Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2005 Sep 13.
Published in final edited form as: J Psychopharmacol. 2004 Dec;18(4):457–474. doi: 10.1177/0269881104047273

Nicotinic receptor mechanisms and cognition in normal states and neuropsychiatric disorders

Kristi A Sacco 1,, Katie L Bannon 2, Tony P George 3
PMCID: PMC1201375  NIHMSID: NIHMS2471  PMID: 15582913

Abstract

Cigarette smoking rates in the American population are approximately 23%, whereas rates of smoking in clinical and population studies of individuals with neuropsychiatric disorders are typically two- to four-fold higher. Studies conducted in a variety of neuropsychiatric populations [e.g. attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), Alzheimer’s disease, schizophrenia] have collectively suggested that nicotine may be efficacious in remediating selected cognitive deficits associated with these disorders, thus providing a framework for understanding the specific vulnerability of these patients to smoking initiation and maintenance. However, the specific gain in cognitive performance produced by nicotine administration in healthy subjects with normal cognitive function is less clear. This article reviews our current understanding of central nicotinic acetylcholine receptor (nAChRs) systems in normal and neuropsychiatric disease states and, specifically, their role with respect to cognitive dysfunction and clinical symptoms in several specific neuropsychiatric populations, including ADHD, Alzheimer’s disease, Parkinson’s disease, Tourette’s Disorder, schizophrenia and affective disorders. The potential benefits of nicotinic agents for therapeutic use in neuropsychiatric disorders is discussed, as well as directions for further research in this area.

Keywords: attention, cigarette smoking, cognitive function, executive function, nicotinic receptor, neuropsychiatric disorder, working memory

Introduction

It has been long-appreciated that cigarette smokers may derive some gains in memory and attention from smoking, and smokers often endorse psychomotor and cognitive impairment during tobacco abstinence. Nonetheless, experimental support for the procognitive effects of cigarette smoking has been scarce because it has been hard to separate direct gains in cognitive performance from nicotine administration per se from the reversal of nicotine withdrawal effects. This has lead to the proposal that nicotine may ultimately produce no benefits on mood, anxiety and cognition (Heishman, 1999), and may even contribute to their worsening (Parrott, 2003). However, while this may be true in non-pathological states, there is increasing evidence that in a number of neuropsychiatric disorders [e.g. schizophrenia, attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder, Parkinson’s disease, Alzhemier’s disease, affective disorders], individuals typically smoke cigarettes at higher prevalence rates than in the general population (Lasser et al., 2000; George and O’Malley, 2004), and/or that neurocognitive deficits which are well-described clinical features of these disorders may be remediated by nicotine administration or smoking (Newhouse et al., 1997; Levin et al., 1998; Levin, 2002; Shytle et al., 2002). Accordingly, the use of nicotine and nicotinic agents has received considerable attention as a therapeutic strategy in these disorders, and the development of subtype-selective agonists and antagonists for the nicotinic acetylcholine receptor (nAChR) is of considerable academic and commercial interest. Furthermore, a better understanding of how nicotine may benefit neurocognitive dysfunction in particular disorders may have relevance for our understanding of why many of these individuals are vulnerable to tobacco addiction and dependence, and might suggest targeted strategies for smoking cessation in these populations. This review evaluates the evidence obtained from basic science and clinical studies which support the notion that nicotine and nicotinic agents may have beneficial effects on various aspects of neurocognitive and clinical function, particularly in individuals with well-defined cognitive compromise, such as in neuropsychiatric disease states, and makes recommendations for further animal and human research in this area.

Methods for study selection

We systematically surveyed the English-language literature using MEDLINE database searches for the period 1966 to January 2004 for the search terms ‘neuropsychiatric disorder’, ‘psychiatric disorder’, ‘nicotine’, ‘smoking’, ‘cognition’ and ‘neuropsychology’. Proceedings of major conferences in the last 3 years were also surveyed. English-language human studies incorporating an adequate control group were included in the review. Bibliographies of relevant studies were also reviewed for completeness.

For the classification of studies shown in Tables 1 and 2, two authors (K.A.S. and K.L.B.) independently and blindly rated each original article for determination of the strength of evidence of the article. Articles were rated: ‘1’ for ‘strong evidence’ if there was substantiation of results by at least one replication study; ‘2’ for ‘modest evidence’ if there was a single well-controlled study with no replication data or several moderately well controlled studies with supporting findings; and ‘3’ for ‘little evidence’ if the study design was not well controlled and there was no evidence of replication.

Table 1.

Binding affinities (Kd or Ki, μM) of various nicotinic agents to nicotinic acetylcholine receptor (nAChR) subtypes

nAChR Subtypes (Kd or Ki, μM)
Nicotinic agent α4β2 (brain) α3β4 (brain) α1β1 (muscle) α7 (brain)
Acetylcholinea 7–20 34 1100
Nicotinea 4.2 10.2 > 1000 40–83
Mecamylamineb 2.5 0.6 30 6.9
Dihydro-β-erythroidineb 3.5 23.1 20.0
Galanthaminea 10 17 0.9
Epibatidineb 3.0 × 10−5 3.0 × 10−4 2.7 × 10−3 2.1 × 10−2
ABT-594a 3.7–5.5 × 10−5 10.0 1.6
Bupropionb 10.0 1.4 10.5 50
Fluoxetineb 2.2 2.5 2.1 10.7

nAChR, Nicotinic acetylcholine receptor

a

Kd, concentration for half-maximal binding

Table 2.

