Skip to main content
Taylor & Francis Open Select logoLink to Taylor & Francis Open Select
. 2025 Mar 13;35(3-4):159–178. doi: 10.1080/1047840X.2024.2442906

The Appraisal Model of Conspiracy Theories (AMCT): Applying Appraisal Theories to Understand Emotional and Behavioral Reactions to Conspiracy Theories

Lotte Pummerer a,b,, Theofilos Gkinopoulos c, Karen M Douglas d, Daniel Jolley e, Kai Sassenberg f,g
PMCID: PMC11912957  PMID: 40104213

Abstract

Beliefs in conspiracy theories are related to a diverse set of emotional and behavioral consequences. At the same time, a theoretical model detailing when a conspiracy theory is more likely to elicit confrontation compared to withdrawal, indirect aggression or community-building is missing. We argue that appraisals provide the missing link between conspiracy beliefs and their consequences, proposing the Appraisal Model of Conspiracy Theories (AMCT). Based on appraisal theories of emotions, we outline how the focus on different features that vary between conspiracy theories and the situations in which they are embedded (i.e., focus on secrecy vs. gained knowledge; powerlessness vs. option of confrontation; harm to oneself vs. others) facilitate specific appraisals, resulting in different behavioral outcomes. We also outline how the AMCT helps to reconcile inconsistent research on conspiracy beliefs by providing better predictions about their emotional and behavioral consequences.

Keywords: Anger, appraisal theory, conspiracy theories, disgust, emotion, fear, pride, schadenfreude


Conspiracy theories are defined as explanations for significant events and circumstances as outcomes of secret actions by powerful individuals or groups who aim to suit their interests at the expense of others (e.g., Goertzel, 1994). Beliefs in conspiracy theories – or conspiracy beliefs – are culturally ubiquitous and widespread (Oliver & Wood, 2014; West & Sanders, 2003; YouGov, 2020), and there is an increasing interest in the study of conspiracy theories from scholars, journalists, and policymakers alike. This attention is warranted because of the consequences of conspiracy beliefs. These consequences are diverse and range from distrust toward the government (Pummerer et al., 2022), vaccination hesitancy (Hornsey et al., 2018; Jolley & Douglas, 2014a), to detrimental intergroup outcomes such as violent protests and physical attacks (Imhoff et al., 2021). At the same time, conspiracy theories might also feel entertaining (Brotherton & Eser, 2015; J.-W. van Prooijen et al., 2022), arguably not resulting in immediate negative consequences, but having more positive connotations for conspiracy believers such as sensation, thrill and adventure seeking.

We argue that the broad range of consequences of conspiracy beliefs is a result of the variations in characteristics of conspiracy theories that are accessible or salient to individuals, due to their content and the situational context that conspiracy theories are embedded in. For example, theories such as Elvis Presley or Princess Diana faking their deaths likely elicit very different emotions and behavioral consequences than conspiracy theories claiming that the current government is trying to install a dictatorship. Likewise, it is possible that even the same conspiracy theory might bright up different emotions depending on which aspects are emphasized. For example, the belief that COVID-19 is a hoax might elicit positive (focusing on not having to worry or strictly keep to guidelines) or negative emotions (focusing on how the population is being manipulated). Overall, distinguishing between different aspects that are commonly part of conspiracy theories (such as their secrecy vs. their explanatory power, the alleged powerlessness vs. revelation of agents, and the harm directed either at one’s own group vs. others) but differently emphasized should be crucial in determining the emotional and behavioral consequences of a conspiracy theory in ways that can be predicted by theoretical models.

In this article, we argue that the integration of appraisal theories of emotions (for a summary, see Moors et al., 2013) within the research on conspiracy beliefs provides the key for making such predictions. That is, the depth of existing research on how appraisals are affected by salient characteristics of an event allows predictions regarding how different aspects of conspiracy theories elicit different appraisals, which, in turn, are connected to behavioral consequences (C. A. Smith & Lazarus, 1993). Specifying how different aspects of conspiracy theories elicit different appraisals, we provide a novel model of conspiracy beliefs and their emotional and behavioral consequences. In doing so, we integrate and reconcile research on conspiracy beliefs that has thus far been inconsistent, while making “risky predictions” (Anjum & Mumford, 2018; Popper, 1977) when conspiracy beliefs are most likely to lead to specific kinds of behavioral consequences, ranging from withdrawal, confrontation, aggression aimed at exclusion or community-seeking.

We will first provide a short review of conspiracy beliefs and appraisal theories relevant to the question at hand. Then we will elaborate our Appraisal Model of Conspiracy Theories (AMCT), specifying how the different characteristics of conspiracy theories work together with the current focus of the individual work in eliciting specific appraisals, and consequently behaviors. We also provide an overview of how the model informs existing research and propose empirical hypotheses for future research, resulting from the integration of appraisal theories into the literature on conspiracy theories. A final section will present key conclusions and discuss limitations.

Conspiracy Beliefs

Conspiracy theories allege that secret groups of typically powerful people have colluded for their own benefit, and at the expense of others (e.g., Goertzel, 1994). While conspiracy theories concern events that are of public interest (Douglas & Sutton, 2023), they are also found on a local level such as allegations that conspiracies occur in a specific workplace that employees should know about (Douglas & Leite, 2017) or doubts about transparency regarding measures in favor of renewable energy on the community level (Winter et al., 2022, 2024). These conspiracy beliefs can be very specific, for example, when people assume that a certain group (e.g., a political party) or a specific individual (e.g., Bill Gates) engages in conspiratorial acts. When people are confronted with a conspiracy theory, they subsequently tend to agree with it more than people who have not been exposed (Jolley & Douglas, 2014a, 2014b; Pummerer et al., 2022), even if they are not aware that their endorsement of the conspiracy theory has increased (Douglas & Sutton, 2008). Additionally, beliefs in a conspiracy theory can be formed and strengthened by (repeated) engagement with content and situations suggesting a specific conspiracy (Béna et al., 2023). We therefore choose the term conspiracy belief to describe the process of engaging with a specific conspiracy theory for the first time, or continued engagement with a conspiracy theory.

Beyond the belief in a specific conspiracy theory, some researchers assess broader suspicions of ongoing conspiracies, which is sometimes termed conspiracy mindset, conspiracy ideation, or conspiracy mentality (e.g., Imhoff & Bruder, 2014). In particular, conspiracy mentality may give rise to more specific beliefs (Imhoff & Bruder, 2014). In contrast, recent discussions also highlight general differences between conspiracy mentality and specific conspiracy beliefs (e.g., Douglas & Sutton, 2023; Imhoff et al., 2022; Nera, 2024; Pummerer, 2024; Sutton et al., 2024; Sutton & Douglas, 2020) and some studies report that the two concepts at times can be related to different behavioral outcomes (e.g., Imhoff & Lamberty, 2020; Pummerer et al., 2022; Trella et al., 2024). One reason for this might be differences in appraisals – a question that we also turn to below.

Current State of Research on Emotions and Conspiracy Beliefs

Researchers have made some attempts to address the impact of conspiracy beliefs on emotions. Several authors have argued that beliefs in conspiracy theories increase negative emotions such as fear and anger (e.g., Jolley & Paterson, 2020; Liekefett et al., 2023, p. 202; van Prooijen, 2022a). The argument is that conspiracy theories confront people with powerlessness considering actors with harmful intent and secret actions. While this argument is reasonable, it fails to account for differences in empirical findings regarding the relation between conspiracy beliefs and emotions. For example, while some authors report a positive correlation between conspiracy beliefs and fear (e.g. Bowes et al., 2021; Grzesiak-Feldman, 2013; Peitz et al., 2021; Sallam et al., 2020), others do not (Grzesiak-Feldman, 2007; Imhoff & Lamberty, 2020; Šrol et al., 2021; Swami et al., 2016; Wagner-Egger & Bangerter, 2007). Even more striking, it has been argued that conspiracy beliefs can also cause positive emotions such as pride or situational joy in light of gained insights, feelings of belonging or the derogation of an enemy or outgroup (e.g., Bilewicz et al., 2013; Van de Cruys et al., 2023; van Prooijen, 2016, 2022; van Prooijen & van Lange, 2014); and there are anecdotal accounts that conspiracy beliefs are connected to joy in the form of schadenfreude, as for example, in sports (Scheepers et al., 2003) and pride in one’s country in the form of collective narcissism (Cichocka et al., 2016; Cislak et al., 2021; Molenda et al., 2023; Sternisko et al., 2023). van Prooijen et al. (2022) even outlined one study where a conspiracy theory (about the Notre Dame fire) induced negatively valanced emotions alongside a study with a different conspiracy theory (speculating about the causes of death for Jeffrey Epstein) eliciting positively valanced emotions (see also Harmon-Jones et al., 2024). Overall, existing research shows inconsistent, if not contradictory, findings regarding the effects of conspiracy theories on emotions.

One reason for the inconclusiveness of the findings might be that existing research rarely distinguishes between different kinds of conspiracy theories (for an exception see Imhoff & Lamberty, 2020), nor does it often consider the specific features of conspiracy theories that make them distinguishable from each other, such as their differing degrees of malevolent intent (Douglas & Sutton, 2023), differences regarding whether specific actors are named or left in the dark, and how powerful or approachable the alleged conspirators are. Some measurement scales combine conspiracy theories implying great uncertainty and powerlessness (e.g., that a powerful and secretive group known as the New World Order is planning to take over sovereign governments) with conspiracy theories about past events that arguably have little consequence for everyday life today (e.g., NASA faked the moon landing; Lewandowsky et al., 2015). Other studies measure only the belief in one specific conspiracy theory such as the belief that the COVID-19 virus is a hoax (Imhoff & Lamberty, 2020) or that the assassination of John F. Kennedy was part of a broader conspiracy involving weapons manufacturers and CIA officials (Butler et al., 1995). Again others measure the belief in a fictitious conspiracy theory, such as the notion that a fictitious bus accident in an African Country was the outcome of a conspiracy (Lantian et al., 2017). Given that these conspiracy theories differ in the alleged conspirators, the extent of harm they imply and who the harm is directed at, it comes as no surprise that they might also evoke different emotions and behavioral responses.

Also, existing research has not yet examined whether even the same conspiracy theory might elicit different emotional and behavioral responses, depending on which aspects of a conspiracy theory are salient. This might also change depending on situational characteristics. For example, anecdotal accounts such as those from the Capitol siege on January 6, 2021 (Parramore, 2021) suggest that being in a group might convey strength and perceptions of control that might not exist when sitting alone at home. In sum, the time seems ripe to replace the one-size-fits-all approach with a more fine-grained one.

Rather than only building on studies about conspiracy beliefs, there is a lot to learn from other areas of research that over the years have accumulated evidence of how behavioral reactions are shaped by the evaluation of an event in a specific situation. More specifically, we argue that appraisal theories of emotions provide a guiding framework. In developing the AMCT, we specify how different aspects of a conspiracy theory salient to an individual are connected to emotions and action tendencies. Furthermore, the AMCT allows for the integration and reconciliation of so far inconsistent research on conspiracy beliefs and emotions, as well as new hypotheses of how the salience of different aspects of conspiracy theories and situational factors such as being in a group shape behavioral outcomes of conspiracy beliefs.

Appraisal Theories of Emotions

Appraisal theories of emotions (e.g., Arnold, 1960; Ellsworth, 2013; Frijda, 1986; Lazarus, 1991; Ortony et al., 1988; Roseman, 2013; Scherer, 2009) circle around the idea that the generation and differentiation of emotions can be predicted by individuals’ evaluations (appraisals) of events. These appraisals are driven and shaped by cognitive processes in several ways: In any given moment, individuals evaluate an event in relation to their goals and desires. Doing so, they arrive, for example, at the appraisal of an event as “pleasant” (if it matches a current goal or desire) or “unpleasant” (if it does not match a current goal or desire). Individuals further do so by using existing cognitive schemas that are salient in a given situation. For example, appraisals can be elicited and modified by guiding the focus of attention to specific aspects of a situation (see, e.g., Bailey et al., 2017; Briñol et al., 2018; Hemenover & Dienstbier, 1996; Neumann, 2000). Appraisals are also influenced by the current emotional state of the individual, since activated appraisals carry on to other situations that are then interpreted in a similar light (Lerner & Keltner, 2000; Quigley & Tedeschi, 1996). For example, anger in one situation automatically elicits appraisal of blame and, thus, additional anger, in another situation (Quigley & Tedeschi, 1996). Thus, appraisals are shaped by the respective nature of an event, but also by characteristics of the situation that make specific aspects of that event salient. The appraisals, in turn, shape action tendencies (e.g., Lerner & Keltner, 2000; Mackie et al., 2000; Scherer, 2019).

Individuals might evaluate an event on many different appraisal dimensions, and over the years, researchers have derived more and more distinctions (see, for example, Ellsworth & Scherer, 2003; Manstead & Fischer, 2001; Moors et al., 2013; Ritzenhöfer et al., 2019; Stöber & Muijs, 2001), thereby adding more nuance to the study of appraisals and emotions. Notwithstanding this complexity, there are four appraisal dimensions that can be thought of as a first “guiding grid” in the clustering of appraisals, which are relatively fundamental and which appear early on in the evaluation process (Gentsch et al., 2015; Lanctôt & Hess, 2007; Scherer, 1999, 2009, 2013). These four appraisals are also the appraisals many appraisal theories agree on (see e.g., Fontaine et al., 2007; Lerner & Keltner, 2000; Moors et al., 2013), which are the dimensions of pleasantness (i.e., whether or not an event represents a desired outcome), certainty (i.e., whether the future seems predictable and comprehensible), control (i.e., the ability to react and/or cope with the situation), and agency (i.e., whether oneself or someone else caused the event).

