Skip to main content
BMJ Public Health logoLink to BMJ Public Health
. 2024 May 27;2(1):e000868. doi: 10.1136/bmjph-2023-000868

Associations between adverse childhood experiences and trust in health and other information from public services, professionals and wider sources: national cross sectional survey

Mark A Bellis 1,2,3,, Karen Hughes 2,3, Kat Ford 3, Catherine Sharp 2, Rebecca Hill 2
PMCID: PMC11812900  PMID: 40018105

Abstract

ABSTRACT

Introduction

Trust in health and other systems can affect uptake of public health advice and engagement with health services. Individuals who had adverse childhood experiences (ACEs) are more likely to experience ill health at earlier ages. Ensuring their engagement with health and other services is important in improving their life course prospects, but little is known about how ACEs affect trust in such services and the information they provide.

Methods

Data were collected via a national household survey of residents in Wales (aged ≥18 years, n=1880, November 2022–March 2023). Questions measured ACE exposure and trust in health, social, police, charities and government, and health and general information provided by a variety of professionals and sources.

Results

Individuals with ACEs were more likely to report low trust in health advice from hospital doctors, general practitioners (GPs), nurses, pharmacists, and NHS 111, an online and telephone urgent care service (eg, adjusted low trust prevalence: GPs, 0 ACEs 5.3%, ≥4 ACEs 10.4%; NHS 111, 0 ACEs 11.9%, ≥4 ACEs 24.1%). Low trust in services also increased with ACEs, with low trust in police being 3.8 times more likely with ≥4 ACEs (vs 0 ACEs). The highest adjusted prevalence of low trust in a service was for government, rising from 48.4% (0 ACEs) to 73.7% (≥4 ACEs). Low trust in general advice and information from TV/radio programmes rose from 17.6% (0 ACEs) to 30.1% (≥4 ACEs); low trust in social media was higher with an equivalent rise from 61.6% to 75.6%.

Conclusion

Breaking intergenerational cycles of ill health and inequity requires engaging and influencing those with ACEs. However, a history of ACEs was associated with lower trust in supporting institutions, systems and professionals. Empirical data on which resources are most trusted by those with ACEs should be used to facilitate better communications with this vulnerable group.

Keywords: Community Health, Public Health, Cross-Sectional Studies, Preventive Medicine


WHAT IS ALREADY KNOWN ON THIS TOPIC

  • A history of adverse childhood experiences (ACEs) is a common feature in many national populations, with greater exposure to ACEs increasing risks of poor health outcomes across the life course.

  • Although individuals with ACEs could benefit substantively from health and social advice and support, little research has examined which systems, services and professionals those with ACEs distrust and if trust preferences differ from those with no ACEs.

WHAT THIS STUDY ADDS

  • Results provide empirical information that ACEs are associated with lower levels of trust in health, social, criminal justice, government and other systems and professionals, as well as online sources of health and other information.

HOW THIS STUDY MIGHT AFFECT RESEARCH, PRACTICE OR POLICY

  • A better understanding of which sources of advice and support are distrusted and trusted by those with a history of ACEs should inform messaging, outreach and other forms of service engagement with this vulnerable group.

Introduction

Adverse childhood experiences (ACEs) include child physical, verbal and sexual maltreatment as well as exposure to other potential sources of trauma, such as growing up in domiciles with domestic violence, mental ill health and substance misuse.1 Exposure to ACEs is consistently associated with low educational attendance and attainment, school suspension and expulsion, and reduced employment prospects.2,5 ACEs are also linked to the adoption of health harming behaviours, such as smoking, alcohol misuse, illicit drug use and sexual risk taking6; involvement in antisocial behaviours, crime and violence7 8; and poorer diets and obesity.9 10 Across the life course, higher levels of ACE exposure are predictive of poorer mental health6 11 and associated with earlier development of non-communicable disease, including cancer, cardiovascular disease, and subsequent premature mortality.6 12 13 Increasingly biological and epigenetic markers of ill health and premature ageing are being disproportionately identified in those exposed to ACEs, indicative of causal relationships.14,16 ACEs are a common feature of populations in high, middle and low income countries, and predict poorer life course outcomes across each economic level.17,19 A meta-analysis of data from 206 ACE studies in 22 countries found that 60% of adults had been exposed to at least one ACE while growing up, with 16% exposed to four or more ACEs.17 Consequently, preventing ACEs and mitigating their potential impacts are major public health considerations as well as factors of substantial economic importance, with the health burden attributable to ACEs having been estimated to cost over US$1 trillion per annum across North America and Europe alone.20

Individuals who have suffered ACEs may benefit from health promotion information that, for instance, advises reductions in substance use or adherence to health guidelines and encourages healthier diets and exercise. Equally, health protection advice on safer sex, routine infection, biometric screening and vaccine uptake is likely to be especially pertinent to those with ACEs, and therefore at higher risks of infection or ill health.6 15 21 Early engagement with health services when ill health develops is also a predictor of better outcomes22 23 and likely to be especially important to those with ACEs who may develop chronic disease earlier in life. However, studies suggest that exposure to ACEs can influence individuals’ likelihood of: adopting health advice on behaviour change;24 vaccination uptake;25 engaging in screening programmes which might lead to earlier health and social interventions;26 27 and complying with medication and other clinical treatment regimens.28 Recent pandemic conditions highlighted poorer uptake of both health protection advice and COVID-19 vaccination in those with ACEs,29 increasing risks of infection and transmission.

A major factor associated with adoption of public sector advice and successful engagement with health, social and other support services is levels of trust in sources and providers.30,32 Moreover, with commercial websites, charities, TV, radio, peers and other sources often providing information independent of the public sector sources, relative trust in public sector versus other commercial, charity and social media outlets is an important consideration, especially when trying to reach and support vulnerable groups.33 However, relatively little is known about how ACEs may impact levels of trust in health advice, public and other systems, or general sources of health and other information.

Here, we used data from a national survey to examine levels of trust in different public, private, social and charitable agencies and professionals. We tested the hypothesis that levels of low trust will vary according to the number of ACEs experienced. Based on the findings, we considered which individuals, agencies and communication platforms are best placed to engage and influence those with a history of ACEs.

Materials and methods

A national household survey of residents in Wales (aged ≥18 years) was conducted between November 2022 and March 2023. A methodology using face-to-face surveys at participants’ places of residence was chosen as this has provided higher levels of compliance in previous ACE studies than, for instance, telephone based surveys.34 Moreover, data on location of domicile (used in residence based surveying) provides an ecological measure of deprivation that can be used in stratified sampling. Such data may not be available when sampling through telephone number lists. Sampling and data collection were undertaken by a professional market research company (MRC). To ensure adequate capture of individuals with exposure to multiple ACEs (≥4, n~200), a target sample of 2000 participants was set based on ACE prevalence rates in previous national surveys.35 A stratified quota sampling methodology was used to obtain a sample representative of national sociodemographics. The sampling unit was lower super output areas (LSOAs; geographical units with a population of approximately 1500 people), with sampling stratified by Welsh Health Board and deprivation quintile (using Welsh index of multiple deprivation (WIMD) 2019 scores).36 WIMD is a combination of eight separate domains of deprivation (income, employment, health, education, access to services, community safety, physical environment and housing), with each domain compiled from a range of different indicators.

