Skip to main content
Wiley Open Access Collection logoLink to Wiley Open Access Collection
. 2024 Oct 27;45(1):43–49. doi: 10.1111/opo.13409

Developmental eye movement test results of Hebrew‐speaking children with cross‐linguistic comparisons

Hadas Ben‐Eli 1,2,, Hadas Blique 1, Mitchell Scheiman 3, Rachel Eichler 1
PMCID: PMC11629855  PMID: 39462238

Abstract

Introduction and Purpose

The developmental eye movement (DEM) test is designed to assess saccadic eye movements and visual‐verbal automaticity in children. This study aimed to assess whether there is a need for independent DEM Hebrew norms and to compare DEM results for Hebrew‐speaking children with eight other language norms.

Methods

The DEM test was administered to 224 Hebrew‐speaking children aged 6–13 years who met the inclusion criteria and read the numbers in Hebrew. Test C of the DEM was performed twice, once from right (R) to left (L) and once from L to R, in random order. Age group and language comparisons, including vertical and horizontal reading speeds, errors and horizontal/vertical (H/V) ratios in both directions were analysed.

Results

The participants were almost evenly distributed between the sexes (46.8% female). Statistically significant differences were found between age groups (6–9 and 10–13 years) for vertical and horizontal reading speeds and H/V ratios in both directions (p < 0.001). Older children, as compared to younger children, exhibited faster vertical and horizontal times, with fewer errors, as well as lower ratios (p < 0.001). No significant difference was noted between reading directions for horizontal time and H/V ratio within both age groups (6–9 year olds: p = 0.27 and p = 0.06; 10–13 year olds: p = 0.89 and p = 0.49, respectively). Comparison of DEM norms across languages showed significant differences, with post‐hoc analysis revealing specific language‐related variations. DEM results for Hebrew‐speaking children had similar outcomes to both original English and French values.

Conclusions

This study compared DEM results of Hebrew‐speaking children and scores across nine languages. DEM test values for Hebrew‐speaking children aligned with norms from other languages, particularly the French and original English norms, with consistent ratio scores. It is recommended for practitioners who test Hebrew‐speaking children to continue using the original English norms and to enable the children to read using their preferred reading direction.

Keywords: children, developmental eye movement (DEM) test, eye movements, Hebrew, languages, saccades


Key points.

  • First time comparison of the developmental eye movement test results for Hebrew‐speaking children with the norms of eight other languages.

  • Developmental eye movement test values for Hebrew‐speaking children aligned with norms from other languages, particularly the French and original English norms, with consistent ratio scores.

  • It is recommended for practitioners who test Hebrew‐speaking children to continue to use the English norms and to enable the children to read using their preferred reading direction.

INTRODUCTION

Reading requires the eyes to make fast, precise movements separated by short pauses to allow high‐resolution visual information to be received from the text. 1 These eye movements, known as saccades, are associated with perception, visual processing, memory and attention during reading. 2 In order to understand the complex visual motor and cognitive processes required for efficient reading, various tests have been developed.

The developmental eye movement (DEM) test is designed to assess saccadic eye movements and visual‐verbal automaticity in children in a reading‐like condition. It was introduced in 1987 and is widely used by optometrists. 3 The DEM is easy to administer and assesses speed and accuracy of ocular movements in children. 4 Although widely used, previous studies have found that the DEM outcomes do not correlate directly with specific parameters of ocular movements. However, it was concluded that the DEM test outcomes were correlated with reading and reading development, and thus may serve a diagnostic role in clinical practice. 5 , 6

In a previous study, the horizontal adjusted time in the DEM, rather than the DEM ratio, demonstrated the strongest association with Visagraph outcome measures. 7 However, this study did not find a significant relationship between the DEM findings and reading rate. In contrast, other research has shown that poor readers exhibit deficient horizontal scanning, as assessed by the DEM, which correlates with slower reading speed. 8 This suggests the utility of the DEM as a screening tool for identifying poor reading skills in school children at an early stage. Additionally, further studies have supported the DEM as a valuable tool for assessing oculomotor behaviour in dyslexic children during reading. 9

The original DEM test norms were established from 556 English‐speaking American school children aged from 6 to 13 years. 4 Researchers have hypothesised that children from non‐English‐speaking countries might have varying reading speeds necessitating the establishment of appropriate datasets. Several studies have been published for different languages with mixed results. For example, Mandarin Chinese, 10 Italian, 11 Spanish, 12 , 13 Portuguese, 14 Cantonese Chinese 15 and Latvian 16 studies found significantly different norms than English, suggesting language‐specific norms be used for those populations. Comparing the English and French DEM norms revealed no differences, leading to the conclusion that French clinicians could use the original English norms, 17 while one of the Spanish norms similarly found mean reference values closely aligned with the English norms. 13 In Hebrew, DEM norms have yet to be compared with other languages.