Appraisals are thought of in continuous dimensions; that is, individuals might evaluate a given event high, low or anywhere in between the dimensions of pleasantness, certainty, control and agency, with evaluations happening spontaneously, recursively and often unconsciously (e.g., Frijda, 1993; Lazarus, 1982; Scherer, 2009, 2019). Despite the almost endless possibility of combinations of appraisals, there are some appraisal combinations – summarized as emotions – which appear more often, most likely because they serve evolutionary adaptation (Ekman, 1999; Scherer, 1984; Scherer & Wallbott, 1994). These are called “modal emotions” (Scherer, 1984, 2009; Scherer & Wallbott, 1994), also resembling the basic emotions articulated by Ekman (1992, 1999) or Izard (2007). In this article, we mainly focus on the appraisals and action tendencies of such “modal emotions” (fear, anger, disgust, pride and joy in the form of schadenfreude). While the naming might sound like emotions are thought of as different categories, emotion research largely characterizes emotions as dimensional, with emotions being more or less present, and emotions shifting depending on the dimensional shifting of their underlying appraisals (see e.g., Briñol et al., 2018; Ekman, 1992; Moors et al., 2013). Many researchers also consider emotions not as singular occurrents, but as an interplay with feedback loops between arousal, facial expressions and physiological concomitants (see, for example, Ekman et al., 1983; Moors et al., 2013; Scherer, 2009). While we do consider these important aspects of emotion, the focus of the current model is on appraisals and resulting action tendencies as these promise the strongest added value to the understanding of responses to conspiracy beliefs.1

While complex emotions have properties that differ across cultures (e.g., Izard, 2007; Mauro et al., 1992), researchers have repeatedly shown that the relationship between appraisals and modal emotions is culturally ubiquitous; that is, the same appraisals will lead to similar modal emotions across cultures (Ellsworth, 1994; Gentsch et al., 2015; Izard, 2007; Kuppens et al., 2008; Mauro et al., 1992; Scherer, 1997, 1999; Tong, 2015). However, whether an event elicits the appraisals of pleasantness, certainty, control and agency is influenced by culture as well as other cognitive schemas and motives. These encompass situational influences (e.g., current emotional or motivational state), chronic person characteristics (e.g., personality traits), group-related aspects (e.g., being in a group that shares the same goal) and cultural appraisal tendencies. We summarize different ways in which these aspects shape appraisals below.

Overall, the appraisal of an individual occurs in reaction to an event. In the case of conspiracy beliefs, this event is the respective conspiracy theory that is evaluated by the individual in terms of the implied certainty, control and pleasantness. Due to the current focus of an individual or situational aspects, certain features of a conspiracy theory hereby might be salient, giving rise to specific appraisals. Applying appraisal theories to conspiracy beliefs, we will now outline how different features of conspiracy theories facilitate different appraisals and resulting action tendencies, allowing predictions about under which circumstances conspiracy beliefs are more (vs. less) likely to result in behaviors like withdrawal, confrontation, indirect aggression or community-building.

Appraisals Elicited by Conspiracy Theories

So far, research on conspiracy beliefs has mostly treated conspiracy theories as a homogeneous concept, thereby overlooking important differences between conspiracy theories (for an exception, see Douglas & Sutton, 2023), as well as how the focus on different aspects of a conspiracy theory – salient due to individual or cultural tendencies as well as the current situation – shapes the connected emotions and resulting behaviors. As specified above, the appraisal dimensions of pleasantness (dimension from unpleasant to pleasant), certainty (dimension from uncertain to certain), control (dimension from low control to high control) and agency (dimension from other-responsibility to self-responsibility) can serve as a first guiding grid that events typically are evaluated on.

The appraisal dimension of agency hereby is the only core appraisal dimension which is relatively fixed regarding conspiracy theories, since conspiracy theories, by definition, assume that other agents are responsible for societal events and their harmful consequences. Even in cases where the individual fights the conspirator, this action is understood and interpreted as a reaction, rendering the conspiracy belief always connected to the appraisal of other-responsibility. However, there is a considerable variation regarding the appraisal dimensions of certainty, control and pleasantness, which we will outline below (for an overview, see Table 1).

Table 1.

Different appraisals elicited by conspiracy theories.

Appraisal Dimension Conspiracy Theory Aspects that Elicit This Appraisal
Certainty Uncertainty Emphasis on secrecy and ambiguity regarding actors, their actions, goals and plans
Certainty Emphasis on the explanatory power of the conspiracy theory
Control Low control Emphasis on the power of the evil actor
High Control Emphasis on the ability to stop the actor because their actions are now revealed
Pleasantness Unpleasant Emphasis on harm to oneself or group
  Pleasant Emphasis on superior knowledge or superior morality; harm to the enemy

Certainty Dimension

Conspiracy theories, by definition, claim that powerful agents are acting in secrecy, implying that their actions cannot be trusted. As such, believing in conspiracy theories can relate to appraisals of uncertainty, since they can indicate unreliable knowledge about the situation and the future (i.e., powerful actors act in secrecy), cast doubt concerning important structures of society (e.g., the government, institutes, academia) and call into question commonly shared knowledge convictions and expectations about the future (e.g., Imhoff et al., 2018; Lewandowsky, 2018). Importantly, this should be especially the case for conspiracy theories that imply a great deal of secrecy and ambiguity regarding the actors, actions and their plans, such as conspiracy theories speaking of elites taking over the world (where questions like “Who?”, “When?” or “How?” are completely left unanswered). As such, studies show that the relatively vague notion of conspiracy theories happening as measured through the conspiracy mentality questionnaire (Bruder et al., 2013) increases uncertainty in the long-term (Liekefett et al., 2023). There are also situational aspects that should highlight exactly these characteristics of conspiracy theories. For example, an already activated appraisal dimension guides the focus of attention, making it more likely to see similar aspects in new situations (see, e.g., Bailey et al., 2017; Briñol et al., 2018; Hemenover & Dienstbier, 1996; Neumann, 2000). Thus, appraisals of uncertainty activated, for example, through societal change or uncertainty in a previous situation, should also highlight aspects of secrecy and ambiguity within conspiracy theories. Overall, the appraisal of uncertainty should be stronger the greater the emphasis is (both, through the content of the conspiracy theory as well as the situation) on secrecy and ambiguity regarding actors, actions and their goals/plans.

At the same time, conspiracy beliefs might also lead to appraisals of certainty since conspiracy theories can offer explanations for societally relevant events (J.-W. van Prooijen, 2020) and help to “connect the dots” of so-far unrelated events which might have otherwise left an individual puzzled (Douglas et al., 2017; Van de Cruys et al., 2023). This should be especially the case when conspiracy theories provide “meaningful” answers to questions that are otherwise left open or where existing answers are unsatisfactory to individuals. Research has shown that people are drawn to (we would add: specific kinds of) conspiracy theories because they intuitively assume that big events also have “big” causes. For example, they are more ready to assume that the killing of a president (vs. a shooting with no consequences) is the consequence of a conspiracy committed by a group rather than by one person alone (Leman & Cinnirella, 2007; McCauley & Jacques, 1979). One might also think back to the beginning of the COVID-19 pandemic, where the conspiracy theory of China deliberately developing the virus that was responsible for a world-wide change of markets and societies for some people sounded more convincing than the tentative speculations that the virus might stem from one (of probably thousands per day) human-animal interaction at a Wuhan market. Overall, the greater the emphasis on the explanations provided by a conspiracy theory, the greater the appraisal of certainty.

Control Dimension

Conspiracy theories arguably can also be related to appraisals of high or low control. Given that conspiracy theories allege evil or at least selfish intentions (i.e. pursuing interests at the expense of others) and secret actions of powerful actors, they are likely often to be connected to appraisals of low control. This is, for example, evident in studies showing that individuals feel powerless after exposure to a conspiracy theory claiming evil actions of powerful actors within one’s society (Jolley & Douglas, 2014b). There also are studies showing that conspiracy beliefs are related to the perception of low personal and political control (for a meta-analysis, see Biddlestone et al., 2022), and that exposure to conspiracy theories increases feelings of perceived victimhood (Bertin, 2024). Overall, the more the emphasis is on one’s own powerlessness in face of an evil and powerless actor, the more a conspiracy theory should elicit appraisals of low control.

However, there are also anecdotal accounts where conspiracy beliefs seem to empower people to react against a system they perceive to be unfair, and thus, perceptions of high control, as for example seen in protests like the March to the Capitol on January 6, 2021 (Frontline, 2021; Parramore, 2021) or protests during the COVID-19 pandemic (Fürstenau, 2021); Thus, it seems that conspiracy beliefs may also at times restore the perception of control when someone else can be blamed for things that are seen as problematic in society (Jolley et al., 2018). This effect should even be heightened when individuals act together in a group, as group cohesion supports control appraisals (see e.g., Beal et al., 2003), and perceived discrimination from powerholders has been shown to increase identification with the group of conspiracy theory believers (Nera et al., 2022). The appraisal of control seems to be closely tied to the perception of certainty, as individuals perceive that they have understood the situation (certainty) and have a target to fight against (Van de Cruys et al., 2023). Overall, the more the emphasis is on the gained ability to stop the actor (individually, or as a group) because their actions are now revealed, the more a conspiracy theory should elicit appraisal of high control.

Pleasantness Dimension

One might also argue that conspiracy beliefs are typically connected to the appraisal of unpleasantness, as they often (though not necessarily) assume harm done toward oneself or one’s own group through secret actions of malevolent powerholders. This is also the case for most newer experimental research – potentially driven by the COVID-19 pandemic – which predominantly features conspiracy theories that indeed imply unpleasantness for the participant. Given that the expectation of harm is prototypical for unpleasantness, the more the emphasis is on harm to oneself or the group, the more a conspiracy theory should elicit appraisals of unpleasantness.

However, definitions of conspiracy theories do not specify that harm is done toward one’s own group and there is also a research tradition of examining conspiracy theories that do not imply direct harm to the individual or their group. For example, theories such as that NASA faked the Apollo moon landing or Elvis Presley faking his death –measured also among individuals outside the US–, long dominated the (mostly correlational) research on conspiracy beliefs before the pandemic (e.g., Lewandowsky et al., 2015). In these cases, conspiracy theories might even evoke appraisals of pleasantness, for example, when conspiracy theories are experienced as entertaining (J.-W. van Prooijen et al., 2022), when they inspire feelings of being (morally) superior to the conspirators or their unaware adherents (J.-W. van Prooijen, 2022b), when they confirm ingroup values, or when they allege that harm is being done to an opponent, which can inspire schadenfreude (Spears & Leach, 2004). Research has, for example, shown that reading about a conspiracy theory casting blame on others can increase positive feelings about one’s own system (Jolley et al., 2018; Mao et al., 2024). Overall, the more the emphasis on a conspiracy theory providing superior knowledge, superior morality and/or harm to an enemy instead of harm to one’s own group, the more the conspiracy theory should elicit appraisals of pleasantness.

Personal and Cultural Aspects Influencing Appraisals

We have already mentioned ways in which the appraisals elicited by conspiracy theories are shaped by the current situation, such as the specific aspect of the conspiracy theory that the individual pays attention to, a preexisting appraisal (reinforcing similar appraisals), or whether the individual is in a group or not. In addition, there are chronic person characteristics that have an influence on appraisal tendencies, reflected in personality traits or the self-concept (for an overview, see van Reekum & Scherer, 1997). Such personality traits and self-concepts should lead to interindividual differences in what aspects of conspiracy theories dominate the perception, and consequently the appraisals and action tendencies the engagement with conspiracy theories brings forward.

Similarly, appraisals are also shaped by cultures (for an overview, see Keltner & Haidt, 1999; Scherer, 1997; Scherer & Brosch, 2009). For example, individualistic cultures are more quick to attribute events to the acts of other (powerful) individuals and groups than more collectivistic cultures (An et al., 2001; Choi et al., 1999; Matsumoto et al., 1988; Mauro et al., 1992; Morris & Peng, 1994), which should facilitate perceptions of low control. However, given that people from collectivist cultures place more emphasis on their social reference group and the interrelatedness (Oishi et al., 2004; Suh, 2002), they should feel more affected (i.e., greater experience of unpleasantness) when believing that one’s own population is being targeted by conspirators. Cultures also differ in the extent to which they are tolerant toward uncertainty (Hofstede, 2001; House et al., 2004), with countries from Central and Latin Europe, Japan, South Korea, Russia, the Middle East and Pakistan scoring higher on uncertainty avoidance, while English-speaking countries (e.g., United Kingdom, United States), China, Scandinavia, India, and Southeast Asia score lower on uncertainty avoidance (Hofstede et al., 2005). Uncertainty avoidance also encompasses appraising situations of mild uncertainty as strongly uncertain (DeWees & Lerner, 2020), which should increase the likelihood that conspiracy theories elicit appraisals of uncertainty in respective countries. So far, cross-cultural differences have been rarely considered in research on conspiracy theories (for exceptions see Adam-Troian et al., 2021; Cordonier et al., 2021; Hornsey et al., 2023; Hornsey & Pearson, 2022; Imhoff, 2022; Jovančević & Milićević, 2020). Through focusing on appraisals and their relations to action tendencies, the AMCT also provides a fruitful basis for examining how the consequences of conspiracy beliefs differ between cultures.

In sum, conspiracy theories should elicit some variation in the appraisals of certainty, control, and pleasantness, depending on which features of the conspiracy theory are salient. We hereby assume that there are differences between conspiracy theories as a whole, with some conspiracy theories being more likely to generally elicit, for example, appraisals of low control than others. Additionally, conspiracy theories might differ in the variety of appraisals they elicit. There are likely some conspiracy theories that elicit relatively fixed appraisals. For example, assuming that a secretive group known as the New World Order is planning to take over sovereign governments should mostly elicit appraisals of uncertainty, low control and unpleasantness. However, there are other theories in which there is extreme variation in possible appraisals depending on which aspects are salient to the individual. For example, believing that COVID-19 is a hoax might elicit appraisals of uncertainty, low control and unpleasantness when focusing on the alleged wrongdoing of governments and their possibilities of restricting the population, but appraisals of high certainty, control and pleasantness when focusing on one’s own superior knowledge about the situation that bears the option to safely to ignore the guidelines.

Here, we want to highlight that the respective abilities of conspiracy theories to elicit the different appraisals is based on typical examples from appraisals theories. Thus, while we have provided examples of conspiracy theories matching situations typically eliciting appraisals of certainty and uncertainty, high and low control, as well as pleasantness and unpleasantness, whether these conspiracy theories indeed reliably elicit the respective appraisals in individuals needs to be established by future research.

The Appraisal Model of Conspiracy Theories

The combinations of appraisals described above are connected to different emotions, as specified in appraisal theories (e.g., Arnold, 1960; Ellsworth, 2013; Frijda, 1986; Lazarus, 1991; Ortony et al., 1988; Roseman, 2013; Scherer, 2009). While all combinations of appraisals are possible, there are some appraisal combinations that occur more often, potentially due to evolutionary reasons (Ekman, 1999; Scherer, 1984; Scherer & Wallbott, 1994). Among them are the combinations of uncertainty, unpleasantness and low control being connected to fear, appraisals of certainty, unpleasantness, high control to anger, appraisals of certainty, unpleasantness, low control to disgust, appraisals of certainty, pleasantness, high control to pride, and appraisals of certainty, pleasantness, and low control to joy (Lerner & Keltner, 2000; C. A. Smith & Ellsworth, 1985; Tiedens & Linton, 2001). These combinations are also the focus of the AMCT. In addition to these five emotions, surprise is also often part of the list of modal (sometimes also called “basic”) emotions (e.g., Ekman, 1999; Izard, 2007; Scherer, 2009). However, recent research suggests that surprise is only elicited in the beginning, later giving rise to other emotions (Noordewier et al., 2016), and thus, surprise will not be discussed here at length.