Summary of evidence of nicotinic modulation of cognition in healthy human subjects: is there evidence for nicotinic enhancement of cognitive function?

Domain Findings Reference
Healthy subjects (smokers and non-smokers) Attention (sustained) Rating = 1 Acute smoking improved response time on RVIP and overall DSST performance in 12 smokers Petrie and Deary (1989)
Smoking deprivation for 24 h increased reaction time, reaction time variability and commission errors on CPT Hatsukami et al. 1989
Attentional measures of vigilance (reaction time, target detection) in the RVIP task were dose-dependently improved by nicotine gum (0, 2 and 4 mg), cigarette smoking Parrott and Craig (1992)
Subcutaneous nicotine (0.0, 0.3 and 0.6 mg) administration in deprived smokers improved correct responses on logical reasoning, vigilance and accuracy on RVIP, and improved word recognition. Positive effects in never smokers were confined to reaction times in RVIP and digit recall Foulds et al. 1996
Transdermal nicotine (21 mg/24 h) compared to placebo improved hit rate on the RVIP task, and this was accompanied by task-induced activation of regions associated with visual arousal, attention and motor activation, including parietal cortex, thalamus, caudate and occipital cortex Lawrence and Ross (2002)
Administration of transdermal nicotine patch significantly improves performance on CPT in non-smoking adults without attentional deficits Levin et al. 1998
Smoking enhanced P300 ERPs under non-deprived conditions, these effects were reduced with 12-h deprivation Pineda et al. 1998
Transdermal nicotine (21 mg) increased electroencephalogram arousal, reduced reaction times and increased P300 ERP amplitudes relative to placebo Knott et al. 1999
Acute and repeated cigarette smoking improves visuospatial expectancies, but not non-spatial expectancies Murphy and Klein (1998)
Attention (selective) Rating = 2 Nicotine administration, or smoking faciltitates classical Stroop Test (1935) performance, whereas abstinence impairs it Wesnes and Revell (1984); Provost and Woodward (1991); Pomerleau et al. 1994
Neither high- or low-nicotine content cigarettes modifies classical Stroop performance; Nicotine gum (0, 2 and 4 mg) and acute smoking or smoking abstinence did not alter Stroop performance Suter et al. 1983; Parrott and Craig (1992); George et al. 2002
Smoking improves smoking-context specific Stroop performance (e.g. reduce Stroop interferenceSI), whereas abstinence impairs smoking-word related Stroop interference Gross et al. 1993; Rusted et al. 2000; Powell et al. (2002)
Transdermal nicotine improved reaction times and interference scores on the classical Stroop, but not other attentional measures Mancuso et al. 1999
Sensorimotor gating (pre-attentional) Rating = 2 Smoking abstinence reduces, and acute smoking increases PPI of the startle response Kumari et al. 1996; Della Casa et al. 1998; Kumari and Gray (1999); Duncan et al. 2001
Acute smoking of high-nicotine content cigarettes (versus nicotine-free) inhibits PPI in deprived smokers Hutchison et al. 2000
Subcutaneous administration improves PPI in non-smokers Kumari et al. 1997
Verbal learning Rating = 3 Increasing doses of mecamylamine yielded more errors on repeated acquisition task in non-smokers Newhouse et al. 1992
Administration of oral cotinine (nicotine’s proximal metabolite) dose-dependently impaired performance on a verbal recall task, while also decreasing N100 ERP latencies and reaction time Herzig et al. 1998
Smoking abstinence slowed responses on a letter search test, while acute smoking reversed letter test performance to baseline Bell et al. 1999
Acute smoking (1–2 cigarettes) had negligible effects on Buschke’s selective reminding task and a letter fluency task in both smokers and non-smokers Sakurai and Kanazawa (2002)
Nonverbal learning Rating = 3 Acute smoking (1–2 cigarettes) had negligible effects on mental arithmetic task in both smokers and non-smokers Sakurai and Kanazawa (2002)
Processing Rating = 1 Smoking deprivation (up to 24 h) increased response latencies (reaction times) on a variety of tasks including attention, recall, working memory and logical reasoning speed Snyder et al. 1989
In non-smokers, nicotine improved reaction times without changes in accuracy on a reaction time task Le Houezec et al. 1994
Nicotine polacrilex (4 mg) showed improvements in reaction time on the two letter search task (visual attention), rather than in accuracy for smokers and non-smokers Ernst et al. 2001a
Low dose transdermal nicotine (7 mg/24 h) maintained choice reaction time performance through increasing overall arousal Davranche and Audiffren (2002)
Working memory Rating = 3 Acute smoking after overnight abstinence impaired spatial working memory performance in smokers Park et al. 2000
Smokers showed deficits in working memory, independent of nicotine gum administration Ernst et al. 2001b
Prolonged smoking abstinence for up to 8 weeks improved spatial working memory performance in non-psychiatric smokers George et al. 2002
Recognition memory improved with higher doses of mecamylamine in non-smokers Newhouse et al. 1992
Executive function Rating = 3 Smoking deprivation for 4 and 24 h led to decrements in Trails B performance, but this was likely related to impairment of psychomotor performance Hatsukami et al. 1989
In smokers, logical reasoning was unchanged by deprivation, but improved once participants resumed smoking Bell et al. 1999