A complicating factor related to developing DEM age‐expected norms is the issue of reading direction in different languages. In most languages, children are taught to read from left to right. However, in some languages such as Arabic, Hebrew, Persian and Kurdish, children read from right to left. Investigators have explored whether this variable of direction might affect the norms. A study with Arabic‐speaking children reported that right‐to‐left reading speed was faster with fewer reading errors than left‐to‐right reading. 18 In a previous publication, we found that reading direction for Hebrew‐speaking children was significantly different for horizontal reading speed on the DEM test, with a directional preference of right to left in children under 10 years of age. 19

Currently, there has not been an investigation regarding whether it is appropriate to use the original normative data of DEM that were developed for English‐speaking children for children who speak only Hebrew. This investigation was designed to compare DEM results of Hebrew‐speaking children to other language norms and to assess whether there is a need for independent Hebrew DEM norms.

METHODS

Participants

The study included 224 healthy children, aged 6–13 years, whose mother tongue is Hebrew. Children included in the study had minimum near visual acuity of Jaeger 1 (6/7.5) at 40 cm, no strabismus (verified by cover test), stereoacuity (Randot test) of ≤60 s of arc and a near point of convergence of ≤6 cm. The DEM test was performed twice, once from right (R) to left (L) and once from L to R, randomly ordered. This study was approved by the IRB committee at Hadassah Academic College (#480). The parents of the participants signed the informed consent form after receiving an explanation on study aims and methods. All data were coded and analysed anonymously.

Procedure

The children were instructed to recite in Hebrew the numbers on Test Plates A and B vertically, as quickly and accurately as possible, without using a finger for support. The examiner recorded the time taken with a stopwatch and any errors made while reading the 80 numbers, with the vertical score serving as a baseline measure of the child's automaticity in number calling. 3

Subsequently, the children were tested with Card C. Card C consists of the same 80 numbers as Cards A + B; however, they are arranged horizontally, resembling a paragraph of text. The examiner recorded the time with a stopwatch and kept track of errors (addition, subtraction, substitution and transposition) during this task. The ratio score, calculated by dividing the adjusted horizontal time (AHT) by the vertical baseline, assessed the child's speed in horizontal number calling.

Sample size

Based on the mean and SD of horizontal time from the mid‐range aged children (9 years old) in English‐speakers (51.13 ± 13.30 s) 3 versus Spanish‐speakers (54.54 ± 11.60 s) 3 with α = 0.05 and power of 80% the required total sample size is 212 children (computed by statulator.com; ©Statulator 2023, on November 15, 2023).

Statistical analysis

Data normality was approved by the Kolmogorov–Smirnov test. When analysing subgroups of age, nonparametric tests were used due to small samples within each subgroup. Continuous variables such as vertical time, horizontal time, number of errors and horizontal/vertical (H/V) ratio were presented as means with standard deviation and analysed using Mann–Whitney U test when comparing between groups and by Wilcoxon test for comparisons within groups. Analysis of these parameters by sex and age groups was performed by Chi‐square test. In comparison of vertical and horizontal reading times among different languages, a one‐way ANOVA test was used and post‐hoc Tukey–Kramer was employed for multiple comparison test. A statistically significant result was considered as p < 0.05 in a two‐tailed test. The analysis was performed using SPSS software (ibm.com).

Power analysis

A study including 224 patients yielded a power of 95% with α = 0.05 and effect size of 0.23 (calculated by G*Power calculator version 3.1.9.7, psychologie.hhu.de/arbeitsgruppen/allgemeine‐psychologie‐und‐arbeitspsychologie/gpower).

RESULTS

A total of 224 children (47% female) with a mean age of 9.7 ± 1.9 years, ranging from 6.0 to 13.8 years were included. Means of vertical and horizontal reading speeds, errors and H/V ratios of all study participants are presented in Table 1.