Emotions have a core appraisal theme (Fredrickson, 1998; Lazarus, 1991; C. A. Smith & Lazarus, 1993), which specifies those kinds of situations in which the combination of appraisals are most likely to occur. Many emotion researchers agree that modal emotions can be thought of as “emotion families” (e.g., Ekman, 1992; Fredrickson, 1998; Izard, 2007; Scherer, 1984, 2009), for example, that fear can have different gradients and eliciting events, matching the core appraisal theme more or less perfectly.

Combining research on appraisal theories and conspiracy belief, we derive to the predictions of the AMCT that an emphasis on the secrecy and ambiguity regarding the actors and their actions, goals and plans, combined with a perceived powerlessness in light of the abilities of the conspirators, and harm to oneself or one’s own group is connected to the emotion of fear. An emphasis on the explanatory power of the conspiracy theory and the ability to stop the actor because their actions are now revealed when expecting harm to oneself or one’s group, is connected to the emotion of anger. Conspiracy theories revealing the contamination of purity (the core appraisal theme of disgust) which is hard to control while assuming harm to oneself or one’s group are connected to the emotion of disgust. In turn, when no harm or harm to enemies is implied and when the superior knowledge or morality through the explanatory power of the conspiracy theory is highlighted, then conspiracy beliefs should be related to pride (emphasis on one’s own actions) or joy in the form of schadenfreude (emphasis on actions from others).

Modal emotions as combinations of appraisals, in turn, are related to specific action tendencies, as established in emotion research (e.g., Carver & Harmon-Jones, 2009; Fredrickson, 1998; Frijda et al., 1989; Kreibig et al., 2010; Lerner & Keltner, 2001; Mercadante et al., 2021; Roseman et al., 1994). We thereby do not assume that emotional and behavioral reactions to conspiracy beliefs are different from emotional and behavioral reactions in other situations. Taken together, we put forward at the AMCT (see Figure 1), linking different features of conspiracy theories to different action tendencies.

Figure 1.

Figure 1.

Appraisal model of conspiracy theories (AMCT). Figure: Pawel Nuwaltzew.

In the next section, we will provide further details on how the different appraisal combinations of fear, anger, disgust, pride and joy in the form of schadenfreude are related to action tendencies and how the emphasis on different features of conspiracy beliefs facilitates specific action tendencies. We will also refer to existing research on conspiracy beliefs, pointing out how the integration of appraisal theories can help to make sense of inconsistencies between studies measuring emotional reactions to conspiracy beliefs.

Fear

Fear is related to the appraisals of uncertainty, unpleasantness and low control (Lerner & Keltner, 2000). This appraisal combination often emerges from perceiving danger or threat, no matter whether physical or goal-related (Frijda, 1986; Lench et al., 2011, p. 20). In terms of action tendencies, fear will increase vigilance toward threatening stimuli (Wenzel & Lystad, 2005) and the tendency to avoid or withdraw from a situation (Carver & Harmon-Jones, 2009).

As outlined above, the appraisals are likely to be elicited by focusing on those aspects of conspiracy theories that imply current or future harm to the participant or their ingroup (i.e., unpleasantness), secrecy and ambiguity regarding actors, actions and their goals/plans (i.e. uncertainty) and conspirators, who are so powerful or unidentifiable that they are not able to fight them, either alone, or collectively as a group (i.e., low control). An example of a conspiracy theory where the content should very dependably elicit uncertain, low-control and unpleasant appraisals is the theory of the New World Order (i.e., “A powerful and secretive group known as the New World Order are planning to eventually rule the world through an autonomous world government which would replace sovereign governments.”, Lewandowsky et al., 2015). Here, the alleged collusions are so vague, yet the allegedly involved people are so powerful that it seems impossible to escape their evil intentions. The emotion of fear should further be more likely to be elicited by this (or a similar) conspiracy theory if the mentioned aspects of unpleasantness, uncertainty and low control are salient, for example when the person already is experiencing fear through an unrelated event, or among individuals and cultures higher in uncertainty aversion.

Applying the lens of appraisal theories to existing research on conspiracy belief can help to get a better understanding of previous inconsistencies, for example regarding the relations between conspiracy beliefs and fear. In line with the model, measures of conspiracy beliefs that include vague and threatening statements (implying uncertainty, unpleasantness and low control) should be more related to fear than measures of conspiracy theories stating specific conspirators (i.e., certainty) that might be confronted (i.e., high control) and/or do not imply harm to an individual (i.e., allowing for pleasantness). This assumption has not been directly tested. However, longitudinal analyses using measures of conspiracy mentality or the flexible inventory of conspiracy suspicions, which both built on rather vague but threatening items (e.g., “There are secret organizations that greatly influence political decisions”, Bruder et al., 2013; “Reporters, scientists, and government officials are involved in a conspiracy to cover up important information about ___”, Wood, 2017), show that conspiracy beliefs on these measures predict more fear at a later time point in US-Canadian and German participants (Leibovitz et al., 2021; Liekefett et al., 2023). At the same time, conspiracy beliefs about Jews in Poland or conspiracy beliefs concerning different minorities in Switzerland – which arguably should be less threatening to people from majority groups – were unrelated to fear (Grzesiak-Feldman, 2007; Wagner-Egger & Bangerter, 2007).

Similarly, looking at the findings in light of appraisal theories can also explain why US participants seeing a film about the conspiracy theory surrounding the assassination of President John F. Kennedy (i.e., certainty regarding conspirators, no harm to participants) did not report more fear than the control group (Butler et al., 1995), while Greek participants reading a text suggesting that unnamed foreign forces were the cause of a huge wildfire that was spreading in Greece (i.e., uncertainty regarding conspirators and the future, potential future harm to participants) reported much more fear (d = 3.10) than participants reading a non-conspiracy control text about the wildfire (Gkinopoulos & Mari, 2023). As indicated above, some conspiracy theories should even reduce fear, namely, when the content and focus of the individual is about denying an existing threat (as long as the focus is not on the alleged concomitant manipulation of the general population). For example, believing that COVID-19 is a hoax was related to less perceived threat by COVID-19, both cross-sectionally (in UK and US samples, Imhoff & Lamberty, 2020) as well as longitudinally (in the Netherlands, J. van Prooijen et al., 2023). A similar effect could be expected for conspiracy theories claiming that anthropogenic climate change is a hoax when the focus is on the alleviation of the climate threat rather than the alleged evil governments. Overall, while existing research has not directly tested the impact of conspiracy theories on appraisals, the patterns in existing research are in line with the prediction that the more narratives emphasize uncertainty, harm and powerlessness regarding the conspirators, the more the exposure to conspiracy theories leads to fear, while conspiracy theories denying harm are expected to reduce existing fear.

When conspiracy beliefs induce fear, the suggested action tendency is withdrawal, as there is no point in confrontation. Such withdrawal from politics and society has also been reported in relation to conspiracy beliefs (e.g., Jolley & Douglas, 2014b; van der Linden, 2015). In line with our starting point that conspiracy beliefs are not all the same and that different features of conspiracy theories being differently salient in conspiracy theories should also bring forward different action tendencies, other studies have found a relation between conspiracy beliefs and more political or broader civic engagement (e.g., Jolley et al., 2018; Pantazi et al., 2022; Winter et al., 2023, see also Imhoff et al., 2021). We argue that this is due to differences in the underlying appraisals and, thus, connected emotions. More specifically, such increased engagement is more in line with the emotion of anger – the emotion which we turn to next.

Anger

Just like fear, anger is connected to appraisals of unpleasantness. But different to fear, anger is connected to appraisal of certainty and high control (Lerner & Keltner, 2000; C. A. Smith & Ellsworth, 1985). This appraisal combination is especially likely to result when people are not able to reach their desired goal, when they are hurt intentionally by another person (Carver & Harmon-Jones, 2009; Frijda, 1986), or due to the perception of injustice and inequality (Leach, 2008). In terms of action tendencies, anger prepares individuals to fight and to seek risks (Lerner & Keltner, 2001) to redress perceived wrongdoings or injustices (Carver & Harmon-Jones, 2009), often by attacking the anger evoking target (e.g., Mackie et al., 2000). Anger is, thus, related to more hostility (Wenzel & Lystad, 2005), aggression (Veenstra et al., 2018), and non-normative behaviors (Hastings & O’Neill, 2009).

As outlined above, conspiracy theories should especially lead to anger when there is expected harm to the individual or their group (i.e., unpleasantness) conspirators and their method of conspiring are known (i.e., certainty), and when the individual perceives that the threat can be prevented, mitigated or confronted (i.e., high control) – either by the individual or by a group that the individual identifies with. Examples for such theories are theories that imply a specific government or company malfeasance when an individual feels able to confront this malfeasance, such as in theories assuming pharmaceutical manipulation when the vaccine or drug can be avoided, or when the government can be confronted through collective action. As already evident in the examples, the experienced anger should vary with the perceived control in that moment, which can be individually or situationally heightened, for example when focusing on specific aspects of a theory, when being in a group, or when already being in a hostile mindset. Thus, the theory of 5 G masts spreading COVID-19 should especially evoke anger when focusing on the alleged malfeasance of the companies, when being with like-minded others, and when generally feeling that the threat can be mitigated (e.g., through turning off the Wi-Fi). Additionally, anger should vary depending on appraisal tendencies and values of the respective culture, with previous research suggesting that anger is more likely to be elicited by violations of harmony and injustice to the group in collectivist cultures compared to violations of autonomy in individualistic cultures (see e.g. Liu et al., 2019; Triandis, 2001).

Experimental studies conducted in the US confirm that conspiracy theories can increase anger in individuals, as evidenced in a study about governments and pharmacies covering up dangers of vaccinations (Featherstone & Zhang, 2020), about the death of John F. Kennedy (Butler et al., 1995), and about the theory of alleged election interference by Democrats or Russians, which elicited anger both among Democrats as well as Republicans, respectively (Albertson & Guiler, 2020). Additionally, there is a correlation between anger and the conspiracy beliefs that COVID-19 is caused by 5 G technology (Jolley & Paterson, 2020), that authorities hid the truth about COVID-19 (Peitz et al., 2021), and scales including items of the state’s interference regarding chemtrails or the attacks on 9/11 (Bowes et al., 2021; Šrol et al., 2021), conducted in the UK, US and Slovakia. Since arguably all these conspiracy theories imply harm while mentioning the respective conspirators and offering the chance to (collectively) confront the threat, they fit the typical appraisal structures proposed to elicit the emotion of harm and respective action tendencies. Future studies might systematically vary perceptions of control and certainty to examine differences in emotions and resulting action tendencies.

Anger has been shown to mediate the link between conspiracy beliefs and violence against objects related to the conspiracy in the UK (Jolley & Paterson, 2020), underlining the importance of understanding emotions in predicting action tendencies. Given the typical action tendencies of anger, it is likely that this emotion also plays a role in the relations between conspiracy beliefs and intentions to engage in everyday crimes (in the UK, Jolley et al., 2019), collective action (in German facebook groups, Frischlich, 2022), non-normative behaviors (in Greece and Germany, Gkinopoulos & Mari, 2023; Pummerer et al., 2022) and aggression, including the intention of violent attacks against the government (among German and US participants, Imhoff et al., 2021) or hostility toward the outgroup accused of the conspiracy (among participants in the Middle East, Poland, and Greece, Nyhan & Zeitzoff, 2018; Oleksy et al., 2021; Sapountzis & Condor, 2013). In addition, the fact that individuals derive certainty from the experience of a shared reality and higher appraisals of control when group cohesion is high, suggests that situations of protests are specifically likely to result in anger and aggression - even if the same conspiracy theory might be more linked to fear when the individual is again alone.

Disgust

Disgust is associated with appraisals of high certainty, unpleasantness and low control (Briñol et al., 2018; Tiedens & Linton, 2001; Smith & Ellsworth, 1985). The core appraisal theme (Lazarus, 1991) is to serve the function of protecting individuals from danger and illness, for example, by keeping one’s own objects clean, and avoiding contaminants. As such, the activated goal of disgust is to restore purity and cleanliness (Roseman et al., 1994; Schnall, 2017). Importantly, apart from objects, disgust can also be directed against individuals and groups, here also serving as a warning signal about the moral status of an action or person (Chapman et al., 2009; Pizarro et al., 2011). It can result from seeing moral transgressions (Haidt et al., 1997; Rozin et al., 1999) and is related to moral condemnation (Schnall, 2017; Schnall et al., 2008). In terms of action tendencies, disgust leads to the desire to create distance away from the object (Roseman et al., 1994; Rozin & Fallon, 1987), which may result in cleaning, creating visual or spatial boundaries, or distancing toward, and expulsion of morally condemned people. While both anger and disgust are related to aggression, the form and goal of the aggression is different. Anger is characterized by short-term aggression aimed at reaching a specific goal (e.g., coercing change in another person’s behavior) but with the long-term goal of reconciliation (e.g., Fischer & Roseman, 2007). Disgust, in turn, is focused on long-term distancing and exclusion, including elimination. While this might include direct aggressive behaviors in some instances, it is also connected to so-called indirect forms of aggression that maintain distance against the transgressor (while still being aggressive and harmful, e.g., being manipulative, excluding from groups, harming others’ reputations through gossiping, encouraging punishment by others; Fan et al., 2024; Molho et al., 2017).

Conspiracy theories should evoke disgust when they imply contamination or moral transgression unfavorably for the individual (i.e. unpleasantness), when the conspirator is known (i.e., certainty), yet when the threat is not easily confronted (i.e., low control), for example due to its contaminative nature. Admittedly, the emotion of disgust is somewhat special compared to the broader emotions of fear or anger, in that it is typically elicited by the specific violations of purity, cleanliness, and only in the broader context by violations of morality. However, there are also conspiracy theories which touch upon such violations. For example, they might create disgust against commonly used treatments such as medications or vaccinations by framing them in terms of a “violation of the purity of the body” (Hornsey et al., 2018) or, for example, by liking face masks to muzzles commonly used for restraining animals, as during the COVID-19 pandemic (Aratani, 2020). As we have outlined, appraisals and emotional reactions can change depending on which aspects are salient to an individual. Thus, a vaccination conspiracy theory might elicit disgust when thinking about the process of vaccination and how vaccinations allegedly violate the purity of the body but elicit anger when thinking about the wrongdoings of pharmaceutical companies.

A prominent example of a disgust related conspiracy theory is the Jewish Conspiracy Theory as it was spread by the propaganda in Nazi Germany which, among other things, stated that Jewish blood endangers the purity of the German race. Similar ideas can be identified for the conspiracy belief that the “pure” European race is being slowly and purposefully replaced by migrants (Jedinger et al., 2023), or that gender equality measures have been designed to harm traditional (“pure”) family values. Thus, while the groups eliciting disgust are still perceived as being threatening and not easily confronted through their alleged contamination, they might also be from groups that are typically more thought of as being from a minority or low-status group.

Experimental and longitudinal studies measuring disgust and conspiracy beliefs are rare. In a study conducted in the US by Albertson and Guiler (2020), participants who were exposed to the theory of an alleged election interference by Democrats or Russians showed more disgust, but also more anger, compared to participants who were not exposed to the theory, with effect sizes being similar in size for both emotions. Correlational studies further report a link between believing that people have not been told the full truth about COVID-19 by the authorities and disgust toward the political system in Italy, the UK, the US and South Korea (Moon & Travaglino, 2021), as well as vaccine hesitancy and disgust, as measured in a 24-nation study (Hornsey et al., 2018). It is, therefore, likely that antisemitic conspiracy theories and gender conspiracy beliefs also lead to disgust. While studies measuring this link are missing, believing in such theories is related to typical action tendencies of disgust. That is, believing in antisemitic conspiracy beliefs has been related to the desire to create physical and relational distance between oneself and Jewish individuals (Bilewicz et al., 2013), and gender conspiracy beliefs are linked to being less accepting of gay men and lesbians as coworkers, neighbors or family members (Marchlewska et al., 2019). While aggressive tendencies establishing a distance between oneself and the object are typically seen in behaviors called indirect aggression (e.g., exclusion, discrimination, bad-mouthing), disgust can also inspire direct forms of aggression aimed at seeking long-term distance and elimination. Two examples are the Holocaust and the terror attacks by Anders Breivik – both inspired by conspiracy beliefs built on the idea of the purity and contamination of one’s race, and marked not by the idea of confrontation and a potential later reconciliation, but elimination. Future studies might not only further investigate the link between different narratives of conspiracy beliefs, cultural differences and disgust, but also specifically investigate the differential outcomes for anger versus disgust in types and goals of aggression (i.e., more direct forms of aggression aimed at long-term reconciliation for anger, and forms of aggression maintaining or creating distance aimed at long-term exclusion for disgust).

Pride

A combination of certainty, pleasantness, and high control appraisals is associated with the experience of pride (e.g., C. A. Smith & Ellsworth, 1985; Tracy & Robins, 2004b, 2004a). This appraisal combination is especially likely to occur in response to an achievement that is perceived to have social value (Michie, 2009; Tracy & Robins, 2007), which might either be connected to an individual or a group (Delvaux et al., 2016). In terms of action tendencies, pride reinforces the action that elicited pride when it reflects the desired self-presentation (Tracy & Robins, 2004a, 2004b), potentially through boosting self-esteem (Brown & Marshall, 2001). It can also prepare the individual for the engagement with the environment, which can include activities like sharing the achievements and new insights with other people (Kreibig et al., 2010) with the goal to draw attention on the behavior and to gain acceptance and status in a group (Tracy & Robins, 2004a). Thus, in line with other positive emotions (Fredrickson, 2001), pride serves the overall goal of mastery (Lazarus et al., 1980) as well as connection and self-development.

Conspiracy theories might elicit pride through the pleasantness derived when individuals feel that they understand what is going on while many other do not (see e.g., Imhoff & Lamberty, 2017; Lantian et al., 2017) – a tendency which should especially be found in individualistic cultures emphasizing individual achievements (P. B. Smith & Bond, 2020). In addition, conspiracy theories should also elicit pride (in individuals as well as groups), when they perceive themselves as morally superior to another individual or outgroup alleged in a conspiracy (J.-W. van Prooijen, 2024). Thus, the knowledge gained contributes to the appraisal of certainty. To allow for pleasantness, there should also be no expected harm to the individual or ingroup implied in the conspiracy theory – or at least one that can be avoided or confronted through the knowledge gained and as a group (i.e., control). To date, experimental and longitudinal research examining the effect of conspiracy beliefs on pride is missing, rendering the idea that conspiracy beliefs can elicit pride open for debate. It is for future research to more systematically investigate this link. However, there are qualitative and theoretical manuscripts which already highlight this possibility, offering a basis for the aforementioned assumptions. For example, one qualitative study in the US reports that people report pride because of their beliefs since they feel like they have come to the respective conclusions by “doing their own research” (Carrion, 2018). Furthermore, for some conspiracy theories, individuals seem to have “aha moments” when solving riddles or being able to “connect the dots” (Van de Cruys et al., 2023). In digital environments, individuals also share conspiracy theories to build connections and receive likes (Ren et al., 2023), to signal group membership (Mus et al., 2022), or even to advance their group’s goals (Miller et al., 2016), which map with typical action tendencies of pride.

In sum, engagement with conspiracy theories should lead to pride and action tendencies of community-building under appraisals of high certainty, pleasantness and control, which should be especially likely when the gained insights or the superior morality are emphasized and when the situation of the individual fosters appraisals of pleasantness, certainty and control. However, while the idea of conspiracy beliefs eliciting pride is not new, studies experimentally testing these assumptions are so far missing.

Schadenfreude (Gloating)

Similarly, we want to inspire debate and future empirical studies regarding the emotion of joy in the form of schadenfreude (gloating), which so far has received little attention in conspiracy theory research. Joy is connected to the appraisal combination of high certainty, pleasantness and control (C. A. Smith & Ellsworth, 1985). In the realm of conspiracy theories, this is most likely to occur in the form of schadenfreude, which is a positive emotion in response to a misfortune of others that leads to personal gain (R. H. Smith et al., 2009). It typically arises when the misfortune happens to an enemy, or a person the individuals feel envy or resentment against, (Levy, 2020; R. H. Smith et al., 2009), and when this person initially displays dominance (Lange & Boecker, 2019). Experiencing the misfortune of such a person can reduce a perceived threat to oneself or one’s group (Leach et al., 2003; Leach & Spears, 2008; Van Dijk et al., 2011). Just like pride, the resulting action tendency is aimed at building connections (Fredrickson, 1998, 2001), for example in the form of celebration (Levy, 2020; Roseman & Steele, 2018).

Conspiracy theories imply potential misfortune, as they speak about evil actions of power holders. Often, the misfortune is perceived to be directed against people’s ingroups, but this does not need to be the case. In fact, conspiracy belief is often measured by asking people how much they agree with conspiracy theories that allege evil intentions against two or more outgroups, be they different countries or populations. For example, people might be asked about conspiracy theories surrounding 9/11 (i.e., evil intentions of the American government against American people) and Princess Diana (i.e., evil intentions of the British government against the British people) in the same scale (e.g., Douglas & Sutton, 2011; Leman & Cinnirella, 2007; Lewandowsky et al., 2015; Pummerer et al., 2023). Thus, conspiracy theories might also allege harm to outgroups, including potential enemies which fits the prototypical situation for schadenfreude. One example could be the theory that 9/11 was planned and conducted by the US government from the perspective of individuals that are anti-Western. This is not very far-fetched since this specific conspiracy theory is spread in the Middle East (Kohut et al., 2011; Rabasa, 2004), and since there are accounts that the event of 9/11 itself caused schadenfreude in some cities (Rabasa, 2004). Instances of schadenfreude are also reported after terror attacks in Europe (some of them itself inspired by conspiracy theories) from Israelis deriving pleasure from pain to countries that have before criticized Israel for anti-terror-measures (Levy, 2020).

Another area in which the engagement with conspiracy theories likely elicits schadenfreude is among sports fans. Schadenfreude is often found in relation to sports events, since such events include competition (e.g., McNamee, 2003; R. H. Smith et al., 2009; Van Dijk et al., 2011). Sport also seems to be a place for conspiracy theories to thrive (Bertin et al., 2023). Some of these conspiracy theories blame the referee or opponent team, but there are also conspiracy theories that allege harm to the opponent team as caused by a secretive third party (Scheepers et al., 2003). Despite these anecdotal accounts, there so far is no research systematically examining the effect of conspiracy beliefs on schadenfreude, rendering our theorizing novel but speculative. However, rather than denying such instances and anecdotal accounts of individuals deriving pleasure from the alleged harm of others, the AMCT provides a model to examine such instances in future studies, allowing a theoretical framework that considers the many instances in which individuals report to derive positive feelings from the engagement with conspiracy theories.

Discussion

Summary

In this article, we put forward the AMCT as a model that proposes different emotional and behavioral responses to conspiracy beliefs. In our effort to unpack the principles, rationale and assumptions that underpin AMCT, we have synthesized insights from appraisal theories with existing research on conspiracy beliefs to outline when conspiracy beliefs are most likely to elicit fear, anger, disgust, pride or joy in the form of schadenfreude, respectively. We assume that emotional reactions to conspiracy beliefs are like other appraisal processes. Thus, we have argued that conspiracy beliefs that emphasize harm, secrecy regarding conspirators, actions and their goals, as well as powerlessness are most likely to lead to fear and withdrawal. Anger, in turn, is more likely to occur when the explanatory power of a conspiracy theory is emphasized and when expecting harm, but also perceiving that the threat somehow can be confronted, either as an individual or as a group, which should lead to confrontation. Since disgust focuses on contaminations or moral transgressions, it is most likely to occur when conspiracy beliefs imply impurity or contamination by an identifiable group, which should lead to aggression aimed at distancing and extinction toward this group. Again, conspiracy beliefs that imply no harm or loss of control to the individual or their group but provide (superior) knowledge should elicit pride and, consequently, foster behaviors of community-building. A similar action tendency is expected to follow from conspiracy beliefs that do not imply harm to one’s own group, but to a hated enemy which should elicit schadenfreude. As mentioned, the ability of conspiracy beliefs to elicit positive emotions and respective action tendencies remains open for debate and needs to be tested in future research.

Limitations

In the AMCT, we have mostly drawn upon research on appraisal theories, interindividual and cross-cultural differences in appraisals, as well as their consequences in the form of action tendencies, and then applied the insights to the study on conspiracy beliefs. Overall, there are many studies confirming the general assumptions of appraisal theories (see e.g. Frijda et al., 1989; Kuppens et al., 2008; Mauro et al., 1992; Power & Hill, 2010; Scherer, 1997) and these continue to be influential in guiding new theory development (see e.g. Briñol et al., 2018; Ellsworth, 2013; Scherer, 2019; Tong, 2015). As such, we deemed that appraisal theories are ideally suited to also inform theorizing about the consequences of conspiracy beliefs. One advantage of appraisal theories is that they are relatively parsimonious in highlighting some core appraisal themes and modal emotions, thus offering a guiding grid in categorizing different outcomes.

However, it is exactly this parsimony of modal emotions that is questioned by some recent developments in emotion research. For example, some authors have questioned whether there are modal emotions at all, noting that the same emotion can have different autonomic nervous system (ANS) patterns, and arguing that the grouping of different emotion categories is learned rather than innate (Siegel et al., 2018, see also Barrett, 2006; Kreibig, 2010). Thus, there might not be one generalizable (modal) emotion of, for example, anger, and not even a specific prototype of anger in the sense of an emotion family, but different emotions depending on in which situation the “anger” is felt, also connecting to different functional action tendencies. Future studies might help to pinpoint such different types of emotions that so far are clustered within one emotion category. Future readers might evaluate whether the framework provided in the AMCT still provides some guiding framework for providing a broader cluster of emotions and action tendencies.

We have also referred to appraisal tendencies in binary terms (e.g., certainty vs. uncertainty, unpleasantness vs. pleasantness, high vs. low control), although we understand appraisals as dimensions where individuals score higher or lower in one of these domains, as is also the understanding of most appraisal theorists (e.g. C. A. Smith & Ellsworth, 1985). Indeed, while the appraisal of low control is a very important aspect in the experience of the emotion of fear (e.g., Lerner & Keltner, 2001; C. A. Smith & Ellsworth, 1985), this is not so much the case for the experience of disgust (see e.g., Russell & Giner-Sorolla, 2011; C. A. Smith & Ellsworth, 1985). However, for simplicity, we decided not to display these kinds of nuances in our descriptions. For the same reason, we also decided to stick with modal (sometimes also called basic) emotions, fully acknowledging that existing research (building on appraisal theories or not) has much to provide regarding complex emotions which would likewise be applicable to the study of conspiracy beliefs. We look forward to future studies and future theoretical models exploring them further, adding even more variety of responses elicited by conspiracy theories.

We also want to add as a caveat that most of the empirical studies on conspiracy theories have been conducted in the geographic West, which is a constraint on generality (see also Butter & Reinkowski, 2014) and (experimental) research on conspiracy beliefs from a more diverse set of countries is much needed (for a start, see Adam-Troian et al., 2021; Douglas et al., 2023; Hornsey et al., 2018, 2023; Hornsey & Pearson, 2022; Imhoff et al., 2022; Nyhan & Zeitzoff, 2018). We believe that our proposed model also provides a helpful tool in deriving hypotheses about cross-cultural differences and studies therein, in the hopes that predictions about cross-cultural differences will become more nuanced over time.

Considering cultural differences, the goal of the model itself might be regarded as a culturally biased attempt. Overall, we focused on the question of how appraisals elicited by conspiracy beliefs make one emotion and connected behavioral tendency more likely than others, thus, focusing on the differentiation between emotions. However, it seems that non-Western countries have less problems with acknowledging and experiencing several emotions at once, even thinking of emotions in dialectical terms (Grossmann et al., 2014, 2016), thus adding another layer and perspective to cross-cultural research on emotions and their behavioral tendencies, also regarding conspiracy beliefs.

One might also debate whether and under which circumstances the emotions elicited through a conspiracy theory are indeed the consequences of the alleged conspiracy rather than emotions caused by its (non-conspiracy) content. For example, one might say that schadenfreude elicited by conspiracy theories that imply harm to one’s enemy is caused simply because of the harm, and not necessarily because of the assumed conspiracy causing the harm. Arguments of this kind imply that conspiracy theories can and should somehow be clearly separated into conspiracy and non-conspiracy content, or that there is a content-free core of conspiracy theories that can be somehow extrapolated and measured. Recent discussions show that this in itself is a difficult task heavily debated in the field of conspiracy beliefs research in psychology (see e.g. Bruder et al., 2013; Douglas & Sutton, 2023; Imhoff, Bertlich, et al., 2022; Meuer et al., 2023; Nera, 2024; Nera & Schöpfer, 2023; Oleksy et al., 2021; Pummerer, 2024; Sutton et al., 2024; Sutton & Douglas, 2020), and one questioned (both, in terms of meaningfulness and success) by researchers from neighboring disciplines such as Cultural History (see e.g. Butter, 2024; Butter & Knight, 2015).

One difficulty is that conspiracy theories typically are deeply embedded in cultural contexts and social identities, making it difficult (if not impossible) to study one without the other, or to prime conspiracy content without also priming content that typically comes with conspiracy theories. There is also the practical question of why one would attempt such a separation. The goal of the AMCT was to outline how different appraisals elicited through conspiracy theories spreading in real-life and being embedded in cultural contexts lead to different behavioral reactions. Rather than assuming conspiracy theories always lead to fear and withdrawal, confrontation or community-building, we argued that the content of the conspiracy theory as well as the current focus and situation of the individual needs to be considered. Thus, the model itself allows for appraisals being elicited through aspects that are automatically included in or logical consequences of a conspiracy theory, even if they do not constitute a” pure” conspiracy part (if distinguishable at all). For example, we argued that conspiracy theories denying the existence of a threat such as COVID-19 or climate change (i.e. consequences or concomitants of believing that governments and scientists exaggerate or manipulate climate-related findings for their own goals) should decrease fear.

The question might remain whether a “pure” conspiracy part of a theory would still be able to elicit fear, anger, disgust, pride, or schadenfreude above and beyond otherwise implied content. We would certainly argue that it can, since the implied secrecy of a malevolent act should make expected harmful events even more fear-eliciting, increase anger against powerholders not keeping to the rules, and highlight an unmoral and disgust-eliciting character. Likewise, it can be specifically the unveiled secrecy that elicits pride, or harm that comes unexpectedly for the enemies increasing schadenfreude. Overall, albeit we argue that focusing on such a “pure” conspiracy part is not necessary for the model, it also does not constitute a threat for its validity, but rather highlights its core aspects.

Conclusions

We have argued that conspiracy beliefs can evoke different appraisals and associated with this, different emotions, depending on what features of a conspiracy theory are emphasized. The action tendencies elicited by these combinations of appraisals should also determine the consequences of conspiracy beliefs, ranging from withdrawal and confrontation to more indirect aggression or even community-building. Given the noticeable differences in emotions experienced in the context of conspiracy theories and in the action tendencies, we argued that research on conspiracy theories needs to consider the differences between conspiracy beliefs and what aspects of conspiracy theories are salient in any given situation. Beyond this main message, several conclusions result from our discussion.

The Variety of Responses Elicited by Conspiracy Theories

As argued in the sections above, conspiracy theories can elicit appraisals of uncertainty as well as certainty, and high as well as low control, depending on the highlighted aspects of a conspiracy theory. Additionally, it seems possible that conspiracy beliefs also elicit appraisals of pleasantness – a possibility that needs to be established in future research. Given this diversity, any well-meant attempt to single out one appraisal or one emotion that is related to conspiracy beliefs (e.g., anger or fear) is determined to fail from the beginning. At the same time, this does not mean that all appraisals are equally frequent. We assume that appraisals of low control and uncertainty are rather frequent considering the assumed evil intentions from powerholders.

It might also be possible, however, that conspiracy theories are most examined in these contexts by researchers. Research in recent years, to a great extent, has focused on conspiracy theories surrounding the COVID-19 pandemic, during which appraisals of low control and uncertainty were especially prevalent. While this research was certainly important and timely, it seems somewhat disconnected to conspiracy theories such as the Apollo moon landing or Elvis Presley not being alive, which long have played an important role in the assessment of conspiracy beliefs (e.g., Lewandowsky et al., 2015).

One reason that those two lines of research might report different results in terms of their emotional and behavioral consequences is that both research lines focus on different features in conspiracy theories, and were conducted during times that differently gave preference to certain appraisals (e.g., that of uncertainty and low control, as conveyed by the situation of the pandemic). Additionally, what became apparent in this review is that research on conspiracy beliefs and disgust, pride or schadenfreude as well as the appraisal patterns eliciting these emotions is strikingly missing considering anecdotal accounts of conspiracy beliefs eliciting these emotions. We hope that our model helps to put forward more research in these domains by providing a better theoretical framework for studying those emotional reactions to conspiracy beliefs and believe that research focusing on these emotions will greatly advance the field and produce a better understanding of how the content of conspiracy theories shapes different consequences.

Consequences of Conspiracy Beliefs as Action Tendencies Associated with Specific Emotions

Existing research has found that beliefs in different conspiracy theories (even conspiracy theories concerning the same event such as the COVID-19 pandemic) lead to different consequences (Imhoff & Lamberty, 2020) and that the consequences of conspiracy beliefs are mediated by emotions such as anger (Jolley & Paterson, 2020; Peitz et al., 2021b) and anxiety (Peitz et al., 2021). The current article goes one step further by detailing which features of conspiracy theories are likely to elicit which action tendencies. Overall, conspiracy beliefs eliciting the appraisal patterns associated with fear (i.e., uncertainty, unpleasantness, low control) should lead to withdrawal, while confrontation and non-normative action is expected in cases where conspiracy theories elicit appraisal patterns associated with anger (i.e., certainty, unpleasantness, high control). Likewise, indirect aggression and behaviors aimed at distancing, cleaning and elimination of aggressors is expected from conspiracy content and orientation connected to disgust (i.e., certainty, unpleasantness, low control). In addition, we have outlined how conspiracy beliefs might also elicit the positive emotions of pride and schadenfreude connected to action tendencies of community-building.

Overall, this framework offers a glimpse into the many possible consequences of conspiracy beliefs, by making predictions of consequences as well as processes underlying the consequences that can, and should, be tested by future research, in the hopes that this also provides more insights into how conspiracy theories prepare different actions.

The Importance of Studying Appraisals and Emotions as Occurring in the Moment

The proposed framework also highlights the importance of studying appraisals in the moment to understand the specific consequences of conspiracy beliefs. As shown above, the consequences of conspiracy beliefs can be wide-ranging both in terms of the versatility (ranging from withdrawal over sharing of insights to confrontation) as well as the severity of the consequences for society. Appraisals might, however, vary within individuals and across situations as outlined above. For example, an individual might experience fear when thinking about a conspiracy theory and being alone at home, while the same thought might increase anger when being at a protest, together with a perceived powerful mass. Thus, considering the appraisals in the very moment seems crucial to understand how the content of the conspiracy theory and the current orientation of the individual shape behavioral consequences.

Additionally, which aspects of conspiracy theories are salient might also be driven by more stable personality traits or cultural aspects, implying that conspiracy theories can mean different things for different individuals – even within one culture. For example, while some individuals might focus on the alleviating thought that COVID-19 is a hoax, others might focus on the alleged manipulation of scientific data in order to make such a conspiracy possible, which resemble two sides of the same “conspiracy coin”. Future studies have much to uncover by not only assessing the broader belief in a conspiracy theory, but also examining, which aspects of a conspiracy theory are more or less salient to an individual. This is even more true when looking at different cultures. Given that cultures differ in their appraisal tendencies, it seems likely that the same conspiracy theory elicits different appraisals (and, thus, different behavioral reactions) across different cultures, and we strongly encourage researchers to examine conspiracy beliefs more cross-culturally.

Final Remarks

Currently, the field lacks theories concerning the relation between conspiracy beliefs and emotions, how conspiracy beliefs elicit the diverse set of outcomes thus far described in the literature, and differences between specific conspiracy theories (but, see Douglas & Sutton, 2023). Drawing from research on appraisal theories and emotions, we provide a framework for research on conspiracy beliefs describing how the focus on different features of conspiracy theories relates to different action tendencies determining the consequence of conspiracy beliefs The different types of reactions to conspiracy theories advance the existing literature by offering a framework that makes it possible to integrate different strands of research on conspiracy beliefs that thus far seem fragmented or inconsistent, such as anecdotal accounts and research indicating that conspiracy beliefs can elicit positive emotions, while other report on conspiracy beliefs eliciting anger and fear. We thus offer a framework that helps to integrate existing research while preserving and exemplifying the diversity of conspiracy beliefs and emotions. Overall, we encourage researchers to discover differences between conspiracy theories and to pay attention to what features in a conspiracy theory are highlighted rather than – as is currently more common – treating them as interchangeable, and propose using appraisal dimensions to systematize those differences. Paying attention to the different kinds of conspiracy theories and the different aspects within one conspiracy theory that might be (situationally) highlighted or downplayed will greatly improve predictions regarding the consequences of conspiracy beliefs, pointing to new interventions and a future research agenda regarding the understudied subject of conspiracy beliefs and emotions.

Acknowledgements

The authors thank Lukas Fock for his help in the literature review and Pawel Nuwaltzew for creating Figure 1.

Funding Statement

The first author received funding as part of the Central Research Funding of the University of Bremen (ZF01/2024). The third author acknowledges the support of the European Research Council (grant number 101018262).

Footnotes

1

This focus on appraisals and action tendencies implies that our theorizing does not focus on emotions as a phenomenon but builds on elements of appraisal theories of emotions to provide a better understanding of the phenomenon we are interested in (i.e., responses to conspiracy theories). We acknowledge that due to this focus, some of our theorizing could also be categorized as motivational. However, a discussion about commonalities and differences between emotional and motivational aspects in general or regarding the AMCT would not only exceed the scope of the paper but would also be beyond our goal to derive a theory of how different aspects of conspiracy theories relate to different behavioral tendencies.

Disclosure Statement

No potential conflict of interest was reported by the author(s).