RATINGS: 1 = Strong Evidence; 2 = Modest Evidence; 3 = Little or No Evidence. CPT, Continuous Performance Test; DSST, Digit Symbol Substitution Test; ERP, event-related potential; PPI, pre-pulse inhibition; RVIP, rapid visual information processing.

Definition of cognition

Cognition is broadly defined as the information-handling aspect of behaviour (Lezak, 1995). Several constructs in cognition are often studied as the major focus of neuropsychiatric disorders, and these typically include attention, memory and executive functioning. Attention is defined as the ability to focus behaviour, or the ability to avoid distractibility. An intact attentional system is thought to be fundamental for the successful execution of other higher-order functions (Gitelman, 2003), although impairments in attention are incorporated in the most common deficiencies among most types of brain pathology, including neuropsychiatric disease (Lezak, 1995). Memory is the complex process of registering, storing, retaining and retrieving previously encountered information, many stages of which are disrupted in individuals with brain impairment, and is related to attentional functioning because it requires the ability to attend to new information for adequate learning and recall (Lezak, 1995). Executive functions have been broadly defined as several abilities that incorporate organization and planning and cognitive flexibility, and that help to sustain the processes of attention and memory (Wecker et al., 2000); thus, it is often argued that many of these functions overlap and require the operation of one for optimal performance of another. For example, in the literature concerning schizophrenia, spatial working memory function has been described as a ‘master’ cognitive process that contributes intrinsically to performance on other important domains, such as executive function and attention, which are also known to be abnormal in this disorder (Green, 1996; Green et al., 1997; Goldman-Rakic, 1999).

Nicotinic acetylcholine receptor

The neurotransmitter acetylcholine has been associated with the processes of arousal, learning and memory, and it is well-established that cholinergic agonists are known to improve memory while, in general, cholinergic antagonists impair memory (Bartus et al., 1985). Such a finding linked research of cholinergic neurones to studies of various forms of progressive dementia. There are two major cholinergic systems which contribute to cognition (i.e. muscarinic and nicotinic). Several reviews are available on muscarinic nAChR systems in cognitive function, in both normal and neuropsychiatric disease states (Avery et al., 1997; Terry and Buccafisco, 2003; Zhou et al., 2003). Here, we will focus on nAChR systems.

Pre-clinical studies have demonstrated that nicotine alters the function of several central neurotransmitter systems, including dopamine (DA), norepinephrine (NE), serotonin (5-HT), glutamate, γ-aminobutyric (GABA) and endogenous opioid peptides (Mansvelder and McGehee, 2002; Picciotto, 2003). Nicotine’s receptors in the brain comprise nAChRs, which are diverse members of the neurotransmitter-gated ion channel superfamily, and which play critical neuromodulatory roles in the central nervous system (Picciotto et al., 2000; Picciotto, 2003). The endogenous neurotransmitter for nAChRs is acetylcholine. There are two general families of central nAChRs (Picciotto, 2000): (i) high-affinity (β2 subunit-containing nAChRs, which exist in a heteropentameric configuration of α subunits combined with β subunits) receptors, which are sensitive to the nicotinic antagonists mecamylamine and dihydro-β-erythrodine and (ii) low-affinity (α7 subunit-containing nAChR homopentameric complexes) receptors which are sensitive to the snake venom toxin α-bungarotoxin and the selective antagonist methyllycaconitine. Both high- and low-affinity nAChRs appear to be present on mesocorticolimbic DA neurones (Wooltorton et al., 2003), and α7 nAChRs are enriched in the hippocampus and cortex and appear to facilitate information processing and sensory integration (Leonard and Bertrand, 2001). Stimulation of pre-synaptic nAChRs on these neurones by nicotine increases transmitter release and metabolism. Unlike most agonists which produce receptor downregulation with chronic exposure, chronic nicotine administration leads to desensitization and inactivation of nAChRs, and a ‘paradoxical’ upregulation of nAChR sites, a process that occurs within 7 days of repeated exposure to nicotine (Gentry and Lukas, 2002). Interestingly, it has been shown that normalization of nAChR upregulation with smoking cessation occurs after 4–8 weeks (Breese et al., 1997). Tolerance is another important aspect of the nicotine dependence syndrome but, interestingly, is not thought to be related to this phenomenon of nAChR desensitization and upregulation, which appears to be more closely related to the process of nicotine withdrawal (Littleton, 2001). Several human studies have suggested that although there is consistent evidence to suggest that tolerance to the effects of nicotine on mood and aversive effects occur, there is little or no evidence for the development of tolerance to the effects of nicotine on cardiovascular measures or psychomotor performance (Heishman and Henningfield, 2000; Perkins, 2002); however, lighter smokers who are not dependent (e.g. ‘chippers’) may have less nicotine tolerance (Shiffman et al., 1992). In general, non-smokers and non-dependent smokers appear to experience more positive and aversive effects and more cardiovascular reactivity compared to non-smokers and dependent smokers, respectively (Srivastava et al., 1991; Shiffman et al., 1992; Perkins et al., 1994; Perkins, 2002).