A proportionate sample of 200 LSOAs was randomly selected from each deprivation quintile in each health board. A target of 10 interviews was set for each selected LSOA, with quota samples by age and sex. Residential addresses within selected LSOAs were identified using the postcode address file. Only one individual per household was eligible to participate, with study inclusion criteria being age ≥18 years, resident in a selected LSOA and cognitively able to participate.

Interviewers were a mix of men and women, all employed by the MRC. Each was trained to ensure they understood the purpose and content of the survey, with training including information on ACEs and the need for sensitivity and objectivity when undertaking interviews. Households were visited by interviewers and, on contact, potential participants were provided with a participant information sheet and a letter of authority from Public Health Wales. Interviewers detailed: the purpose of the survey and how findings would be used; survey content; and the voluntary, confidential and anonymous nature of the survey. All study materials were provided in Welsh and English language, and participants were able to complete the survey in the language of their choice. All participants provided informed consent, recorded electronically within the survey. Face-to-face interviews were conducted at the door via computer assisted personal interviewing with computer assisted self-interviewing for more sensitive questions (eg, ACE questions). Participation took an average of 22 min, following which all participants were provided with a thank you leaflet containing information for appropriate national support services. Just under half (49%) of all individuals contacted agreed to participate in the survey (n=2007).

Study questionnaire

The study questionnaire included questions on trust in health advice and general information from various sources, and overall trust in health services, social services, police, government and charities; exposure to ACEs before the age of 18 years; and a range of demographics. Online supplemental table A1 provides the questions used to measure ACEs and outcome measures included in this study.

Guidelines for measuring trust recommend a scale of 0 to 10.37 Consequently, trust in health advice for each item was measured by the question “On a scale of 0 to 10 where 0 is not at all and 10 is completely, how much would you trust health advice given to you by: GPs (general practitioners); hospital doctors; nurses; pharmacists/chemists (hereafter termed pharmacists); health professionals available through NHS 111 (hereafter termed NHS 111); health professionals accessed via other means (eg, an app; hereafter termed virtual health professionals); and friends, family or colleagues”. NHS 111 is an online and telephone urgent care health service which provides advice and signposting to appropriate information and services. Trust in service/systems was measured by the question “On a scale of 0 to 10 where 0 is not at all and 10 is completely, how much do you trust: health services; social services; police; charities/voluntary organisations (hereafter termed charities); and government”. Trust in general advice/information was measured by the question “On a scale of 0 to 10 where 0 is not at all and 10 is completely, how much do you trust general advice and information from: NHS websites; health apps for smartphones or tablets (hereafter termed health apps); other internet sites/internet searches (eg, Google, YouTube, Wikipedia; hereafter termed general internet sites); social media (such as Twitter and Facebook); and TV/radio programmes”. Guidelines for measuring trust also include the use of thresholds for categorising trust levels.37 However, with no specific threshold for low trust stipulated, for all outcomes we adopted ratings below the midpoint of the scale, <5, as having low trust. This consistent approach across outcomes also assisted with comparisons of low trust percentages between different sources of advice and information. For all trust measurements, those who were unable to provide a rating were recorded as ‘don’t know/not applicable’.

An adapted version of the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention short ACE tool38 was used to measure exposure to nine ACE types: verbal, physical and sexual abuse; parental separation/divorce; witnessing domestic violence; and living within a household where mental illness, alcohol or drug misuse was present, or where a household member was incarcerated. Out of the nine types of ACEs, the number individuals reported was summed to generate an ACE count (0 ACEs, 1 ACE, 2–3 ACEs, ≥4 ACEs).

Participants’ sex, age and ethnicity (UK census categories) were collected. Age was re-categorised into four age groups (18–29, 30–49, 50–69 and ≥70 years). For the purpose of analysis and due to low levels in other than white ethnicities, ethnicity was categorised into white and other than white. Residential deprivation was measured using the WIMD quintile (1=most deprived, 5=least deprived) for LSOA of residence.

Statistical analysis

For the purpose of analyses, the sample was restricted to participants who had complete demographic and ACE count data, for a final sample of 1880. Statistical analyses were conducted in SPSS V.24. χ2 tests were used to measure bivariate relationships for all outcome measures and ACE counts and participant demographics. Binary logistic regression (enter method) was used to measure independent associations between ACEs and all outcomes, controlling for participant age, sex, ethnicity and residential deprivation quintile. Generalised linear modelling was used to generate estimated marginal means (adjusted means) from binary logistic models.

Patient and public involvement

Patients (other than as participants) were not involved in the study. However, findings are being made publicly available through open access journal articles and study reports.

Results

The sample and, for comparison, national demographics are shown in online supplemental table A2. Just over half of the sample were women (54.5%); 47.1% were aged 18–49 years and 52.9% were aged ≥50 years; 95.6% were of white ethnicity; and proportions in each deprivation quintile ranged from 19.3% (quintile 1, most deprived) to 20.6% (quintile 5, least deprived). Overall, the sample was a good match to general population characteristics for ethnicity and deprivation but included a lower percentage of individuals aged 18–29 years (14.4%) and more women (54.5%) than the national population (19.2% and 51.1%, respectively). Over half of all participants reported 0 ACEs (56.6%), with 17.9% reporting 1 ACE, 14.6% 2–3 ACEs and 10.9% ≥4 ACEs.

For each trust rating, some individuals were categorised as ‘don’t know/not applicable’. In particular, online sources such as websites, health apps and social media were not rated as frequently by older respondents and several health service sources were not rated as frequently by those of other than white ethnicities. Individuals with ACEs were more likely to be able to provide a trust rating for health advice from NHS 111 and for social services. Overall reporting levels for each question are shown in table 1 with breakdowns by demographics and ACE count in online supplemental tables A3–A6.

Table 1. Question response rates and percentage of individuals reporting a low trust rating for different sources of advice and services by exposure to adverse childhood experiences.

No of individuals Response rate(%) All(%) ACE count (%)
0 1 2–3 ≥4 χ2, P value
Low trust in health advice from
 General practitioners 1845 98.1 7.7 6.4 8.3 7.4 13.9 13.641, 0.003
 Hospital doctors 1819 96.8 4.9 3.5 4.6 5.9 11.1 21.379, <0.001
 Nurses 1827 97.2 3.2 2.6 1.8 3.4 8.7 22.272, <0.001
 Pharmacists 1807 96.1 3.2 2.4 1.8 4.5 7.9 18.723, <0.001
 NHS 111 1307 69.5 10.3 9.2 7.3 11.9 17.9 14.079, 0.003
 Virtual health professionals 831 44.2 19.1 17.1 18.1 18.3 29.6 8.965, 0.030
 Friends, family or colleagues 1700 90.4 15.5 13.4 16.3 19.3 19.8 8.839, 0.032
Low trust in services/systems
 Health services 1846 98.2 12.6 10.0 10.2 15.6 26.8 46.560, <0.001
 Social services 1101 58.6 25.0 20.1 24.6 26.4 41.7 30.693, <0.001
 Police 1705 90.7 18.8 15.0 16.5 19.4 41.3 72.730, <0.001
 Charities 1580 84.0 9.4 7.7 6.8 13.9 16.0 20.131, <0.001
 Government 1831 97.4 62.2 55.8 65.4 69.5 80.0 52.667, <0.001
Low trust in general advice/info from
 NHS websites 1416 75.3 7.9 6.2 5.5 12.7 13.8 19.309, <0.001
 Health apps 923 49.1 34.5 32.2 35.8 29.5 49.5 13.970, 0.003
 General internet sites 1517 80.7 34.4 30.7 36.8 36.5 44.9 14.817, 0.002
 Social media 1414 75.2 71.5 68.1 74.5 74.8 77.5 9.701, 0.021
 TV/radio programmes 1750 93.1 24.6 20.9 25.5 30.0 35.5 23.153, <0.001

ACEadverse childhood experience

Trust in health advice

Ability to provide a trust rating in health advice was high for most types of health service staff (>96%) and friends, family or colleagues (90.4%), falling to 69.5% for NHS 111 and 44.2% for virtual health professionals (table 1). Less than 5% of those rating health advice from hospital doctors, nurses and pharmacists, and 7.7% of those rating GPs, reported low trust in this advice (table 1). Low trust increased for remote health advice delivery methods, to 1 in 10 (10.3%) of those rating NHS 111 and almost 1 in 5 (19.1%) of those rating virtual health professionals. Overall, friends, family or colleagues were less trusted than NHS 111 but more trusted than virtual health professionals (table 1).