References

  1. Adam‐Troian, J., Wagner‐Egger, P., Motyl, M., Arciszewski, T., Imhoff, R., Zimmer, F., Klein, O., Babinska, M., Bangerter, A., Bilewicz, M., Blanuša, N., Bovan, K., Bužarovska, R., Cichocka, A., Çelebi, E., Delouvée, S., Douglas, K. M., Dyrendal, A., Gjoneska, B., … van Prooijen, J. (2021). Investigating the Links Between Cultural Values and Belief in Conspiracy Theories: The Key Roles of Collectivism and Masculinity. Political Psychology, 42(4), 597–618. 10.1111/pops.12716 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  2. Albertson, B., & Guiler, K. (2020). Conspiracy theories, election rigging, and support for democratic norms. Research & Politics, 7(3), 2053168020959859. 10.1177/2053168020959859 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  3. An, K., Hui, M. K., & Leung, K. (2001). Who should be responsible? Effects of voice and compensation on responsibility attribution, perceived justice, and post-complaint behaviors across cultures. International Journal of Conflict Management, 12(4), 350–364. 10.1108/eb022863 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  4. Anjum, R. L., & Mumford, S. (2018). Risky predictions. In Anjum R. L. & Mumford S. (Eds.), Causation in science and the methods of scientific discovery (p. 0) Oxford University Press. 10.1093/oso/9780198733669.003.0022 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  5. Aratani, L. (2020). How did face masks become a political issue in America? In The Guardian. https://www.theguardian.com/world/2020/jun/29/face-masks-us-politics-coronavirus
  6. Arnold, M. B. (1960). Emotion and personality. Vol. I. Psychological aspects (pp. xiv, 296) Columbia University Press. [Google Scholar]
  7. Bailey, H. R., Kurby, C. A., Sargent, J. Q., & Zacks, J. M. (2017). Attentional focus affects how events are segmented and updated in narrative reading. Memory & Cognition, 45(6), 940–955. 10.3758/s13421-017-0707-2 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Barrett, L. F. (2006). Solving the emotion paradox: Categorization and the experience of emotion. Personality and Social Psychology Review: An Official Journal of the Society for Personality and Social Psychology, Inc, 10(1), 20–46. 10.1207/s15327957pspr1001_2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Beal, D. J., Cohen, R. R., Burke, M. J., & McLendon, C. L. (2003). Cohesion and performance in groups: A meta-analytic clarification of construct relations. The Journal of Applied Psychology, 88(6), 989–1004. 10.1037/0021-9010.88.6.989 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Béna, J., Rihet, M., Carreras, O., & Terrier, P. (2023). Repetition could increase the perceived truth of conspiracy theories. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 30(6), 2397–2406. 10.3758/s13423-023-02276-4 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Bertin, P. (2024). The victimizing effects of conspiracy beliefs. Zeitschrift Für Psychologie, 232(1), 26–37. 10.1027/2151-2604/a000542 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  12. Bertin, P., Delouvée, S., McColl, K., & van Prooijen, J. W. (2023). Rage against the machine: Investigating conspiracy theories about the video assistant referee on Twitter during the 2018 FIFA World Cup. Sport Management Review, 26(4), 495–516. 10.1080/14413523.2022.2127179 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  13. Biddlestone, M., Green, R., Cichocka, A., Douglas, K. M., & Sutton, R. M. (2022). A systematic review and meta-analysis of the motives associated with conspiracy beliefs. PsyArXiv. 10.31234/osf.io/rxjqc [DOI] [PubMed]
  14. Bilewicz, M., Winiewski, M., Kofta, M., & Wójcik, A. (2013). Harmful ideas, the structure and consequences of anti-semitic beliefs in Poland. Political Psychology, 34(6), 821–839. 10.1111/pops.12024 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  15. Bowes, S. M., Costello, T. H., Ma, W., & Lilienfeld, S. O. (2021). Looking under the tinfoil hat: Clarifying the personological and psychopathological correlates of conspiracy beliefs. Journal of Personality, 89(3), 422–436. 10.1111/jopy.12588 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Briñol, P., Petty, R. E., Stavraki, M., Lamprinakos, G., Wagner, B., & Díaz, D. (2018). Affective and cognitive validation of thoughts: An appraisal perspective on anger, disgust, surprise, and awe. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 114(5), 693–718. 10.1037/pspa0000118 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Brotherton, R., & Eser, S. (2015). Bored to fears: Boredom proneness, paranoia, and conspiracy theories. Personality and Individual Differences, 80, 1–5. 10.1016/j.paid.2015.02.011 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  18. Brown, J. D., & Marshall, M. A. (2001). Self-esteem and emotion: Some thoughts about feelings. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 27(5), 575–584. 10.1177/0146167201275006 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  19. Bruder, M., Haffke, P., Neave, N., Nouripanah, N., & Imhoff, R. (2013). Measuring individual differences in generic beliefs in conspiracy theories across cultures: Conspiracy mentality questionnaire. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 225. 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00225 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Butler, L. D., Koopman, C., & Zimbardo, P. G. (1995). The psychological impact of viewing the film “JFK”: Emotions, beliefs, and political behavioral intentions. Political Psychology, 16(2), 237–257. 10.2307/3791831 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  21. Butter, M. (2024). Die psychologische Forschung aus Sicht der Kulturgeschichte. In Imhoff R. (Ed.), Die Psychologie der Verschwörungstheorien: Von dunklen Mächten sonderbar belogen (pp. 157–175). Hogrefe. [Google Scholar]
  22. Butter, M., & Knight, P. (2015). Bridging the great divide: Conspiracy theory research for the 21st century. Diogenes, 62(3-4), 17–29. 10.1177/0392192116669289 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  23. Butter, M., & Reinkowski, M. (2014). Introduction: Mapping conspiracy theories in the United States and the Middle East. In Conspiracy theories in the United States and the Middle East: A comparative approach (pp. 1–32). 10.1515/9783110338270.1 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  24. Carrion, M. L. (2018). “You need to do your research”: Vaccines, contestable science, and maternal epistemology. Public Understanding of Science (Bristol, England), 27(3), 310–324. 10.1177/0963662517728024 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Carver, C. S., & Harmon-Jones, E. (2009). Anger is an approach-related affect: Evidence and implications. Psychological Bulletin, 135(2), 183–204. 10.1037/a0013965 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Chapman, H. A., Kim, D. A., Susskind, J. M., & Anderson, A. K. (2009). In bad taste: Evidence for the oral origins of moral disgust. Science (New York, N.Y.), 323(5918), 1222–1226. 10.1126/science.1165565 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Choi, I., Nisbett, R. E., & Norenzayan, A. (1999). Causal attribution across cultures: Variation and universality. Psychological Bulletin, 125(1), 47–63. 10.1037/0033-2909.125.1.47 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  28. Cichocka, A., Marchlewska, M., & De Zavala, A. G. (2016). Does self-love or self-hate predict conspiracy beliefs? Narcissism, self-esteem, and the endorsement of conspiracy theories. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 7(2), 157–166. 10.1177/1948550615616170 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  29. Cislak, A., Marchlewska, M., Wojcik, A. D., Śliwiński, K., Molenda, Z., Szczepańska, D., & Cichocka, A. (2021). National narcissism and support for voluntary vaccination policy: The mediating role of vaccination conspiracy beliefs. Group Processes & Intergroup Relations, 24(5), 701–719. 10.1177/1368430220959451 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  30. Cordonier, L., Cafiero, F., & Bronner, G. (2021). Why are conspiracy theories more successful in some countries than in others? An exploratory study on Internet users from 22 Western and non-Western countries. Social Science Information, 60(3), 436–456. 10.1177/05390184211018961 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  31. Delvaux, E., Meeussen, L., & Mesquita, B. (2016). Emotions are not always contagious: Longitudinal spreading of self-pride and group pride in homogeneous and status-differentiated groups. Cognition & Emotion, 30(1), 101–116. 10.1080/02699931.2015.1018143 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. DeWees, B. R., & Lerner, J. S. (2020). Uncertainty avoidance. In Zeigler-Hill V. & Shackelford T. K. (Eds.), Encyclopedia of personality and individual differences (pp. 5647–5652). Springer International Publishing. 10.1007/978-3-319-24612-3_806 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  33. Douglas, K. M., & Leite, A. C. (2017). Suspicion in the workplace: Organizational conspiracy theories and work-related outcomes. British Journal of Psychology (London, England: 1953), 108(3), 486–506. 10.1111/bjop.12212 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Douglas, K. M., & Sutton, R. M. (2008). The hidden impact of conspiracy theories: Perceived and actual influence of theories surrounding the death of Princess Diana. The Journal of Social Psychology, 148(2), 210–221. 10.3200/SOCP.148.2.210-222 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Douglas, K. M., & Sutton, R. M. (2011). Does it take one to know one? Endorsement of conspiracy theories is influenced by personal willingness to conspire. The British Journal of Social Psychology, 50(3), 544–552. 10.1111/j.2044-8309.2010.02018.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Douglas, K. M., & Sutton, R. M. (2023). What are conspiracy theories? A definitional approach to their correlates, consequences, and communication. Annual Review of Psychology, 74(1), 271–298. 10.1146/annurev-psych-032420-031329 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Douglas, K. M., Sutton, R. M., & Cichocka, A. (2017). The psychology of conspiracy theories. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 26(6), 538–542. 10.1177/0963721417718261 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Douglas, K. M., Sutton, R. M., Van Lissa, C. J., Stroebe, W., Kreienkamp, J., Agostini, M., Bélanger, J. J., Gützkow, B., Abakoumkin, G., Khaiyom, J. H. A., Ahmedi, V., Akkas, H., Almenara, C. A., Atta, M., Bagci, S. C., Basel, S., Berisha Kida, E., Bernardo, A. B. I., Buttrick, N. R., … Leander, N. P. (2023). Identifying important individual- and country-level predictors of conspiracy theorizing: A machine learning analysis. European Journal of Social Psychology, 53(6), 1191–1203. 10.1002/ejsp.2968 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  39. Ekman, P. (1992). An argument for basic emotions. Cognition and Emotion, 6(3-4), 169–200. 10.1080/02699939208411068 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  40. Ekman, P. (1999). Basic emotions. In Dalgleish T. & Power M. (Eds.), Handbook of cognition and emotion (pp. 45–60). [Google Scholar]
  41. Ekman, P., Levenson, R. W., & Friesen, W. V. (1983). Autonomic nervous system activity distinguishes among emotions. Science (New York, N.Y.), 221(4616), 1208–1210. 10.1126/science.6612338 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Ellsworth, P. C. (1994). Sense, culture, and sensibility. In Emotion and culture: Empirical studies of mutual influence (pp. 23–50). American Psychological Association. 10.1037/10152-001 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  43. Ellsworth, P. C. (2013). Appraisal theory: Old and new questions. Emotion Review, 5(2), 125–131. 10.1177/1754073912463617 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  44. Ellsworth, P. C., & Scherer, K. R. (2003). Appraisal processes in emotion. In Davidson R. J. (Ed.), Handbook of affective sciences (pp. 572–595). Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
  45. Fan, L., Molho, C., Kupfer, T. R., Sauter, D. A., & Tybur, J. M. (2024). Beyond outrage: Observers anticipate different behaviors from expressors of anger versus disgust. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 15(4), 450–460. 10.1177/19485506231176954 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  46. Featherstone, J. D., & Zhang, J. (2020). Feeling angry: The effects of vaccine misinformation and refutational messages on negative emotions and vaccination attitude. Journal of Health Communication, 25(9), 692–702. 10.1080/10810730.2020.1838671 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Fischer, A. H., & Roseman, I. J. (2007). Beat them or ban them: The characteristics and social functions of anger and contempt. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 93(1), 103–115. 10.1037/0022-3514.93.1.103 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Fontaine, J. R. J., Scherer, K. R., Roesch, E. B., & Ellsworth, P. C. (2007). The world of emotions is not two-dimensional. Psychological Science, 18(12), 1050–1057. 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.02024.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Fredrickson, B. L. (1998). What good are positive emotions? Review of General Psychology: journal of Division 1, of the American Psychological Association, 2(3), 300–319. 10.1037/1089-2680.2.3.300 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Fredrickson, B. L. (2001). The role of positive emotions in positive psychology: The broaden-and-build theory of positive emotions. American Psychologist, 56(3), 218–226. 10.1037/0003-066X.56.3.218 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Frijda, N. H. (1986). The emotions. Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
  52. Frijda, N. H. (1993). The place of appraisal in emotion. Cognition & Emotion, 7(3-4), 357–387. 10.1080/02699939308409193 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  53. Frijda, N. H., Kuipers, P., & ter Schure, E. (1989). Relations among emotion, appraisal, and emotional action readiness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57(2), 212–228. 10.1037/0022-3514.57.2.212 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  54. Frischlich, L. (2022). “Resistance!”: Collective action cues in conspiracy theory-endorsing facebook groups. Media and Communication, 10(2), 130–143. 10.17645/mac.v10i2.5182 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  55. Frontline . (2021, January 12). What Alex Jones Said in the Lead Up to the Capitol Riot. https://www.pbs.org/wgbh/frontline/article/what-conspiracy-theorist-alex-jones-said-in-the-lead-up-to-the-capitol-riot/
  56. Fürstenau, M. (2021, March 4). Germany’s Querdenker COVID protest movement. Dw.Com. https://www.dw.com/en/meet-germanys-querdenker-covid-protest-movement/a-57049985
  57. Gentsch, K., Grandjean, D., & Scherer, K. R. (2015). Appraisals generate specific configurations of facial muscle movements in a gambling task: Evidence for the component process model of emotion. PloS One, 10(8), e0135837. 10.1371/journal.pone.0135837 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Gkinopoulos, T., & Mari, S. (2023). How exposure to real conspiracy theories motivates collective action and political engagement? Τhe moderating role of primed victimhood and underlying emotional mechanisms in the case of 2018 bushfire in Attica. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 53(1), 21–38. 10.1111/jasp.12923 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  59. Goertzel, T. (1994). Belief in conspiracy theories. Political Psychology, 15(4), 731–742. 10.2307/3791630 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  60. Grossmann, I., Huynh, A. C., & Ellsworth, P. C. (2016). Emotional complexity: Clarifying definitions and cultural correlates. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 111(6), 895–916. 10.1037/pspp0000084 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Grossmann, I., Karasawa, M., Kan, C., & Kitayama, S. (2014). A cultural perspective on emotional experiences across the life span. Emotion (Washington, D.C.), 14(4), 679–692. 10.1037/a0036041 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Grzesiak-Feldman, M. (2007). Conspiracy thinking and state-trait anxiety in young polish adults. Psychological Reports, 100(1), 199–202. 10.2466/pr0.100.1.199-202 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Grzesiak-Feldman, M. (2013). The effect of high-anxiety situations on conspiracy thinking. Current Psychology, 32(1), 100–118. 10.1007/s12144-013-9165-6 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  64. Haidt, J., Rozin, P., McCauley, C., & Imada, S. (1997). Body, psyche, and culture: The relationship between disgust and morality. Psychology and Developing Societies, 9(1), 107–131. 10.1177/097133369700900105 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  65. Harmon-Jones, E., Szymaniak, K., Edgeworth, D., Sebban, G., & Harmon-Jones, C. (2024). Evil perceptions but not entertainment value appraisals relate to conspiracy beliefs. Frontiers in Social Psychology, 2. 10.3389/frsps.2024.1350584 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  66. Hastings, S. E., & O’Neill, T. A. (2009). Predicting workplace deviance using broad versus narrow personality variables. Personality and Individual Differences, 47(4), 289–293. 10.1016/j.paid.2009.03.015 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  67. Hemenover, S. H., & Dienstbier, R. A. (1996). The effects of an appraisal manipulation: Affect, intrusive cognitions, and performance for two cognitive tasks. Motivation and Emotion, 20(4), 319–340. 10.1007/BF02856521 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  68. Hofstede, G. (2001). Culture’s consequences: Comparing values, behaviors, institutions and organizations across nations. SAGE Publications. [Google Scholar]
  69. Hofstede, G., Hofstede, G. J., & Minkov, M. (2005). Cultures and organizations: Software of the mind (Vol. 2). Mcgraw-hill. [Google Scholar]
  70. Hornsey, M. J., Bierwiaczonek, K., Sassenberg, K., & Douglas, K. M. (2023). Individual, intergroup and nation-level influences on belief in conspiracy theories. Nature Reviews Psychology, 2(2), 85–97. 10.1038/s44159-022-00133-0 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Hornsey, M. J., Harris, E. A., & Fielding, K. S. (2018). The psychological roots of anti-vaccination attitudes: A 24-nation investigation. Health Psychology: official Journal of the Division of Health Psychology, American Psychological Association, 37(4), 307–315. 10.1037/hea0000586 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Hornsey, M. J., & Pearson, S. (2022). Cross-national differences in willingness to believe conspiracy theories. Current Opinion in Psychology, 47, 101391. 10.1016/j.copsyc.2022.101391 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. House, R. J., Hanges, P. J., Javidan, M., Dorfman, P. W., & Gupta, V. (2004). Culture, leadership, and organizations: The GLOBE study of 62 societies. Sage Publications. [Google Scholar]