TABLE 1.

Basic characteristics and developmental eye movement (DEM) mean results of study population.

Parameter Mean ± SD
N 224
Female, N (%) 105 (47)
Mean age ± SD (Y) 9.6 ± 1.9
Age range (Y) 6.0–13.8
Vertical (s) 45.56 ± 12.19
Error vertical 0.65 ± 2.72
Horizontal left to right (s) 60.85 ± 23.52
Error horizontal left to right 4.13 ± 5.61
Ratio H/V left to right 1.32 ± 0.25
Horizontal right to left (S) 59.77 ± 22.52
Error horizontal right to left 4.07 ± 5.19
Ratio H/V right to left 1.31 ± 0.29

Abbreviations: H/V, horizontal/vertical; SD, standard deviation; Y, years.

Table 2 compares the vertical and horizontal reading speeds in both directions (R–L and L–R), errors and H/V ratios for the 6–9 and 10–13 year age groups. The comparison between the older and younger children in this study follows a previous publication and was repeated here due to the larger sample size. 19 Statistically significant differences were found between age groups for vertical and horizontal reading speeds and H/V ratios in both directions (p < 0.001). Older children, compared with the younger age group, exhibited faster vertical (39.00 ± 8.07 vs. 50.58 ± 12.45, respectively) and horizontal times (48.28 ± 13.14 vs. 70.45 ± 25.17, respectively), with fewer errors vertically (0.21 ± 0.70 vs. 0.96 ± 3.46, respectively) and horizontally (2.01 ± 3.28 vs. 5.81 ± 6.46, respectively), as well as lower ratios (1.22 ± 0.17 vs. 1.39 ± 0.27, respectively; p < 0.001).

TABLE 2.

Comparison of developmental eye movement (DEM) scores by age groups.

DEM test parameter 6–9 years (n = 127); Mean ± SD 10–13 years (n = 97); Mean ± SD p‐Value a
Vertical (s) 50.58 ± 12.45 39.00 ± 8.07 <0.001
Error vertical 0.96 ± 3.46 0.21 ± 0.70 0.01
Horizontal left to right (s) 70.45 ± 25.17 48.28 ± 13.14 <0.001
Error horizontal left to right 5.81 ± 6.46 2.01 ± 3.28 <0.001
Ratio H/V left to right 1.39 ± 0.27 1.22 ± 0.17 <0.001
Horizontal right to left (s) 68.63 ± 24.49 48.17 ± 12.23 <0.001
Error horizontal right to left 5.88 ± 5.88 1.67 ± 2.64 <0.001
Ratio H/V right to left 1.38 ± 0.32 1.20 ± 0.19 <0.001

Abbreviation: H/V, horizontal/vertical.

a

Analysed by Mann–Whitney U test.

No statistically significant difference was noted between reading directions (R–L vs. L–R) for horizontal time and H/V ratio for all study participants as well as for both age groups; thus, comparisons to different languages were analysed using the standard L–R direction (Table 3).

TABLE 3.

Comparison of reading direction in developmental eye movement (DEM) test by intra‐age groups.

DEM test parameter All children (n = 224); time (s); mean ± SD p‐Value a 6–9 years (n = 127); p‐Value a 10–13 years (n = 97); time (s); mean ± SD p‐Value a
Horizontal left to right 60.85 ± 23.52 0.27 70.45 ± 25.17 0.14 48.28 ± 13.14 0.89
Horizontal right to left 59.77 ± 22.52 68.63 ± 24.49 48.17 ± 12.23
Error horizontal left to right 4.13 ± 5.61 0.86 5.81 ± 6.46 0.50 2.01 ± 3.28 0.26
Error horizontal right to left 4.07 ± 5.19 5.88 ± 5.88 1.67 ± 2.64
Ratio H/V left to right 1.32 ± 0.24 0.06 1.39 ± 0.27 0.06 1.22 ± 0.17 0.47
Ratio H/V right to left 1.31 ± 0.29 1.38 ± 0.32 1.20 ± 0.19

Abbreviation: H/V, horizontal/vertical.

a

Analysed by Wilcoxon test.

Moreover, no difference was found in distribution of sexes between younger and older children (p = 0.91). DEM parameter scores were not found to be statistically significant different when divided by sex for vertical (p=0.88), horizontal (p=0.80), errors (p=0.26) and ratio (p=0.35).

