship problems, and prosocial behavior) in 4–17 year old children and adolescents [46]. Scores in the normal range lie between 0–13 whereas scores between 17–40 represent severe psychological symptoms. The SDQ also assesses how severely the observed symptoms interfere with family relationships, friendships, learning in the classroom, and leisure activities. This ‘impact score’ is assessed by means of a 0 to 4 Likert scale (‘not at all’ to ‘a great deal’), with a score of 2 or greater representing problems in such areas [28]. The SDQ was completed by the caregiver every four weeks.

The Child Swanson, Nolan, and Pelham-IV Questionnaire 26 (SNAP-IV 26). The SNAP-IV 26 is a shorter version of the long SNAP-IV [47] and measures symptoms of Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) and Oppositional Defiant Disorder (ODD). It is a caregiver-rated scale with three subscales: inattention (items 1–9), hyperactivity-impulsivity (items 10–18), and ODD symptoms (items 19–26), each scored on a 4-point Likert scale (0 = not at all, 1 = just a little, 2 = quite a bit, and 3 = very much), with higher scores representing more symptom severity. For the inattention, hyperactivity/impulsivity and ODD subscale, scores of less than 13, 13, and 8 represent normal symptoms, while scores from 23–27, 23–27, and 19–24 represent severe symptoms, respectively. The SNAP-IV displays high internal consistency (α = .94) and sufficient interrater reliability between teachers and caregivers (r = .43-.49) [48].

Eating behavior questionnaire [49]. Caregivers were asked to report on their child‘s diet and eating pattern every four weeks using a 5-point Likert scale, rating servings of fruit and vegetables eaten/day (1 = less than one serving a day, 5 = 4 or more servings per day), how much food is eaten/meal, regularity of eating breakfast, and consumption frequently of processed and fast-food (5 = every day, 1 = less than once a week). The child’s diet overall is rated from 1 (not very healthy) to 7 (very healthy). The total score ranges from 9 to 47, with a higher score representing a healthier diet.

The Depression Anxiety and Stress Scale 21 (DASS-21). The DASS-21 [50] is a publicly available questionnaire that has three subscales (depression, anxiety, and stress), using a 3-point Likert scale. Cut-offs have been provided to indicate normal, mild, moderate, severe, or extremely severe problems; anything below 10 (for depression), 8 (for anxiety) and 15 (for stress) is considered within the normal to mild range. The scale has excellent reliability (ICC = .93) as well as a sufficient construct validity [51]. The DASS-21 was completed by the caregivers about their own mental health to capture their symptoms every four weeks.

The revised Side-Effect Checklist (SEC). Safety, or side effects were assessed using the SEC, a questionnaire based on the Antidepressant Side-Effect Checklist (ASEC), which is normally a self-report measure looking at adverse events associated with antidepressants [52]. The SEC was modified to assess common side effects associated with taking nutritional supplement capsules, such as nausea, headaches, or insomnia. The SEC includes 21 items that are scored on a 0- to 3-point Likert scale, investigating the type, presence, and severity of multiple side effects that may be related to the intervention. However, in this study, the SEC was completed by the participant’s caregiver. They were also asked to indicate whether they thought that the experienced side effect was related to the intervention or was due to other reasons.

Adherence and product likability. Every two weeks, caregivers reported how many doses the participants missed (“How many doses did your child miss over the past two weeks? One dose is equal to one Stick”) and rated on a Likert scale how easy it was to take the product (1 = extremely difficult, 7 = extremely easy) and how much the Sticks were liked by the children (1 = dislike a great deal, 7 = like a great deal) on behalf of their child.

Randomization (random number generation, allocation concealment, implementation) and blinding

Participants were randomized in blocks of four to the initial treatment and initial waitlist control group (ITG and IWLG respectively) using a computer-generated randomization sequence (from www.randomization.com) by an independent person not involved in the recruitment of participants. The website asked about the set of numbers that needed to be generated, the numbers per set, and the number range. Once the randomization list had been generated, participants were assigned to the next sequential number that had been written on a paper and concealed in envelopes by an independent person not involved in the recruitment process. During the baseline meeting, the envelopes were opened by the study coordinator in front of the participants, who had been blinded to participant randomization and assignment up to this point.

Data analysis

Data were analysed using Jamovi 2.2.5 (jamovi.org), software by Lakens [53] and Cumming, Fidler [54] for effect sizes (ES) and 95% Confidence Intervals (95%CI) on ES, and Sigma Plot (systatsoftware.com). Demographic data were tabulated descriptively. Standardized mean difference effect sizes were reported as Cohen’s ds for between-groups and as dav for within-group comparisons (with 95%CI on d) (Lakens, 2103). The Common Language ES (CLES) [53] was additionally reported for the CL-ARI at the end of RCT, giving the probability that a randomly selected individual in the ITG improved relative to a randomly selected IWLG participant. At baseline, the variables were tested for normality using the Shapiro-Wilk normality test, and all variables that failed the normality test were analysed using the Welch’s test, a parametric test that controls for homogeneity of variance. Remaining mean group differences in the RCT and IRP were analysed by two-sided t-tests at each time point while within-group mean changes were compared with t-tests comparing baseline with later time points. During treatment maintenance phases, change over time and between-group differences were examined by a Groups (2) by Time (2) ANOVA. For all statistical tests α was set to.05. Correlations were conducted using Pearson’s r.

Modified Brinley Plots [55] examined the patterns of response to treatment over study phases, displaying individual change across time in the context of all other participants’ scores at that time, along with means (M), standard deviations (SD), and relevant effect sizes.

Results

Demographic information

All participants completed the RCT phase, 22 (91.7%) the full trial in the ITG, and 19 (79.2%) in the IWLG. Reasons for drop-out are shown in Fig 1. Both groups were predominantly of NZ European ethnicity and closely matched on age, but the ITG was more male dominated than the IWLG group (Table 1). In both groups, participants had a moderate-to-high socio-economic status (SES) as determined by the New Zealand Socio-economic Index 2018 [56]. Primary ethnicity was categorized using the New Zealand Ministry of Health prioritization tables based on the Ethnicity New Zealand Standard Classification 2005 V2.0 [57].

Table 1. Sociodemographic characteristics of participants at baseline.

Baseline characteristic ITG (N = 24) IWLG (N = 24)
N % N %
Age 7.9 (M) 1.76 (SD) 7.7 (M) 1.5 (SD)
Gender
 Female 7 29.2 10 41.2
 Male 17 70.8 13 54.2
 Non-binary 0 0 1 4.2
Ethnicity
 NZ Māori 4 16.7 4 16.7
 Pacific Peoples 0 0 0 0
 Asian 2 8.3 0 0
 MELAA 0 0 1 4.2
 Other 2 8.3 2 8.3
 NZ European 16 66.7 17 70.8
SES 66.1 (M) 11.1 (SD) 64.8 (M) 15.3 (SD)
Diagnosis a
 Anxiety Disorders 3 6.3 5 10.4
 Behavioral Problems 5 10.4 7 14.6
 Learning Difficulties 3 6.3 4 8.3
Previous psychological treatment a 6 25.0 4 16.7
Previous psychotropic medication a 1 4.2 1 4.2
Current psychological treatment a 1 4.2 4 16.7

Note. MELAA = Middle Eastern, Latin American, and African; SES = Socio-economic status

a based on parent-report

Randomized Controlled Phase (RCT) and Immediate Replication Phases (IRP)

Results for the CL-ARI between groups and across time are shown in Fig 2 and group data for the primary and secondary measures are displayed in Table 2.

Fig 2. Changes for the ITG and IWLG on the CL-ARI from baseline to week 4, 8, and 12.

Fig 2

Note. Modified Brinley Plots showing individual scores and average change (as shown with the cross) at baseline and at week 4, 8, and 12 for the Revised Clinician-Rated Temper and Irritability Scale (CL-ARI). The solid diagonal line indicates no change. The between- (ds) and within-group (dav) effect sizes are shown at each time point. The 95% Confidence Interval is shown in [] .

Table 2. Between group baseline and week 4, 6, 8, and 12 data on primary and secondary outcome measures.