  74. Imhoff, R. (2022). Conspiracy theories through a cross-cultural lens. Online Readings in Psychology and Culture, 5(3). 10.9707/2307-0919.1175 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  75. Imhoff, R., Bertlich, T., & Frenken, M. (2022). Tearing apart the “evil” twins: A general conspiracy mentality is not the same as specific conspiracy beliefs. Current Opinion in Psychology, 46, 101349. 10.1016/j.copsyc.2022.101349 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Imhoff, R., & Bruder, M. (2014). Speaking (un-)truth to power: Conspiracy mentality as a generalised political attitude. European Journal of Personality, 28(1), 25–43. 10.1002/per.1930 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  77. Imhoff, R., Dieterle, L., & Lamberty, P. (2021). Resolving the puzzle of conspiracy worldview and political activism: Belief in secret plots decreases normative but increases nonnormative political engagement. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 12(1), 71–79. 10.1177/1948550619896491 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  78. Imhoff, R., & Lamberty, P. (2020). A bioweapon or a hoax? The link between distinct conspiracy beliefs about the coronavirus disease (COVID-19) outbreak and pandemic behavior. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 11(8), 1110–1118. 10.1177/1948550620934692 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Imhoff, R., & Lamberty, P. K. (2017). Too special to be duped: Need for uniqueness motivates conspiracy beliefs. European Journal of Social Psychology, 47(6), 724–734. 10.1002/ejsp.2265 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  80. Imhoff, R., Lamberty, P., & Klein, O. (2018). Using power as a negative cue: How conspiracy mentality affects epistemic trust in sources of historical knowledge. Personality & Social Psychology Bulletin, 44(9), 1364–1379. 10.1177/0146167218768779 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. Imhoff, R., Zimmer, F., Klein, O., António, J. H. C., Babinska, M., Bangerter, A., Bilewicz, M., Blanuša, N., Bovan, K., Bužarovska, R., Cichocka, A., Delouvée, S., Douglas, K. M., Dyrendal, A., Etienne, T., Gjoneska, B., Graf, S., Gualda, E., Hirschberger, G., … van Prooijen, J.-W. (2022). Conspiracy mentality and political orientation across 26 countries. Nature Human Behaviour, 6(3), 392–403. 10.1038/s41562-021-01258-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  82. Izard, C. E. (2007). Basic emotions, natural kinds, emotion schemas, and a new paradigm. Perspectives on Psychological Science: a Journal of the Association for Psychological Science, 2(3), 260–280. 10.1111/j.1745-6916.2007.00044.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  83. Jedinger, A., Masch, L., & Burger, A. M. (2023). Cognitive reflection and endorsement of the “great replacement” conspiracy theory. Social Psychological Bulletin, 18, 1–12. 10.32872/spb.10825 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  84. Jolley, D., & Douglas, K. M. (2014a). The effects of anti-vaccine conspiracy theories on vaccination intentions. PloS One, 9(2), e89177. 10.1371/journal.pone.0089177 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  85. Jolley, D., & Douglas, K. M. (2014b). The social consequences of conspiracism: Exposure to conspiracy theories decreases intentions to engage in politics and to reduce one’s carbon footprint. British Journal of Psychology (London, England: 1953), 105(1), 35–56. 10.1111/bjop.12018 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  86. Jolley, D., Douglas, K. M., Leite, A. C., & Schrader, T. (2019). Belief in conspiracy theories and intentions to engage in everyday crime. British Journal of Social Psychology, 58(3), 534–549. 10.1111/bjso.12311 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  87. Jolley, D., Douglas, K. M., & Sutton, R. M. (2018). Blaming a few bad apples to save a threatened barrel: The system-justifying function of conspiracy theories. Political Psychology, 39(2), 465–478. 10.1111/pops.12404 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  88. Jolley, D., & Paterson, J. L. (2020). Pylons ablaze: Examining the role of 5G COVID-19 conspiracy beliefs and support for violence. The British Journal of Social Psychology, 59(3), 628–640. 10.1111/bjso.12394 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  89. Jovančević, A., & Milićević, N. (2020). Optimism-pessimism, conspiracy theories and general trust as factors contributing to COVID-19 related behavior – A cross-cultural study. Personality and Individual Differences, 167, 110216. 10.1016/j.paid.2020.110216 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  90. Keltner, D., & Haidt, J. (1999). Social functions of emotions at four levels of analysis. Cognition & Emotion, 13(5), 505–521. 10.1080/026999399379168 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  91. Kohut, A., Wike, R., Horowitz, J. M., Poushter, J., Barker, C. (2011). Muslim-Western tensions persist. Pew Research Center. https://www.pewresearch.org/wp-content/uploads/sites/2/2011/07/Pew-Global-Attitudes-Muslim-Western-Relations-FINAL-FOR-PRINT-July-21-2011.pdf
  92. Kreibig, S. D. (2010). Autonomic nervous system activity in emotion: A review. Biological Psychology, 84(3), 394–421. 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2010.03.010 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  93. Kreibig, S. D., Gendolla, G. H. E., & Scherer, K. R. (2010). Psychophysiological effects of emotional responding to goal attainment. Biological Psychology, 84(3), 474–487. 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2009.11.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  94. Kuppens, P., Van Mechelen, I., & Rijmen, F. (2008). Toward disentangling sources of individual differences in appraisal and anger. Journal of Personality, 76(4), 969–1000. 10.1111/j.1467-6494.2008.00511.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  95. Lanctôt, N., & Hess, U. (2007). The timing of appraisals. Emotion (Washington, D.C.), 7(1), 207–212. 10.1037/1528-3542.7.1.207 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  96. Lange, J., & Boecker, L. (2019). Schadenfreude as social-functional dominance regulator. Emotion (Washington, D.C.), 19(3), 489–502. 10.1037/emo0000454 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  97. Lantian, A., Muller, D., Nurra, C., & Douglas, K. M. (2017). “I know things they don’t know!” The role of need for uniqueness in belief in conspiracy theories. Social Psychology, 48(3), 160–173. 10.1027/1864-9335/a000306 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  98. Lazarus, R. S. (1982). Thoughts on the relations between emotion and cognition. American Psychologist, 37(9), 1019–1024. 10.1037/0003-066X.37.9.1019 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  99. Lazarus, R. S. (1991). Emotion and adaptation. Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
  100. Lazarus, R. S., Kanner, A. D., & Folkman, S. (1980). Chapter 8 - Emotions: A cognitive–phenomenological analysis. In Plutchik R. & Kellerman H. (Eds.), Theories of emotion (pp. 189–217). Academic Press. 10.1016/B978-0-12-558701-3.50014-4 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  101. Leach, C. W. (2008). Envy, inferiority, and injustice: Three bases of anger about inequality. In Smith R. (Ed.), Envy: Theory and Research (p. 0). Oxford University Press. 10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195327953.003.0006 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  102. Leach, C. W., & Spears, R. (2008). “A vengefulness of the impotent”: The pain of in-group inferiority and schadenfreude toward successful out-groups. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 95(6), 1383–1396. 10.1037/a0012629 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  103. Leach, C. W., Spears, R., Branscombe, N. R., & Doosje, B. (2003). Malicious pleasure: schadenfreude at the suffering of another group. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84(5), 932–943. 10.1037/0022-3514.84.5.932 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  104. Leibovitz, T., Shamblaw, A. L., Rumas, R., & Best, M. W. (2021). COVID-19 conspiracy beliefs: Relations with anxiety, quality of life, and schemas. Personality and Individual Differences, 175, 110704. 10.1016/j.paid.2021.110704 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  105. Leman, P. J., & Cinnirella, M. (2007). A major event has a major cause: Evidence for the role of heuristics in reasoning about conspiracy theories. Social Psychological Review, 9(2), 18–28. 10.53841/bpsspr.2007.9.2.18 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  106. Lench, H. C., Flores, S. A., & Bench, S. W. (2011). Discrete emotions predict changes in cognition, judgment, experience, behavior, and physiology: A meta-analysis of experimental emotion elicitations. Psychological Bulletin, 137(5), 834–855. 10.1037/a0024244 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  107. Lerner, J. S., & Keltner, D. (2000). Beyond valence: Toward a model of emotion-specific influences on judgement and choice. Cognition & Emotion, 14(4), 473–493. 10.1080/026999300402763 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  108. Lerner, J. S., & Keltner, D. (2001). Fear, anger, and risk. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81(1), 146–159. 10.1037/0022-3514.81.1.146 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  109. Levy, A. (2020). Understanding international Schadenfreude: The pains of Europe as a protective shield of the Israeli self-perception. Journal of International Relations and Development, 23(4), 996–1025. 10.1057/s41268-019-00179-3 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  110. Lewandowsky, S. (2018). In Whose Hands the Future?. In Uscinski J. E. (Ed.), Conspiracy Theories and the people who believe them (p. 0). Oxford University Press. 10.1093/oso/9780190844073.003.0010 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  111. Lewandowsky, S., Gignac, G. E., & Oberauer, K. (2015). Erratum: The role of conspiracist ideation and worldviews in predicting rejection of science. PloS One, 10(8), e0134773. 10.1371/journal.pone.0134773 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  112. Liekefett, L., Christ, O., & Becker, J. C. (2023). Can conspiracy beliefs be beneficial? Longitudinal linkages between conspiracy beliefs, anxiety, uncertainty aversion, and existential threat. Personality & Social Psychology Bulletin, 49(2), 167–179. 10.1177/01461672211060965 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  113. Liu, S. S., Morris, M., Talhelm, T., & Yang, Q. (2019). Ingroup vigilance in collectivistic cultures. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 116(29), 14538–14546. 10.1073/pnas.1817588116 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  114. Mackie, D. M., Devos, T., & Smith, E. R. (2000). Intergroup emotions: Explaining offensive action tendencies in an intergroup context. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79(4), 602–616. 10.1037/0022-3514.79.4.602 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  115. Manstead, A. S., & Fischer, A. H. (2001). Social appraisal: The social world as object of and influence on appraisal processes. In Appraisal Processes in Emotion: Theory, Methods, Research (pp. 221–232). [Google Scholar]
  116. Mao, J.-Y., Zeng, Z.-X., Yang, S.-L., Guo, Y.-Y., & van Prooijen, J.-W. (2024). Explaining the paradox of conspiracy theories and system-justifying beliefs from an intergroup perspective. Political Psychology, 45(2), 299–318. 10.1111/pops.12924 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  117. Marchlewska, M., Cichocka, A., Łozowski, F., Górska, P., & Winiewski, M. (2019). In search of an imaginary enemy: Catholic collective narcissism and the endorsement of gender conspiracy beliefs. The Journal of Social Psychology, 159(6), 766–779. 10.1080/00224545.2019.1586637 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  118. Matsumoto, D., Kudoh, T., Scherer, K., & Wallbott, H. (1988). Antecedents of and reactions to emotions in the United States and Japan. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 19(3), 267–286. 10.1177/0022022188193001 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  119. Mauro, R., Sato, K., & Tucker, J. (1992). The role of appraisal in human emotions: A cross-cultural study. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 62(2), 301–317. 10.1037/0022-3514.62.2.301 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  120. McCauley, C., & Jacques, S. (1979). The popularity of conspiracy theories of presidential assassination: A Bayesian analysis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37(5), 637–644. 10.1037/0022-3514.37.5.637 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  121. McNamee, M. (2003). Schadenfreude in sport: Envy, justice, and self-esteem. Journal of the Philosophy of Sport, 30(1), 1–16. 10.1080/00948705.2003.9714556 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  122. Mercadante, E., Witkower, Z., & Tracy, J. L. (2021). The psychological structure, social consequences, function, and expression of pride experiences. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 39, 130–135. 10.1016/j.cobeha.2021.03.010 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  123. Meuer, M., Oeberst, A., & Imhoff, R. (2023). How do conspiratorial explanations differ from non‐conspiratorial explanations? A content analysis of real‐world online articles. European Journal of Social Psychology, 53(2), 288–306. 10.1002/ejsp.2903 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  124. Michie, S. (2009). Pride and gratitude: How positive emotions influence the prosocial behaviors of organizational leaders. Journal of Leadership & Organizational Studies, 15(4), 393–403. 10.1177/1548051809333338 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  125. Miller, J. M., Saunders, K. L., & Farhart, C. E. (2016). Conspiracy endorsement as motivated reasoning: The moderating roles of political knowledge and trust. American Journal of Political Science, 60(4), 824–844. 10.1111/ajps.12234 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  126. Molenda, Z., Marchlewska, M., & Rogoza, M. (2023). Nothing hurts like (in-group) love? National narcissism, conspiracy intentions, and non-prosocial managing emotions of others. Personality and Individual Differences, 201, 111947. 10.1016/j.paid.2022.111947 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  127. Molho, C., Tybur, J. M., Güler, E., Balliet, D., & Hofmann, W. (2017). Disgust and anger relate to different aggressive responses to moral violations. Psychological Science, 28(5), 609–619. 10.1177/0956797617692000 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  128. Moon, C., & Travaglino, G. A. (2021). Examining conspiracy beliefs and COVID-19 in four countries: The role of disgust towards the political system and implications for prosocial behavior. Journal of Pacific Rim Psychology, 15, 1–11. 10.1177/18344909211056855 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  129. Moors, A., Ellsworth, P. C., Scherer, K. R., & Frijda, N. H. (2013). Appraisal theories of emotion: State of the art and future development. Emotion Review, 5(2), 119–124. 10.1177/1754073912468165 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  130. Morris, M. W., & Peng, K. (1994). Culture and cause: American and Chinese attributions for social and physical events. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67(6), 949–971. 10.1037/0022-3514.67.6.949 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  131. Mus, M., Bor, A., & Petersen, M. B. (2022). Do conspiracy theories efficiently signal coalition membership? An experimental test using the “who said what?” design. PloS One, 17(3), e0265211. 10.1371/journal.pone.0265211 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  132. Nera, K. (2024). Analyzing the causation between conspiracy mentality and belief in conspiracy theories. Zeitschrift Für Psychologie, 232(1), 44–49. 10.1027/2151-2604/a000533 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  133. Nera, K., Jetten, J., Biddlestone, M., & Klein, O. (2022). ‘Who wants to silence us’? Perceived discrimination of conspiracy theory believers increases ‘conspiracy theorist’ identification when it comes from powerholders – But not from the general public. The British Journal of Social Psychology, 61(4), 1263–1285. 10.1111/bjso.12536 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  134. Nera, K., & Schöpfer, C. (2023). What is so special about conspiracy theories? Conceptually distinguishing beliefs in conspiracy theories from conspiracy beliefs in psychological research. Theory & Psychology, 33(3), 287–305. 10.1177/09593543231155891 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  135. Neumann, R. (2000). The causal influences of attributions on emotions: A procedural priming approach. Psychological Science, 11(3), 179–182. 10.1111/1467-9280.00238 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  136. Noordewier, M. K., Topolinski, S., & Van Dijk, E. (2016). The temporal dynamics of surprise. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 10(3), 136–149. 10.1111/spc3.12242 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  137. Nyhan, B., & Zeitzoff, T. (2018). Conspiracy and misperception belief in the Middle East and North Africa. The Journal of Politics, 80(4), 1400–1404. 10.1086/698663 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  138. Oishi, S., Diener, E., Napa Scollon, C., & Biswas-Diener, R. (2004). Cross-situational consistency of affective experiences across cultures. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 86(3), 460–472. 10.1037/0022-3514.86.3.460 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  139. Oleksy, T., Wnuk, A., Maison, D., & Łyś, A. (2021). Content matters. Different predictors and social consequences of general and government-related conspiracy theories on COVID-19. Personality and Individual Differences, 168, 110289. 10.1016/j.paid.2020.110289 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  140. Oliver, J. E., & Wood, T. J. (2014). Conspiracy theories and the paranoid style(s) of mass opinion. American Journal of Political Science, 58(4), 952–966. 10.1111/ajps.12084 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  141. Ortony, A., Clore, G. L., & Collins, A. (1988). The cognitive structure of emotions. Cambridge University Press. 10.1017/CBO9780511571299 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  142. Pantazi, M., Papaioannou, K., & Van Prooijen, J. (2022). Power to the People: The Hidden Link Between Support for Direct Democracy and Belief in Conspiracy Theories. Political Psychology, 43(3), 529–548. 10.1111/pops.12779 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  143. Parramore, L. S. (2021). Like QAnon’s Capitol rioters, the Nashville bomber’s lizard people theory is deadly serious. In NBC News. https://www.nbcnews.com/think/opinion/qanon-s-capitol-rioters-nashville-bomber-s-lizard-people-theory-ncna1253819