After a period of overnight abstinence, nAChRs become resensitized, and then these nAChRs are thought to be fully responsive to nicotine as an exogenous agonist, which might explain why most smokers report that the most satisfying cigarette of the day is the first one in the morning. Mesolimbic DA (reward pathway) neurones are of particular importance because these neurones project from the ventral tegmental area (VTA) in the midbrain to anterior limbic forebrain structures such as the nucleus accumbens and cingulate cortex, and mediate the rewarding effects of nicotine. These neurones express high affinity nAChRs both on their cell bodies and terminals (Zoli et al., 2002), and receive inputs from glutamatergic and GABAergic neurones that have low and high affinity nAChRs on their terminals, respectively (Mansvelder et al., 2002). Similarly, there are nAChRs present pre-synaptically on midbrain DA neurones that project from the VTA to the prefrontal cortex (PFC) and that evoke DA release and metabolism when pre-synaptic nAChRs are activated by nicotine (Marshall, 1997). Such nAChR regulation may mediate the effects of nicotine and smoking on PFC-dependent cognitive function, including spatial working memory and executive function (Goldman-Rakic, 1999; George et al., 2000d; George et al., 2002; Sacco et al., 2004).

There is evidence that, similar to nicotine, competitive nAChR agonists are more likely to induce receptor desensitization (and upregulation) compared to allosteric modulators of nAChRs (e.g. galanthamine), which appear to be less prone to producing such nAChR desensitization (Maelicke et al., 2001), thereby maximizing nAChR stimulation effects with repeated drug dosing. A summary of the binding affinities of nicotine and various nicotinic agonists and antagonists, including allosteric modulators, is presented in Table 1 (Bertrand et al., 1992; Harvey et al., 1996; Donnelly Roberts et al., 1998; Parker et al., 1998; Fryer and Lukas, 1999a, 1999b; Maelicke et al., 2001; Shytle et al., 2002; Eaton et al., 2003; Samochocki et al., 2003; Zhao et al., 2003).

In addition to nicotine, there are approximately 4000 chemical constituents in tobacco, and some of these have psychopharmacological effects and thus contribute to the nicotine dependence state in humans. For example, an unidentified component of tobacco smoke (not nicotine, but likely to comprise harman and norharman alkaloids, which are known benzodiazepine inverse agonists) (Rommelspacher et al., 2002) inhibits both monoamine oxidase (MAO) A and B subtypes which are responsible for the metabolism of monoamine neurotransmitters such as NE and 5-HT (MAO-A) and DA (MAO-B) (Fowler et al., 1996a,b), and may contribute to the reinforcing properties of tobacco and to smoking-related cognitive enhancement. In addition, carbon monoxide (CO) is a combustion product of tobacco which, being a gaseous second messenger like nitric oxide (NO), known to be involved in neurotransmission (Baranano et al., 2001), could also contribute to the process of tobacco dependence.

Non-human studies of nAChRs and cognition

Several animal models of cognitive performance have been developed in the past 15 years which are considered to be analogues of cognitive processing in human subjects, and which have been used in cross-species validation of the effects of a number of psycho-active drugs, including nicotine (Levin, 1992, Levin and Simon, 1998; Levin, 2002). For example, there is strong evidence that acute and chronic nicotine (or selective nicotinic agonist) administration can enhance working memory (e.g. radial arm maze) (Levin, 1996; Levin, 1998; Levin et al., 1999; Levin, 2002) and attentional function in rodents (Stolerman et al., 2000; Hahn et al., 2003) and, in non-human primates (Schneider et al., 2003), cognitive processes that are known to depend, at least in part, on central DA and NE function. In rats, nicotine administration increases catecholamine (e.g. DA and NE) release and turnover in nigrostriatal, mesolimbic and mesocortical circuits, while acute nicotine abstinence (< 24 h) has been shown to result in decreases in central catecholamine function in both rodents (Vezina et al., 1992; Fung et al., 1996; George et al., 1998; Hildebrand et al., 1998) and humans (West et al., 1984; Ward et al., 1991). Further support of the effects of nicotine on DA-mediated behaviour comes from studies suggesting that nicotine administration enhances long-lasting, reward-related learning in the rat (Olausson et al., 2003), as well as the observation that nicotine improves response accuracy, reduces omission rates and shortens response latency in the 5-choice serial reaction time task (5-CSRTT), a rodent model of attention (Stolerman et al., 2000; Hahn et al., 2003) However, there is accumulating evidence that other neurotransmitters, such as acetylcholine, glutamate and 5-HT (Arnsten, 2000; Robbins, 2000), also contribute to cognitive performance in these animal models.