Variable Baseline (N = 48) Week 4 (RCT) (N = 48) Week 8 (IRP) (N = 48) Week 12 (IRP) (N = 47)
ITG IWLG ITG IWLG ITG IWLG ITG (N = 23) IWLG (N = 24)
M (SD) M (SD) M (SD) M (SD) d s p M (SD) M (SD) d s p M (SD) M (SD) d s p
CL-ARI+
 Temper Outbursts 0.44 (0.12) 0.46 (0.10) 0.30 (0.17) 0.42 (0.12) -0.82 .006* 0.35 (0.13) 0.33 (0.15) 0.12 .673 0.29 (0.18) 0.40 (0.12) -0.71 .019*
 Irritable Mood 0.56 (0.22) 0.48 (0.22) 0.30 (0.20) 0.53 (0.20) -1.19 <.001* 0.42 (0.28) 0.33 (0.24) -0.36 .224 0.26 (0.22) 0.45 (0.23) -0.83 .007*
 Impairment 0.365 (0.15) 0.31 (0.17) 0.15 (0.11) 0.29 (0.16) -1.01 .001* 0.20 (0.16) 0.18 (0.13) 0.18 .532 0.12 (0.12) 0.25 (0.17) -0.88 .004*
 Total 44.9 (11.3) 41.6 (11.7) 24.9 (13.8) 41.4 (12.7) -1.25 <.001* 32.2 (16.3) 27.7 (15.7) 0.29 .316 22.2 (15.4) 36.4 (13.7) -0.98 .002*
CGI-I+ 2.33 (0.76) 3.88 (0.99) 1.74 < .001* 3.17 (1.17) 2.79 (1.10) 0.33 .258 2.39 (0.78) 3.64 (1.17) -1.23 <.001*
CGI-S 3.96 (0.55) 4.00 (0.65) 2.92 (1.02) 3.92 (0.66) -1.17 < .001* 3.42 (0.88) 3.38 (0.77) 0.05 .862 2.88 (1.08) 3.83 (0.76) -1.03 <.001*
CGAS 55.1 (6.33) 55.3 (6.55) 64.2 (9.50) 55.8 (7.09) 1.00 .001* 60.2 (8.92) 61.0 (8.09) -0.10 .736 65.1 (9.11) 57.4 (7.62) 0.92 .003*
PTP 3.21 (1.32) 4.71 (0.91) -1.33 < .001* 4.08 (1.41) 3.63 (1.28) 0.34 .245 3.13 (1.25) 4.38 (1.17) -1.03 .001*
ARI 8.38 (1.84) 8.38 (2.02) 3.67 (2.32) 7.08 (2.0) -1.58 < .001* 5.75 (2.98) 4.83 (3.02) 0.31 .295 3.30 (2.34) 5.79 (3.11) -0.90 .003*
SDQ
 Externalising Score 11.0 (2.77) 11.7 (4.33) 8.85 (2.95) 11.6 (4.07) -0.84 .006* 9.29 (3.32) 9.79 (4.80) -0.121 .677 7.96 (3.46) 10.33 (4.34) -0.60 .044*
 Internalising Score 5.88 (3.13) 6.96 (4.14) 3.58 (2.55) 7.08 (3.46) -1.15 < .001* 4.42 (3.73) 5.42 (3.17) -0.29 .323 2.87 (2.75) 6.04 (3.78) -0.10 .002*
 Total Difficulties Score 16.6 (3.93) 18.6 (7.13) 12.2 (4.1) 18.7 (5.89) -1.28 < .001* 13.71 (5.19) 15.21 (6.55) -0.254 .384 10.83 (4.67) 16.38 (6.76) -0.95 .002*
 Impact Score 3.63 (1.66) 3.92 (1.91) 2.21 (1.59) 3.25 (2.21) -0.54 .068 2.42 (1.38) 2.83 (2.41) -0.212 .467 1.26 (1.52) 3.38 (2.83) -0.93 .003*
SNAP-IV
 ADHD 29.6 (10.2) 33.0 (12.5) 22.6 (11.1) 29.3 (13.1) -0.55 .062 24.2 (11.7) 26.9 (13.3) -0.21 .465 22.1 (10.5) 29.8 (14.8) -0.60 .046*
 ODD 15.5 (4.88) 15.1 (4.74) 10.0 (4.74) 13.6 (4.11) -0.82 .007* 11.0 (5.20) 11.50 (5.73) -0.08 .773 9.04 (5.60) 12.7 (5.30) -0.66 .027*
EBQ 34.8 (5.47) 34.4 (5.95) 35.7 (4.85) 34.3 (5.70) 0.27 .059 34.9 (5.73) 34.1 (5.33) 0.14 .641 35.0 (5.70) 34.4 (5.72) 0.10 .728
DASS-21
 Depression 4.67 (5.49) 7.08 (6.49) 3.83 (3.82) 4.17 (5.17) -0.07 .800 2.92 (2.83) 4.75 (7.39) -0.33 .265 2.26 (2.12) 5.67 (6.15) -0.74 .016*
 Anxiety 2.58 (2.73) 4.42 (5.66) 1.83 (2.76) 3.00 (4.04) -0.34 .250 2.00 (2.57) 2.75 (3.58) -0.24 .409 1.83 (2.82) 3.17 (3.77) -0.40 .174
 Stress 13.50 (7.42) 14.50 (6.76) 9.25 (5.59) 11.25 (6.97) -0.32 .279 10.83 (6.40) 10.17 (6.27) 0.11 .717 10.00 (6.06) 13.42 (9.06) -0.44 .138

Note. M = mean; SD = standard deviation, d = Cohen’s d effect size; RCT = randomized control trial phase; IRP = immediate replication phase; CL-ARI = Revised Clinician-rated Affective Reactivity Scale; CGI-I = Clinical Global Impressions Improvement Scale; PTP = Parent Target Problem Rating Scale; ARI = Affective Reactivity Index; SDQ = Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire; SNAP-IV = The Child Swanson, Nolan, and Pelham-IV Questionnaire 26; EBQ = Eating Behavior Questionnaire.

+ = primary measure.

* p < .05.

Primary outcomes

CL-ARI. As shown in Figs 2 and 3, both the ITG and IWLG displayed similar scores and variance baseline. At the end of RCT phase, the ITG largely improved their total score (t(23) = -8.54, p < .001, dav = 1.6) while the IWLG showed negligible change (t(23) = -0.09, p = .0 = 926, dav = -0.015). At the end of RCT phase, the ITG improved largely and statistically significantly more than the IWLG (t(46) = -4.33, p < .001, ds = 1.25). Further, at the end of the RCT phase, the CLES was large (81%), indicating a high probability of severe symptom reduction in the treatment group. This pattern of improvement is also evident for the CL-ARI subscales.

Fig 3. CL-ARI total mean scores for the ITG and IWLG across baseline and week 4, 8, and 12.

Fig 3

Note. This Fig presents the Total Revised Clinician-Rated Temper and Irritability Scale (CL-ARI) scores for the ITG and IWLG across baseline until the end of the immediate replication phase (IRP)) of the trial. * p < .05 (between-group comparison).

As displayed in Fig 2, for the ITG, going off the micronutrients (week 8) did not lead to a systematic return to baseline levels on the CL-ARI, although there was a reduction in dav (t(23) = -3.91, p < .001, dav = .90). Receiving treatment for the first time in the IWLG (week 8) led to a similar but attenuated effect (Fig 2a and 2e), (t(23) = -4.84, p < .001, dav =.-1.0). The between-group ds at week 8 was small and statistically not significant (t(46) = 1.01, p = .32, ds = .29). Return to micronutrients for the ITG (week 12) replicated the initial positive effect with a large statistically significant effect (t(22) = -8.28, p < .001, dav = 1.69) (Fig 2a and 2c), while withdrawal of treatment for the IWLG led to a trend towards the retention of a treatment effect into the OFF-phase, with a small-to-moderate ES (t(23) = -1.92, p = .067, dav = -.41).

CGI-I. The ITG was rated as significantly and largely more improved than the IWLG at the end of RCT (t(46) = -6.04, p < .001, ds = 1.74). In addition, 66% (N = 16) of the ITG were identified as responders to the treatment (‘much-’ or ‘very much improved’), while only 8% (N = 2) ‘much’ or ‘very much’ improved in the IWLG group. Going off treatment for the ITG lead to 33% (N = 8) responders relative to baseline, while starting treatment for the IWLG resulted in 54% responders (N = 13) (week 8). Upon continuing the micronutrients, 60% (N = 14) of the ITG responded to treatment, while going off treatment led to 21% (N = 5) of responders in the IWLG compared to baseline.

Secondary outcomes

Data for the secondary outcome variables are shown in Table 2.

CGI-S. At the end of RCT, the ITG was functioning significantly better than the IWLG at the end of RCT (Welch’s t(39.2) = -4.05, p < .001, ds = 1.17). The ITG mean fell to within the ‘mildly ill’ range, while the IWLG mean stayed within the ‘moderately ill’ range. Further, nine (37.5%) participants were identified as ‘not at all ill’ or ‘borderline ill’ in the ITG versus none in the IWLG.

CGAS. At the end of RCT, the ITG had large and significantly better overall functioning than the IWCG (t(46) = 3.46, p < .001, ds = 1.00). Specifically, at the end of RCT, the CGAS scores in the ITG represented “some difficulty in a single area but generally functioning well” (M = 64.2, SD = 9.50) compared to the IWLG, which indicated “variable functioning with sporadic difficulties in several but not all social areas” (M = 55.8, SD = 7.09).

PTP. At the end of RCT, 67% (N = 16) of participants in the ITG had ‘definitely’, ‘much’, or ‘extremely improved’ in terms of emotion dysregulation symptoms, including anger, temper, frustration, or emotional outbursts. In contrast, only 8% (N = 2) in the IWLG were rated as such. Overall, at the end of RCT, the treatment group average fell in the ‘definitely improved’ range, while the IWLG mean fell in the ‘no change from baseline’.

DASS-21. At the end of RCT, a positive correlation between scores on the Total CL-ARI and Total DASS-21 was found (r = 0.38, p = .007), indicating that the lower the participants’ symptoms of irritability, the lower the caregivers’ symptoms of depression, anxiety, and stress, and vice versa.

Treatment maintenance phase

See Table 3 for all maintenance phase results on the primary and secondary measures.

Table 3. Results for treatment maintenance phases on primary and secondary measures.

Variable Baseline Month 1 Month 2
ITG (N = 24)
M (SD)
IWLG (N = 24)
M (SD)
ITG (N = 22)
M (SD)
IWLG (N = 20)
M (SD)
ITG (N = 22)
M (SD)
IWLG (N = 19)
M (SD)
η p 2 (between) p (between) η p 2 (within) Month 1–2 p (within)
Month 1–2
CL-ARI+
 Temper Outbursts 0.44 (0.12) 0.46 (0.10) 0.25 (0.19) 0.33 (0.14) 0.23 (0.18) 0.30 (0.18) 0.07 .103 .04 .233
 Irritable Mood 0.56 (0.22) 0.48 (0.22) 0.25 (0.26) 0.37 (0.24) 0.28 (0.25) 0.37 (0.27) 0.06 .123 .00 .911
 Impairment 0.365 (0.15) 0.31 (0.17) 0.12 (0.11) 0.20 (0.21) 0.14 (0.16) 0.21 (0.19) 0.07 .102 .014 .460
 Total 44.9 (11.3) 41.6 (11.7) 20.2 (16.6) 29.7 (17.0) 21.3 (18.3) 29.5 (18.9) 0.08 .080 0.00 .981
CGI-I+ 2.27 (0.88) 3.05 (1.32) 2.27 (0.94) 3.16 (1.34) 0.15 .011* 0.00 1.00
CGI-S 3.96 (0.55) 4.00 (0.65) 2.77 (1.15) 3.45 (1.10) 2.64 (1.26) 3.16 (0.96) 0.10 .049* 0.14 .015*
CGAS 55.1 (6.33) 55.3 (6.55) 66.2 (10.5) 61.3 (9.58) 68.2 (13.4) 62.8 (8.88) 0.07 .088 0.19 .005*
PTP 2.86 (1.17) 3.70 (1.69) 2.82 (1.33) 3.37 (1.42) 0.08 .080 0.05 .147
ARI 8.38 (1.84) 8.38 (2.02) 3.25 (2.61) 5.00 (3.16) 2.82 (3.00) 4.37 (3.08) 0.09 .065 0.20 .004*
SDQ
 Externalising Score 11.0 (2.77) 11.7 (4.33) 7.52 (3.67) 8.80 (3.79) 6.86 (3.58) 8.53 (3.88) 0.04 .206 0.09 .062
 Internalising Score 5.88 (3.13) 6.96 (4.14) 3.13 (2.83) 5.00 (3.43) 2.32 (2.08) 4.63 (3.37) 0.13 .022* 0.11 .036*
 Total Difficulties Score 16.6 (3.93) 18.6 (7.13) 10.7 (4.82) 13.8 (6.27) 9.18 (4.98) 13.2 (5.93) 0.10 .040* 0.20 .003*
 Impact Score 3.63 (1.66) 3.92 (1.91) 1.39 (1.59) 2.15 (2.41) 1.00 (1.48) 2.00 (2.47) 0.06 .140 0.07 .086
SNAP-IV
 ADHD 29.6 (10.2) 33.0 (12.5) 22.0 (11.3) 23.1 (11.2) 21.0 (11.1) 25.4 (12.4) 0.02 .409 0.02 .421
 ODD 15.5 (4.88) 15.1 (4.74) 8.17 (5.37) 9.95 (5.24) 8.86 (6.88) 9.95 (5.19) 0.02 .382 0.00 .776
EBQ 34.8 (5.47) 34.4 (5.95) 35.3 (5.64) 34.8 (5.20) 36.3 (5.61) 35.1 (5.01) 0.01 .576 .030 .276
DASS-21
 Depression 4.67 (5.49) 7.08 (6.49) 2.70 (2.93) 4.70 (7.00) 2.27 (3.40) 4.32 (5.34) 0.06 .128 0.02 .388
 Anxiety 2.58 (2.73) 4.42 (5.66) 1.65 (3.60) 3.60 (6.34) 2.18 (3.75) 2.74 (5.26) 0.02 .375 0.01 .469
 Stress 13.50 (7.42) 14.50 (6.76) 9.30 (6.54) 10.7 (8.24) 8.91 (8.72) 10.1 (5.55) 0.01 .548 0.003 .732