  144. Peitz, L., Lalot, F., Douglas, K., Sutton, R., & Abrams, D. (2021). COVID-19 conspiracy theories and compliance with governmental restrictions: The mediating roles of anger, anxiety, and hope. Journal of Pacific Rim Psychology, 15, 1–13. 10.1177/18344909211046646 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  145. Pizarro, D., Inbar, Y., & Helion, C. (2011). On disgust and moral judgment. Emotion Review, 3(3), 267–268. 10.1177/1754073911402394 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  146. Popper, K. R. (1977). The logic of scientific discovery. Systematic Zoology, 26(3), 361. 10.2307/2412687 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  147. Power, T. G., & Hill, L. G. (2010). Individual differences in appraisal of minor, potentially stressful events: A cluster analytic approach. Cognition & Emotion, 24(7), 1081–1094. 10.1080/02699930903122463 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  148. Pummerer, L. (2024). On different operationalizations and conceptualizations of the tendency to believe in conspiracy theories. Zeitschrift Für Psychologie, 232(1), 55–58. 10.1027/2151-2604/a000547 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  149. Pummerer, L., Böhm, R., Lilleholt, L., Winter, K., Zettler, I., & Sassenberg, K. (2022). Conspiracy theories and their societal effects during the COVID-19 pandemic. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 13(1), 49–59. 10.1177/19485506211000217 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  150. Pummerer, L., Ditrich, L., Winter, K., & Sassenberg, K. (2023). Think about it! Deliberation reduces the negative relation between conspiracy belief and adherence to prosocial norms. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 14(8), 952–963. 10.1177/19485506221144150 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  151. Quigley, B. M., & Tedeschi, J. T. (1996). Mediating effects of blame attributions on feelings of anger. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 22(12), 1280–1288. 10.1177/01461672962212008 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  152. Rabasa, A. M. (2004). Effect of September 11 and the War on Terrorism. In The Muslim World after 9/11. (pp. 50–52) RAND Corporation. 10.2307/20034326 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  153. Ren, Z., Schweitzer, M., & Dimant, E. (2023). Beyond belief: How social engagement motives influence the spread of conspiracy theories. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 104, 104421. 10.1016/j.jesp.2022.104421 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  154. Ritzenhöfer, L., Brosi, P., & Welpe, I. M. (2019). Share your pride: How expressing pride in the self and others heightens the perception of agentic and communal characteristics. Journal of Business and Psychology, 34(6), 847–863. 10.1007/s10869-018-9595-0 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  155. Roseman, I. J. (2013). Appraisal in the emotion system: Coherence in strategies for coping. Emotion Review, 5(2), 141–149. 10.1177/1754073912469591 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  156. Roseman, I. J., & Steele, A. K. (2018). Concluding commentary: Schadenfreude, gluckschmerz, jealousy, and hate—what (and when, and why) are the emotions? Emotion Review, 10(4), 327–340. 10.1177/1754073918798089 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  157. Roseman, I. J., Wiest, C., & Swartz, T. S. (1994). Phenomenology, behaviors, and goals differentiate discrete emotions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67(2), 206–221. 10.1037/0022-3514.67.2.206 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  158. Rozin, P., & Fallon, A. E. (1987). A perspective on disgust. Psychological Review, 94(1), 23–41. 10.1037/0033-295X.94.1.23 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  159. Rozin, P., Lowery, L., Imada, S., & Haidt, J. (1999). The CAD triad hypothesis: A mapping between three moral emotions (contempt, anger, disgust) and three moral codes (community, autonomy, divinity). Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76(4), 574–586. 10.1037/0022-3514.76.4.574 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  160. Russell, P. S., & Giner-Sorolla, R. (2011). Moral anger, but not moral disgust, responds to intentionality. Emotion (Washington, D.C.), 11(2), 233–240. 10.1037/a0022598 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  161. Sallam, M., Dababseh, D., Yaseen, A., Al-Haidar, A., Ababneh, N. A., Bakri, F. G., & Mahafzah, A. (2020). Conspiracy beliefs are associated with lower knowledge and higher anxiety levels regarding COVID-19 among students at the University of Jordan. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 17(14), 4915. 10.3390/ijerph17144915 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  162. Sapountzis, A., & Condor, S. (2013). Conspiracy accounts as intergroup theories: Challenging dominant understandings of social power and political legitimacy. Political Psychology, 34(5), 731–752. 10.1111/pops.12015 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  163. Scheepers, D., Spears, R., Doosje, B., & Manstead, A. S. R. (2003). Two functions of verbal intergroup discrimination: Identity and instrumental motives as a result of group identification and threat. Personality & Social Psychology Bulletin, 29(5), 568–577. 10.1177/0146167203251531 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  164. Scherer, K. R. (1984). On the nature and function of emotion: A component process approach. In Approaches to emotion. Psychology Press. [Google Scholar]
  165. Scherer, K. R. (1997). The role of culture in emotion-antecedent appraisal. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73(5), 902–922. 10.1037/0022-3514.73.5.902 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  166. Scherer, K. R. (1999). Appraisal theory. In Dalgleish T. & Power M. J. (Eds.), Handbook of cognition and emotion (1st ed., pp. 637–663). Wiley. 10.1002/0470013494.ch30 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  167. Scherer, K. R. (2009). The dynamic architecture of emotion: Evidence for the component process model. Cognition and Emotion, 23(7), 1307–1351. 10.1080/02699930902928969 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  168. Scherer, K. R. (2013). The nature and dynamics of relevance and valence appraisals: Theoretical advances and recent evidence. Emotion Review, 5(2), 150–162. 10.1177/1754073912468166 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  169. Scherer, K. R. (2019). Studying appraisal-driven emotion processes: Taking stock and moving to the future. Cognition & Emotion, 33(1), 31–40. 10.1080/02699931.2018.1510380 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  170. Scherer, K. R., & Brosch, T. (2009). Culture-specific appraisal biases contribute to emotion dispositions. European Journal of Personality, 23(3), 265–288. 10.1002/per.714 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  171. Scherer, K. R., & Wallbott, H. G. (1994). Evidence for universality and cultural variation of differential emotion response patterning. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 66(2), 310–328. 10.1037/0022-3514.66.2.310 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  172. Schnall, S. (2017). Disgust as embodied loss aversion. European Review of Social Psychology, 28(1), 50–94. 10.1080/10463283.2016.1259844 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  173. Schnall, S., Haidt, J., Clore, G. L., & Jordan, A. H. (2008). Disgust as embodied moral judgment. Personality & Social Psychology Bulletin, 34(8), 1096–1109. 10.1177/0146167208317771 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  174. Siegel, E. H., Sands, M. K., Van den Noortgate, W., Condon, P., Chang, Y., Dy, J., Quigley, K. S., & Barrett, L. F. (2018). Emotion fingerprints or emotion populations? A meta-analytic investigation of autonomic features of emotion categories. Psychological Bulletin, 144(4), 343–393. 10.1037/bul0000128 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  175. Smith, P. B., & Bond, M. H. (2020). Cultural dimensions theory. In Zeigler-Hill V. & Shackelford T. K. (Eds.), Encyclopedia of personality and individual differences (pp. 970–976) Springer International Publishing. 10.1007/978-3-319-24612-3_2093 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  176. Smith, C. A., & Ellsworth, P. C. (1985). Patterns of cognitive appraisal in emotion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 48(4), 813–838. 10.1037/0022-3514.48.4.813 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  177. Smith, C. A., & Lazarus, R. S. (1993). Appraisal components, core relational themes, and the emotions. Cognition & Emotion, 7(3-4), 233–269. 10.1080/02699939308409189 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  178. Smith, R. H., Powell, C. A. J., Combs, D. J. Y., & Schurtz, D. R. (2009). Exploring the when and why of schadenfreude. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 3(4), 530–546. 10.1111/j.1751-9004.2009.00181.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  179. Spears, R., & Leach, C. W. (2004). Intergroup schadenfreude: Conditions and consequences. In Tiedens L. Z. & Leach C. W. (Eds.), The social life of emotions (1st ed., pp. 336–356). Cambridge University Press. 10.1017/CBO9780511819568.018 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  180. Šrol, J., Čavojová, V., & Ballová Mikušková, E. (2021). Finding someone to blame: The link between COVID-19 conspiracy beliefs, prejudice, support for violence, and other negative social outcomes. Frontiers in Psychology, 12, 726076. 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.726076 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  181. Sternisko, A., Cichocka, A., Cislak, A., & Van Bavel, J. J. (2023). National narcissism predicts the belief in and the dissemination of conspiracy theories during the COVID-19 pandemic: evidence from 56 countries. Personality & Social Psychology Bulletin, 49(1), 48–65. 10.1177/01461672211054947 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  182. Stöber, J., & Muijs, M. (2001). Patterns of situational appraisal in experiences of worry and anxiety. Anxiety, Stress, & Coping, 14(4), 367–390. 10.1080/10615800108248362 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  183. Suh, E. M. (2002). Culture, identity consistency, and subjective well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83(6), 1378–1391. 10.1037/0022-3514.83.6.1378 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  184. Sutton, R. M., & Douglas, K. M. (2020). Conspiracy theories and the conspiracy mindset: Implications for political ideology. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 34, 118–122. 10.1016/j.cobeha.2020.02.015 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  185. Sutton, R. M., Douglas, K. M., & Trella, C. (2024). Conspiracy mentality versus belief in conspiracy theories. Zeitschrift Für Psychologie, 232(1), 50–54. 10.1027/2151-2604/a000549 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  186. Swami, V., Furnham, A., Smyth, N., Weis, L., Lay, A., & Clow, A. (2016). Putting the stress on conspiracy theories: Examining associations between psychological stress, anxiety, and belief in conspiracy theories. Personality and Individual Differences, 99, 72–76. 10.1016/j.paid.2016.04.084 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  187. Tiedens, L. Z., & Linton, S. (2001). Judgment under emotional certainty and uncertainty. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81(6), 973–988. 10.1037/0022-3514.81.6.973 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  188. Tong, E. M. W. (2015). Differentiation of 13 positive emotions by appraisals. Cognition & Emotion, 29(3), 484–503. 10.1080/02699931.2014.922056 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  189. Tracy, J. L., & Robins, R. W. (2004a). Putting the self into self-conscious emotions: A theoretical model. Psychological Inquiry, 15(2), 103–125. 10.1207/s15327965pli1502_01 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  190. Tracy, J. L., & Robins, R. W. (2004b). Show your pride: Evidence for a discrete emotion expression. Psychological Science, 15(3), 194–197. 10.1111/j.0956-7976.2004.01503008.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  191. Tracy, J. L., & Robins, R. W. (2007). The psychological structure of pride: A tale of two facets. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 92(3), 506–525. 10.1037/0022-3514.92.3.506 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  192. Trella, C., Sutton, R. M., & Douglas, K. M. (2024). Semantic and causal relations between the conspiracy mentality and belief in conspiracy theories. Zeitschrift Für Psychologie, 232(1), 7–17. 10.1027/2151-2604/a000545 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  193. Triandis, H. C. (2001). Individualism-collectivism and personality. Journal of Personality, 69(6), 907–924. 10.1111/1467-6494.696169 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  194. Van de Cruys, S., Bervoets, J., Gadsby, S., Gijbels, D., & Poels, K. (2023). Insight in the conspiracist ‘ s mind, 1–66. 10.31234/osf.io/6ef9u [DOI] [PubMed]
  195. van der Linden, S. (2015). The conspiracy-effect: Exposure to conspiracy theories (about global warming) decreases pro-social behavior and science acceptance. Personality and Individual Differences, 87, 171–173. 10.1016/j.paid.2015.07.045 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  196. Van Dijk, W. W., Ouwerkerk, J. W., Wesseling, Y. M., & Van Koningsbruggen, G. M. (2011). Towards understanding pleasure at the misfortunes of others: The impact of self-evaluation threat on schadenfreude. Cognition & Emotion, 25(2), 360–368. 10.1080/02699931.2010.487365 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  197. van Prooijen, J.-W. (2016). Sometimes inclusion breeds suspicion: Self-uncertainty and belongingness predict belief in conspiracy theories. European Journal of Social Psychology, 46(3), 267–279. 10.1002/ejsp.2157 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  198. van Prooijen, J.-W. (2020). An existential threat model of conspiracy theories. European Psychologist, 25(1), 16–25. 10.1027/1016-9040/a000381 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  199. van Prooijen, J.-W. (2022a). Injustice Without Evidence: The Unique Role of Conspiracy Theories in Social Justice Research. Social Justice Research, 35(1), 88–106. 10.1007/s11211-021-00376-x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  200. van Prooijen, J.-W. (2022b). Psychological benefits of believing conspiracy theories. Current Opinion in Psychology, 47, 101352. 10.1016/j.copsyc.2022.101352 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  201. van Prooijen, J.-W. (2024). Group-oriented motivations underlying conspiracy theories. Group Processes & Intergroup Relations, 27 (5), 1050–1067. 10.1177/13684302241240696 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  202. van Prooijen, J., Amodio, D. M., Boot, A., Eerland, A., Etienne, T., Krouwel, A. P. M., Onderco, M., Verkoeijen, P., & Zwaan, R. A. (2023). A longitudinal analysis of conspiracy beliefs and Covid-19 health responses. Psychological Medicine, 53(12), 5709–5716. 10.1017/S0033291722002938 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  203. van Prooijen, J.-W., Ligthart, J., Rosema, S., & Xu, Y. (2022). The entertainment value of conspiracy theories. British Journal of Psychology (London, England: 1953), 113(1), 25–48. 10.1111/bjop.12522 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  204. van Prooijen, J.-W., & van Lange, P. A. M. (2014). The Social Dimension of Belief in Conspiracy Theories. In Power, politics, and paranoia: Why people are suspicious of their leaders. (pp. 237–253). [Google Scholar]
  205. van Reekum, C. M., & Scherer, K. R. (1997). Levels of processing in emotion-antecedent appraisal. In Matthews G. (Ed.), Advances in psychology (Vol. 124, pp. 259–300). North-Holland. 10.1016/S0166-4115(97)80123-9 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  206. Veenstra, L., Bushman, B. J., & Koole, S. L. (2018). The facts on the furious: A brief review of the psychology of trait anger. Current Opinion in Psychology, 19, 98–103. 10.1016/j.copsyc.2017.03.014 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  207. Wagner-Egger, P., & Bangerter, A. (2007). The Truth Lies Elsewhere: Correlates of Belief in Conspiracy Theories. Revue Internationale de Psychologie Sociale, 20(4), 31–61. [Google Scholar]
  208. Wenzel, A., & Lystad, C. (2005). Interpretation biases in angry and anxious individuals. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 43(8), 1045–1054. 10.1016/j.brat.2004.02.009 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  209. West, H. G., & Sanders, T. (2003). Transparency and conspiracy: Ethnographies of suspicion in the new world order. Duke University Press. [Google Scholar]
  210. Winter, K., Hornsey, M. J., Pummerer, L., & Sassenberg, K. (2022). Anticipating and defusing the role of conspiracy beliefs in shaping opposition to wind farms. Nature Energy, 7(12), 1200–1207. 10.1038/s41560-022-01164-w [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  211. Winter, K., Hornsey, M. J., Pummerer, L., & Sassenberg, K. (2024). Public agreement with misinformation about wind farms. Nature Communications, 15(1), 8888. 10.1038/s41467-024-53278-2 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  212. Winter, K., Pummerer, L., & Sassenberg, K. (2023). Change by (almost) all means: The role of conspiracy mentality in predicting support for social change among the political left and right. European Journal of Social Psychology, 53(7), 1563–1575. 10.1002/ejsp.2995 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  213. Wood, M. J. (2017). Conspiracy suspicions as a proxy for beliefs in conspiracy theories: Implications for theory and measurement. British Journal of Psychology (London, England: 1953), 108(3), 507–527. 10.1111/bjop.12231 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  214. YouGov . (2020). Globalism2020 guardian conspiracy theories. https://docs.cdn.yougov.com/msvke1lg9d/Globalism2020GuardianConspiracyTheories.pdf

Articles from Psychological Inquiry are provided here courtesy of Taylor & Francis

RESOURCES