Non-psychiatric human populations (Table 2)

There is some evidence for nicotinic receptor-mediated cognitive enhancement in non-psychiatric healthy controls. Recent studies have suggested that nicotine itself can alter the function of brain regions thought to contribute to neurocognitive function in humans. In functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) studies, intravenous (i.v.) nicotine has been shown to dose-dependently increase subjective feelings of reward, and this is accompanied by activation of limbic and paralimbic brain areas such as the nucleus accumbens, amygdala, anterior cingulate cortex and prefrontal cortex (Stein et al., 1998). Similarly, the same group most recently demonstrated that transdermal nicotine administration improved task-related activation of the parietal cortex, thalamus and caudate during performance of the rapid visual information processing (RVIP) test, as well as generalized occipital cortical activity in an fMRI study of the effects of nicotine on cognitive mechanisms, indicating that areas that typically mediate visual attention, arousal and motor activation are activated with administration of nicotine (Lawrence and Ross, 2002). In both smokers and non-smokers, nicotine administration decreases regional cerebral blood flow (rCBF) in the anterior cingulate cortex and the cerebellum, and increases rCBF to the occipital cortex, which further illustrates the positive cognitive effects of nicotine on cortical areas mediating mood and attention, and also on the visual cortex (Ghatan et al., 1998). Other recent functional imaging studies have suggested that differences in cognitive activation of neural substrates in smokers and non-smokers using a working memory task (the ‘N-back’ test) may be related to trait factors rather than the direct effects of nicotine or smoking (Ernst, 2001a), and that previous exposure to smoking can alter brain responsiveness to nicotine administration (Ernst, 2001b). Accordingly, these neuroimaging studies illustrate the complexities of determining the effects of nicotine and smoking effects on brain function and complex behaviours such as cognitive performance, and suggest that their may be functional pathology that determines vulnerability to nicotine addiction.

Several studies have suggested that nicotine administration and smoking may improve sustained attentional function on a variety of classical neuropsychological tests of attention, vigilance and accuracy such as the Continuous Perfromance Test (CPT) and RVIP (Petrie and Deary, 1989; Parrott and Craig, 1992; Foulds et al., 1996; Levin et al., 1998; Murphy and Klein, 1998; Lawrence and Ross, 2002), and that smoking abstinence can impair attentional function (Hatsukami et al., 1989; Eisenberg et al., 1996) (Table 2). Similar effects of acute smoking and smoking abstinence (Pineda et al., 1998) and of transdermal nicotine (Knott et al., 1999) have been obtained using non-neuropsychological measures of attention, namely event-related potentials such as the P300 response. Interestingly, acute smoking or nicotine administration have been shown to improve, and abstinence to impair, selective attention as assessed by the classical Stroop Colour Word Test (Wesnes and Revell, 1984; Provost and Woodward, 1991; Pomerleau et al., 1994; Mancuso et al., 1999), but this effect on Stroop performance was not found in all studies (Suter et al., 1983; Parrott and Craig, 1992; George et al., 2002), and differences may be due to different drug administration and testing methodologies involved. Furthermore, in a modified version of the Stroop Test which involves selective attention for smoking-related words, acute smoking improves, whereas abstinence impairs, smoking word-related reaction time responses (e.g. Stroop interference) (Gross et al., 1993; Rusted et al., 2000; Powell et al., 2002).

A classic measure of pre-attention is pre-pulse inhibition (PPI) of the startle response, an operational measure of sensorimotor gating (Swerdlow et al., 1992), and PPI is known to be deficient in a number of neuropsychiatric disorders (Swerdlow et al., 1992). There is strong evidence from pre-clinical studies that PPI is negatively modulated by dopamine (Swerdlow et al., 1992; Ralph-Williams et al., 2002). Nicotine and cigarette smoking effects have been evaluated in smokers and non-smokers without neuropsychiatric illness and, in some cases, smoking abstinence may impair PPI (Della Casa et al., 1999; Duncan et al., 2001), and acute smoking may improve sensorimotor gating (Kumari et al., 1996; Della Casa et al., 1999; Kumari and Gray, 1999; Duncan et al., 2001), while nicotine administration in non-smokers may modestly improve PPI (Kumari et al., 1997). Other studies have shown that acute cigarette smoking inhibits PPI (Hutchison et al., 2000), which is consistent with animal studies suggesting that augmentation of DA function inhibits PPI (Swerdlow et al., 1992). Methodological differences between these studies, such as the degree of nicotine dependence and length of smoking deprivation, may contribute to these discrepant findings.

With respect to the effect of nicotine on psychomotor performance, early studies suggested that smoking abstinence (up to 24 h) can reduce psychomotor performance as assessed by slowing of reaction times (Snyder et al., 1989). Subsequently, studies involving administration of nicotine gum (Ernst et al., 2001a) and a transdermal patch (Davranche and Audiffren, 2002) have shown that these routes of administration can improve reaction times probably via non-specific effects on arousal in both smokers and non-smokers. Furthermore, Le Houzec et al. (1994) demonstrated that subcutaneous nicotine can improve reaction times in non-smokers, without improvements in accuracy or vigilance.