Note. M = mean; SD = standard deviation, d = Cohen’s d effect size measure; RCT = randomized control trial phase; IRP = immediate replication phase; CL-ARI = Revised Clinician-rated Affective Reactivity Scale; CGI-I = Clinical Global Impressions Improvement Scale; PTP = Parent Target Problem Rating Scale; ARI = Affective Reactivity Index; SDQ = Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire; SNAP-IV = The Child Swanson, Nolan, and Pelham-IV Questionnaire 26; EBQ = Eating Behavior Questionnaire.

+ = primary measure

* p < .05

Primary outcomes

CL-ARI. No significant group-, time-, or interaction effects were found on the total CL-ARI or any of the subscales in the maintenance phase.

CGI-I. Post-hoc analysis showed that across the treatment maintenance phase, the ITG showed a significantly greater improvement over both month 1 and 2 relative to the IWLG, although the difference was small (t(39) = -2.66, p = .011, d = 0.15). While in month 1, 65% (N = 15) responded to treatment in the ITG, 45% (N = 9) did so in the IWLG. Similarly, at month 2, 68% (N = 15) in the ITG and 47% (N = 9) in the IWLG responded to treatment.

Secondary outcomes

CGI-S. At the end of the trial, participants in the ITG functioned slightly but significantly better than the IWLG (F(1,39) = 6.43, p = .015, ηp2 = 0.10). Further, 55% (N = 12) of the ITG were rated ‘not at all ill’ or ‘borderline ill’, while 32% (N = 6) were rated as such in the IWLG.

PTP. In month 1, 72.7% (N = 16) definitely- much-, or extremely improved in the ITG regarding parent-identified emotion dysregulation-related issues, while 50% did so in the IWLG. In month 2, 68% (N = 15) were classified as such in the ITG, and 47% in the IWLG.

EMO-I. As shown in Fig 4, at baseline, 83% of children argued, whined or sulked, or became verbally insulting, swore, or shouted. Also, 80% became physically aggressive, such as slamming doors, punching walls, making a mess, or destroying property, and 58% hit, kicked, bit, or spit, and 19% needed physical restraint. Moreover, 79% had outbursts at least weekly, and 58% of those lasted for half an hour or more. In comparison, at the end of the trial, 80% still argued, whined or sulked, and but only 31% became verbally insulting, swore, or shouted. Further, only 31% became physically aggressive, 29% hit, kicked, bit or spit, and 10% required physical restraint. A Table displaying detailed EMO-I outcomes is shown in the supplemental material (S2 Table).

Fig 4. Descriptive information (%) for the EMO-I for participants at baseline and end of trial.

Fig 4

Adverse events, adherence rates, and product likability

Across the whole study period, 168 side effects were reported while taking the micronutrient Sticks, and the caregiver believed that 29% (N = 48) of those were linked to the intervention. No serious adverse events were reported. Fifty-one percent (N = 85) either persisted for at least four weeks or reoccurred throughout the trial. At the end of RCT, the IWLG reported experiencing significantly more headaches (χ2(3) = 8.29, p = .040) and sweating (χ2(1) = 4.36, p = .037) than the ITG; in particular, headaches were experienced by 50% (N = 12) of the IWLG and 25% (N = 6) of the ITG, and sweating by 17% (N = 4) and 0% (N = 0) in the IWLG and ITG, respectively. Throughout the trial, increased appetite was the most frequently reported side effect linked by parents to the intervention, although nausea and vomiting was the most reported adverse event overall. For more information on overall side effects, see Table 4. Side effects during the RCT phase can be found in the supplemental material (S3 Table).

Table 4. Treatment-emerging adverse events.

Adverse events Participants (N = 48) (%) Linked to interventiona
(N = 48) (%)
Persistentb and/or recurrent (N = 48) (%)
Dry mouth 7 (14.6) 2 (4.2) 4 (8.3)
Drowsiness 9 (18.8) 1 (2.1) 5 (10.4)
Insomnia 11 (22.9) 7 (14.6) 10 (20.8)
Blurred vision 3 (6.3) 0 (0) 0 (0)
Headache 13 (27.1) 3 (6.3) 9 (18.8)
Constipation 3 (6.3) 2 (4.2) 0 (0)
Diarrhea 14 (29.2) 1 (2.1) 3 (6.3)
Increased appetite 17 (35.4) 12 (25) 14 (29.2)
Decreased appetite 11 (22.9) 4 (8.3) 5 (10.4)
Nausea and vomiting 20 (41.7) 4 (8.3) 9 (18.8)
Problems with urination 3 (6.3) 0 (0) 2 (4.2)
Palpitations 2 (4.2) 1 (2.1) 1 (2.1)
Feeling light-headed on standing 6 (12.5) 2 (4.2) 2 (4.2)
Feeling like the room is spinning total 11 (22.9) 1 (2.1) 1 (2.1)
Sweating 7 (14.6) 1 (2.1) 5 (10.4)
Increased body temperature 8 (16.7) 2 (4.2) 3 (6.3)
Tremor 0 (0) 0 (0) 0 (0)
Disorientation 1 (2.1) 0 (0) 0 (0)
Yawning 11 (22.9) 1 (2.1) 3 (6.3)
Weight gain 8 (16.7) 4 (8.3) 6 (12.5)
Eczema 1 (2.1) 2 (4.2) 3 (6.3)

Note.

aBased on parent-report and clinical judgement.

bPersistent = present for at least four weeks.

Based on self-report, the adherence rate was 92% in the ITG and 94.9% in the IWLG (overall 93%). The caregivers rated it to be ‘slightly easy’ (M = 5.4, SD = 1.4) for the children to take the product and reported that children liked the micronutrient powder ‘a little’ (M = 4.8, SD = 1.4). 44% of the participants liked banana, 32% tropical punch, and 28% sour berry the most. Most caregivers of participants (95%) reported they would recommend the micronutrient product and 87% would purchase it at some point in the future. The main reason reported for not wanting to buy it in the future was cost.

Discussion

This study presents the first reported trial to investigate the effects of micronutrients absorbed by the oral mucosa on emotion dysregulation symptoms in 5-10-year-old children. By the end of the RCT phase, emotion dysregulation and overall improvement were significantly greater in the ITG than in the IWLG. In addition, the ITG improved on the ODD subscale of the SNAP-IV to a significantly larger degree than the IWLG and there was a trend towards a greater improvement on the ADHD subscale. The reduction in symptoms was clinically meaningful in the ITG, reducing ADHD scores from the mild to the non-clinical range, whereas the IWLG stayed within the mild range. Similarly, on the ODD subscale, scores in the ITG reduced from the moderate to the mild range, while the IWLG stayed within the moderate range.

Comparing the results from the current study to CBT-based interventions on emotion regulation, micronutrients reduced symptoms to a larger degree. In particular, a meta-analysis by Mingebach, Kamp-Becker [58] found that parent-based psychological interventions for children under the age of 13 yielded overall long-term moderate improvements in children’s externalizing (SMD = 0.45, p < .001) and overall behavior (SMD = 0.46, p < .001). Further, Kennedy, Bilek [59] found that a transdiagnostic intervention targeting areas related to emotional reactivity and anxiety-focused CBT in children with emotional disorders led to approximately 70% of treatment response over 15 and 10 sessions respectively. These findings are comparable to the current study, where 65% of children in the ITG had responded to treatment; however, micronutrients had only been administered for four weeks at this point, showing a more rapid effect. In a recent randomized controlled study by Helander, Enebrink [60], 120 8-12-year old children with ODD, ODD combined with CD or Disruptive Behavior Disorder Not Otherwise Specified (NOS) were assigned to 11 group sessions of parent treatment alone or combined with group-CBT for children. Both treatment groups showed a large decrease in symptoms related to ODD from pre-treatment to 2-year follow-up (d = -0.80, p < .001). Comparing these findings to our results, micronutrients have a more immediate and potentially larger effect on emotion dysregulation symptoms in children. In addition, micronutrients are convenient to administer, require less time, effort, and money both by parents and children, and place fewer demands on health professionals and the broader health system.

Regarding psychiatric medication, antipsychotics have been shown to have a positive impact on anger and disruptive behavior related to psychiatric illnesses in children and adults [61, 62]; nevertheless, concerns have been raised regarding significant adverse side effects, and in most cases, medication alone does not suffice for children with severe emotion dysregulation and must be administered in addition to CBT-based interventions (n.b. the same may potentially be true for micronutrients) [62, 63]. A recent prospective naturalistic study investigated the effect of low and high dose PRN (as-needed) psychiatric medication on anger outbursts in 104 children in a psychiatric hospital, where children did not show any improvements in outburst duration [64]. Our findings, in contrast, showed that at the end of the trial, outbursts became less severe, less frequent, and shorter in duration compared to baseline.

Our positive findings are underlined by previous evidence illustrating that micronutrients can alleviate emotion dysregulation in children and adults. For example, a double-blind RCT by Rucklidge, Eggleston [29] investigated the effects of taking a broad-spectrum micronutrient supplement for 10 weeks for children with ADHD compared to a placebo. Results revealed that relative to the control group, the micronutrients moderately improved symptoms of emotion dysregulation (d = 0.46, p = .029). In addition, 47% of the micronutrient group overall ‘much’ or ‘very much’ improved, while only 28% did so in the placebo group. Treatment response rates were slightly higher in our study, where 67% in the ITG ‘much’ or ‘very much improved’ at the end of RCT, and 59% at the end of the trial. Nevertheless, it is worth noting that the Rucklidge, Eggleston [29] study was blinded compared to our non-blinded study, representing a possible explanation for the lowered improvement rates, as non-blinded studies can overestimate treatment effects.