After accounting for sociodemographic confounders (age, sex, ethnicity and deprivation), ACE count was found to be significantly associated with low trust in health advice from each health professional source, with the likelihood of a low trust response being more than twice as high in those with ≥4 ACEs (vs 0 ACEs, table 2). There was a smaller increase in low trust in health advice from friends, family or colleagues but this was still significantly different between the 0 and ≥4 ACEs categories (table 2). Adjusted means identified an increase in low trust prevalence for health advice from hospital doctors from 3.3% (0 ACEs) to 8.8% (≥4 ACEs), and for health advice from GPs from 5.3% to 10.4%, respectively (figure 1). For health advice from NHS 111, low trust increased from 11.9% (0 ACEs) to 24.1% (≥4 ACEs) and for virtual health professionals from 19.2% to 34.2%, respectively (figure 1).

Table 2. Logistic regression analysis of low trust in health advice from different sources by exposure to adverse childhood experiences and sociodemographics.

General practitioners Hospital doctors Nurses Pharmacists NHS 111 Virtual health professionals Friends, family or colleagues
aOR (95% CI, P value) aOR (95% CI, P value) aOR (95% CI, P value) aOR (95% CI, P value) aOR (95% CI, P value) aOR (95% CI, P value) aOR (95% CI, P value)
ACE count
 0 Ref (0.027) Ref (0.004) Ref (0.004) Ref (0.007) Ref (0.001) Ref (0.022) Ref (0.058)
 1 1.33 (0.84 to 2.13, 0.228) 1.27 (0.68 to 2.36, 0.458) 0.65 (0.26 to 1.60, 0.349) 0.68 (0.28 to 1.69, 0.410) 0.75 (0.43 to 1.31, 0.314) 1.10 (0.67 to 1.79, 0.711) 1.23 (0.86 to 1.76, 0.259)
 2–3 1.13 (0.67 to 1.91, 0.656) 1.62 (0.87 to 2.98, 0.126) 1.20 (0.55 to 2.61, 0.643) 1.74 (0.86 to 3.55, 0.126) 1.41 (0.86 to 2.33, 0.178) 1.10 (0.66 to 1.83, 0.709) 1.55 (1.07 to 2.24, 0.020)
 ≥4 2.09 (1.29 to 3.40, 0.003) 2.84 (1.60 to 5.03, <0.001) 2.87 (1.50 to 5.52, 0.002) 2.74 (1.39 to 5.41, 0.004) 2.36 (1.44 to 3.89, 0.001) 2.19 (1.32 to 3.62, 0.002) 1.53 (1.00 to 2.33, 0.049)
Age (years)
 18–29 Ref (0.035) Ref (0.026) Ref (0.088) Ref (0.077) Ref (0.180) Ref (0.001) Ref (0.018)
 30–49 1.01 (0.58 to 1.73, 0.986) 0.62 (0.33 to 1.15, 0.129) 0.71 (0.34 to 1.51, 0.376) 0.90 (0.41 to 1.96, 0.784) 0.80 (0.46 to 1.38, 0.416) 0.97 (0.57 to 1.64, 0.896) 1.16 (0.76 to 1.77, 0.504)
 50–69 1.41 (0.82 to 2.42, 0.211) 1.03 (0.57 to 1.88, 0.916) 1.09 (0.52 to 2.29, 0.814) 1.31 (0.60 to 2.83, 0.497) 1.30 (0.75 to 2.26, 0.349) 2.12 (1.26 to 3.59, 0.005) 1.30 (0.85 to 2.00, 0.232)
 ≥70 0.64 (0.33 to 1.24, 0.189) 0.38 (0.17 to 0.88, 0.023) 0.33 (0.11 to 0.98, 0.046) 0.32 (0.10 to 1.07, 0.064) 1.17 (0.62 to 2.23, 0.628) 1.21 (0.60 to 2.45, 0.601) 0.69 (0.42 to 1.14, 0.144)
Sex*
 Women 1.19 (0.83 to 1.69, 0.340) 1.37 (0.88 to 2.14, 0.169) 1.68 (0.96 to 2.94, 0.070) 1.06 (0.62 to 1.82, 0.821) 0.70 (0.48 to 1.00, 0.051) 0.97 (0.68 to 1.39, 0.852) 0.93 (0.71 to 1.21, 0.586)
Ethnicity*
 Other than white 0.79 (0.31 to 2.03, 0.626) 0.98 (0.34 to 2.83, 0.976) 1.17 (0.35 to 3.95, 0.803) 1.60 (0.55 to 4.67, 0.390) 1.91 (0.89 to 4.10, 0.095) 1.68 (0.79 to 3.60, 0.181) 0.88 (0.44 to 1.76, 0.720)
Deprivation
 (Least) 5 Ref (0.011) Ref (0.165) Ref (0.249) Ref (0.328) Ref (0.665) Ref (0.121) Ref (0.011)
 4 1.16 (0.66 to 2.02, 0.613) 0.96 (0.45 to 2.06, 0.921) 0.87 (0.33 to 2.30, 0.779) 1.25 (0.46 to 3.40, 0.670) 1.03 (0.59 to 1.79, 0.929) 1.07 (0.64 to 1.78, 0.798) 0.67 (0.45 to 1.00, 0.049)
 3 0.56 (0.29 to 1.09, 0.086) 0.93 (0.44 to 2.00, 0.860) 0.91 (0.35 to 2.33, 0.841) 1.78 (0.70 to 4.54, 0.229) 1.12 (0.65 to 1.93, 0.692) 0.68 (0.39 to 1.18, 0.171) 0.48 (0.32 to 0.74, 0.001)
 2 1.44 (0.84 to 2.48, 0.188) 1.56 (0.78 to 3.15, 0.212) 1.67 (0.71 to 3.90, 0.238) 1.55 (0.59 to 4.08, 0.373) 0.73 (0.40 to 1.34, 0.303) 0.53 (0.29 to 0.96, 0.036) 0.60 (0.40 to 0.90, 0.014)
 (Most) 1 1.63 (0.96 to 2.77, 0.074) 1.79 (0.91 to 3.53, 0.094) 1.78 (0.78 to 4.09, 0.172) 2.38 (0.97 to 5.86, 0.059) 0.87 (0.49 to 1.56, 0.638) 0.75 (0.44 to 1.28, 0.295) 0.69 (0.46 to 1.03, 0.066)

P values in Rreference (Ref) rows refer to overall contribution of variable to model.

*

Reference categories for sex and ethnicity awere maleen and white, respectively.