Table 4 presents a comparison of DEM scores across various languages, including Hebrew, English, Spanish, Cantonese, Mandarin, Portuguese, Italian, French and Latvian. The data are categorised by age and measures four parameters: vertical time, horizontal time, error rates and the H/V ratio. Children speaking English, Portuguese, Italian and Hebrew included those 6.0–13.11 years of age, while those speaking Mandarin and French included up to 12.11 years of age and Spanish and Cantonese up to 11.11 years. The data on the Latvian‐speaking children included only the ratio outcomes in children ages 6.0–13.11 years. Comparison of DEM norms across languages showed statistically significant differences, with post‐hoc analysis revealing specific language‐related variations.

TABLE 4.

Comparison of developmental eye movement (DEM) scores among Hebrew, English, Spanish, Chinese (Cantonese), Chinese (Mandarin), Portuguese, Italian, French and Latvian studies.

Age group (Y) Time (s); mean ± SD Hebrew (a) English 3 (b) Spanish 12 (c) Cantonese 15 (d) Mandarin 10 (e) Portuguese 14 (f) Italian 11 (g) French 17 (h) Latvian 16 (i) p‐Value a
6.0–6.11 N 13 52 224 53 238 78 65 32 NA
Vertical 59.13 ± 13.88(c) 63.11 ± 15.59(c,d) 86.30 ± 23.02(d,e,f,g,h) 50.98 ± 13.16(e,f,g) 62.84 ± 17.97(f,g) 72.44 ± 17.49 72.29 ± 20.99 62.29 ± 13.51 NA <0.001
Horizontal 77.87 ± 23.46(c,f,g) 98.26 ± 32.61(c,d) 146.90 ± 41.60(d,e,f,g) 71.27 ± 18.45(e,f,g,h) 86.67 ± 34.10(f,g) 107.99 ± 24.49 108.12 ± 30.49 99.20 ± 23.77 NA <0.001
Error 7.69 ± 7.88 (b) 15.22 ± 11.49(c,d,e,f) 7.70 ± 4.43(h) 9.66 ± 7.51(g) 7.17 ± 8.22(g,h) 8.41 ± 6.97(g) 14.9 ± 8.3 13.0 ± 11.15 NA <0.001
Ratio 1.36 ± 0.36(c) 1.58 ± 0.45(e) 1.67 ± 0.37(d,e) 1.41 ± 0.25 1.38 ± 0.28(f,g,h) 1.53 ± 0.34 1.53 ± 0.29 1.60 ± 0.31 NA <0.001
7.0–7.11 N 36 75 227 63 183 92 196 42 141
Vertical 54.13 ± 13.69(d) 54.83 ± 9.20(d) 58.27 ± 13.04(d,e,g,h) 43.34 ± 8.82 50.48 ± 9.72(f) 60.39 ± 14.48(g,h) 52.74 ± 10.17 51.83 ± 9.38 NA <0.001