Further supporting our findings, a recent study with 126 children with ADHD aged 6–12 found that at week 8, 38% ‘definitely-‘, ‘much-’, or ‘extremely improved’ in the micronutrient compared to 25% in the placebo group (p = .04) [65]; however, regarding emotion dysregulation and irritability specifically, no group difference was found. This stands in contrast to the current study, where regarding emotion dysregulation-related symptoms, 67% ‘definitely improved’ or better in the ITG compared to 8% in the IWLG.

Additionally, a positive correlation between scores on the Total CL-ARI and Total DASS-21 at the end of RCT phase was found, indicating that the lower the participants’ symptoms of irritability, the lower the caregivers’ symptoms of depression, anxiety, and stress, and vice versa. Further, the beneficial effects of the initial exposure to micronutrients did not fully wash out during the first OFF phase for either group. This may be related to the direct effects of the micronutrients, or it might be related to more positive interactions between child and parent, where potential changes in the relationship that occurred when behavior improved in the ON phase were maintained to an extent when coming off the micronutrients. This highlights an important connection between the caregiver’s and children’s mental health, which has also been shown in previous research; for example, Karimzadeh, Rostami [66] found that different aspects of the parent’s functioning (physical and social functioning, anxiety, and depression) were positively correlated with externalizing and internalizing symptoms in their preschool children, including aggression, ignorance, and symptoms of anxiety.

During the maintenance phase, individuals in the ITG showed a significantly greater overall improvement on the CGI-I than the IWLG, and in addition, more individuals from the IWLG withdrew from the study compared to the ITG (15% (N = 5) and 10% (N = 2) respectively). A previous study found that length of time spent on a waitlist predicted treatment dropout among individuals with an eating disorder [67]. As motivation to engage in treatment is likely at its maximum when first seeking help, and may weaken over time, waiting might result in diminished commitment to treatment [67]. Although it was observed that no participants dropped out after the 4-week waiting period, their motivation to stay in the study might have been weakened throughout the waiting period, leading to more dropouts later in the study. Additionally, waitlist participants were in the trial for four weeks longer than the treatment group, which may also have contributed. Overall, this suggests some disadvantages for participants who are placed on a waitlist.

With regards to treatment-emergent side effects, at the end of RCT, the IWLG reported significantly more headaches and sweating than the ITG. Overall, an increase in appetite was the most frequently reported treatment-emergent side effect considered to be related to the intervention. These findings are consistent with previous research finding increased food consumption following nutritional supplementation [6870].

The overall dropout rate was low (14.5%) and adherence to the intervention was high. Further, participants reported that they liked the micronutrients ‘a little’ and that they were ‘slightly easy’ to take. Our results confirm previous findings by Katta, Blampied [33], indicating that the micronutrient Sticks are a convenient mode of taking micronutrients.

Overall, our results show micronutrient Sticks lead to significant improvements in emotional dysregulation in children, improvements in ODD symptoms and parental functioning, and had an appealing side effect profile. Although future trials and replication of these findings is necessary to strengthen and better understand the impact of micronutrients absorbed via the oral mucosa on emotion dysregulation, the results from this study indicate that they might be an effective treatment for children with moderate-to severe symptoms of emotion dysregulation.

Limitations and future directions

All online measures were completed by the participants’ caregivers, and no one was blinded to the intervention or waitlist group allocation. This introduces potential bias due to expectancy effects, including performance bias, as knowing about taking an active treatment is likely to influence participants’ expectations. Moreover, we recognize that compared to active control groups, waitlist participants improve less or not at all, as receiving no treatment may interfere with potential spontaneous symptom alleviation by reinforcing participant’s belief that they will not improve [71]. This can contribute to an overestimated treatment effect.

Furthermore, it needs to be underlined that the current findings are exclusively based on samples recruited from New Zealand. Larger future studies including samples from a variety of geographical backgrounds need to be conducted to support the efficacy of the intervention among other populations.

In addition, the study findings established some disadvantages of being placed in the waitlist group in terms of treatment effectiveness and completion, which are ethical issues that need to be considered in future research. As our trial was exploratory in nature, future double-blinded randomized trials need to address these issues to strengthen internal validity.

As past research has established a dose-response curve for the micronutrients used in the Sticks [72], it would be interesting to look into the effects of an increased Stick dose on emotion dysregulation in future studies, especially in those who did not show a clinical response to the initial dose. It might also be useful to further investigate the potential relationship between micronutrient supplement intake and appetite in children that was observed in this trial. Lastly, future studies should explore the pharmacokinetics of oral mucosal and gastrointestinal micronutrient administration to evaluate which absorption method might be more effective [33].

Supporting information

S1 Table. Ingredients and doses of the micronutrients.

(DOCX)

pone.0311794.s001.docx (14.5KB, docx)
S2 Table. Descriptive information for the EMO-I for participants at baseline and end of trial.

(DOCX)

pone.0311794.s002.docx (16.4KB, docx)
S3 Table. Recorded adverse events during the RCT phase.

(DOCX)

pone.0311794.s003.docx (14.9KB, docx)
S1 Fig. Study phases.

(TIF)

pone.0311794.s004.tif (525.6KB, tif)
S1 Appendix. CONSORT 2010 checklist.

(DOC)

pone.0311794.s005.doc (219.5KB, doc)
S2 Appendix. Study protocol for micronutrients and emotion dysregulation in children.

(DOCX)

pone.0311794.s006.docx (194.9KB, docx)

Acknowledgments

Thanks go to the participants and their families, and to Truehope for donating the micronutrient product researched in this study (Truehope Ultimate Sticks).

Data Availability

Throughout the paper, we are using Figures and Tables to summarise larger amounts of data, including participant’s demographic information and study results. The paper also includes a detailed methods section that outlines the study procedure, the measures used, and the way data were analysed. The Figures and Tables in the supplementary material support the data in the paper, illustrating the study procedure, additional information on side effects, and additional summarised data. The datasets generated and analysed in this study that contain the participant’s raw data were conducted in form of Excel spreadsheets and spreadsheets using a statistical software (Jamovi org.). At this stage, the raw data underlying the Figures and Tables in the paper and/or supplementary material have not been provided due to ethical restrictions, so the participant’s privacy is protected. The data sets contain potentially identifying and sensitive information, and since the sample recruited in the study is small, and the country the study took place in (New Zealand) has a small population, the identities of study participants could possibly be identified even if a de-identified data set was provided. During the consent procedure, participants and their caregivers were assured that confidentiality will be respected at all times and no material which could personally identify the participants would be used in any reports on this study. Further, during the consent procedure, participants were made aware that the obtained information will remain confidential and will not be disclosed or used in any way unless the researchers have concerns about the child’s safety or the safety of others. Additionally, participants were told that the only people who will have access to the information are the study investigators and members of the Mental Health and Nutrition Lab Te Puna Toiora at the University of Canterbury, and approved auditors. It was also made clear that any information that was disclosed about the caregivers and the participant will be kept in a confidential file and stored in a locked filing cabinet, and that digital information will be stored on a password protected computer system, complying with Rule 5 of the Health Information Privacy Code. This ensures that the participant’s information is kept safe from loss, unauthorised access, use, modification, or disclosure. Further, it was mentioned that all data that would be provided will be stored at the University of Canterbury for 10 years after the participant turns 16, and will then be destroyed, as per university and health regulations and Rule 9 of the Health Information Privacy Code. By storing the data in a different place, we would be going against this procedure as approved by the Northern Health and Disability Ethics Committee. The study and these study procedures have been approved by Northern Health and Disability Ethics Committee (21/NTA/169). Their contact email address is hdecs@health.govt.nz. We did obtain consent from the participants that coded information may be used for future research, in which case the participants’ information may be widely shared with other researchers or companies. Therefore, the minimal anonymized data set necessary to replicate the study will be made available following publication, after de-identification, on reasonable request, to researchers who provide a methodologically sound proposal and meet the criteria for access to confidential data. Proposals should be directed to the corresponding author; to gain access, data requestors will need to sign a data access agreement. An alternative point of contact is the Northern Health and Disability Ethics Committee.

Funding Statement

University of Canterbury Child and Well-being Research Institute (CWRI) N.M.K https://www.canterbury.ac.nz/research/about-uc-research/research-groups-and-centres/child-wellbeing-research-institute The the sponsors or funders did not play any role in the study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.