ACEadverse childhood experienceaOR, adjusted ORRef, reference category

Figure 1. Adjusted percentages reporting low trust in different sources of advice and information, by count of adverse childhood experiences (ACE).

Figure 1

Low trust in hospital doctors and nurses was least common in those aged ≥70 years, while low trust in virtual health professionals was highest in those aged 50–69 years (table 2). Sex and ethnicity were not independently associated with trust in any source of health advice, while low trust in GPs was higher in the more deprived quintiles and low trust in friends, family or colleagues highest in the least deprived quintile (table 2 and online supplemental table A7).

Trust in services and systems

Nearly all (>97%) respondents provided a trust rating for government and health services, and 90.7% rated police. However, only 84.0% provided a rating for charities and 58.6% for social services (table 1). Trust was highest for charities, for which only 9.4% of respondents provided a low trust rating, followed by health services (12.6%). The prevalence of low trust was nearly twice as high for social services (25.0%) with police at 18.8%. However, low trust was substantively higher for government (62.2%) than for any other service or system.

Low trust in all services and systems increased significantly with ACE count, with those with ≥4 ACEs being over three times more likely to have low trust in health services (adjusted OR (aOR) 3.23) and police (aOR 3.81) than those with 0 ACEs (table 3). The likelihood of a low trust rating increased with even one ACE (vs 0 ACEs) for government and with two or more ACEs for health services, social services and charities. Adjusted means identified an increase in low trust in health services from 8.4% with 0 ACEs to 23.0% with ≥4 ACEs and for police from 17.0% to 43.8%, respectively (figure 1).

Table 3. Logistic regression analysis of low trust in different services and systems by exposure to adverse childhood experiences and sociodemographics.

Health services Social services Police Charities Government
aOR (95% CI, P value) aOR (95% CI, P value) aOR (95% CI, P value) aOR (95% CI, P value) aOR (95% CI, P value)
ACE count
 0 Ref (<0.001) Ref (<0.001) Ref (<0.001) Ref (<0.001) Ref (<0.001)
 1 1.01 (0.67 to 1.52, 0.981) 1.26 (0.86 to 1.86, 0.235) 1.08 (0.76 to 1.53, 0.688) 0.92 (0.54 to 1.58, 0.772) 1.45 (1.11 to 1.88, 0.006)
 2–3 1.68 (1.13 to 2.49, 0.011) 1.50 (1.01 to 2.24, 0.046) 1.37 (0.95 to 1.98, 0.092) 2.10 (1.34 to 3.31, 0.001) 1.82 (1.36 to 2.45, <0.001)
 ≥4 3.23 (2.19 to 4.77, <0.001) 2.81 (1.89 to 4.18, <0.001) 3.81 (2.67 to 5.44, <0.001) 2.35 (1.44 to 3.84, 0.001) 2.99 (2.05 to 4.36, <0.001)
Age (years)
 18–29 Ref (0.001) Ref (0.009) Ref (<0.001) Ref (0.076) Ref (<0.001)
 30–49 0.75 (0.48 to 1.17, 0.197) 0.93 (0.61 to 1.42, 0.737) 0.57 (0.39 to 0.83, 0.003) 0.85 (0.48 to 1.50, 0.566) 1.14 (0.83 to 1.56, 0.415)
 50–69 1.42 (0.92 to 2.18, 0.110) 1.39 (0.91 to 2.12, 0.132) 1.12 (0.78 to 1.62, 0.532) 1.49 (0.86 to 2.58, 0.156) 1.60 (1.16 to 2.21, 0.004)
 ≥70 0.78 (0.48 to 1.30, 0.343) 0.68 (0.41 to 1.14, 0.146) 0.62 (0.40 to 0.96, 0.031) 1.25 (0.68 to 2.29, 0.479) 0.79 (0.57 to 1.10, 0.164)
Sex*
 Women 0.95 (0.72 to 1.26, 0.738) 0.75 (0.56 to 0.99, 0.041) 0.74 (0.57 to 0.95, 0.018) 0.71 (0.51 to 1.01, 0.055) 0.73 (0.60 to 0.89, 0.002)
Ethnicity*
 Other than white 0.71 (0.32 to 1.59, 0.408) 1.01 (0.49 to 2.08, 0.972) 1.39 (0.75 to 2.55, 0.294) 0.81 (0.31 to 2.08, 0.654) 0.52 (0.32 to 0.83, 0.006)
Deprivation
 (Least) 5 Ref (0.535) Ref (0.445) Ref (0.021) Ref (0.051) Ref (0.003)
 4 1.05 (0.66 to 1.67, 0.842) 1.08 (0.69 to 1.68, 0.744) 1.30 (0.86 to 1.97, 0.218) 1.13 (0.63 to 2.03, 0.691) 1.38 (1.02 to 1.87, 0.035)
 3 1.20 (0.76 to 1.88, 0.441) 0.78 (0.49 to 1.25, 0.304) 1.02 (0.66 to 1.56, 0.939) 1.13 (0.63 to 2.04, 0.688) 1.29 (0.96 to 1.74, 0.092)
 2 1.41 (0.90 to 2.21, 0.137) 1.21 (0.78 to 1.89, 0.402) 1.52 (1.01 to 2.30, 0.045) 1.37 (0.77 to 2.43, 0.281) 1.80 (1.32 to 2.45, <0.001)
 (Most) 1 1.30 (0.83 to 2.05, 0.251) 1.04 (0.66 to 1.62, 0.869) 1.75 (1.17 to 2.62, 0.007) 2.04 (1.19 to 3.51, 0.010) 1.59 (1.17 to 2.17, 0.003)

P values in reference (Ref) rows refer to overall contribution of variable to model.

*

Reference categories for sex and ethnicity awere maleen and white, respectively.

ACEadverse childhood experienceaOR, adjusted ORRef, reference category

For all services and systems, adjusted odds of low trust ratings were highest among those aged 50–69 years (table 3). Men were more likely to report low trust in social services, police and government than women. Being resident in more deprived quintiles was also associated with low trust in police and government, and white ethnicity with low trust in government (table 3 and online supplemental table A7).

Trust in general advice and information

Nearly all respondents provided a trust rating for TV/radio programmes (93.1%), 80.7% for general internet sites, and around three quarters for NHS websites and social media, although less than half (49.1%) provided a rating for health apps (table 1). Only 7.9% of those rating NHS websites were categorised as low trust, increasing to a quarter (24.6%) for TV/radio programmes and to over a third for general internet sites (34.4%) and health apps (34.5%; table 1). Trust in general advice/information from social media was lowest, with trust levels of 7 in 10 respondents (71.5%) categorised in the low category (table 1).

ACE count was independently associated with low trust responses across all sources, with the likelihood of a low trust response being around double in those with≥4 ACEs (vs 0 ACEs; table 4). For social media and general internet sites, low trust increased significantly even with one ACE (vs 0) and for NHS websites and TV/radio programmes with ≥2 ACEs. Adjusted means identified an increase in prevalence of low trust in NHS websites from 5.7% with 0 ACEs to 12.8% with ≥4 ACEs and for TV/radio programmes from 17.6% to 30.1%, respectively (figure 1).

Table 4. Logistic regression analysis of low trust in different sources of general advice and information by exposure to adverse childhood experiences and sociodemographics.