Horizontal 76.90 ± 22.03(d) 87.94 ± 28.18(d,e,g,h) 81.38 ± 26.91(d,e,f,g) 57.14 ± 14.21(f,g,h) 65.86 ± 15.26(f,g) 81.47 ± 21.48 75.01 ± 19.33 73.38 ± 15.27 NA <0.001
Error 8.55 ± 7.84(e) 12.50 ± 12.91(c,d,e,f,g,h) 6.97 ± 6.41(e) 5.59 ± 6.72 4.26 ± 6.19(g) 5.00 ± 5.84(g) 7.9 ± 7.6 6.88 ± 5.96 NA <0.001
Ratio 1.44 ± 0.25(c) 1.60 ± 0.41(d,e,f,g,h) 1.70 ± 0.29(d,e,f,g,h,i) 1.32 ± 0.16(i) 1.31 ± 0.22(g,i) 1.36 ± 0.24(i) 1.43 ± 0.25 1.42 ± 0.18 1.49 ± 0.27 <0.001
8.0–8.11 N 36 93 240 54 195 90 200 43 114
Vertical 48.49 ± 10.83(d) 46.76 ± 7.89(d,f) 49.60 ± 9.65(d,e) 38.50 ± 7.99(e,f,g,h) 43.64 ± 9.14(f,h) 50.97 ± 10.61(g) 45.77 ± 9.68 50.15 ± 6.99 NA <0.001
Horizontal 69.31 ± 28.52(b,d,e,g) 57.73 ± 12.32(c,d,f,h) 68.30 ± 19.57(d,e,g) 47.55 ± 10.60(f,g,h) 52.76 ± 12.74(f,g,h) 65.67 ± 17.62 59.91 ± 14.87 67.13 ± 13.62 NA <0.001
Error 5.60 ± 5.35(e,f) 4.61 ± 6.91(e,f) 5.55 ± 5.97(e,f,g) 3.96 ± 3.74 1.86 ± 3.18(g,h) 2.33 ± 3.31(h) 4.0 ± 4.6 6.44 ± 6.79 NA <0.001
Ratio 1.41 ± 0.28(b,d,e) 1.24 ± 0.18(c,i) 1.40 ± 0.31(d,e,f,g) 1.24 ± 0.15(i) 1.21 ± 0.20(g,i) 1.30 ± 0.27(i) 1.31 ± 0.20(i) 1.33 ± 0.18 1.45 ± 0.35 <0.001
9.0–9.11 N 42 84 238 50 156 65 188 50 178
Vertical 46.67 ± 10.29(c,d,e,g) 42.33 ± 8.20(d,e,f) 42.37 ± 7.46(d,e,f) 36.53 ± 6.61(f,g,h) 37.18 ± 7.76(f,g,h) 48.10 ± 10.45(g) 41.98 ± 7.89 44.66 ± 9.92 NA <0.001
Horizontal 63.60 ± 23.95(b,c,d,e,g) 51.13 ± 13.30(d,e,f,h) 54.54 ± 11.60(d,e) 43.00 ± 7.83(f,g,h) 43.03 ± 10.66(f,g,h) 57.72 ± 12.11(g) 52.04 ± 12.78(h) 58.87 ± 15.91 NA <0.001
Error 2.97 ± 3.95 2.17 ± 4.10 1.69 ± 2.66(h) 2.98 ± 3.28 1.45 ± 3.83(h) 1.72 ± 3.67 2.6 ± 3.8 3.58 ± 4.66 NA <0.001
Ratio 1.35 ± 0.24(b,d,e,f,g) 1.21 ± 0.19(c,i) 1.31 ± 0.22(d,e,f,g) 1.18 ± 0.12(i) 1.16 ± 0.18(g,h,i) 1.22 ± 0.21(i) 1.24 ± 0.18(i) 1.30 ± 0.18 1.34 ± 0.22 <0.001
10.0–10.11 N 34 73 258 52 184 88 184 38 135
Vertical 42.20 ± 8.91(d,e,g) 40.28 ± 4.73(d,e,f) 40.19 ± 7.65(d,e,f) 29.38 ± 6.00(f,g,h) 32.33 ± 6.04(f,g,h,g) 43.70 ± 9.85(g) 38.13 ± 6.35 41.31 ± 5.79 NA <0.001