References

  • 1.Shaw P, Stringaris A, Nigg J, Leibenluft E. Emotion dysregulation in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Am J Psychiatry. 2014;171(3):276–93. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2013.13070966 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Pylypow J, Quinn D, Duncan D, Balbuena L. A measure of emotional regulation and irritability in children and adolescents: The clinical evaluation of emotional regulation–9. Journal of Attention Disorders. 2020;24(14):2002–11. doi: 10.1177/1087054717737162 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Beauchaine TP, Gatzke-Kopp L, Mead HK. Polyvagal theory and developmental psychopathology: Emotion dysregulation and conduct problems from preschool to adolescence. Biol Psychol. 2007;74(2):174–84. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2005.08.008 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Thompson RA. Emotion dysregulation: A theme in search of definition. Dev Psychopathol. 2019;31(3):805–15. doi: 10.1017/S0954579419000282 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Beauchaine TP. Future directions in emotion dysregulation and youth psychopathology. J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol. 2015;44(5):875–96. doi: 10.1080/15374416.2015.1038827 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Stringaris A. Irritability in children and adolescents: A challenge for DSM-5. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2011;20(2):61–6. doi: 10.1007/s00787-010-0150-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Brotman MA, Kircanski K, Leibenluft E. Irritability in children and adolescents. Annu Rev Clin Psychol. 2017;13:317–41. doi: 10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-032816-044941 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Zeman J, Cassano M, Perry-Parrish C, Stegall S. Emotion regulation in children and adolescents. J Dev Behav Pediatr. 2006;27(2):155–68. doi: 10.1097/00004703-200604000-00014 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Fergusson DM, John Horwood L, Ridder EM. Show me the child at seven: The consequences of conduct problems in childhood for psychosocial functioning in adulthood. Journal of child psychology and psychiatry. 2005;46(8):837–49. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.00387.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Leibenluft E, Cohen P, Gorrindo T, Brook JS, Pine DS. Chronic Versus Episodic Irritability in Youth: A Community-Based, Longitudinal Study of Clinical and Diagnostic Associations. J Child Adolesc Psychopharmacol. 2006;16(4):456–66. doi: 10.1089/cap.2006.16.456 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Riley AW, Spiel G, Coghill D, Döpfner M, Falissard B, Lorenzo MJ, et al. Factors related to health-related quality of life (HRQoL) among children with ADHD in Europe at entry into treatment. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2006;15(1):i38–i45. doi: 10.1007/s00787-006-1006-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Slutske WS, Moffitt TE, Poulton R, Caspi A. Undercontrolled temperament at age 3 predicts disordered gambling at age 32: A longitudinal study of a complete birth cohort. Psychol Sci. 2012;23(5):510–6. doi: 10.1177/0956797611429708 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Moffitt TE, Arseneault L, Belsky D, Dickson N, Hancox RJ, Harrington H, et al. A gradient of childhood self-control predicts health, wealth, and public safety. Proceedings of the national Academy of Sciences. 2011;108(7):2693–8. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1010076108 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Wakschlag LS, Choi SW, Carter AS, Hullsiek H, Burns J, McCarthy K, et al. Defining the developmental parameters of temper loss in early childhood: implications for developmental psychopathology. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2012;53(11):1099–108. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2012.02595.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Calkins SD. The emergence of self-regulation: Biological and behavioral control mechanisms supporting toddler competencies. Socioemotional development in the toddler years: Transitions and transformations. 2007:261–84. [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Brotman MA, Schmajuk M, Rich BA, Dickstein DP, Guyer AE, Costello EJ, et al. Prevalence, clinical correlates, and longitudinal course of severe mood dysregulation in children. Biol Psychiatry. 2006;60(9):991–7. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.08.042 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Sobanski E, Banaschewski T, Asherson P, Buitelaar J, Chen W, Franke B, et al. Emotional lability in children and adolescents with attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD): Clinical correlates and familial prevalence. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2010;51(8):915–23. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2010.02217.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Stringaris A, Goodman R. Mood lability and psychopathology in youth. Psychol Med. 2009;39(8):1237–45. doi: 10.1017/S0033291708004662 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Blader JC, Pliszka SR, Jensen PS, Schooler NR, Kafantaris V. Stimulant-responsive and stimulant-refractory aggressive behavior among children with ADHD. Pediatrics. 2010;126(4):e796–e806. doi: 10.1542/peds.2010-0086 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Waxmonsky J, Pelham WE, Gnagy E, Cummings MR, O’Connor B, Majumdar A, et al. The efficacy and tolerability of methylphenidate and behavior modification in children with Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder and severe mood dysregulation. J Child Adolesc Psychopharmacol. 2008;18(6):573–88. doi: 10.1089/cap.2008.065 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Pliszka SR, Browne RG, Olvera RL, Wynne SK. A double-blind, placebo-controlled study of Adderall and methylphenidate in the treatment of Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2000;39(5):619–26. doi: 10.1097/00004583-200005000-00016 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Biederman J, Krishnan S, Zhang Y, McGough JJ, Findling RL. Efficacy and tolerability of lisdexamfetamine dimesylate (NRP-104) in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a phase III, multicenter, randomized, double-blind, forced-dose, parallel-group study. Clin Ther. 2007;29(3):450–63. doi: 10.1016/s0149-2918(07)80083-x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Posner J, Kass E, Hulvershorn L. Using stimulants to treat ADHD-related emotional lability. Current Psychiatry Reports. 2014;16(10):1–8. doi: 10.1007/s11920-014-0478-4 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Scott S, O’Connor TG. An experimental test of differential susceptibility to parenting among emotionally‐dysregulated children in a randomized controlled trial for oppositional behavior. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2012;53(11):1184–93. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2012.02586.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Miller BF, Petterson S, Burke BT, Phillips RL Jr, Green LA. Proximity of providers: Colocating behavioral health and primary care and the prospects for an integrated workforce. Am Psychol. 2014;69(4):443. doi: 10.1037/a0036093 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Daley D, Van der Oord S, Ferrin M, Cortese S, Danckaerts M, Doepfner M, et al. Practitioner review: Current best practice in the use of parent training and other behavioural interventions in the treatment of children and adolescents with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2018;59(9):932–47. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12825 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Daley D, Van der Oord S, Ferrin M, Danckaerts M, Doepfner M, Cortese S, et al. Behavioral interventions in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: A meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials across multiple outcome domains. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2014;53(8):835–47. e5. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2014.05.013 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Gordon HA, Rucklidge JJ, Blampied NM, Johnstone JM. Clinically significant symptom reduction in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder treated with micronutrients: An open-label reversal design study. J Child Adolesc Psychopharmacol. 2015;25(10):783–98. doi: 10.1089/cap.2015.0105 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Rucklidge JJ, Eggleston MJ, Johnstone JM, Darling K, Frampton CM. Vitamin‐mineral treatment improves aggression and emotional regulation in children with ADHD: A fully blinded, randomized, placebo‐controlled trial. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2018;59(3):232–46. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12817 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Johnstone JM, Hatsu I, Tost G, Srikanth P, Eiterman LP, Bruton AM, et al. Micronutrients for attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in youths: a placebo-controlled randomized clinical trial. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2022;61(5):647–61. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2021.07.005 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Polaha J, Dalton WT 3rd, Lancaster BM. Parental report of medication acceptance among youth: Implications for everyday practice. South Med J. 2008;101(11):1106–12. doi: 10.1097/SMJ.0b013e318180645d [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Boztepe H, Özdemir H, Karababa Ç, Yıldız Ö. Difficulties experienced during preparation and administration of oral drugs. Turkish Archives of Pediatrics/Türk Pediatri Arşivi. 2014;49(3):231. doi: 10.5152/tpa.2014.1795 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Katta NM, Blampied NM, Mulder RT, Rucklidge JJ. Micronutrients absorbed via the oral mucosa reduce irritability and anger but not stress in university students during COVID-19: A randomized placebo-controlled trial. International Journal of Stress Management. 2023. [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Hearnden V, Sankar V, Hull K, Juras DV, Greenberg M, Kerr AR, et al. New developments and opportunities in oral mucosal drug delivery for local and systemic disease. Advanced Drug Delivery Reviews. 2012;64(1):16–28. doi: 10.1016/j.addr.2011.02.008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Zhang H, Zhang J, Streisand JB. Oral mucosal drug delivery. Clin Pharmacokinet. 2002;41(9):661–80. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Stringaris A, Goodman R, Ferdinando S, Razdan V, Muhrer E, Leibenluft E, et al. The Affective Reactivity Index: A concise irritability scale for clinical and research settings. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2012;53(11):1109–17. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2012.02561.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Horner RH, Carr EG, Halle J, McGee G, Odom S, Wolery M. The use of single-subject research to identify evidence-based practice in special education. Except Child. 2005;71(2):165–79. [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Kratochwill T, Hitchcock J, Horner R, Levin J, Odom S, Rindskopf D, et al. What works clearinghouse. Retrieved Sep. 2010;25:2011. [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Haller SP, Kircanski K, Stringaris A, Clayton M, Bui H, Agorsor C, et al. The clinician affective reactivity index: validity and reliability of a clinician-rated assessment of irritability. Behav Ther. 2020;51(2):283–93. doi: 10.1016/j.beth.2019.10.005 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Busner J, Targum SD. The Clinical Global Impressions scale: Applying a research tool in clinical practice. Psychiatry (edgmont). 2007;4(7):28. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Kadouri A, Corruble E, Falissard B. The improved Clinical Global Impression Scale (iCGI): Development and validation in depression. BMC Psychiatry. 2007;7(1):1–7. doi: 10.1186/1471-244X-7-7 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Shaffer D, Gould MS, Brasic J, Ambrosini P, Fisher P, Bird H, et al. A Children’s Global Assessment Scale (CGAS). Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1983;40(11):1228–31. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1983.01790100074010 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Ezpeleta L, Penelo E, de la Osa N, Navarro JB, Trepat E. How the Affective Reactivity Index (ARI) works for teachers as informants. J Affect Disord. 2020;261:40–8. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2019.09.080 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Carlson GA, Silver J, Klein DN. Psychometric Properties of the Emotional Outburst Inventory (EMO-I): Rating What Children Do When They Are Irritable. The Journal of Clinical Psychiatry. 2022;83(2):39382. doi: 10.4088/JCP.21m14015 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Goodman R. The Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire: A research note. Journal of child psychology and psychiatry. 1997;38(5):581–6. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1997.tb01545.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Stone LL, Otten R, Engels RC, Vermulst AA, Janssens JM. Psychometric properties of the parent and teacher versions of the strengths and difficulties questionnaire for 4-to 12-year-olds: A review. Clin Child Fam Psychol Rev. 2010;13(3):254–74. doi: 10.1007/s10567-010-0071-2 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Swanson JM. The SNAP Rating Scale for the Diagnosis of the Attention Deficit Disorder. 1981.
  • 48.Bussing R, Fernandez M, Harwood M, Hou W, Garvan CW, Eyberg SM, et al. Parent and teacher SNAP-IV ratings of Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder symptoms: Psychometric properties and normative ratings from a school district sample. Assessment. 2008;15(3):317–28. doi: 10.1177/1073191107313888 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Baker CW, Little TD, Brownell KD. Predicting adolescent eating and activity behaviors: The role of social norms and personal agency. Health Psychol. 2003;22(2):189. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Lovibond PF, Lovibond SH. The structure of negative emotional states: Comparison of the Depression Anxiety Stress Scales (DASS) with the Beck Depression and Anxiety Inventories. Behav Res Ther. 1995;33(3):335–43. doi: 10.1016/0005-7967(94)00075-u [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Henry JD, Crawford JR. The short‐form version of the Depression Anxiety Stress Scales (DASS‐21): Construct validity and normative data in a large non‐clinical sample. Br J Clin Psychol. 2005;44(2):227–39. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Uher R, Farmer A, Henigsberg N, Rietschel M, Mors O, Maier W, et al. Adverse reactions to antidepressants. The British Journal of Psychiatry. 2009;195(3):202–10. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.108.061960 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Lakens D. Calculating and reporting effect sizes to facilitate cumulative science: A practical primer for t-tests and ANOVAs. Front Psychol. 2013:863. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00863 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Cumming G, Fidler F, Kalinowski P, Lai J. The statistical recommendations of the American Psychological Association Publication Manual: Effect sizes, confidence intervals, and meta‐analysis. Aust J Psychol. 2012;64(3):138–46. [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Blampied NM. Analyzing therapeutic change using modified Brinley plots: History, construction, and interpretation. Behav Ther. 2017;48(1):115–27. doi: 10.1016/j.beth.2016.09.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Boven N, Shackelton N, Bolton L, Sporle A, Milne B. New Zealand Socio-economic Index 2018. Auckland: COMPASS Research Centre, University of Auckland; 2022.
  • 57.Health Information Standards Organisation. Ethnicity data protocols (HISO Standard No. 10001:2017): Ministry of Health.; 2017.
  • 58.Mingebach T, Kamp-Becker I, Christiansen H, Weber L. Meta-meta-analysis on the effectiveness of parent-based interventions for the treatment of child externalizing behavior problems. PLoS One. 2018;13(9):e0202855. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0202855 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Kennedy SM, Bilek EL, Ehrenreich-May J. A randomized controlled pilot trial of the unified protocol for transdiagnostic treatment of emotional disorders in children. Behav Modif. 2019;43(3):330–60. doi: 10.1177/0145445517753940 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Helander M, Enebrink P, Hellner C, Ahlen J. Parent management training combined with group-CBT compared to parent management training only for oppositional defiant disorder symptoms: 2-year follow-up of a randomized controlled trial. Child Psychiatry Hum Dev. 2023;54(4):1112–26. doi: 10.1007/s10578-021-01306-3 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Mercer D, Douglass AB, Links PS. Meta-analyses of mood stabilizers, antidepressants and antipsychotics in the treatment of borderline personality disorder: Effectiveness for depression and anger symptoms. Journal of personality disorders. 2009;23(2):156–74. doi: 10.1521/pedi.2009.23.2.156 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Loy JH, Merry SN, Hetrick SE, Stasiak K. Atypical antipsychotics for disruptive behaviour disorders in children and youths. Cochrane Database Syst Rev. 2017;8(8):CD008559. doi: 10.1002/14651858.CD008559.pub3 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Carlson GA, Singh MK, Amaya-Jackson L, Benton TD, Althoff RR, Bellonci C, et al. Narrative review: impairing emotional outbursts: what they are and what we should do about them. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2023;62(2):135–50. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2022.03.014 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Carlson GA, Spring L, Schwartz JE. Does Pro Re Nata Oral Medication Shorten Outburst Duration in Children? J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2021;61(2):111–4. e3. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2021.09.415 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Tost G, Srikanth P, Bruton A, Hatsu IE, Leung BM, Ast HK, et al. Problems most concerning to parents of children with ADHD and emotional dysregulation in a randomized controlled trial of multinutrients: MADDY secondary analysis. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2024:1–13. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Karimzadeh M, Rostami M, Teymouri R, Moazzen Z, Tahmasebi S. The association between parental mental health and behavioral disorders in pre-school children. Electronic Physician. 2017;9(6):4497. doi: 10.19082/4497 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Carter O, Pannekoek L, Fursland A, Allen KL, Lampard AM, Byrne SM. Increased wait-list time predicts dropout from outpatient enhanced cognitive behaviour therapy (CBT-E) for eating disorders. Behav Res Ther. 2012;50(7–8):487–92. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2012.03.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Mda S, van Raaij JM, MacIntyre UE, de Villiers FP, Kok FJ. Improved appetite after multi-micronutrient supplementation for six months in HIV-infected South African children. Appetite. 2010;54(1):150–5. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2009.09.021 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Kusumastuti AC, Ardiaria M, Hendrianingtyas M. Effect of zinc and iron supplementation on appetite, nutritional status and intelligence quotient in young children. The Indonesian Biomedical Journal. 2018;10(2):133–9. [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Ohinata K, Takemoto M, Kawanago M, Fushimi S, Shirakawa H, Goto T, et al. Orally administered zinc increases food intake via vagal stimulation in rats. The Journal of nutrition. 2009;139(3):611–6. doi: 10.3945/jn.108.096370 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Basham RB. Scientific and practical advantages of comparative design in psychotherapy outcome research. J Consult Clin Psychol. 1986;54(1):88. doi: 10.1037//0022-006x.54.1.88 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 72.Rucklidge JJ, Andridge R, Gorman B, Blampied N, Gordon H, Boggis A. Shaken but unstirred? Effects of micronutrients on stress and trauma after an earthquake: RCT evidence comparing formulas and doses. Human Psychopharmacology: Clinical and Experimental. 2012;27(5):440–54. doi: 10.1002/hup.2246 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Decision Letter 0