There is little evidence that nicotine administration and smoking per se can improve verbal and non-verbal learning in smokers (independent of deprivation-induced deficits) and non-smokers (Bell et al., 1999; Sakurai and Kanazawa, 2002) and, interestingly, it has been shown that the nAChR antagonist mecamylamine (5–20 mg) dose-dependently worsens verbal learning and recall in non-smokers (Newhouse et al., 1992). However, a study by Min et al. (2001) demonstrated significantly improved learning in both smoking and non-smoking elderly non-demented adults over several trials on a verbal learning and memory task, and significantly improved verbal learning, visual object learning, as well as delayed recall and word retrieval performances, suggesting that these beneficial effects can be seen in otherwise healthy, or perhaps, marginally at-risk, elderly adults. Furthermore, there is some evidence that acute smoking (Park et al., 2000) or a history of smoking (Ernst et al., 2001a) is associated with impairment of working memory; smoking abstinence for up to 8 weeks was associated with improvement of spatial working memory in non-psychiatric smokers (George et al., 2002). Finally, Newhouse et al. (1994) administered single doses of the non-selective high-affinity nAChR antagonist mecamylamine at doses of 0, 5, 10 and 20 mg/day in healthy young controls and elderly subjects in a double-blind, placebo-controlled manner. Their findings suggested that, although at the highest dose (20 mg/day), there was a significant impairment in both groups in the learning condition of the Repeated Acquisition Task, elderly subjects appeared to be more sensitive to mecamylamine effects because there was a significant impairment with the 10 mg/day dose that was not observed in younger subjects. There was a similar enhanced sensitivity to mecamylamine on recognition memory, which was preferentially observed in elderly versus younger subjects. These results suggest an increased sensitivity to nAChR blockade, possibly due to an age-related dysfunction in nAChRs.

With respect to executive functioning, there is some evidence that acute smoking abstinence (up to 24 h) can impair performance on Trails B (Hatsukami et al., 1989), but these effects are probably non-specific and related to decrements in psychomotor performance. Furthermore, performance on a logical reasoning task was unchanged by smoking abstinence, but modest performance improvements were observed after acute smoking (Bell et al., 1999).

There has been much discussion in the literature about whether non-psychiatric cigarette smokers derive any beneficial effects on cognitive performance, or whether any cognitive enhancement that is reported is simply due to reversal of tobacco withdrawal induced cognitive decrements (Spilich, 1994; Parrott, 2003). The preponderance of the evidence suggests that, in nicotine dependent smokers, cigarette smoking may simply reverse abstinence-related impairment in cognitive function (Hatsukami, 1989), and nicotine abstinence is known to impair attention and reaction time performance within 24 h after smoking abstinence (Hatsukami et al., 1989; Eissenberg et al., 1996). Interestingly, oral administration of cotinine (the proximal metabolite of nicotine) impaired performance on a verbal recall task and of N100 event-related potential latencies (Herzig et al., 1998), suggesting that tobacco abstinence-related cognitive impairment may be due in part to accumulation of this long-acting metabolite. Further studies of the direct effects of nicotine (and its metabolite cotinine) on cognitive performance in non-smoking (never smoking), healthy human subjects will be required to address these questions, independent of the effects of nicotine withdrawal.

Involvement of nAChR systems in clinical and cognitive function in neuropsychiatric disorders (Table 3)

Table 3.

Summary of evidence for nicotinic modulation of cognition in patients with neuropsychiatric disorders: is there evidence for nicotinic enhancement of cognitive function?