NHS websites Health apps General internet sites Social media TV/radio programmes
aOR (95% CI, P value) aOR (95% CI, P value) aOR (95% CI, P value) aOR (95% CI, P value) aOR (95% CI, P value)
ACE count
 0 Ref (0.001) Ref (0.001) Ref (<0.001) Ref (0.001) Ref (<0.001)
 1 0.88 (0.48 to 1.62, 0.684) 1.21 (0.83 to 1.75, 0.322) 1.37 (1.03 to 1.83, 0.031) 1.48 (1.07 to 2.04, 0.018) 1.27 (0.94 to 1.71, 0.121)
 2–3 2.19 (1.32 to 3.64, 0.003) 0.92 (0.62 to 1.36, 0.664) 1.41 (1.04 to 1.92, 0.028) 1.53 (1.09 to 2.16, 0.015) 1.60 (1.17 to 2.19, 0.003)
 ≥4 2.45 (1.38 to 4.33, 0.002) 2.32 (1.50 to 3.59, <0.001) 2.10 (1.49 to 2.96, <0.001) 1.93 (1.29 to 2.90, 0.002) 2.02 (1.43 to 2.86, <0.001)
Age (years)
 18–29 Ref (0.007) Ref (0.001) Ref (0.001) Ref (<0.001) Ref (0.018)
 30–49 0.91 (0.47 to 1.73, 0.765) 1.25 (0.83 to 1.88, 0.297) 1.19 (0.86 to 1.65, 0.294) 1.65 (1.21 to 2.26, 0.002) 1.27 (0.89 to 1.82, 0.187)
 50–69 1.98 (1.07 to 3.67, 0.031) 2.13 (1.40 to 3.26, <0.001) 1.75 (1.25 to 2.44, 0.001) 2.22 (1.57 to 3.15, <0.001) 1.60 (1.12 to 2.30, 0.010)
 ≥70 1.70 (0.80 to 3.60, 0.170) 1.81 (1.05 to 3.11, 0.034) 1.77 (1.18 to 2.63, 0.005) 1.86 (1.19 to 2.92, 0.007) 1.06 (0.71 to 1.59, 0.768)
Sex*
 Women 1.12 (0.75 to 1.69, 0.573) 0.94 (0.71 to 1.25, 0.669) 0.88 (0.71 to 1.10, 0.266) 0.93 (0.73 to 1.18, 0.561) 0.90 (0.72 to 1.12, 0.351)
Ethnicity*
 Other than white 0.97 (0.34 to 2.82, 0.961) 1.36 (0.71 to 2.59, 0.352) 0.92 (0.54 to 1.57, 0.766) 0.59 (0.36 to 0.97, 0.038) 0.70 (0.38 to 1.28, 0.247)
Deprivation
 (Least) 5 Ref (0.005) Ref (0.483) Ref (0.749) Ref (0.031) Ref (0.041)
 4 2.60 (1.37 to 4.92, 0.003) 1.29 (0.85 to 1.96, 0.235) 0.86 (0.62 to 1.20, 0.377) 0.69 (0.47 to 1.02, 0.062) 1.51 (1.05 to 2.16, 0.026)
 3 1.35 (0.67 to 2.74, 0.401) 1.09 (0.71 to 1.68, 0.698) 0.81 (0.58 to 1.13, 0.211) 0.60 (0.41 to 0.87, 0.007) 1.74 (1.22 to 2.48, 0.002)
 2 1.23 (0.59 to 2.59, 0.582) 0.87 (0.55 to 1.37, 0.549) 0.84 (0.60 to 1.18, 0.319) 0.75 (0.51 to 1.11, 0.151) 1.37 (0.95 to 1.99, 0.092)
 (Most) 1 2.50 (1.29 to 4.85, 0.006) 0.96 (0.62 to 1.50, 0.862) 0.84 (0.59 to 1.18, 0.305) 0.56 (0.38 to 0.83, 0.004) 1.53 (1.06 to 2.21, 0.024)

P values in reference (Ref) rows refer to overall contribution of variable to model.

*

Reference categories for sex and ethnicity weare maleen and white, respectively.

ACEadverse childhood experienceaOR, adjusted ORRef, reference category

For all sources, higher proportions of low trust were reported by those aged ≥50 years, with the exception of trust in TV/radio programmes and NHS websites by those aged ≥70 years (table 4). Low trust did not vary significantly with sex or ethnicity, except for a lower likelihood of low trust in social media in other than white ethnicities. Low trust in NHS websites was highest in deprivation quintiles 1 (most deprived) and 4. Low trust in social media was most likely in deprivation quintile 5 (least deprived), while low trust in TV/radio programmes was least likely in this quintile (table 4 and online supplemental table A7).

Discussion

Positive relationships with parents and other caregivers in childhood are related to stronger attachments,39 40 and help develop the ability to trust others.41 When children have been hurt or neglected by caregivers, their ability to trust others may be diminished along with their ability to form attachment bonds,42,44 even subsequently with their own children.45 Moreover, maltreated children can be more likely to interpret neutral facial expressions as angry or aggressive,46 with immediate implications for developing attachment and trust.43 While such limited findings illustrate the impacts ACEs may have on an individual’s ability to trust others, less empirical information is available on how ACEs may affect trust in information and advice from professionals, and public and private organisations. In a largely separate literature, trust in organisations often considers issues such as their consistency, reliability, transparency, ethics and integrity, and proven competence.47,50 However, such work has generally not considered how individuals’ experiences of ACEs may affect their ability to trust information and advice from organisations and professionals for health and other purposes.

Here, our results identified strong associations between exposure to ACEs and greater prevalence of low trust in different support systems, as well as health and other advice and information from professionals, agencies and information sources. Of particular concern to healthcare and public health systems, individuals with ≥4 ACEs (vs 0 ACEs) were between two and three times more likely to have low trust in GPs, hospital doctors, nurses and pharmacists (table 2). Critically, those with a high ACEs count are more likely to have greater healthcare, health protection and health promotion needs.6 Consequently, low trust may result in poorer uptake of health improving behaviour advice, specifically in those most likely to develop non-communicable diseases, infections and other physical and mental health issues at earlier ages. Furthermore, trust in sources of advice is an important predictor of compliance with such advice.51 Consequently, individuals with higher ACE counts may also be less likely to adopt and maintain fidelity to medication regimens, other treatment plans or vaccination schedules.26 52 Health systems should recognise that those most in need of their support may also be least likely to trust their advice. Recent developments in trauma informed approaches to health and well being have begun considering how all systems may better meet the needs of those who have experienced trauma, including ACEs.53 54 However, further empirical work is urgently needed, especially when exposure to ACEs remains a common experience (here, 43.4% had at least one ACE and 10.9% ≥4 ACEs).

Overall, low trust was much more prevalent in advice and information from virtual (online and apps) or remote (NHS 111) health advice sources than directly from health professionals (table 1), although response levels varied by source (see limitations). Sources carrying the NHS brand or associated with health professions were more trusted than their more generic counterparts (eg, low trust in NHS websites vs general internet sites, table 1), with social media the least trusted source (71.5% low trust; table 1). Regardless, there were strong positive associations between multiple ACE exposure and low trust for all such sources (tables24). With increasing proportions of public health advice and service contact moving to remote platforms (eg, phone and internet), lower trust in these services should be of concern. Moreover, individuals with higher ACE counts are often greater users of health systems and, for instance, adjusted prevalence of low trust in health advice from the NHS 111 service rose from 11.9% with 0 ACEs to 24.1% with ≥4 ACEs (figure 1).