Ebenezer Wiafe

19 Jun 2024

PONE-D-24-09885Micronutrients absorbed via the oral mucosa reduce emotion dysregulation in 5-10-year-old children: A three-phased randomized wait-list-controlled trialPLOS ONE

Dear Dr. Rucklidge,

Thank you for submitting your manuscript to PLOS ONE. After careful consideration, we feel that it has merit but does not fully meet PLOS ONE’s publication criteria as it currently stands. Therefore, we invite you to submit a revised version of the manuscript that addresses the points raised during the review process.

Please submit your revised manuscript by Aug 03 2024 11:59PM. If you will need more time than this to complete your revisions, please reply to this message or contact the journal office at plosone@plos.org. When you're ready to submit your revision, log on to https://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/ and select the 'Submissions Needing Revision' folder to locate your manuscript file.

Please include the following items when submitting your revised manuscript:

  • A rebuttal letter that responds to each point raised by the reviewer(s). You should upload this letter as a separate file labeled 'Response to Reviewers'.

  • A marked-up copy of your manuscript that highlights changes made to the original version. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Revised Manuscript with Track Changes'.

  • An unmarked version of your revised paper without tracked changes. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Manuscript'.

If you would like to make changes to your financial disclosure, please include your updated statement in your cover letter. Guidelines for resubmitting your figure files are available below the reviewer comments at the end of this letter.

If applicable, we recommend that you deposit your laboratory protocols in protocols.io to enhance the reproducibility of your results. Protocols.io assigns your protocol its own identifier (DOI) so that it can be cited independently in the future. For instructions see: https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/submission-guidelines#loc-laboratory-protocols. Additionally, PLOS ONE offers an option for publishing peer-reviewed Lab Protocol articles, which describe protocols hosted on protocols.io. Read more information on sharing protocols at https://plos.org/protocols?utm_medium=editorial-email&utm_source=authorletters&utm_campaign=protocols.

We look forward to receiving your revised manuscript.

Kind regards,

Ebenezer Wiafe, PhD, MPharm, Pharm D

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Journal Requirements:

When submitting your revision, we need you to address these additional requirements.

1. Please ensure that your manuscript meets PLOS ONE's style requirements, including those for file naming. The PLOS ONE style templates can be found at 

https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=wjVg/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_main_body.pdf and 

https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=ba62/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_title_authors_affiliations.pdf

2. We note that you have indicated that there are restrictions to data sharing for this study. For studies involving human research participant data or other sensitive data, we encourage authors to share de-identified or anonymized data. However, when data cannot be publicly shared for ethical reasons, we allow authors to make their data sets available upon request. For information on unacceptable data access restrictions, please see http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/data-availability#loc-unacceptable-data-access-restrictions

Before we proceed with your manuscript, please address the following prompts:

a) If there are ethical or legal restrictions on sharing a de-identified data set, please explain them in detail (e.g., data contain potentially identifying or sensitive patient information, data are owned by a third-party organization, etc.) and who has imposed them (e.g., a Research Ethics Committee or Institutional Review Board, etc.). Please also provide contact information for a data access committee, ethics committee, or other institutional body to which data requests may be sent.

b) If there are no restrictions, please upload the minimal anonymized data set necessary to replicate your study findings to a stable, public repository and provide us with the relevant URLs, DOIs, or accession numbers. Please see http://www.bmj.com/content/340/bmj.c181.long for guidelines on how to de-identify and prepare clinical data for publication. For a list of recommended repositories, please see https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/recommended-repositories. You also have the option of uploading the data as Supporting Information files, but we would recommend depositing data directly to a data repository if possible.

Please update your Data Availability statement in the submission form accordingly.

3. Please review your reference list to ensure that it is complete and correct. If you have cited papers that have been retracted, please include the rationale for doing so in the manuscript text, or remove these references and replace them with relevant current references. Any changes to the reference list should be mentioned in the rebuttal letter that accompanies your revised manuscript. If you need to cite a retracted article, indicate the article’s retracted status in the References list and also include a citation and full reference for the retraction notice.

Reviewers' comments:

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Author

1. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.

Reviewer #1: Partly

Reviewer #2: Yes

Reviewer #3: Yes

**********

2. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

Reviewer #1: No

Reviewer #2: Yes

Reviewer #3: Yes

**********

3. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #1: No

Reviewer #2: Yes

Reviewer #3: Yes

**********

4. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

Reviewer #3: Yes

**********

5. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #1: This manuscript presents data analysis from a 3-phase, randomized, open-label, waitlist-controlled trial to compare

the effectiveness of orally-absorbed micronutrients intervention in 5-10 year old children. The topic is of importance, and the study was registered as a RCT, and was approved by the respective IRB/Ethics Committee. While the study objectives sound interesting, is important, and on target, some shortcomings were observed, in regards to abiding by the CONSORT guidelines for conducting and reporting results of high-quality randomized controlled trials (RCTs). Some other (statistical) comments were also provided.

1. Abstract:

For a better presentation, Abstract needs to follow the Objective/Methods/Results/Conclusion framework.

2. Methods:

Methods reporting need some work. An orderly manner is suggested, following CONSORT guidelines, without repeating information, such as Trial Design, Participant Eligibility Criteria and settings, Interventions, Outcomes, sample size/power considerations, Interim analysis and stopping rules, Randomization (details on random number generation, allocation concealment, implementation), Blinding issues, etc, should be mentioned. The authors are advised to create separate subsections for each of the possible topics (whichever necessary), and that way produce a very clear writeup. They are advised to write it carefully, following nice examples in the manuscript below:

https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0889540619300010

Specific comments:

(a) For instance, the randomization and allocation concealment should be made very clear (they are NOT the same thing); the trial staff recruiting patients should NOT have the randomization list. Randomization should be prepared by the trial statistician, and he/she would not participate in the recruiting.

(b) More details on the randomization is needed; saying "a computer-generated randomization" was used is half-hearted.

(c) Sample size: The sample size/power statement appeared pretty vague; in usual waitlist-controlled trials, there are previous attempts of producing a desirable sample size/power statements, using the ANCOVA design; see here:

https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC5109669/ The authors are recommended to do the same. If not possible, then some power/sample size statements should be produced, based on an objective effect size, and at a reasonable 5% level of significance.

(d) Statistical Analysis:

(d1) The section Data Analysis should be renamed Statistical Methods. While mentioning tests, whether a 1-sided, or a 2-sided test will be used needs to be mentioned. Furthermore, t-test was used (for continuous variables) which is heavily based on normality assumptions. Under violations, alternative nonparametric tests, such as Wilcoxon rank sum tests, should be mentioned. There is no mention of such alternative testing methods.