Domain Findings Reference
ADHD Attention Rating = 1 TNP improved CPT reaction time and hit rate variability in adults with ADHD, but more so in smokers than nonsmokers Conners et al. 1996; Levin et al. 1996a, 2001
Clinical outcomes Rating = 1 Nicotine increased self-reported vigor on POMS Conners et al. 1996; Levin et al. 1996a, 2001
TNP improved symptoms as measured by CGI in both smokers and nonsmokers Conners et al. 1996; Levin et al. 1996b
The nicotinic agonist ABT-418 improved clinical measures of attentiveness and impulsivity in adults with ADHD Wilens et al. 1999
TNP reduced symptoms on Conners’ Parent Rating Scale in ‘Learning Problems and ‘Hyperactivity’ domains in children and adolescents with ADHD Shytle et al. 2002b
Alzheimer’s disease Attention Rating = 1 Intravenous nicotine (0.125, 0.25 and 0.50 mg/kg/min) dose-dependently improved intrusion errors on a verbal recall task in non-smokers with AD, with maximal benefits at the 0.25 mg/kg/min dose, suggesting an ‘inverted U’ dose effect Newhouse et al. 1988
TNP improved performance on CPT by reducing errors of omission after 1 day, and this effect was sustained with chronic nicotine exposure. Also reduced variability of response speed in patients with AD White and Levin (1999)
MMN’s amplitude increases with nicotine gum (2 mg) and MMN latencies shortened by nicotine treatment in patients with AD Engeland et al. 2002
Nicotine gum (2 mg) did not alter auditory P300 in either Tacrine treated or non-treated AD subjects Knott et al. 2002
Patients with AD improved detection performance on a flicker fusion test compared to control groups with i.v. nicotine, as well as discriminative sensitivity and reaction times Sahakian et al. 1989
Subcutaneous administration of nicotine improved sustained visual attention (RVIP and delayed response matching tasks) and perception (flicker fusion task) in AD patients Jones et al. 1992
Learning and memory Rating = 2 TNP improved acquisition of information compared to placebo patch in patients with probable AD Wilson et al. 1995
TNP did not improve performance of a letter memory test (Sternberg) in patients with AD White and Levin (1999)
Subcutaneous nicotine versus saline administration did not improve verbal or visual Stroop test memory in patients with AD Jones et al. 1992
No difference between TNP and placebo patch on measures of short-term memory Snaedal et al. 1996
Executive function Rating = 3 TNP did not improve performance on the DRS compared to PLA in patients with probable AD Wilson et al. 1995
Psychomotor performance Rating = 2–3 Subcutaneous nicotine improved finger tapping performance in patients with AD in this single-blind study Jones et al. 1992
Parkinson’s disease Attention Rating = 1 Acute intravenous nicotine (up to 1.25 μg/kg/min) in comparison to saline significantly dose-dependently improved reaction time, processing speed, and reduced tracking errors Kelton et al. 2000
Clinical outcomes Rating = 2–3 Chronic treatment with TNP (14 mg/day) compared to placebo improved extrapyramidal symptoms Kelton et al. 2000
NRT did not significantly alter ratings of timed walking, fine motor skills, hand tremor, or depressive symptoms in a nonsmoking sample with PD Vieregge et al. 2001
After 12 h of exposure to NRT, 16 patients with PD demonstrated a worsening of motor performance compared to placebo Ebersbach et al. 1999
Schizophrenia Attention (sustained) Rating = 1 SCZ smokers showed improved sustained attention (on CPT Hits) with administrated of 14 mg patch compared to non-psychiatric controls Depatie et al. 2002
Cigarette smoking improved abstinence-induced impairment of CPT Hit Rate and Attentional Variability Index in Schizophrenics compared to smoking controls. These improvements were blocked by the high-affinity nAChR antagonist mecamylamine Sacco et al. 2003
Attention (selective) Rating = 3 SCZ patients quitting smoking for up to 10 weeks did not have alteration of Stroop test performance George et al. 2002
Attention (sensory gating) Rating = 1 P50 auditory-evoked ERPs are transiently improved by nicotine administration and cigarette smoking. Such effects are related to stimulation of a7 nAChRs (encoded by CHRNA7) Adler et al. 1993; Leonard et al. 2002
Nicotine administration (to smokers and nonsmokers), or smoking in SCZ patients demonstrated improved leading saccade acceleration in the smooth pursuit eye movement task, with no significant effects in non-psychiatric subjects Olincy et al. 1998; Sherr et al. 2002; Depatie et al. 2002; Avila et al. 2003; Olincy et al. 2003
Acute cigarette smoking selectively improves pre-pulse inhibition in SCZ compared to non-psychiatric controls; these effects are blocked by the high-affinity nAChR antagonist mecamyamine George et al. 2003a
Learning and memory Rating = 2 SCZ smokers treated with haloperidol demonstrated dose-related impairment in delayed matching to sample accuracy, and poorer response time on a complex reaction task. Nicotine dose-dependently reversed these medication induced impairments Levin et al. 1996
Working memory or spatial organization Rating = 1 SCZ smokers had baseline impairments in VSWM compared to SCZ nonsmokers, and after quitting smoking, SCZ smokers had further impairments in VSWM while control quitters had improvements George et al. 2002
Administration of nicotine to SCZ smokers improved performance on a spatial organization task (ANAM battery) Smith et al. 2002
Clinical outcomes Rating = 2 In a group of SCZ patients, smoking high nicotine cigarettes decreased negative symptoms of SCZ more than smoking de-nicotinized cigarettes. However, neither type of cigarettes significantly altered positive symptoms, anxiety, or depression Smith et al. 2002
SCZ patients demonstrated decreased haloperidol-induced bradykinesia-rigidity when administered 21 mg/24 h TNP versus placebo Yang et al. 2002

Ratings: 1 = Strong Evidence; 2 = Modest Evidence; 3 = Little of No Evidence. ADHD, attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder; ANAM, Automated Neuropsychological Assessment Metrics; CGI, Clinical Global Improvement; CPT, Continuous Performance Test; DRS, Dementia Rating Scale; ERP, event-related potential; MMN, mismatch negativity; NRT, nicotine replacement therapy; POMS, Profile of Mood States; RVIP, rapid visual information processing; SCZ, schizophrenic; TNP, transdermal nicotine patch; VSWM, visuospatial working memory.

Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD)

ADHD is characterized by a persistent pattern of inattention and distractibility and/or hyperactivity/impulsivity to the degree that it impairs academic or occupational functioning (First, 1994). Individuals with ADHD typically have demonstrated evidence of this disorder in their childhood years and, of these individuals, conservative estimates suggest that 50% continue to demonstrate clinically significant symptoms into adulthood (Barkley et al., 2002), putting the prevalence rates among adults at approximately 2% of the population (Weiss and Murray, 2003).