Horizontal 52.67 ± 12.03(c,d,e,g) 47.64 ± 10.11(d,e) 47.24 ± 10.37(d,e) 33.69 ± 8.72(f,g,h) 36.04 ± 6.98(d,e,g) 49.30 ± 9.91(g) 44.72 ± 8.08 (h) 50.98 ± 8.04 NA <0.001
Error 3.29 ± 3.84(c,d,e,f) 1.91 ± 2.68(c,e,f) 0.81 ± 1.76(g,h) 1.56 ± 2.16 0.71 ± 1.48(g,h) 0.76 ± 2.47(g,h) 2.0 ± 2.6 2.21 ± 3.86 NA <0.001
Ratio 1.23 ± 0.14(e) 1.19 ± 0.17(i) 1.25 ± 0.16(d,e,f,g) 1.14 ± 0.15(i) 1.12 ± 0.13 1.14 ± 0.13(i) 1.18 ± 0.12(i) 1.23 ± 0.14 1.30 ± 0.29 <0.001
11.0–11.11 N 28 82 252 33 166 83 77 27 118
Vertical 39.19 ± 7.51(d,e) 37.14 ± 5.42(d,e) 36.16 ± 6.32(d,e) 29.83 ± 5.36(f,g,h) 30.35 ± 5.67(f,g,h) 38.33 ± 5.51(g) 35.06 ± 6.41 38.72 ± 8.20 NA <0.001
Horizontal 48.97 ± 14.32(b,c,d,e,f,g) 42.62 ± 7.61(d,e) 43.49 ± 10.04(d,e,g) 32.87 ± 7.03(f,g,h) 32.83 ± 6.48(f,g,h) 42.98 ± 6.35 39.49 ± 8.44 43.94 ± 9.83 NA <0.001
Error 2.00 ± 3.57(c,e,f) 1.68 ± 2.34(c,e,f) 0.51 ± 1.68(d,g,h) 1.70 ± 2.72(e,f) 0.55 ± 1.51(g,h) 0.45 ± 1.63(g,h) 1.7 ± 2.0 2.14 ± 2.74 NA <0.001
Ratio 1.26 ± 0.22(b,d,e,f,g,h) 1.15 ± 0.13(i) 1.18 ± 0.15(e,i) 1.10 ± 0.11(i) 1.09 ± 0.14(i) 1.13 ± 0.12(i) 1.13 ± 0.12(i) 1.13 ± 0.11(i) 1.25 ± 0.18 <0.001
12.0–12.11 N 23 38 NA NA 166 94 79 26 153
Vertical 36.36 ± 6.59(e) 35.14 ± 5.87(e) NA NA 28.06 ± 4.80(f) 35.47 ± 5.57 31.55 ± 5.74 35.96 ± 6.16 NA <0.001
Horizontal 42.73 ± 11.71(e) 39.35 ± 8.11(e) NA NA 30.67 ± 5.56(f) 39.29 ± 5.20 35.34 ± 6.47 41.91 ± 7.31 NA <0.001
Error 0.91 ± 1.85 1.11 ± 1.17 NA NA 0.51 ± 1.34(g,h) 0.43 ± 1.36(g,h) 1.1 ± 1.8 1.61 ± 2.07 NA <0.001
Ratio 1.15 ± 0.17 1.12 ± 0.10(i) NA NA 1.10 ± 0.12(i) 1.12 ± 0.13(i) 1.12 ± 0.09(i) 1.17 ± 0.12 1.23 ± 0.17 <0.001
13.0–13.11 N 13 37 NA NA NA 87 85 NA 171
Vertical 34.56 ± 6.16(g) 33.75 ± 6.53(g) NA NA NA 34.13 ± 4.81(g) 29.71 ± 4.58 NA NA <0.001
Horizontal 44.92 ± 12.57(b,f,g) 37.56 ± 7.23(g) NA NA NA 37.46 ± 6.10(g) 33.16 ± 6.57 NA NA <0.001
Error 0.55 ± 1.16 1.61 ± 2.15(f) NA NA NA 0.48 ± 1.70(g) 1.2 ± 1.9 NA NA <0.001
Ratio 1.20 ± 0.09(f,i) 1.12 ± 0.12(i) NA NA NA 1.10 ± 0.10(i) 1.12 ± 0.12(i) NA 1.71 ± 0.14 <0.001

Note: Letters in parentheses indicate statistically significant result between pair of groups by post‐hoc analysis (p < 0.05).

Abbreviations: H/V, horizontal/vertical; NA, not applicable.

a

Analysed by one‐way ANOVA and post‐hoc Tukey–Kramer.

Vertical and horizontal times tended to decrease as age increased across all language groups, reflecting improved performance with age. Across different age groups (6.0–13.11 years), significant differences in vertical and horizontal times were observed among children speaking different languages. Cantonese children consistently had the shortest times across all age groups, while Spanish children, especially those aged 6.0–6.11 years, had the longest times. Cantonese and Mandarin children generally showed lower ratios, whereas Spanish‐speaking children exhibited higher ratios.

DISCUSSION

The original DEM norms were developed in English and are extensively validated. 3 , 6 The main purpose of this study was to determine if these original norms can be used with children whose primary language is Hebrew. These results indicate that there are significant similarities between the Hebrew results and the original English norms. Vertical time norms across all ages are equivalent between the English and Hebrew values. The horizontal time, errors and ratio of the Hebrew results were most similar to the English and French norms when compared with the other languages.

In the current study, the H/V ratios of the Hebrew‐speaking children were found to be comparable with the established English norms, suggesting similar challenges in horizontal reading despite the differences in reading direction between the two languages. A previous study on the directional effect in the DEM test indicated that younger Hebrew‐speaking children tended to prefer reading numbers from R to L. 19 However, no such preference was observed in the present study. This discrepancy may be attributed to the larger sample size and the inclusion of children from diverse educational backgrounds, with 28% of participants being Ultra‐Orthodox, compared to 67% in the earlier study. In Ultra‐Orthodox schools, there is a strong emphasis on Hebrew reading, while English and mathematics are often not introduced at the early stages of education. Future research on directionality preferences in Arabic‐speaking children, who also read from right to left, could provide further insight into the influence of reading direction on young children's performance.

A secondary aim of this study was to compare data from Hebrew‐speaking children with norms previously published in English, Spanish, Cantonese, Mandarin, Portuguese, Italian, French and Latvian‐speaking children. There were two Spanish studies reporting DEM norms, 12 , 13 and we chose to include the one with the larger sample size in the analysis.

Hebrew‐speaking children exhibited significant differences compared with other language groups. For ages 6.0–7.11 years, they had higher vertical and horizontal times compared with Cantonese and Mandarin children, resulting in similar H/V ratios. Spanish children in this age group had the slowest times but made fewer errors than English speakers, possibly due to an emphasis on accuracy, with Hebrew‐speaking children's error rates being similar to those of Spanish speakers. In ages 8–9.11 years, Hebrew speakers had significantly higher vertical and horizontal times compared to Mandarin and Cantonese speakers and more errors than Mandarin and Portuguese speakers. This trend continued in ages 10–11.11 years, where Hebrew‐speaking children had higher times and more errors than Cantonese, Mandarin, Italian and Spanish speakers, and their ratio was most similar to Latvian. In the oldest group (12–13.11 years), Hebrew‐speaking children showed no significant differences in errors or ratios compared with English, Portuguese, Italian and Latvian children.

The differences between Cantonese and Mandarin‐speaking children and others were highlighted by Xie et al., 10 explaining that in China, children often begin reading earlier than their American counterparts. This explains the norms of the younger aged children. In older aged children, an additional explanation proposed that Chinese is an ideographic language, and numbers are easier to pronounce in Chinese than English. This also applies to Hebrew, where most numbers have several syllables that could lengthen the measured time. The ratios of all languages could theoretically be identical no matter how complex the language pronunciation.

Studies of DEM norms in Cantonese, 11 Portuguese 14 and Italian 15 consider the differences in language, educational systems and cultural practices as explanations for the variations across different languages. 11 , 14 , 15 Regardless of these differences, Moiroud reiterated the universal importance of the DEM test for judging the reading performance and speed of visual processing, particularly in children with reading disorders. 9 Spanish 13 and French 17 studies have concluded that their specific language DEM norms are similar to the original English values. French norms show similarities with Hebrew in terms of vertical and horizontal times, and the errors are somewhat comparable. The DEM results for Hebrew‐speaking children align with other norms, particularly those for English and French. 17

A limitation of this study is the relatively small sample size in the youngest and oldest age groups, which may affect the generalisability of the results for these age ranges. However, the overall sample size was sufficient based on the conducted power analysis. Specific DEM norms per age were not established; rather, only comparisons to other language results.

Based on the findings of this study, it is recommended that practitioners testing Hebrew‐speaking children continue to use the English norms and allow children to read in the direction they are most comfortable with, ensuring their preferred reading orientation.

CONCLUSIONS

This study compared DEM results for Hebrew‐speaking children with eight other languages. DEM test values for Hebrew‐speaking children aligned with the norms from other languages, particularly the French and original English norms, with consistent ratio scores. It is recommended that practitioners who test Hebrew‐speaking children continue to use the English norms and allow children to read using their preferred reading direction.

AUTHOR CONTRIBUTIONS

Hadas Ben‐Eli: Conceptualization (equal); formal analysis (lead); methodology (equal); project administration (lead); validation (equal); writing – original draft (equal). Hadas Blique: Data curation (equal); investigation (equal); writing – review and editing (equal). Mitchell Scheiman: Conceptualization (equal); methodology (equal); supervision (equal); writing – review and editing (equal). Rachel Eichler: Conceptualization (equal); investigation (equal); methodology (equal); project administration (equal); supervision (lead); writing – original draft (equal).

FUNDING INFORMATION

This research received no specific grant from any funding agency in the public, commercial, or not‐for‐profit sectors.

CONFLICT OF INTEREST STATEMENT

The authors report no conflicts of interest and have no proprietary interest in any of the materials mentioned in this article.

CONSENT

A written parental consent for participation in the study was obtained for all participants.

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS

The authors thank all study participants.

Ben‐Eli H, Blique H, Scheiman M, Eichler R. Developmental eye movement test results of Hebrew‐speaking children with cross‐linguistic comparisons. Ophthalmic Physiol Opt. 2025;45:43–49. 10.1111/opo.13409

REFERENCES

  • 1. Azadi R, Zhu EY, McPeek RM. Modulation of saccade trajectories during sequential saccades. J Neurophysiol. 2021;125:796–804. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2. Paterson KB, McGowan VA, Warrington KL, Li L, Li S, Xie F, et al. Effects of normative aging on eye movements during reading. Vision. 2020;4:7. 10.3390/vision4010007 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3. Richman JE, Garzia RP. Developmental eye movement test (DEM): examiner's booklet, version 1. South Bend: Bernell Corp; 1987. [Google Scholar]
  • 4. Garzia RP, Richman JE, Nicholson SB, Gaines CS. A new visual‐verbal saccade test: the development eye movement test (DEM). J Am Optom Assoc. 1990;61:124–135. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5. Ayton LN, Abel LA, Fricke TR, McBrien NA. Developmental eye movement test: what is it really measuring? Optom Vis Sci. 2009;86:722–730. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6. Facchin A. Spotlight on the developmental eye movement (DEM) test. Clin Optom. 2021;13:73–81. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7. Webber A, Wood J, Gole G, Brown B. DEM test, visagraph eye movement recordings, and reading ability in children. Optom Vis Sci. 2011;88:295–302. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8. Palomo‐Álvarez C, Puell MC. Relationship between oculomotor scanning determined by the DEM test and a contextual reading test in schoolchildren with reading difficulties. Graefes Arch Clin Exp Ophthalmol. 2009;247:1243–1249. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9. Moiroud L, Gerard CL, Peyre H, Bucci MP. Developmental eye movement test and dyslexic children: a pilot study with eye movement recordings. PLoS One. 2018;13:e0200907. 10.1371/journal.pone.0200907 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10. Xie Y, Shi C, Tong M, Zhang M, Li T, Xu Y, et al. Developmental eye movement (DEM) test norms for Mandarin Chinese‐speaking Chinese children. PLoS One. 2016;11:e0148481. 10.1371/journal.pone.0148481 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11. Facchin A, Maffioletti S, Carnevali T. Developmental eye movement (DEM) test: normative data for Italian population. Optom Vis Dev. 2012;43:162–179. [Google Scholar]
  • 12. Fernandez‐Velazquez FJ, Fernandez‐Fidalgo MJ. Do DEM test scores change with respect to the language? Norms for Spanish‐speaking population. Optom Vis Sci. 1995;72:902–906. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13. Jiménez R, González MD, Pérez MA, Garcia JA. Evolution of accommodative function and development of ocular movements in children. Ophthalmic Physiol Opt. 2003;23:97–107. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14. Baptista AMG, De Sousa RARC, De Morais Guerra Casal CC, Marques R, Da Silva C. Norms for the developmental eye movement test for Portuguese children. Optom Vis Sci. 2011;88:864–871. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15. Pang PC, Lam CS, Woo GC. The developmental eye movement (DEM) test and Cantonese‐speaking children in Hong Kong SAR, China. Clin Exp Optom. 2010;93:213–223. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16. Serdjukova J, Ekimane L, Valeinis J, Skilters J, Krumina G. How strong and weak readers perform on the developmental eye movement test (DEM): norms for Latvian school‐aged children. Read Writ. 2017;30:233–252. [Google Scholar]
  • 17. Moiroud L, Royo A, Bucci MP. The developmental eye movement test in French children. Optom Vis Sci. 2020;97:978–983. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18. Medland C, Walter H, Margaret WJ. Eye movements and poor reading: does the developmental eye movement test measure cause or effect? Ophthalmic Physiol Opt. 2010;30:740–747. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19. Eichler R, Scheiman M, Ben‐Eli H. Comparing the directional effect of number calling on the developmental eye movement test results in Hebrew‐speaking children. Ophthalmic Physiol Opt. 2023;43:623–628. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Ophthalmic & Physiological Optics are provided here courtesy of Wiley

RESOURCES