(d2) ANOVA was used, however, it is once again heavily based on Gaussian assumptions. No nonparametric alternatived to ANOVA mentioned.

(d3) Is there any reason why a ANCOVA was not used, which is typically the model for waitlist-controlled trials, incorporating covariates.

3. Results & Conclusions:

(a) The authors should check that any statement of significance should be followed by a p-value in the entire Results section. Otherwise, the Results section look OK; it's pretty straightforward.

(b) Conclusions should state that the current findings are ONLY based on the random samples derived from a New Zealand population, and should allude to future studies with much larger sample sizes and collected at other geographical areas to confirm the effectiveness of the intervention.

Reviewer #2: This is a well-written manuscript that contributes to the literature on the use of alternative therapy in emotional dysregulation/ADHD symptom amelioration.

Below are some suggestions for revisions/clarifications:

1. The study’s justification for focusing on children ages 5-10 yr old, need to be made clear. One possible assumption is that it is because this age group has difficulty with pill swallowing. This needs to be clarified in the write-up.

2. It is suggested that the authors clarify the level of involvement of supplement providers with regards to study design, implementation, reporting, etc.

3. CL-ARI is supposed to be administered by a clinician. It is not clear if the study coordinator is a clinician and was therefore qualified to administer this measure. Also, it is not clear if the PTP and CGI-S were completed by clinicians or the research coordinator.

4. Similar to the above comment, it is not clear who completed the CGI and how the determination of treatment response was made.

5. There are about 10 secondary outcomes. What was the justification for assessing all of these? For example, it is not clear why the EBQ was assessed since the study was not targeted at improving diet.

6. Were the reported PTPs similar for all participants? If not, it is not clear how the individual PTPs were classified and grouped to determine symptom improvements per group (i.e ITG vs. IWLG)?

7. The assessment of safety is one of the study goals , however, the methods is missing how safety was operationalized and assessed.

8. Figure ( esp 2 & 3) titles/notes are separated from the actual figures (pages 15 vs. 47/48), which made comprehending the tables difficult.

9. Suggest minimizing reporting results in discussion.

Reviewer #3: ### Review Comments for Manuscript PONE-D-24-09885

#### 1. Technical Soundness and Data Support

The manuscript is technically sound and presents a well-conducted study on the effects of micronutrients absorbed via the oral mucosa on emotion dysregulation in children. The study design, including the randomized wait-list-controlled trial and three-phased approach, is robust and appropriate for the research question. The data support the conclusions, demonstrating significant improvements in emotion dysregulation for children receiving the intervention compared to the control group. The study also includes appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes to ensure the validity of the findings.

#### 2. Statistical Analysis

The statistical analysis is performed rigorously and appropriately. The authors use a range of statistical tests, including t-tests, ANOVA, and effect size calculations, to analyze the data. The reporting of results includes confidence intervals and effect sizes, providing a clear understanding of the magnitude and significance of the findings. The use of Modified Brinley Plots to display individual changes over time is a valuable addition that enhances the interpretation of the data.

#### 3. Data Availability

The authors have made a commendable effort to make the data underlying the findings available, in line with PLOS ONE's data policy. While the raw data are not included in the manuscript to protect participant privacy, the authors have provided a clear statement that de-identified data will be available upon reasonable request. This approach balances the need for data transparency with ethical considerations regarding participant confidentiality.

#### 4. Manuscript Presentation and Language

The manuscript is presented in an intelligible fashion and is written in clear, standard English. The structure is logical, and the flow of information is easy to follow. There are no significant typographical or grammatical errors, and the language is appropriate for a scientific audience.

#### 5. Review Comments to the Author

- The manuscript is a valuable contribution to the field of child psychology and nutrition, addressing a relevant and timely research question.

- The introduction provides a thorough background, highlighting the need for alternative treatments for emotion dysregulation in children and the potential of micronutrient interventions.

- The methodology is well-detailed, ensuring that the study can be replicated. The randomization process, intervention details, and measurement tools are clearly described.

- The results are presented comprehensively, with appropriate use of tables and figures to illustrate key findings. The inclusion of both primary and secondary outcome measures strengthens the study.

- The discussion section effectively interprets the findings, placing them in the context of existing literature. The authors acknowledge the limitations of the study, including the open-label design and potential biases, and suggest directions for future research.

- The ethical considerations and adherence to data availability policies are well-handled, demonstrating a commitment to ethical research practices.

**********

6. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #1: No

Reviewer #2: No

Reviewer #3: Yes: sherif kamal

**********

[NOTE: If reviewer comments were submitted as an attachment file, they will be attached to this email and accessible via the submission site. Please log into your account, locate the manuscript record, and check for the action link "View Attachments". If this link does not appear, there are no attachment files.]

While revising your submission, please upload your figure files to the Preflight Analysis and Conversion Engine (PACE) digital diagnostic tool, https://pacev2.apexcovantage.com/. PACE helps ensure that figures meet PLOS requirements. To use PACE, you must first register as a user. Registration is free. Then, login and navigate to the UPLOAD tab, where you will find detailed instructions on how to use the tool. If you encounter any issues or have any questions when using PACE, please email PLOS at figures@plos.org. Please note that Supporting Information files do not need this step.

PLoS One. 2024 Dec 5;19(12):e0311794. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0311794.r002

Author response to Decision Letter 0


11 Aug 2024

PLOS One response letter

Reviewer #1:

1. Abstract

Comment: For a better presentation, the Abstract needs to follow the Objective/Methods/Results/Conclusion framework.

Response: The Abstract has been adjusted to the Objective/Methods/Results/Conclusion framework.

2. Methods:

Comment: Methods reporting need some work. An orderly manner is suggested, following CONSORT guidelines, without repeating information, such as Trial Design, Participant Eligibility Criteria and settings, Interventions, Outcomes, sample size/power considerations, Interim analysis and stopping rules, Randomization (details on random number generation, allocation concealment, implementation), Blinding issues, etc, should be mentioned. The authors are advised to create separate subsections for each of the possible topics (whichever necessary), and that way produce a very clear writeup. They are advised to write it carefully, following nice examples in the manuscript below:

https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0889540619300010

A) For instance, the randomization and allocation concealment should be made very clear (they are NOT the same thing); the trial staff recruiting patients should NOT have the randomization list. Randomization should be prepared by the trial statistician, and he/she would not participate in the recruiting.

B) Comment: More details on the randomization is needed; saying "a computer-generated randomization" was used is half-hearted.

Response to comments A and B: The Methods section has been adjusted to the suggested format and the randomization process has been clarified on p. 13: “Participants were randomized in blocks of four to the initial treatment and initial waitlist control group (ITG and IWLG respectively) using a computer-generated randomization sequence (from www.randomization.com) by an independent person not involved in the recruitment of participants. The website asked about the set of numbers that needed to be generated, the numbers per set, and the number range. Once the randomization list had been generated, participants were assigned to the next sequential number that had been written on a paper and concealed in envelopes by an independent person not involved in the recruitment process. During the baseline meeting, the envelopes were opened by the study coordinator in front of the participants, who had been blinded to participant randomization and assignment up to this point.”

C) Comment: Sample size: The sample size/power statement appeared pretty vague; in usual waitlist-controlled trials, there are previous attempts of producing a desirable sample size/power statements, using the ANCOVA design; see here:

https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC5109669/ The authors are recommended to do the same. If not possible, then some power/sample size statements should be produced, based on an objective effect size, and at a reasonable 5% level of significance.

Response: The sample size/power statement was adjusted and made more specific (p. 5). “The number of participants was based on the What Works Clearinghouse Standards for single-case designs (37, 38), being considered a sufficient sample size to determine whether the treatment is successful in reducing emotion dysregulation with sufficient power at a 5% level of significance. The researchers anticipated a moderate effect size (~d=0.5) based on previous studies investigating the same intervention (i.e. True Hope Ultimate Sticks), establishing moderate effect sizes on emotion dysregulation (33).”

The design standard for single-case experimental designs is based on validity considerations following the What Works Clearinghouse Standards. As the study employed an initial RCT phase followed by a replication crossover withdrawal design drawn from a single-case ABAB design, the number of participants was based on the What Works Clearinghouse Standards for single-case designs, being considered a sufficient sample size to determine whether the treatment is successful in reducing emotion dysregulation with sufficient power. Therefore, no conventional power calculation was conducted. The anticipated effect size was based on findings on emotion dysregulation from previous research (Katta et al., 2023) investigating the same product as in the current study.

D) Statistical analysis

Results and conclusions

(d1) Comment: The section Data Analysis should be renamed Statistical Methods. While mentioning tests, whether a 1-sided, or a 2-sided test will be used needs to be mentioned. Furthermore, t-test was used (for continuous variables) which is heavily based on normality assumptions. Under violations, alternative nonparametric tests, such as Wilcoxon rank sum tests, should be mentioned. There is no mention of such alternative testing methods.

Response: A two-sided t-test was used. Because our design is balanced, with equal n in each group, and additionally is an RCT, our design is particularly robust against violations of parametric test assumptions (Blanca, 2018; Schmider et al., 2010). Nevertheless, we tested for normality using the Shapiro-Wilk Test, and for all variables that failed the normality test at baseline, the data was analysed using the Welch’s Test. The Welch’s Test is a parametric test that controls for homogeneity of variance, and in addition, it delivers an effect size. The results were consistent with the original findings. All this information was added to the Data Analysis section (p. 12) and adjusted in the Results section (p. 13-23).

(d2) ANOVA was used, however, it is once again heavily based on Gaussian assumptions. No nonparametric alternatives to ANOVA mentioned.

Response: ANOVA is widely accepted to be robust from normality when sample sizes are relatively large (Mena et al., 2017, Schmider et al., 2010). In addition, our design is robust against test assumption violations. Therefore, we decided that data analysis with a nonparametric alternative was not necessary.

(d3) Is there any reason why an ANCOVA was not used, which is typically the model for waitlist-controlled trials, incorporating covariates.

Response: The study employed an initial RCT phase followed by a replication crossover design. At the end of each replication phase, there we compared two groups at one time point; therefore, the t-test appeared to be a valid and sufficient way of analyzing the data. This is supported by Wilkinson (1999), the Task Force on Statistical Inference, stating that a minimally sufficient analysis should be chosen to provide a simple yet adequate way to address the data. Further, the variance of scores was initially compressed using strict eligibility criteria, reducing the occurrence of a large variance in scores.

Nevertheless, we re-analyzed the RCT data using ANCOVA with the baseline data as a covariate, and the results did not change. Therefore, we decided to keep reporting the outcomes using t-tests.

Results & Conclusions:

A) Comment: The authors should check that any statement of significance should be followed by a p-value in the entire Results section. Otherwise, the Results section look OK; it's pretty straightforward.

Response: The authors checked each statement of significance and ensured that each statement was followed by a p-value.

B) Conclusions should state that the current findings are ONLY based on the random samples derived from a New Zealand population, and should allude to future studies with much larger sample sizes and collected at other geographical areas to confirm the effectiveness of the intervention.

Response: This has been added to the Limitations and Future Directions section (p. 30): “Furthermore, it needs to be underlined that the current findings are exclusively based on samples recruited from New Zealand. Larger future studies including samples from a variety of geographical backgrounds need to be conducted to support the efficacy of the intervention among other populations.”

Reviewer #2:

1. Comment: The study’s justification for focusing on children ages 5-10 yr old, need to be made clear. One possible assumption is that it is because this age group has difficulty with pill swallowing. This needs to be clarified in the write-up.

Response: The justification for focusing on 5- to 10-year-old children has been clarified on pages 3-5 in the introduction. One reason for focusing on this age group is that emotion regulation abilities typically develop during the first 5 years of life and represent normative behavior in early childhood/preschool age, which needs to be differentiated from clinically significant symptoms. Another reason is that we wanted to minimize confounding variables as best as possible, for example developmental levels (e.g. puberty). Further, some of the measures used, such as the SDQ, are applicable to an age range from 4-10 years. Finally, children often struggle with swallowing pills.

2. Comment: It is suggested that the authors clarify the level of involvement of supplement providers regarding the study design, implementation, reporting, etc.

Response: The supplement providers were not involved in any stage of the study design, including study planning, design, implementation, or reporting. This has been added on p. 8.

3. CL-ARI is supposed to be administered by a clinician. It is not clear if the study coordinator is a clinician and was therefore qualified to administer this measure. Also, it is not clear if the PTP and CGI-S were completed by clinicians or the research coordinator.

Response: The CL-ARI was administered by the study coordinator, a postgraduate PhD student in Psychology and supervised by a Clinical Psychologist. As part of the supervision, the supervisor conducted reliability checks and assessed whether the CL-ARI procedure was followed correctly. This has been added to the methods section.

4. Comment: Similar to the above comment, it is not clear who completed the CGI and how the determination of treatment response was made.

Response: The CGI was completed by the study coordinator, a postgraduate PhD student in Psychology who was supervised by a Clinical Psychologist. Treatment response was determined based on all information that was available to the study coordinator, including all parent-report and clinician-administered measures. This has been added to the methods section.

5. There are about 10 secondary outcomes. What was the justification for assessing all of these? For example, it is not clear why the EBQ was assessed since the study was not targeted at improving diet.

Response: A range of secondary outcomes were assessed to ensure a comprehensive picture of symptoms related to emotion dysregulation. As emotion dysregulation can present in various ways, and there are inconsistencies in its definition in the literature, it was important to include multiple emotion dysregulation measures.

The EBQ was assessed to be able to measure whether the participant’s diet changed over time and being able to exclude that diet might have confounded the results.

The DASS-21 was related to the caregiver’s wellbeing to assess a potential connection between the children’s symptoms of emotion dysregulation and the caregiver’s wellbeing, or vice versa.

6. Were the reported PTPs similar for all participants? If not, it is not clear how the individual PTPs were classified and grouped to determine symptom improvements per group (i.e ITG vs. IWLG)?

Response: The analyzed PTPs were related to emotion dysregulation, including temper outbursts, emotion dysregulation, frustration (outbursts), anger (outbursts), and emotional outbursts. Given the limited sample size, other PTPs not related to emotion dysregulation were not categorized as these largely varied between participants.

7. The assessment of safety is one of the study goals , however, the methods is missing how safety was operationalized and assessed.

Response: The safety was assessed by capturing side effects with an established side effect measure (the Revised Side-Effect Checklist). This has been added to the Measures section under Methods (p. 12).

8. Figure (esp 2 & 3) titles/notes are separated from the actual figures (pages 15 vs. 47/48), which made comprehending the tables difficult.

Response: We followed the PLOS One formatting guidelines which outlined to only insert the figure titles in the manuscript.

9. Suggest minimizing reporting results in discussion.

Response: Statistical results that had already been mentioned previously in the results were removed from the discussion.

Reviewer #3:

Response: Thank you for the very positive reflection, it is much appreciated.

Attachment

Submitted filename: Response to Reviewers.docx

pone.0311794.s007.docx (28.4KB, docx)

Decision Letter 1

Ebenezer Wiafe

25 Sep 2024

Micronutrients absorbed via the oral mucosa reduce emotion dysregulation in 5-10-year-old children: A three-phased randomized wait-list-controlled trial

PONE-D-24-09885R1

Dear Dr. Rucklidge,

We’re pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been judged scientifically suitable for publication and will be formally accepted for publication once it meets all outstanding technical requirements.

Within one week, you’ll receive an e-mail detailing the required amendments. When these have been addressed, you’ll receive a formal acceptance letter and your manuscript will be scheduled for publication.

An invoice will be generated when your article is formally accepted. Please note, if your institution has a publishing partnership with PLOS and your article meets the relevant criteria, all or part of your publication costs will be covered. Please make sure your user information is up-to-date by logging into Editorial Manager at Editorial Manager® and clicking the ‘Update My Information' link at the top of the page. If you have any questions relating to publication charges, please contact our Author Billing department directly at authorbilling@plos.org.

If your institution or institutions have a press office, please notify them about your upcoming paper to help maximize its impact. If they’ll be preparing press materials, please inform our press team as soon as possible -- no later than 48 hours after receiving the formal acceptance. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information, please contact onepress@plos.org.

Kind regards,

Ebenezer Wiafe, PhD, MPharm, Pharm D

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Acceptance letter

Ebenezer Wiafe

18 Oct 2024

PONE-D-24-09885R1

PLOS ONE

Dear Dr. Rucklidge,

I'm pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been deemed suitable for publication in PLOS ONE. Congratulations! Your manuscript is now being handed over to our production team.

At this stage, our production department will prepare your paper for publication. This includes ensuring the following:

* All references, tables, and figures are properly cited

* All relevant supporting information is included in the manuscript submission,

* There are no issues that prevent the paper from being properly typeset

If revisions are needed, the production department will contact you directly to resolve them. If no revisions are needed, you will receive an email when the publication date has been set. At this time, we do not offer pre-publication proofs to authors during production of the accepted work. Please keep in mind that we are working through a large volume of accepted articles, so please give us a few weeks to review your paper and let you know the next and final steps.

Lastly, if your institution or institutions have a press office, please let them know about your upcoming paper now to help maximize its impact. If they'll be preparing press materials, please inform our press team within the next 48 hours. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information, please contact onepress@plos.org.

If we can help with anything else, please email us at customercare@plos.org.

Thank you for submitting your work to PLOS ONE and supporting open access.

Kind regards,

PLOS ONE Editorial Office Staff

on behalf of

Dr. Ebenezer Wiafe

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Associated Data

    This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

    Supplementary Materials

    S1 Table. Ingredients and doses of the micronutrients.

    (DOCX)

    pone.0311794.s001.docx (14.5KB, docx)
    S2 Table. Descriptive information for the EMO-I for participants at baseline and end of trial.

    (DOCX)

    pone.0311794.s002.docx (16.4KB, docx)
    S3 Table. Recorded adverse events during the RCT phase.

    (DOCX)

    pone.0311794.s003.docx (14.9KB, docx)
    S1 Fig. Study phases.

    (TIF)

    pone.0311794.s004.tif (525.6KB, tif)
    S1 Appendix. CONSORT 2010 checklist.

    (DOC)

    pone.0311794.s005.doc (219.5KB, doc)
    S2 Appendix. Study protocol for micronutrients and emotion dysregulation in children.

    (DOCX)

    pone.0311794.s006.docx (194.9KB, docx)
    Attachment

    Submitted filename: Response to Reviewers.docx

    pone.0311794.s007.docx (28.4KB, docx)

    Data Availability Statement

    Throughout the paper, we are using Figures and Tables to summarise larger amounts of data, including participant’s demographic information and study results. The paper also includes a detailed methods section that outlines the study procedure, the measures used, and the way data were analysed. The Figures and Tables in the supplementary material support the data in the paper, illustrating the study procedure, additional information on side effects, and additional summarised data. The datasets generated and analysed in this study that contain the participant’s raw data were conducted in form of Excel spreadsheets and spreadsheets using a statistical software (Jamovi org.). At this stage, the raw data underlying the Figures and Tables in the paper and/or supplementary material have not been provided due to ethical restrictions, so the participant’s privacy is protected. The data sets contain potentially identifying and sensitive information, and since the sample recruited in the study is small, and the country the study took place in (New Zealand) has a small population, the identities of study participants could possibly be identified even if a de-identified data set was provided. During the consent procedure, participants and their caregivers were assured that confidentiality will be respected at all times and no material which could personally identify the participants would be used in any reports on this study. Further, during the consent procedure, participants were made aware that the obtained information will remain confidential and will not be disclosed or used in any way unless the researchers have concerns about the child’s safety or the safety of others. Additionally, participants were told that the only people who will have access to the information are the study investigators and members of the Mental Health and Nutrition Lab Te Puna Toiora at the University of Canterbury, and approved auditors. It was also made clear that any information that was disclosed about the caregivers and the participant will be kept in a confidential file and stored in a locked filing cabinet, and that digital information will be stored on a password protected computer system, complying with Rule 5 of the Health Information Privacy Code. This ensures that the participant’s information is kept safe from loss, unauthorised access, use, modification, or disclosure. Further, it was mentioned that all data that would be provided will be stored at the University of Canterbury for 10 years after the participant turns 16, and will then be destroyed, as per university and health regulations and Rule 9 of the Health Information Privacy Code. By storing the data in a different place, we would be going against this procedure as approved by the Northern Health and Disability Ethics Committee. The study and these study procedures have been approved by Northern Health and Disability Ethics Committee (21/NTA/169). Their contact email address is hdecs@health.govt.nz. We did obtain consent from the participants that coded information may be used for future research, in which case the participants’ information may be widely shared with other researchers or companies. Therefore, the minimal anonymized data set necessary to replicate the study will be made available following publication, after de-identification, on reasonable request, to researchers who provide a methodologically sound proposal and meet the criteria for access to confidential data. Proposals should be directed to the corresponding author; to gain access, data requestors will need to sign a data access agreement. An alternative point of contact is the Northern Health and Disability Ethics Committee.


    Articles from PLOS ONE are provided here courtesy of PLOS

    RESOURCES