Similar increases in low trust with higher ACEs were seen for all services, including health services (tables1 3). The greatest increase in the likelihood of low trust with ACEs was seen for police (≥4 ACEs vs 0 ACEs; aOR 3.81), although adjusted odds also at least doubled in those with ≥4 ACEs for health and social services, charities and government (table 3). While evidence supports individuals with higher ACEs having more experiences as perpetrators and victims of crime,7 55 results here identified an estimated increase in prevalence of low trust in police services from 17.0% (0 ACEs) to 43.8% (≥4 ACEs; figure 1). Some police forces are already considering how they make services more trauma informed in order to better engage and build trust with those with a history of adversity.56 57 Typically, however, in the UK and elsewhere, such measures are not yet an integrated feature of police training and operations.

Across all services and systems, the highest prevalence of reported low trust was seen for government (table 1). However, public health advice is frequently referred to as government guidance or guidelines and issued by government departments. Examples include advice for healthy eating58 and physical activity,59 while in some countries health warning labels on alcohol products are identified as governmental rather than health service or medical advice (eg, USA;60). Our results suggested labelling information and advice as governmental may lead to lower levels of trust in it and potentially provide commercial organisations wishing to lessen its uptake an opportunity to exploit a less trusted title (eg, governmental guidance). The differences in trust for those with high levels of ACEs were particularly stark. An estimated 23.0% of those with ≥4 ACEs reported low trust in health services, rising to 73.7% with low trust in government (figure 1). Consequently, our results suggest that provision of national health related or other guidance should consider the benefits of an exclusive health service brand rather than a governmental one.

Finally, our results suggested very low levels of trust in social media, with considerably greater trust in more traditional information platforms, such as TV/radio programmes (table 1). Again, for all communication platforms, low trust was significantly increased in those with higher ACEs (table 4, figure 1). However, increasing amounts of public funding are being invested in social media messaging in order to reach different population groups. The value of such investments may need recalibrating if three quarters of those with high ACE counts have low trust in social media and even among those with no ACE exposure most individuals have low trust in advice and information on such platforms.

Limitations

The study relied on recall of ACEs by adults, which may be incomplete or inaccurate and, in some cases, participants may have chosen not to disclose certain ACEs despite the reassurance of confidentiality and anonymity. Moreover, an average interview time of 22 min may have impacted some respondents’ focus and compliance over the course of their interview. However, ACE prevalence was comparable with other studies undertaken in the UK.35 While compliance levels were also generally consistent with other ACE surveys (49%35) we cannot identify how ACEs and levels of trust in those choosing not to complete the survey may have affected the results. Here, respondents were able to say if they could not rate trust in any particular individual, agency or service (table 1; online supplemental tables A3–A6) as not all respondents may feel adequately exposed to or informed on each source. The majority of respondents (>90%) could rate each type of individual health professional (ie, GPs, hospital doctors, nurses and pharmacists) and there were no significant relationships between ACE counts and providing such trust ratings (online supplemental table A4). For some information/advice sources and services, ACE count was related to ability to provide a trust rating. Thus social services and NHS 111 were more likely to be given a rating by individuals with higher ACEs (online supplemental tables A4 and A5), which may reflect a greater likelihood of being in contact with such services. Individuals with 2–3 ACEs (vs 0 ACEs) were more likely to rate health apps, general internet sites, virtual health professionals and social media but less likely to rate health services in general (online supplemental tables A4–A6). It is not possible to assess whether this reflects a pattern of accessing remote services more among this group. However, results may better reflect those directly or indirectly exposed to each source rather than any overall population wide prevalence.

We used a range of ACEs typically identified in ACE research tools used by the World Health Organization and US Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. However, there is continuing debate about whether other childhood adversities (eg, peer victimisation, community violence) should be included in ACE measurements61,63 and how this would affect relationships with trust requires further studies. Moreover, our results did not measure the severity of ACEs, length of exposure, frequency and age of occurrence,61 all of which may be important in the development of trust. With little work having already been undertaken on the impacts of adversity on trust in the range of individuals and organisations we examined, there were no validated instruments, and the questions used here require further validation and refinement. Our questions did not examine if levels of trust might vary depending on the type of information or service provided but this is an important consideration for further studies. We chose to dichotomise participants trust ratings into low trust (yes 0–4, or no 5–10 on the scale) with the same single cut-off applied to all items. This reflected the aim of the study to examine factors relating specifically to low trust. However, further studies could examine other categorisations including, for instance, very high or very low trust (eg, 8–10 or 0–2 on the 0–10 scale, respectively). Finally, the survey was carried out after a pandemic and during a cost of living crisis,64 and we were not able to ascertain whether trust in information and advice from different sources was affected by these global events and whether any such affects are sustained.

Conclusions

In the UK and elsewhere, trust in institutions and figures of professional standing or other authority has diminished over recent decades.65 However, public health and healthcare systems rely on trust as a mechanism to ensure advice is followed and for fidelity to treatment. The alternative, seen during the COVID-19 pandemic, is the use of legislation and criminal justice agencies to enforce health measures (eg, restricting movements). The choice of institution or professional with which information is associated may be a critical decision in its credibility. Individuals with ACEs are an important population for health, social, criminal justice and other sectors to influence. Those with ACEs may require disproportionate amounts of public sector or charitable support, and may benefit more from services but be less likely to trust in such services and the advice they provide. Greater trust in services by those with healthier childhoods will only serve to perpetuate intergenerational cycles of adversity and associated inequalities. Thus trauma informed approaches are required that understand who the most credible communicators are and how best to develop trust in essential support services.

supplementary material

online supplemental file 1
bmjph-2-1-s001.pdf (385.1KB, pdf)
DOI: 10.1136/bmjph-2023-000868

Acknowledgements

We are grateful to the residents of Wales who kindly participated in this study. We also thank Katie Cresswell, Sumina Azam, Tracy Black, Joanne Hopkins and Ashley Gould at Public Health Wales for their support with study development and implementation.

Footnotes

Funding: This work was supported by Public Health Wales. MAB received salary support from the UK Prevention Research Partnership (Violence, Health and Society; MR-VO49879/1).

Provenance and peer review: Not commissioned; externally peer reviewed.

Patient consent for publication: Not applicable.

Ethics approval: This study involves human participants and was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. The study was reviewed through the Integrated Research Application System (IRAS; ref 320185) and ethical approval was granted by Bangor University Healthcare and Medical Sciences Academic Ethics Committee (ref 2022-17225). Participants gave informed consent to participate in the study before taking part.

Patient and public involvement: Patients and/or the public were not involved in the design, or conduct, or reporting, or dissemination plans of this research.

Data availability statement

Data are available upon reasonable request.

References

  • 1.Bellis MA, Wood S, Hughes K, et al. Tackling adverse childhood experiences (aces): state of the art and options for action [cardiff: public health wales] 2023. [15-Apr-2023]. https://www.ljmu.ac.uk/-/media/phi-reports/pdf/2023-01-state-of-the-art-report-eng.pdf Available. Accessed.
  • 2.Pierce H, Jones MS, Gibbs BG. Early adverse childhood experiences and exclusionary discipline in high school. Soc Sci Res. 2022;101:102621. doi: 10.1016/j.ssresearch.2021.102621. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Bellis MA, Hughes K, Ford K, et al. Adverse childhood experiences and sources of childhood resilience: a retrospective study of their combined relationships with child health and educational attendance. BMC Public Health. 2018;18:792. doi: 10.1186/s12889-018-5699-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Hardcastle K, Bellis MA, Ford K, et al. Measuring the relationships between adverse childhood experiences and educational and employment success in England and Wales: findings from a retrospective study. Public Health. 2018;165:106–16. doi: 10.1016/j.puhe.2018.09.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Metzler M, Merrick MT, Klevens J, et al. Adverse childhood experiences and life opportunities: shifting the narrative. Child Youth Serv Rev. 2017;72:141–9. doi: 10.1016/j.childyouth.2016.10.021. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Hughes K, Bellis MA, Hardcastle KA, et al. The effect of multiple adverse childhood experiences on health: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Lancet Public Health. 2017;2:e356–66. doi: 10.1016/S2468-2667(17)30118-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Graf GH-J, Chihuri S, Blow M, et al. Adverse childhood experiences and justice system contact: a systematic review. Pediatrics. 2021;147:e2020021030. doi: 10.1542/peds.2020-021030. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Bellis MA, Hughes K, Ford K, et al. Does adult alcohol consumption combine with adverse childhood experiences to increase involvement in violence in men and women? A cross-sectional study in England and Wales. BMJ Open. 2018;8:e020591. doi: 10.1136/bmjopen-2017-020591. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Wiss DA, Brewerton TD. Adverse childhood experiences and adult obesity: a systematic review of plausible mechanisms and meta-analysis of cross-sectional studies. Physiol Behav. 2020;223:112964. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2020.112964. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Aquilina SR, Shrubsole MJ, Butt J, et al. Adverse childhood experiences and adult diet quality. J Nutr Sci. 2021;10:e95. doi: 10.1017/jns.2021.85. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Zarse EM, Neff MR, Yoder R, et al. The adverse childhood experiences questionnaire: two decades of research on childhood trauma as a primary cause of adult mental illness, addiction, and medical diseases. Cogent Medicine. 2019;6:1581447. doi: 10.1080/2331205X.2019.1581447. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Merrick MT, Ford DC, Ports KA, et al. Vital signs: estimated proportion of adult health problems attributable to adverse childhood experiences and implications for prevention — 25 States, 2015–2017. MMWR Morb Mortal Wkly Rep. 2019;68:999–1005. doi: 10.15585/mmwr.mm6844e1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Yu J, Patel RA, Haynie DL, et al. Adverse childhood experiences and premature mortality through mid-adulthood: a five-decade prospective study. Lancet Reg Health Am . 2022;15:100349. doi: 10.1016/j.lana.2022.100349. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Kim K, Yaffe K, Rehkopf DH, et al. Association of adverse childhood experiences with accelerated epigenetic aging in midlife. JAMA Netw Open. 2023;6:e2317987. doi: 10.1001/jamanetworkopen.2023.17987. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Soares S, Rocha V, Kelly-Irving M, et al. Adverse childhood events and health biomarkers: a systematic review. Front Public Health. 2021;9:649825. doi: 10.3389/fpubh.2021.649825. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Linden W, LeMoult J. Editorial perspective: adverse childhood events causally contribute to mental illness – we must act now and intervene early. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2022;63:715–9. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.13541. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Madigan S, Deneault A-A, Racine N, et al. Adverse childhood experiences: a meta-analysis of prevalence and moderators among half a million adults in 206 studies. World Psychiatry. 2023;22:463–71. doi: 10.1002/wps.21122. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Hughes K, Ford K, Bellis MA, et al. Health and financial costs of adverse childhood experiences in 28 European countries: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Lancet Public Health. 2021;6:e848–57. doi: 10.1016/S2468-2667(21)00232-2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Solberg MA, Peters RM. Adverse childhood experiences in non-Westernized nations: implications for immigrant and refugee health. J Immigr Minor Health. 2020;22:145–55. doi: 10.1007/s10903-019-00953-y. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Bellis MA, Hughes K, Ford K, et al. Life course health consequences and associated annual costs of adverse childhood experiences across Europe and North America: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Lancet Public Health. 2019;4:e517–28. doi: 10.1016/S2468-2667(19)30145-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Wood S, Harrison SE, Judd N, et al. The impact of behavioural risk factors on communicable diseases: a systematic review of reviews. BMC Public Health. 2021;21:2110. doi: 10.1186/s12889-021-12148-y. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Simmons LA, Wolever RQ, Bechard EM, et al. Patient engagement as a risk factor in personalized health care: a systematic review of the literature on chronic disease. Genome Med. 2014;6:16. doi: 10.1186/gm533. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Golder AM, Mshihadani A, McMillan DC, et al. Route to diagnosis of colorectal cancer and association with survival within the context of a bowel screening programme. Public Health. 2022;211:53–61. doi: 10.1016/j.puhe.2022.06.032. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Baggett TP, Rigotti NA. Cigarette smoking and advice to quit in a national sample of homeless adults. Am J Prev Med. 2010;39:164–72. doi: 10.1016/j.amepre.2010.03.024. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Zhang S, Richardson BA, Lin J, et al. The association between adverse childhood experiences and human papillomavirus vaccination coverage in US young adults: a cross-sectional study. Sex Transm Dis. 2023;50:656–63. doi: 10.1097/OLQ.0000000000001846. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Brandford AA, Williams EN, Han G, et al. Adverse childhood experiences and preventive cervical cancer screening behavior. Oncol Nurs Forum. 2023;50:679–91. doi: 10.1188/23.ONF.679-691. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Alcalá HE, Mitchell EM, Keim-Malpass J. Heterogeneous impacts: adverse childhood experiences and cancer screening. Cancer Causes Control. 2018;29:343–51. doi: 10.1007/s10552-018-1007-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Draxler JM, Ruppar TM. Treatment adherence in adolescents with histories of adverse childhood experiences: a systematic review. J Psychosoc Nurs Ment Health Serv. 2022;60:11–8. doi: 10.3928/02793695-20211118-04. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Bellis MA, Hughes K, Ford K, et al. Associations between adverse childhood experiences, attitudes towards COVID-19 restrictions and vaccine hesitancy: a cross-sectional study. BMJ Open. 2022;12:e053915. doi: 10.1136/bmjopen-2021-053915. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Figueiras MJ, Ghorayeb J, Coutinho MVC, et al. Levels of trust in information sources as a predictor of protective health behaviors during COVID-19 pandemic: a UAE cross-sectional study. Front Psychol. 2021;12:633550. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.633550. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Qiao S, Friedman DB, Tam CC, et al. COVID-19 vaccine acceptance among college students in South Carolina: do information sources and trust in information matter. J Am Coll Health. 2024;72:859–68. doi: 10.1080/07448481.2022.2059375. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Jennings W, Stoker G, Bunting H, et al. Lack of trust, conspiracy beliefs, and social media use predict COVID-19 vaccine hesitancy. Vaccines (Basel) 2021;9:593. doi: 10.3390/vaccines9060593. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Jackson DN, Peterson EB, Blake KD, et al. Americans’ trust in health information sources: trends and sociodemographic predictors. Am J Health Promot. 2019;33:1187–93. doi: 10.1177/0890117119861280. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Bellis MA, Hughes K, Cresswell K, et al. Comparing relationships between single types of adverse childhood experiences and health-related outcomes: a combined primary data study of eight cross-sectional surveys in England and Wales. BMJ Open. 2023;13:e072916. doi: 10.1136/bmjopen-2023-072916. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Hughes K, Ford K, Kadel R, et al. Health and financial burden of adverse childhood experiences in England and Wales: a combined primary data study of five surveys. BMJ Open. 2020;10:e036374. doi: 10.1136/bmjopen-2019-036374. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.StatsWales Welsh index of multiple deprivation. 2019. [20-Dec-2023]. https://statswales.gov.wales/Catalogue/Community-Safety-and-Social-Inclusion/Welsh-Index-of-Multiple-Deprivation Available. Accessed.