Rates of cigarette smoking in individuals with ADHD have been found to be higher (approximately 40%) compared to the general population of normal adults (Pomerleau et al., 1995). The mechanisms behind these higher rates are hypothesized to be related to self-medication with nicotine of the putative cognitive (attentional and inhibitory) dysfunction associated with ADHD. Several lines of evidence that support this theory include the finding that those ADHD patients who were smokers retrospectively self-reported a higher level of symptoms associated with this disorder in childhood than did never smokers with ADHD, suggesting that the presence of greater symptomatology in childhood may be associated with a greater risk to smoke (Pomerleau et al., 1995). The mechanism for the effect of nicotine on reducing attentional deficits in ADHD may be similar to psychostimulants used to treat this disorder, and probably involves enhancement of central DA and NE function. Methylphenidate and dextroamphetamine are two psychostimulants used to treat ADHD by promoting DA and NE release; nicotine acts an indirect dopamine agonist and, similar to psychostimulants, exerts its effects by improving attention, the trademark deficit in this disorder (Levin, 1992). The most recent drug to be approved for the treatment of ADHD, the NE-specific reuptake inhibitor, atomoxetine, as well as the known efficacy in ADHD of the combined DA/NE reuptake inhibitor, bupropion, further supports the involvement of NE mechanisms in this disorder (Spencer and Beiderman, 2002).

Administration of transdermal nicotine to smokers was shown to improve reaction time in adults with ADHD in multiple studies (Conners et al., 1996; Levin et al., 1996a) on the CPT, a computerized test of attention validated in its use for diagnosing and assessing the severity and course of ADHD (Conners, 1995). A more recent study by the Duke group in adults with ADHD has shown that both the acute and chronic transdermal nicotine patch (TNP) can reduce hit rate reaction time variability on the CPT with a comparable effect size to the psychostimulant methylphenidate, and was also associated with reduced Clinical Global Impression–Severity (CGI–S) and depressed mood (Levin et al., 2001). In clinical treatment research in ADHD subjects, TNP in both smokers and nonsmokers was found to reduce symptoms adults diagnosed with ADHD on the CGI as well as improve self-reported vigor on a measure of measure of current mood states (Conners et al., 1996; Levin et al., 1996a), and reduce symptoms on several key domains related to ADHD on the Conners’ Parent Rating Scale in children and adolescents with ADHD (Shytle, 2002). Recently, Wilens et al. (1999) reported that the novel nicotinic cholinergic agent ABT-418, a nicotinic agonist with selectivity for the α4β2 nAChR, may reduce impulsivity, hyperactivity and attentional deficits in adults with ADHD. Taken together, these collective data strongly suggest the positive effects of (high-affinity) nAChR stimulation on attentional dysfunction and clinical symptomatology in patients with ADHD.

Alzheimer’s disease

Alzheimer’s Disease (AD), the most common form of dementia, is a progressive neuropsychiatric disease in which individuals demonstrate ongoing deterioration of cognitive abilities over time. Rates of AD in the general population range from 2% to 6% of individuals over the age of 65 years, with estimates that this rate will rise given the greater number of elderly adults predicted in the population in the coming years (Rocca et al., 1986). Changes in individuals with AD typically begin with apparently benign symptoms, often observed as a loss of interest in usual activities, or neglect of daily routines, and these eventually lead to more apparent forgetfulness, such as failing to keep appointments, neglecting chores and errands, or recalling names of acquaintances. The more obvious degeneration associated with AD in the middle stages include declines in planning, organization, cognitive adaptability, attention and concentration, and visuospatial deficits, thought to be the result of primary deficits in memory (Hyman et al., 1989), probably due to cholinergic neurone degeneration (Zhou et al., 2003), as well as dysnomia, and memory for recently learned information. Individuals with AD often present as being unaware of their cognitive declines, which is a characteristic feature of this disease. Remote memories remain intact much longer over the course of this disease; however, as the disease progresses into its later stages, memories for the distant past and recognition of close relatives and friends deteriorate, and apraxias and agnosias are likely to develop, leading to a loss of ability to dress or feed oneself (Victor, 2001).

Early detection of dementia symptoms is typically performed using validated dementia screening measures such as the Mattis Dementia Rating Scale (DRS) which taps characteristic areas of early decline in patients with AD, including attention, initiation, perseveration, construction, conceptualization and verbal and non-verbal memory. Sensitivity and specificity of these measures in the ability to differentiate AD from normal controls has been shown to be extremely high (Salmon et al., 2002). Deficits measured through comprehensive neuropsychological evaluation can help detect signs of this disorder years before the clinical diagnosis (Pasquier, 1999).

AD is biologically characterized by a deficit in acetylcholine, and specifically basal forebrain cholinergic neurones in the nucleus basalis of Meynert, with attendant reduction of nAChRs (Terry and Buccafusco, 2003). Changes in nAChRs studied in normally ageing controls have been determined to represent distinctly different changes from those receptors in patients with AD (Court et al., 2001). