Abstract
Objective
The literature on clinical psychophysiology highlights the possibility of using Heart Rate Variability (HRV) as an index of psychophysical balance and resilience to stress. This study investigates the differences in stress reactivity and subsequent recovery between a group of migraineurs and healthy controls.
Methods
Socio-demographic (i.e., sex, age, profession, marital status, and level of education) and psychophysiological (HR and HRV) measures of a group of thirty subjects with migraine (26 migraineurs without aura (86.7%), 2 migraineurs with aura (6.7%), and 2 migraineurs with and without aura (6.7%)) and from thirty healthy control subjects were collected. In particular, HRV was analyzed through frequency-domain parameters, including Low-Frequency (LF; 0.04–0.15 Hz) and High-Frequency (HF; 0.15–0.4 Hz) bands as well as LF/HF ratio during a Psychophysiological Stress Profile (PSP) structured in seven phases: (1) Baseline, (2) Objective stressor 1 (Stroop Test), (3) Rest 1, (4) Objective stressor 2 (Mental Arithmetic Task), (5) Rest 2, (6) Subjective stressor (recount a significant life event), and (7) Rest 3. The LF, HF, and LF/HF ratio values were transformed into a logarithmic scale (i.e., log-LF, log-HF, and log LF/HF ratio). Additionally, LF and HF were converted into normalized units (0-100) (i.e., LF% and HF%) which, in turn, were used to obtain reactivity and recovery to stress through delta values (Δ) calculation.
Results
Subjects with migraine reported greater ΔLF% levels of reactivity and recovery to subjective stressor, demonstrating a prevalence of sympathetic activity while recounting a personal life event. At the same time, a lowering of the same values was found in the subjects of the group control.
Discussion
Our results underline the importance of conducting a psychophysiological assessment in patients with headaches because reduced stress management skills could influence the clinical manifestations of the disease, considering stress as one of the most common triggers for migraine patients.
Supplementary Information
The online version contains supplementary material available at 10.1007/s10072-024-07739-7.
Keywords: Migraine, Psychophysiology, Autonomic imbalance, Heart rate variability, Stress response
Introduction
Migraine is a complex multifactorial neurological disorder that represents one of the most disabling conditions [1]. Despite the recent improvement in knowledge about migraines and the development of new preventive pharmacological options, the burden of migraines has continued to enlarge in recent years [2]. In 2017, migraine became the leading cause of years lived with disability in people aged 15 to 49, with a prevalence of approximately 15% of the general population [3].
As migraine impacts how the central nervous system (CNS) processes sensory information, it can lead to various symptoms, including sensitivity to light and sound, nausea, dizziness, and aversion to certain smells [4]. The disorder is marked by distinct phases, including preictal, ictal, postictal, and interictal. Each phase is linked to the activation of specific regions of the brain and is associated with a variety of clinical manifestations [4].
The pathophysiology of migraine is currently not yet fully understood [5]. Nonetheless, the most accredited hypothesis contemplates the involvement of the trigeminovascular pathway to be primary [6]. The path formed by the trigeminal nerve, with its axonal projections to the meninges and blood vessels, transmits nociceptive information to different regions of the brain, including the cortex [7]. Another hypothesis that received scientific validation concerns central sensitization, a mechanism widely studied in migraine pathophysiology [8–10]. Referring to research, migraine is characterized by elevated reactivity of CNS neurons to normal or subthreshold afferent inputs [11]. In general, sensitization is a common feature of chronic pain conditions as it triggers CNS excitability with repeated nociceptive input, resulting in widespread hyperalgesia and/or allodynia, as well as alterations in cognitive and emotional-affective processing [12, 13].
In addition to eating poorly on time, tiredness, and lack of sleep, the presence of stress/tension is among the most common precipitating factors the scientific community recognizes [14–16]. The stress response is a physiological and hormonal reaction that prepares the body to cope with stress by activating its energy resources [17]. Specifically, the sympathetic branch of the autonomic nervous system (ANS) induces the release of neurotransmitters, such as adrenaline and norepinephrine which, in turn, favor the mobilization of glucose, and the increase in heart rate (HR) and blood pressure [18]. At the same time, the activation of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis favors the release of stress hormones (i.e., cortisol) which further enhance physical and cognitive performance [19, 20]. Nonetheless, prolonged stress can negatively affect an individual’s health, such as compromising the immune and cardiovascular systems. Several studies highlighted that stress is higher among migraine patients than healthy patients and above the clinical threshold level [21, 22]. The impact of stress on migraine patients may be higher, as they show a lower adaptability to changes (i.e., sleep changes, lifestyle changes, hormonal changes, dietary changes, etc.). Indeed migraine is characterized by an augmented cortical excitability [23]. On top of this, stress was confirmed as a primary factor in triggering and perpetuating migraine attacks [24–26] in nearly 70% of migraine patients [27], as demonstrated to contribute to the severity and frequency of migraine attacks [14, 15, 26]. In this regard, the predominant role of autonomic imbalance was manifested [16, 26, 28, 29]. Therefore, migraine has a great disability and burden, which can be a stressor itself, creating a vicious circle between migraine attacks and stressful events [15].
Recent studies found HR variability (HRV) to be a useful parameter for the evaluation of autonomic imbalance connected to the chronic stress response [30–32]. HRV is the variation in time between consecutive heartbeats (RR intervals) [33]. It consists of coupling and synchronizing the cardiac rhythm with the phases of respiration. Deep, regular breathing was validated to augment HR fluctuation and respiratory sinus arrhythmia, and it appears capable of optimizing the balance between the sympathetic (SNS) and parasympathetic (PNS) nervous systems [34]. The two components of the autonomic nervous system (SNS and PNS) are also known as the fight-or-flight mechanism and the tend-and-befriend behaviors, respectively [35, 36]. In other words, a higher prevalence of SNS activity reflects fight-or-flight behaviors, while PNS activity is associated with tend-and-befriend behaviors as well as relaxation [37]. HRV analysis is commonly conducted in either the time domain or frequency domain [38, 39]. In frequency domain analysis, the HR oscillation is divided into four bands: (1) ultra-low frequencies (ULF), (2) very low frequencies (VLF), (3) low frequencies (LF), and (4) high frequencies (HF). LF power (0.04–0.15 Hz) can be produced by both the PNS and SNS, as well as blood pressure regulation via baroreceptors, primarily by the PNS, or by baroreflex activity alone. On the other hand, HF power (0.15–0.4 Hz) is equivalent to the well-known respiratory sinus arrhythmia and is considered to represent vagal control of HR [40] and primarily reflects the PNS activity [35]. In addition, some researchers attested that the LF/HF ratio reflects both sympathovagal balance and sympathetic modulations [41], being sensitive to mental stress [42]. The power of the LF and HF spectral components of HRV can be expressed as absolute (ms2) or normalized units (0-100). The normalization procedure has proved crucial to the interpretation of data [43].
Research suggests that an optimal level of HRV is associated with self-regulatory capacity, adaptability, and resilience, whereas decreased vagal tone is a risk factor for cardiovascular disease and all-cause mortality [38, 44, 45]. Studies further linked decreased vagal tone to increased inflammation and less ability to control pain, especially in patients with headaches [46, 47]. A systematic review with meta-analysis attested that patients with migraines manifested a decrease in both sympathetic and parasympathetic modulation, observed by enlarged P-wave dispersion, longer QTc intervals, and a decrease in the rate of deep breathing, with no significant differences in the time-domain analyses of HRV [48]. Moreover, during the ictal period, episodic migraine patients presented a decrease in parasympathetic modulation documented by lower SDNN (a time-domain parameter) and in sympathetic modulation as reported by LF [49, 50].
HRV analysis was recommended for both long-term (24 h) and short-term (5 min) studies [30]. Although 24-hour HRV analysis is useful for increasing frequency resolution [43], its application in samples of volunteer subjects is difficult. Particularly, changes in the physical or mental states, environments, and even noises in ambulatory recordings can severely affect the results of HRV analysis [41]. In contrast, rigorous experimental control is possible for short recordings (i.e., a few minutes) carried out in well-controlled environments [41].
Long-term recordings are carried out with dynamic electrocardiogram (ECG) according to Holter [51], while short-term recordings can be made with precordial ECG [41]. Some recording devices do not use the traditional QRS complex of an ECG to calculate HR and HRV. To illustrate, PPG uses a photoelectric sensor that estimates changes in blood volume to calculate HR. The PPG technique involves using a photocell (such as an infrared light-emitting diode) positioned on an easily accessible tissue area with blood capillaries (i.e., finger or earlobe). The energy emitted by an infrared source passes through the tissue and is reflected on it. The amount of light can then assess changes in blood volume (due to heartbeats) in an area reflected to the photodetector, and, thus, form the basis for estimating heartbeats [30, 52].
Several studies corroborated PPG as an accurate method for beat-to-beat HR information in different patient groups [53]. Except for breathing protocols based on the resonant frequency exercises which documented lower accuracy of PPG compared to ECG [43], a reasonable correlation between these two measures was validated [53]. Specifically, a study by Parak et al. [54], demonstrated that PPG had an average beat-to-beat absolute error after applying an artifact correction algorithm of 5.94 ms, indicating similar accuracy to ECG and suggesting potential use in a clinical setting [53, 55]. Lastly, although these two measures may diverge under acute stress conditions [56], PPG proved to reliably assess acute stress reactivity [57]. Specifically considering the reactivity to stress, there is evidence that the calculation of the difference between induced stress and baseline as well as that between the rest phase and the previously administered stressor can manifest some psychophysiological phenomena that are not appreciated only in the baseline phase [58].
In light of this evidence, the objective of the present study was to analyze HRV in migraine patients recording a psychophysiological profile. In particular, a group of migraineurs may demonstrate differences in HRV values under various types of stressor stimuli compared to a group of controls.
Materials and methods
Participants and procedure
This case-control study enrolled people between the ages of 18 and 65. People who decided to voluntarily participate responded to posters allowing them to scan a QR code and access the Outlook Calendar to book an appointment.
The clinical group was composed of people diagnosed with migraine. The headache diagnosis was made by a board-certified neurologist using the International Headache Society (IHS) criteria [25]. Criteria for inclusion in the study were age > 18 years old, completion of informed consent, and no history of neuropsychiatric syndromes (i.e., previous head trauma, epilepsy, etc.) and/or physical diseases (i.e., sensory disturbances of sight and/or hearing) that may limit the protocol procedure. Exclusion criteria were the presence of pain in the musculoskeletal and dental systems, history of neck, temporomandibular joint, shoulder, or back surgery, the presence of other neurological deficits or a rheumatoid disorder, the assumption of analgesics within 24 h before the investigation of psychotropic drugs with rebound effects on the ANS in the last three months (i.e., tricyclic antidepressants; antipsychotics; norepinephrine–dopamine reuptake inhibitors, such as bupropion; serotonin modulators, such as mirtazapine and trazodone; serotonin-norepinephrine reuptake inhibitors, such as venlafaxine and duloxetine), and overuse headache medications as defined by the IHS.
The control group consisted of age-matched volunteers. Participants in the control group had no headaches for at least 3 months before the study and had experienced occasional mild headaches (< 5 times per year), for which they had never sought medical care. Criteria for inclusion in the study were age > 18 years old, completion of informed consent, and no history of neuropsychiatric syndromes (i.e., previous head trauma, epilepsy, etc.) and/or physical diseases (i.e., sensory disturbances of sight and/or hearing) that may limit the protocol procedure.
The researchers explained the purpose of the study as well as the psychophysiological assessment procedure, without specifying the single phases so as not to influence the stress response. Subsequently, participants were offered the option to book another appointment with a licensed clinical psychologist to receive an exhaustive commentary on the reports.
The Ethical Committee of the University of Parma approved the study (protocol number: 118310/2024). The research was conducted following the Guidelines for Good Research Practice of the University of Parma (2020). All procedures were conducted following the 1964 Declaration of Helsinki of the World Medical Association as well as the 2005 Universal Declaration on Bioethics and Human Rights defined by the UNESCO regarding research involving human participants.
Measures
All enrolled people underwent a Psychophysiological Stress Profile [59]. The PSP consists of the execution of a 14-minute assessment including 7 phases (2 min each), after a 4-minute adaptation phase in which the subject is asked to sit quietly in a chair. During the experiment, patients were asked to remain as still as possible, with their feet placed on the ground at 45 degrees and their arms on the armrests. The room temperature is maintained between 19 and 21 °C for the entire duration of the recording. The seven phases include (1) Baseline, the person is asked to remain still and with eyes closed; (2) Objective stressor 1, a computerized version of the Stroop Test is proposed; (3) Rest 1, the person is asked to relax as much as possible; (4) Objective stressor 2, the Mental Arithmetic Task is proposed, which consists of mental calculations of serial subtraction (e.g., subtract consecutively the number 7 from 1008); (5) Rest 2, as described in Recovery 1; (6) Subjective stressor, the person is asked to briefly describe a significant life event; and (7) Rest 3, as described in Recovery 1.
Biograph Procomp Infiniti 5.0 (Thought Technology Ltd., Canada) was used to collect data regarding HR and Blood Volume Pressure (BVP) with a blood volume pulse sensing sensor (also known as PPG), using a cuff positioned on the finger of the right upper limb. PPG optically detects pulse waves by assessing changes in light absorption caused by blood flow [60]. Thus, the BVP sends infrared light to the skin surface and measures the amount of reflected light. The more blood there is in the skin, the more light is reflected.
The HRV indices analyzed in the frequency domain were low-frequency (LF, 0.04–0.15 Hz) and high-frequency (HF, 0.15–0.40 Hz) bands, obtained from the nonparametric method fast Fourier transform (FFT). The power spectrum was subsequently quantified into various frequency-domain measurements as defined previously (Table 1) [41]. Due to the skewed distribution of frequency domain variables, a natural logarithmic transformation was applied (log-LF and log-HF, respectively). Both measurements contribute to the formulation of the fraction between LF and HF (LF/HF ratio), which is also transformed into a natural logarithmic scale (log-LF/HF ratio). Additionally, LF and HF were normalized by the percentage of total power to detect SNS and PNS influence on HRV (LF% and HF%, respectively) [61]. Specifically, normalized values for HRV were obtained by calculating the percentage of LF and HF power relative to the total spectrum power minus the VLF band (< 0.04 Hz). The normalization procedure was performed to minimize total power variations in the absolute values of LF and HF [61].
Table 1.
Variable | Units | Definition | Frequency Range |
---|---|---|---|
VLF | ms2 | Power in VLF range | 0.003–0.04 Hz |
LF | ms2 | Power in LF range | 0.04–0.15 Hz |
HF | ms2 | Power in HF range | 0.15–0.4 Hz |
LF/HF | ratio | LF (ms2)/HF (ms2) | |
LF% | nu | LF power in normalized units: LF/(total power - VLF) X 100 | |
HF% | nu | HF power in normalized units: HF/(total power - VLF) X 100 |
Legend: VLF = Very-Low Frequency; LF = Low Frequency; HF = High Frequency; nu = Normalized units
Statistical analysis
All statistical analyses were performed using SPSS (Version 28.0.1.0; IBM Corp, Armonk, NY, USA). First, differences in socio-demographic variables (i.e., gender, age, marital status, educational level, and occupation) between patients and control groups were assessed at baseline, using a Chi-Squared Test or an Independent Samples T-Test.
Concerning the psychophysiological values, all the assumptions for the conduction of parametric statistics were respected for HR and HRV (i.e., HR, log-LF, log-HF, log-LF/HF ratio, LF%, and HF%). A repeated-measures ANOVA was conducted to compare HR, log-LF, log-HF, log-LF/HF ratio, LF%, and HF% between groups in every phase of the PSP (i.e., Baseline, Objective stressor 1, Rest 1, Objective stressor 2, Rest 2, Subjective stressor, and Rest 3), one at time. Post-hoc analyses were made using the Bonferroni method.
Furthermore, delta (Δ) values were calculated for LF% and HF% to have psychophysiological reactivity and recovery according to the recommendations of Laborde and colleagues [40]. Reactivity was obtained by calculating the difference between the stress phases and baseline (i.e., Objective Stressor 1 – Baseline = Reactivity 1; Objective Stressor 2 – Baseline = Reactivity 2; and Subjective Stressor – Baseline = Reactivity 3) to quantify changes in participants’ mean LF and HF HRV percentage values (LF% and HF%, respectively) during stress induction (120-s) compared to the participant’s baseline values (120-s). Similarly, reactivity was obtained by calculating the difference between the rest phase and stress induction (i.e., Recovery 1 - Objective Stressor 1 = Recovery 1; Recovery 2 - Objective Stressor 2 = Recovery 2; and Recovery 3 - Subjective Stressor = Recovery 3) to quantify the changes in the participants’ mean LF and HF percent HRV values during the recovery phase (120-s) compared to the participant’s value during the stress induction (120-s).
Consequently, an Independent Samples T-Test was adopted to compare the delta values (i.e., Reactivity 1, Reactivity 2, Reactivity 3, Recovery 1, Recovery 2, and Recovery 3) between control and migraine groups.
Results
Sample characteristics and comparison between patients and controls
The patient group was composed of 26 patients with migraine without aura (86.7%), 2 patients with migraine with aura (6.7%), and 2 patients with migraine with and without aura (6.7%).
Table 2 shows the comparison between groups. No significant differences emerged in the socio-demographic variables, except for gender which highlights a higher prevalence of women in the sample of migraineurs. Comparisons between migraineurs and controls in HR, log-LF, log-HF, log-LF/HF ratio, LF%, and HF% HRV scores are reported in Table 3. None of the analyses reached statistical significance, although LF% and HF% approached the threshold of 0.05.
Table 2.
Variable | Control group (n=30) | Migraine group (n=30) | T or X2 | p |
---|---|---|---|---|
Age, M (SD) | 28.40 (10.70) | 29.07 (10.34) | t(59) = 0.25 | 0.34 |
Sex, N (%) | χ2 (1, N = 59) = 8.86 | 0.01 | ||
Male | 16 (54.8%) | 5 (16.1%) | ||
Female | 14 (45.2%) | 25 (83.9%) | ||
Marital status, N (%) | χ2 (1, N = 59) = 0.10 | 0.29 | ||
Single/Unmarried | 24 (80.6%) | 23 (77.4%) | ||
Married/Cohabitant | 6 (19.4%) | 7 (22.6%) | ||
Education Level, N (%) | χ2 (2, N = 59) = 1.35 | 0.65 | ||
Middle school | 1 (3.2%) | 3 (9.7%) | ||
High school | 13 (41.9%) | 14 (48.4%) | ||
University | 16 (54.8%) | 13 (41.9%) | ||
Current Occupation, N (%) | χ2 (1, N = 59) = 0.08 | 0.45 | ||
Student | 20 (64.5%) | 21 (67.7%) | ||
Employed | 10 (35.5%) | 9 (32.3%) |
Table 3.
Control group (n = 30) | Migraine group (n = 30) | Effects | ||||||||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Baseline | Objective Stress 1 | Rest 1 | Objective Stress 2 | Rest 2 | Subjective Stress | Rest 3 | Baseline | Objective Stress 1 | Rest 1 | Objective Stress 2 | Rest 2 | Subjective Stress | Rest 3 | Time x group F | p | |
HR (bpm) | 73.32 ± 12.53 | 85.96 ± 13.01 | 72.81 ± 10.82 | 86.31 ± 12.56 | 72.94 ± 10.53 | 90.14 ± 11.13 | 73.16 ± 10.95 | 72.88 ± 10.14 | 85.49 ± 12.83 | 72.67 ± 8.50 | 85.96 ± 13.85 | 71.81 ± 7.96 | 88.71 ± 11.98 | 73.14 ± 8.75 | 0.03 | 0.86 |
log-LF (ms2) | 5.52 ± 1.11 | 5.37 ± 1.27 | 5.29 ± 1.23 | 5.84 ± 1.20 | 5.49 ± 1.27 | 6.75 ± 1.44 | 5.70 ± 1.07 | 4.87 ± 1.11 | 5.67 ± 1.23 | 5.20 ± 1.37 | 6.08 ± 1.25 | 5.12 ± 1.09 | 6.73 ± 0.87 | 5.59 ± 1.56 | 0.32 | 0.57 |
log-HF (ms2) | 5.00 ± 1.10 | 4.92 ± 1.60 | 5.08 ± 1.17 | 5.33 ± 1.61 | 5.38 ± 0.93 | 6.84 ± 1.68 | 5.56 ± 1.09 | 4.85 ± 1.04 | 5.29 ± 1.48 | 5.31 ± 1.47 | 5.78 ± 1.83 | 5.02 ± 1.10 | 6.74 ± 1.52 | 5.50 ± 1.57 | 1.68 | 0.20 |
log-LF/HF ratio | 0.41 ± 1.89 | 0.45 ± 0.81 | 0.24 ± 1.04 | 0.51 ± 0.83 | 0.11 ± 1.05 | -0.09 ± 0.68 | 0.14 ± 0.70 | 0.02 ± 1.10 | 0.38 ± 0.71 | 0.04 ± 1.00 | 0.31 ± 0.95 | 0.10 ± 0.74 | -0.01 ± 1.02 | 0.19 ± 1.14 | 2.71 | 0.11 |
LF% (nu) | 46.20 ± 18.51 | 46.11 ± 16.27 | 41.58 ± 21.03 | 46.92 ± 15.60 | 41.44 ± 18.10 | 34.95 ± 11.86 | 41.92 ± 13.78 | 36.32 ± 18.42 | 40.64 ± 15.26 | 37.43 ± 19.18 | 43.56 ± 16.55 | 35.24 ± 13.25 | 38.77 ± 18.25 | 37.37 ± 15.47 | 3.43 | 0.06 |
HF% (nu) | 32.09 ± 18.44 | 30.90 ± 3.55 | 35.28 ± 18.71 | 27.97 ± 12.89 | 36.44 ± 17.28 | 39.08 ± 12.51 | 37.74 ± 12.11 | 39.28 ± 18.91 | 31.78 ± 12.88 | 38.60 ± 17.32 | 32.98 ± 16.65 | 35.09 ± 15.60 | 38.88 ± 18.34 | 37.53 ± 14.49 | 3.01 | 0.08 |
Note: Data are presented as mean ± standard deviation. Legend: HR = Heart Rate; log-LF = Low-Frequency band transformed into natural logarithm; LF% =Low-Frequency band in normalized units; log-HF = High-Frequency band transformed into natural logarithm; HF% = How-Frequency band in normalized units; logLF/HF ratio = LF/HF ratio transformed into natural logarithm
As suggested by Laborde and colleagues [40], changes in LF% and HF% were operationalized using the delta calculation. Once the relative reactivity and recovery values were obtained, a comparison between groups was carried out (using a T-Test for Independent Samples) for each PSP recording phase. Specifically, significant differences emerged considering the reactivity to Subjective Stress (Reactivity 3) and subsequent recovery (Recovery 3) (Table 4).
Table 4.
Control group (n = 30) | Migraine group (n = 30) | t (59) | p | Cohen’s D | |||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
M | SD | M | SD | ||||
LF% | |||||||
Δ Reactivity 1 | -0.09 | 25.68 | 5.60 | 16.30 | 1.02 | 0.16 | 0.26 |
Δ Recovery 1 | -4.53 | 26.08 | -3.40 | 19.65 | -0.02 | 0.49 | -0.01 |
Δ Reactivity 2 | 0.73 | 23.72 | 6.31 | 22.22 | 0.97 | 0.17 | 0.25 |
Δ Recovery 2 | -5.48 | 26.05 | -6.79 | 17.24 | -0.40 | 0.35 | -0.01 |
Δ Reactivity 3 | -10.76 | 21.08 | 2.96 | 21.30 | 1.41 | 0.01 | 0.62 |
Δ Recovery 3 | -1.32 | 22.40 | -10.76 | 21.08 | -1.67 | 0.05 | -0.43 |
HF% | |||||||
Δ Reactivity 1 | -1.19 | 20.96 | -7.47 | 17.15 | -1.27 | 0.10 | -0.33 |
Δ Recovery 1 | 4.37 | 21.84 | 6.06 | 17.32 | 0.33 | 0.37 | 0.09 |
Δ Reactivity 2 | -4.12 | 21.96 | -5.06 | 23.77 | -1.16 | 0.44 | -0.04 |
Δ Recovery 2 | 8.47 | 22.83 | 0.84 | 22.85 | -1.29 | 0.11 | -0.33 |
Δ Reactivity 3 | 6.99 | 19.99 | 1.01 | 21.72 | 1.11 | 0.14 | 0.29 |
Δ Recovery 3 | -1.34 | 15.72 | -3.66 | 20.03 | -0.50 | 0.31 | -0.13 |
Discussion
The present study aimed to investigate HRV in different phases of a psychophysiological stress profile in a group of subjects with migraine. Therefore, the sample of migraine patients was compared with a control group, as no significant differences emerged between the two groups when looking at the socio-demographic variables, except for gender which was consistent with the literature [62]. Specifically, the objective that guided the study was to analyze the differences in reactivity to various types of induced stressors within an experimental protocol and the consequent recovery in a resting condition. Our findings highlighted that, in addition to a difference at baseline, the stressor that differentiated migraineurs from controls was the subjective one. Recounting a significant life event resulted in increased sympathetic activity in subjects with migraine compared to controls, as a significant augment in delta LF% was observed in the Subjective stressor phase.
Recent research attested that the HRV parameter is useful for describing the autonomic imbalance associated with stress response [30–32]. Specifically, the research highlighted that a high HRV is an indicator of psychophysical health, as it allows the activation of the PNS and SNS systems in a balanced manner, allowing reactivity to stressors that altered homeostasis but, also, self-regulatory and adaptability after it [63, 64]. For this reason, an optimal level of HRV is associated with self-regulation and resilience capabilities [35, 44, 45].
Looking at the migraine literature, interesting results were described by Gass and Glaross [65], who found increased sympathetic activation and reduced parasympathetic activation in a group of patients with mixed tension-type migraine by analyzing HRV. Similar findings were also collected by Perciaccante et al. [66], performing a 24-hour electrocardiographic recording. Consistent with these studies, Druschky and colleagues [67] found a higher LF and a reduced HF during a slow breathing condition, confirming a reduced parasympathetic activity in two types of neurological diseases (migraine and epilepsy), hypothesizing that psychiatric and stress comorbidities could be a common factor for both conditions.
Taking into account studies investigating the HRV parameter on headaches, the results shown by the various research groups are conflicting. For instance, a decrease in both sympathetic and parasympathetic modulation, as well as vagal tone, was described [48]. In particular, Rossi and colleagues [49] observed an alteration in autonomic modulation, with minor oscillations in both LF (sympathetic/vagal modulation) and HF (vagal modulation), consistent with previous work by Tabata and collaborators [68]. Chuang et al. [69], moreover, validated that a reduced parasympathetic/cardiovagal modulation interfered with the therapeutic response to flunarizine. In contrast to these findings, our data did not document alterations in vagal tone (HF oscillations), being more in line with Miglis et al. [70].
Our study did not detect significant differences in HRV between the control and migraine groups considering the reactivity and recovery to induced stress, except for the phase of presentation of subjective stress consisting of having to recount a personal life event. In other words, an increase in the delta LF% value was noted during subjective stress in migraine subjects, while there was a decrease in these values in control subjects. The cause of the difference between our data and those present in the literature could lie precisely in the differences between the stimuli presented, as well as the detection methods. In a similar case, Tabata and colleagues [68] suggested that fluctuations in circadian HR rhythm could be attributable to dysfunction of the serotonergic system. Thus, it is essential to bear in mind that migraine is a cyclical disorder, with changes in cortical excitability between different phases that could influence the response to stressful stimuli [71]. As an illustration, a study analyzing sympathetic and parasympathetic modulation in patients with episodic migration certified reduced autonomic activity even during the ictal phase [50].
To our knowledge, this is the first time that HRV values have been compared between migraineurs and control subjects at different stages of a psychophysiological recording. Furthermore, having administered different types of stressors allowed us to highlight a different response when recounting a personal event. Specifically, while migraine sufferers perceive it as stressful to report a significant life episode, the opposite is true for healthy subjects.
As a result, this evidence enriched a line of studies that took into account the response to psychophysical stress as salient in the genesis of migraine attacks.
ANS dysfunction was repeatedly confirmed in primary headache disorders, including migraine, and is believed to be a notable aspect in headache attacks [16, 24, 28, 29]. Several studies validated a pathophysiology common to the incidence of migraine and mental suffering [72]. It seems that emotional arousal can modulate the transmission of pain, amplifying its perception [73]. First of all, it seems that a fundamental role may be played by the monoaminergic transmission that controls the descending modulation of pain, as the pathway that projects from the periaqueductal gray matter to the serotonergic neurons in the medulla and to the noradrenergic neurons in the pons and tegmentum could be altered [74]. Secondly, serotonergic transmission may be further implicated due to repeated migraine attacks causing its decrease [75]. Bearing in mind that low levels of serotonin can lead to a lowering of the pain perception threshold, the vicious circle created by depression appears evident [76]. Central monoaminergic alterations also include an imbalance between dopamine and norepinephrine in pain matrix neurons due to aberrant tyrosine metabolism, which is, in turn, consistent with dysfunctions of the HPA axis [77–79] frequently found in stress-related physical and emotional disorders [37].
In light of these assumptions, it is plausible to argue that stress is greater among migraine patients in comparison with healthy subjects as well as various psychiatric comorbidities [21, 22]. Furthermore, it is believed to contribute to the incidence, frequency, and severity of migraine attacks [15], confirming its primary role in the activation and perpetuation of attacks [24, 25].
Although our data allowed the underlining of clinically relevant findings, they must be read in light of the main limitation that characterizes HRV. It is crucial to contemplate that the HRV is a simplification of a complex bioengineering exercise and that it could be easy to confuse the part (i.e., the variable derived from the HRV) with the whole (i.e., the sympathetic/parasympathetic command) without an attitude characterized by caution. The interpretation of the LF and HF bands has not yet brought the groups of researchers to agree on the topic. By way of illustration, there is evidence that the LF oscillation corresponds to vasomotor activity and the HF oscillation corresponds to respiratory activity, casting doubt on the initial approach of splitting the frequency range [43]. More specifically, the interpretation of the LF component is the most controversial. Pomeranz et al. [79] suspected that the LF component was influenced by body position (i.e., supine or upright) while studies conducted by Malliani and colleagues as early as 1991 reassured that LF is a main indicator of sympathetic activity in healthy individuals [80], modifiable by inclination and moderate physical exercise as well as mental stress [81]. However, electrophysiological studies have also shown that LF and HF oscillations are present in both impulse activity variability signals recorded simultaneously from sympathetic and vagal efferent fibers participating in cardiac innervation, not allowing limiting LF and HF to a particular neural circuit.
Future studies could also take into account some methodological challenges that characterize PPG-based studies [52]. One limitation of PPG is that it records a delayed cardiac response farther from the heart. Thus, ECG-based estimates that present a sharp peak for the R component are not exactly matched by PPG-based methods that show a less pronounced curved peak of the signal [30]. Furthermore, ECG-based estimates of HR and HRV are recommended to obtain more reliable results because they allow visual inspection and correction of cardiac data artifacts, frequently produced in experimental protocols involving the use of stressful stimuli [56].
Lastly, the cross-sectional study design confuses the nature of the associations, which remains to be determined. Despite several studies corroborating the relationships between psychophysiological factors and headaches, it is possible to hypothesize that stress may worsen headaches which, in turn, may exacerbate emotional problems [74]. Furthermore, the small sample size and imbalance in favor of women represent a further limitation that future studies should overcome.
Nevertheless, the possible clinical implications arising from our findings could be considerable. Highlighting the need to evaluate the role of specific psychological factors and stress on the ANS could explain the manifestations of headaches and help identify patients who are more difficult to treat and who require psychological as well as medical intervention.
Conclusions
Our study aimed to compare HRV in different phases of a psychophysiological evaluation with stressor and rest conditions in a group of migraineurs and a group of control subjects. We found that migraineurs have a different autonomic regulation with higher reactivity in the condition of a subjective stressor. In other words, recounting a significant life event is considered as stressful as a cognitive task. During an experiment where migraineurs were asked to talk about a personal episode, higher emotional involvement was observed. Autonomic imbalance may indicate greater vulnerability to stress and, therefore, lower adaptability and resilience. Our findings are in line with the research on the psychophysiology of migraine, which supports that stress and autonomic imbalance can influence the incidence, frequency, and intensity of migraine attacks.
Electronic supplementary material
Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.
Funding
Open access funding provided by Università degli Studi di Parma within the CRUI-CARE Agreement.
Declarations
Ethical Statement
All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were under the ethical standards of the institutional and national research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki Declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards. The study was approved by the Ethical Review Board of the University of Parma (Protocol No. 118310/2024).
Footnotes
Publisher’s note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
References
- 1.Steiner TJ, Stovner LJ, Vos T, Jensen R, Katsarava Z (2018) Migraine is first cause of disability in under 50s: will health politicians now take notice? J Headache Pain 19(1):17. 10.1186/s10194-018-0846-2 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Ashina M (2020) Migraine. N Engl J Med 383(19):1866–1876. 10.1056/NEJMra1915327 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Steiner TJ, Stovner LJ, Jensen R, Uluduz D, Katsarava Z Lifting The Burden: the Global Campaign against Headache (2020) Migraine remains second among the world’s causes of disability, and first among young women: findings from GBD2019. J Headache Pain, 21(1): 137. 10.1186/s10194-020-01208-0 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 4.Goadsby PJ, Holland PR, Martins-Oliveira M, Hoffmann J, Schankin C, Akerman S (2017) Pathophysiology of migraine: a disorder of sensory Processing. Physiol Rev 97(2):553–622. 10.1152/physrev.00034.2015 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Ashina M, Tepper S, Brandes JL et al (2018) Efficacy and safety of erenumab (AMG334) in chronic migraine patients with prior preventive treatment failure: a subgroup analysis of a randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled study. Cephalalgia 38(10):1611–1621. 10.1177/0333102418788347 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Burstein R, Noseda R, Borsook D (2015) Migraine: multiple processes, complex pathophysiology. J Neuroscience: Official J Soc Neurosci 35(17):6619–6629. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0373-15.2015 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Noseda R, Burstein R (2013) Migraine pathophysiology: anatomy of the trigeminovascular pathway and associated neurological symptoms, CSD, sensitization and modulation of pain. Pain 154(1). 10.1016/j.pain.2013.07.021 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 8.De Tommaso M, Sciruicchio V (2016) Migraine and central sensitization: clinical features, main comorbidities and therapeutic perspectives. Curr Rheumatol Reviews 12(2):113–126. 10.2174/1573397112666151231110813 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Filatova E, Latysheva N, Kurenkov A (2008) Evidence of persistent central sensitization in chronic headaches: a multi-method study. J Headache Pain 9(5):295–300. 10.1007/s10194-008-0061-7 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Suzuki K, Suzuki S, Shiina T, Kobayashi S, Hirata K (2022) Central Sensitization in Migraine: a narrative review. J pain Res 15:2673–2682. 10.2147/JPR.S329280woo [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Woolf CJ (2011) Central sensitization: implications for the diagnosis and treatment of pain. Pain 152(3 Suppl):S2–S15. 10.1016/j.pain.2010.09.030 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Andreatta M, Puschmann AK, Sommer C, Weyers P, Pauli P, Mühlberger A (2012) Altered processing of emotional stimuli in migraine: an event-related potential study. Cephalalgia: Int J Headache 32(15):1101–1108. 10.1177/0333102412459573 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Kropp P, Gerber WD (1995) Contingent negative variation during migraine attack and interval: evidence for normalization of slow cortical potentials during the attack. Cephalalgia: Int J Headache 15(2):123–179. 10.1046/j.1468-2982.1995.015002123.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.An H, Han JW, Jeong HG, Kim TH, Lee JJ, Lee SB, Park JH, Kim KW (2020) Parasympathetic predominance is a risk factor for future depression: a prospective cohort study. J Affect Disord 260:232–237. 10.1016/j.jad.2019.09.015 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Sauro KM, Becker WJ (2009) The stress and migraine interaction. Headache 49(9):1378–1386. 10.1111/j.1526-4610.2009.01486.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Spierings EL (2001) Headache continuum: concept and supporting evidence from a recent study of chronic daily headache. Clin J Pain 17(4):337–340. 10.1097/00002508-200112000-00008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Selye H (1956) What is stress. Metabolism 5(5):525–530 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Selye H (1998) A syndrome produced by diverse nocuous agents. 1936. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci 10(2):230–231. 10.1176/jnp.10.2.230a [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Calabrese EJ (2008) Stress biology and hormesis: the yerkes-Dodson law in psychology–a special case of the hormesis dose response. Crit Rev Toxicol 38(5):453–462. 10.1080/10408440802004007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Pruneti C, Vanello N, Paterni M, Landini L, Guidotti S, Ferdeghini EM (2021) Combined functional magnetic resonance imaging and skin conductance to detect localized neural response to psychological stress: a pilot study. Arch Ital Biol 159(1):21–27. 10.12871/00039829202112 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Jette N, Patten S, Williams J, Becker W, Wiebe S (2008) Comorbidity of migraine and psychiatric disorders–a national population-based study. Headache 48(4):501–516. 10.1111/j.1526-4610.2007.00993.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Shala N, Dreshaj S (2018) Association of depression, anxiety and post-traumatic stress disorder with migraine: data from Kosovo. Neurol Neurochir Pol 52(4):490–494. 10.1016/j.pjnns.2018.03.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Chen WT, Wang SJ, Fuh JL, Lin CP, Ko YC, Lin YY (2011) Persistent ictal-like visual cortical excitability in chronic migraine. Pain 152(2):254–258. 10.1016/j.pain.2010.08.047 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.GBD 2016 Disease and Injury Incidence and Prevalence Collaborators (2017) Global, regional, and national incidence, prevalence, and years lived with disability for 328 diseases and injuries for 195 countries, 1990–2016: a systematic analysis for the global burden of Disease Study 2016. Lancet (London England) 390(10100):1211–1259. 10.1016/S0140-6736(17)32154-2 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Headache Classification Committee of the International Headache Society (IHS) The International Classification of Headache Disorders, 3rd edition (2018) Cephalalgia: an international journal of headache, 38(1): 1–211. 10.1177/0333102417738202 [DOI] [PubMed]
- 26.Liu TH, Wang Z, Xie F, Liu YQ, Lin Q (2021) Contributions of aversive environmental stress to migraine chronification: Research update of migraine pathophysiology. World J Clin Cases 9(9):2136–2145. 10.12998/wjcc.v9.i9.2136 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Theeler BJ, Kenney K, Prokhorenko OA, Fideli US, Campbell W, Erickson JC (2010) Headache triggers in the US military. Headache 50(5):790–794. 10.1111/j.1526-4610.2009.01571.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Wacogne C, Lacoste JP, Guillibert E, Hugues FC, Le Jeunne C (2003) Stress, anxiety, depression and migraine. Cephalalgia: Int J Headache 23(6):451–455. 10.1046/j.1468-2982.2003.00550.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Zivadinov R, Willheim K, Sepic-Grahovac D, Jurjevic A, Bucuk M, Brnabic-Razmilic O, Relja G, Zorzon M (2003) Migraine and tension-type headache in Croatia: a population-based survey of precipitating factors. Cephalalgia: Int J Headache 23(5):336–343. 10.1046/j.1468-2982.2003.00544.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Laborde S, Mosley E, Thayer JF (2017) Heart Rate Variability and Cardiac Vagal Tone in Psychophysiological Research - recommendations for Experiment Planning, Data Analysis, and Data Reporting. Front Psychol 8:213. 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00213 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Lkhagvasuren B, Hiramoto T, Tumurbaatar E, Bat-Erdene E, Tumur-Ochir G, Viswanath V, Corrigan J, Jadamba T (2023) The Brain Overwork Scale: a Population-based cross-sectional study on the Psychometric Properties of a New 10-Item scale to assess Mental Distress in Mongolia. Healthc (Basel Switzerland) 11(7):1003. 10.3390/healthcare11071003 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Tumurbaatar E, Bat-Erdene E, Amartuvshin T, Dashtseren M, Tumur-Ochir G, Boldbaatar D, Jadamba T, Hiramoto T, Oka T, Lkhagvasuren B (2023) Heart rate variability and tension-type headache: a population-based cross-sectional study. IBRO Neurosci Rep 15:194–202. 10.1016/j.ibneur.2023.09.004 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Task Force of the European Society of Cardiology and the North American Society of Pacing and Electrophysiology (1996) Heart rate variability. Standards of measurement, physiological interpretation, and clinical use. Eur Heart J 17(3):354–381 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Russo MA, Santarelli DM, O’Rourke D (2017) The physiological effects of slow breathing in the healthy human. Breathe (Sheff) 13(4):298–309. 10.1183/20734735.009817 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Hoareau V, Godin C, Dutheil F, Trousselard M (2021) The Effect of Stress Management Programs on physiological and psychological components of stress: the influence of baseline physiological state. Appl Psychophysiol Biofeedback 46(3):243–250. 10.1007/s10484-021-09508-0 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Jiménez Morgan S, Molina Mora JA (2017) Effect of Heart Rate Variability Biofeedback on Sport Performance, a systematic review. Appl Psychophysiol Biofeedback 42(3):235–245. 10.1007/s10484-017-9364-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Cacioppo JT, Tassinary LG, Berntson GG (2010) Handbook of psychophysiology, 2nd edn. Cambridge University Press, New York [Google Scholar]
- 38.Moraes JL, Rocha MX, Vasconcelos GG, Vasconcelos Filho JE, de Albuquerque VHC, Alexandria AR (2018) Advances in Photopletysmography Signal Analysis for Biomedical Applications. Sensors 18(6):1894. 10.3390/s18061894 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Shaffer F, Ginsberg JP (2017) An overview of Heart Rate Variability Metrics and norms. Front Public Health 5:258. 10.3389/fpubh.2017.00258 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Laborde S, Mosley E, Mertgen A (2018) Vagal Tank Theory: the three rs of Cardiac Vagal Control functioning - resting, reactivity, and recovery. Front Neurosci 12:458. 10.3389/fnins.2018.00458 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Kuo TB, Lin T, Yang CC, Li CL, Chen CF, Chou P (1999) Effect of aging on gender differences in neural control of heart rate. Am J Physiol 277(6):H2233–H2239. 10.1152/ajpheart.1999.277.6.H2233 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Pagani M, Rimoldi O, Pizzinelli P et al (1991) Assessment of the neural control of the circulation during psychological stress. J Auton Nerv Syst 35(1):33–41. 10.1016/0165-1838(91)90036-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Malliani A, Pagani M, Lombardi F (1994) Physiology and clinical implications of variability of cardiovascular parameters with focus on heart rate and blood pressure. Am J Cardiol 73(10):3 C–9C. 10.1016/0002-9149(94)90617-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Pruneti C, Ferrari S, Guidotti S (2023) A narrative review of Heart Rate Variability as a Good Index of Psychophysical Health in athletes and in Biofeedback Training. J Clin Sport Psychol. 10.1123/jcsp.2022-0016. Advanced online publication [Google Scholar]
- 45.Koenig J, Williams DP, Kemp AH, Thayer JF (2016) Vagally mediated heart rate variability in headache patients–a systematic review and meta-analysis. Cephalalgia: Int J Headache 36(3):265–278. 10.1177/0333102415583989 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Thayer JF, Lane RD (2007) The role of vagal function in the risk for cardiovascular disease and mortality. Biol Psychol 74(2):224–242. 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2005.11.013 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Barloese MC, Mehlsen J, Brinth L, Lundberg HI, Jennum PJ, Jensen RH (2015) Reduced Baroreflex Sensitivity in Cluster Headache patients. Headache 55(6):815–824. 10.1111/head.12586 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Koenig W, Nitschmann S (2016) Prävention kardiovaskulärer Ereignisse: Heart outcomes Prevention evaluation (HOPE) [Prevention of cardiovascular events: Heart outcomes Prevention evaluation (HOPE)]. Der Internist 57(12):1250–1253. 10.1007/s00108-016-0155-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Lee CH, Shin HW, Shin DG (2020) Impact of Oxidative Stress on Long-Term Heart Rate Variability: Linear Versus Non-Linear Heart Rate Dynamics. Heart, lung & circulation, 29(8): 1164–1173. 10.1016/j.hlc.2019.06.726 Leonardi, M., Musicco, M., & Nappi, G. (1998). Headache as a major public health problem: current status. Cephalalgia: an international journal of headache, 18 Suppl 21, 66–69. https://doi.org/10.1177/0333102498018s2116 [DOI] [PubMed]
- 50.Rossi DM, de Souza HCD, Bevilaqua-Grossi D, Vendramim ACC, Philbois SV, Carvalho GF, Dach F, Mascarenhas S, de Oliveira AS (2022) Impairment on Cardiovascular autonomic modulation in women with migraine. Int J Environ Res Public Health 20(1):763. 10.3390/ijerph20010763 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Dias ARL, de Souza KA, Dos Santos KM et al (2022) Ambulatory heart rate variability in overweight and obese men after high-intensity interval exercise versus moderate-intensity continuous exercise. Eur J Sport Sci 22(7):1113–1121. 10.1080/17461391.2021.1900403 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Lohani M, Payne BR, Strayer DL (2019) A review of psychophysiological measures to assess Cognitive States in Real-World Driving. Front Hum Neurosci 13:57. 10.3389/fnhum.2019.00057 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Rinne JKA, Miri S, Oksala N, Vehkaoja A, Kössi J (2023) Evaluation of a wrist-worn photoplethysmography monitor for heart rate variability estimation in patients recovering from laparoscopic colon resection. J Clin Monit Comput 37(1):45–53. 10.1007/s10877-022-00854-w [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Parak J, Tarniceriu A, Renevey P, Bertschi M, Delgado-Gonzalo R, Korhonen I (2015) Evaluation of the beat-to-beat detection accuracy of PulseOn wearable optical heart rate monitor. Annu Int Conf IEEE Eng Med Biol Soc 2015:8099–8102. 10.1109/EMBC.2015.7320273 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Dziembowska I, Izdebski P, Rasmus A, Brudny J, Grzelczak M, Cysewski P (2016) Effects of Heart Rate Variability Biofeedback on EEG Alpha asymmetry and anxiety symptoms in male athletes: a pilot study. Appl Psychophysiol Biofeedback 41(2):141–150. 10.1007/s10484-015-9319-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Schäfer A, Vagedes J (2016) How accurate is pulse rate variability as an estimate of heart rate variability? A review on studies comparing photoplethysmographic technology with an electrocardiogram. Int J Cardiol 166(1):15–29. 10.1016/j.ijcard.2012.03.119 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Charlton PH, Celka P, Farukh B, Chowienczyk P, Alastruey J (2018) Assessing mental stress from the photoplethysmogram: a numerical study. Physiol Meas 39(5):054001. 10.1088/1361-6579/aabe6a [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Ceccarelli LA, Giuliano RJ, Glazebrook CM, Strachan SM (2019) Self-Compassion and Psycho-Physiological Recovery From Recalled Sport Failure. Front Psychol, 10:1564. Published 2019 Jul 5. 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.01564 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 59.Zhang L, Qiu S, Zhao C, Wang P, Yu S (2021) Heart rate variability analysis in episodic migraine: a cross-sectional study. Front Neurol 12:647092. 10.3389/fneur.2021.647092 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Lalanza JF, Lorente S, Bullich R, García C, Losilla JM, Capdevila L (2023) Methods for Heart Rate Variability Biofeedback (HRVB): a systematic review and guidelines. Appl Psychophysiol Biofeedback 48(3):275–297. 10.1007/s10484-023-09582-6 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Heart rate variability (1996) : standards of measurement, physiological interpretation and clinical use. Task Force of the European Society of Cardiology and the North American Society of Pacing and Electrophysiology. Circulation 93(5):1043–1065 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Fuller GD (1979) Biofeedback methods and procedures in clinical practice. Biofeedback
- 63.Stovner LJ, Hagen K, Jensen R, Katsarava Z, Lipton R, Scher A, Steiner T, Zwart JA (2007) The global burden of headache: a documentation of headache prevalence and disability worldwide. Cephalalgia: Int J Headache 27(3):193–210. 10.1111/j.1468-2982.2007.01288.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Kim HG, Cheon EJ, Bai DS, Lee YH, Koo BH (2018) Stress and heart rate variability: a Meta-analysis and review of the literature. Psychiatry Invest 15(3):235–245. 10.30773/pi.2017.08.17 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Lehrer P (2022) My life in HRV Biofeedback Research. Appl Psychophysiol Biofeedback 47(4):289–298. 10.1007/s10484-022-09535-5 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Miglis MG (2018) Migraine and autonomic dysfunction: which is the horse and which is the jockey? Curr Pain Headache Rep 22(3):19. 10.1007/s11916-018-0671-y [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Gass JJ, Glaros AG (2013) Autonomic dysregulation in headache patients. Appl Psychophysiol Biofeedback 38(4):257–263. 10.1007/s10484-013-9231-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Tabata M, Takeshima T, Burioka N, Nomura T, Ishizaki K, Mori N, Kowa H, Nakashima K (2000) Cosinor analysis of heart rate variability in ambulatory migraineurs. Headache 40(6):457–463. 10.1046/j.1526-4610.2000.00068.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Chuang CH, Li JY, King JT, Chen WT, Chen SP, Wang YF, Liu HY, Hsiao FJ, Pan LH, Wang SJ, Lai KL (2023) Abnormal heart rate variability and its application in predicting treatment efficacy in patients with chronic migraine: an exploratory study. Cephalalgia: Int J Headache 43(10):3331024231206781. 10.1177/03331024231206781 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Perciaccante A, Fiorentini A, Valente R, Granata M, Tubani L (2007) Migraine and heart rate variability. Arch Intern Med 167(20):2264–2265. 10.1001/archinte.167.20.2264-a [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71.Barbanti P, Brighina F, Egeo G, Di Stefano V, Silvestro M, Russo A (2020) Migraine as a cortical brain disorder. Headache 60(9):2103–2114. 10.1111/head.13935 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72.Rainville P, Bao QVH, Chrétien P (2005) Pain-related emotions modulate experimental pain perception and autonomic responses. Pain 118(3):306–318. 10.1016/j.pain.2005.08.022 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73.Cathcart S, Petkov J, Winefield AH, Lushington K, Rolan P (2010) Central mechanisms of stress-induced headache. Cephalalgia: Int J Headache 30(3):285–295. 10.1111/j.1468-2982.2009.01917.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74.Walters ET (2014) Neuroinflammatory contributions to pain after SCI: roles for central glial mechanisms and nociceptor-mediated host defense. Exp Neurol 258:48–61. 10.1016/j.expneurol.2014.02.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 75.Chu HT, Liang CS, Lee JT, Yeh TC, Lee MS, Sung YF, Yang FC (2018) Associations between Depression/Anxiety and headache frequency in migraineurs: a cross-sectional study. Headache 58(3):407–415. 10.1111/head.13215 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 76.Dworkin RH, Gitlin MJ (1991) Clinical aspects of depression in chronic pain patients. Clin J Pain 7(2):79–94. 10.1097/00002508-199106000-00004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 77.Baskin SM, Smitherman TA (2009) Migraine and psychiatric disorders: comorbidities, mechanisms, and clinical applications. Neurol Sciences: Official J Italian Neurol Soc Italian Soc Clin Neurophysiol 30(Suppl 1):S61–S65. 10.1007/s10072-009-0071-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 78.Pomeranz B, Macaulay RJ, Caudill MA et al (1985) Assessment of autonomic function in humans by heart rate spectral analysis. Am J Physiol 248(1 Pt 2):H151–H153. 10.1152/ajpheart.1985.248.1.H151 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 79.D’Andrea G, Nordera GP, Perini F, Allais G, Granella F (2007) Biochemistry of neuromodulation in primary headaches: focus on anomalies of tyrosine metabolism. Neurol Sciences: Official J Italian Neurol Soc Italian Soc Clin Neurophysiol 28(Suppl 2):S94–S96. 10.1007/s10072-007-0758-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 80.Malliani A, Pagani M, Lombardi F, Cerutti S (1991) Cardiovascular neural regulation explored in the frequency domain. Circulation 84(2):482–492. 10.1161/01.cir.84.2.482 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 81.Brovelli M, Baselli G, Cerutti S, Guzzetti S, Liberati D, Lombardi F, Malliani A, Pagani M, Pizzinelli P (1983) Computerized analysis for an experimental validation of neurophysiological models of heart rate control. Comput Cardiol, 205–208
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.