  • 37.OECD . Paris: OECD Publishing; 2017. [15-Apr-2024]. OECD guidelines on measuring trust.https://www.oecd-ilibrary.org/governance/oecd-guidelines-on-measuring-trust_9789264278219-en Available. accessed. [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Centers for Disease Control and Prevention Behavioral risk factor surveillance system ACE data. [20-Dec-2023]. https://www.cdc.gov/violenceprevention/aces/ace-brfss.html Available. Accessed.
  • 39.Koehn AJ, Kerns KA. Parent–child attachment: meta-analysis of associations with parenting behaviors in middle childhood and adolescence. Attach Hum Dev. 2018;20:378–405. doi: 10.1080/14616734.2017.1408131. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Wilhelm K. Parental bonding and adult attachment style: the relationship between four category models. Int J Womens Health Wellness . 2016;2:016. doi: 10.23937/2474-1353/1510016. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Wray-Lake L, Flanagan CA. Parenting practices and the development of adolescents’ social trust. J Adolesc. 2012;35:549–60. doi: 10.1016/j.adolescence.2011.09.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Neil L, Viding E, Armbruster-Genc D, et al. Trust and childhood maltreatment: evidence of bias in appraisal of unfamiliar faces. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2022;63:655–62. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.13503. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Hepp J, Schmitz SE, Urbild J, et al. Childhood maltreatment is associated with distrust and negatively biased emotion processing. Borderline Personal Disord Emot Dysregul. 2021;8:5. doi: 10.1186/s40479-020-00143-5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Erozkan A. The link between types of attachment and childhood trauma. Ujer . 2016;4:1071–9. doi: 10.13189/ujer.2016.040517. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Lang AJ, Gartstein MA, Rodgers CS, et al. The impact of maternal childhood abuse on parenting and infant temperament. J Child Adolesc Psychiatr Nurs. 2010;23:100–10. doi: 10.1111/j.1744-6171.2010.00229.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Pfaltz MC, Passardi S, Auschra B, et al. Are you angry at me? Negative interpretations of neutral facial expressions are linked to child maltreatment but not to posttraumatic stress disorder. Eur J Psychotraumatol. 2019;10:1682929. doi: 10.1080/20008198.2019.1682929. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Zhang J, Li H, Yang K. A meta‐analysis of the government performance—trust link: taking cultural and methodological factors into account. Public Administration Review. 2022;82:39–58. doi: 10.1111/puar.13439. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Ozawa S, Sripad P. How do you measure trust in the health system? A systematic review of the literature. Soc Sci Med. 2013;91:10–4. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2013.05.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Murtin F, Fleischer L, Siegerink V, et al. Paris: OECD Publishing; 2018. Trust and its determinants: evidence from the Trustlab experiment. OECD Statistics working papers, No.2018/02. [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Song J, Zahedi FM. Trust in health infomediaries. Decision Support Systems. 2007;43:390–407. doi: 10.1016/j.dss.2006.11.011. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Pak A, McBryde E, Adegboye OA. Does high public trust amplify compliance with stringent COVID-19 government health guidelines? A multi-country analysis using data from 102,627 individuals. Risk Manag Healthc Policy. 2021;14:293–302. doi: 10.2147/RMHP.S278774. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Wang H, Liao Y, Guo L, et al. Association between childhood trauma and medication adherence among patients with major depressive disorder: the moderating role of resilience. BMC Psychiatry. 2022;22:644. doi: 10.1186/s12888-022-04297-0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Tebes JK, Champine RB, Matlin SL, et al. Population health and trauma‐informed practice: implications for programs, systems, and policies. Am J Community Psychol. 2019;64:494–508. doi: 10.1002/ajcp.12382. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.ACE Hub Wales Trauma-informed Wales: a societal approach to understanding, preventing and supporting the impacts of trauma and adversity. Cardiff: Public Health Wales. 2022. [20-Dec-2023]. https://traumaframeworkcymru.com/ Available. Accessed.
  • 55.Ports KA, Ford DC, Merrick MT. Adverse childhood experiences and sexual victimization in adulthood. Child Abuse Negl. 2016;51:313–22. doi: 10.1016/j.chiabu.2015.08.017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Brodie ZP, Gillespie-Smith K, Goodall K, et al. The impact of trauma-awareness session on police officers’ trauma-informed attitudes in Scotland. Psychol Crime Law. 2023:1–15. doi: 10.1080/1068316X.2023.2210736. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Hardcastle K, Bellis M, Hopkins J. Cardiff: Public Health Wales; 2021. [20-Dec-2023]. The early action together programme: outcomes, impacts and lessons for future transformation.https://phwwhocc.co.uk/wp-content/uploads/2021/02/PHW-EAT-Summary-Report-English-Final-09.02.21.pdf Available. accessed. [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Public Health England . London: Public Health England; 2018. A quick guide to the government’s health eating recommendations.https://assets.publishing.service.gov.uk/media/5ba8a087e5274a55c3407c38/A_quick_guide_to_govt_healthy_eating_update.pdf Available. [Google Scholar]
  • 59.US Department of Health and Human Services . Physical Activity Guidelines for Americans. 2nd. Washington DC: US Department of Health and Human Services; 2018. https://health.gov/sites/default/files/2019-09/Physical_Activity_Guidelines_2nd_edition.pdf edn. Available. [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Alcohol and Tobacco Tax and Trade Bureau Wine labeling: health warning statement. [20-Dec-2023]. https://www.ttb.gov/labeling-wine/wine-labeling-health-warning-statement Available. Accessed.
  • 61.Anda RF, Porter LE, Brown DW. Inside the adverse childhood experience score: strengths, limitations, and misapplications. Am J Prev Med. 2020;59:293–5. doi: 10.1016/j.amepre.2020.01.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Karatekin C, Hill M. Expanding the original definition of adverse childhood experiences (ACEs) J Child Adolesc Trauma. 2019;12:289–306. doi: 10.1007/s40653-018-0237-5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.McLennan JD, MacMillan HL, Afifi TO. Questioning the use of adverse childhood experiences (ACEs) questionnaires. Child Abuse Negl. 2020;101:104331. doi: 10.1016/j.chiabu.2019.104331. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Broadbent P, Thomson R, Kopasker D, et al. The public health implications of the cost-of-living crisis: outlining mechanisms and modelling consequences. Lancet Reg Health Eur. 2023;27:100585. doi: 10.1016/j.lanepe.2023.100585. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Perry J. Trust in public institutions: trends and implications for economic security. Policy brief No.108. United Nations Department of Economic and Social Affairs. 2021. [20-Dec-2023]. https://www.un.org/development/desa/dspd/wp-content/uploads/sites/22/2021/08/PB_108.pdf Available. Accessed.

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

online supplemental file 1
bmjph-2-1-s001.pdf (385.1KB, pdf)
DOI: 10.1136/bmjph-2023-000868

Data Availability Statement

Data are available upon reasonable request.


Articles from BMJ Public Health are provided here courtesy of BMJ Publishing Group

RESOURCES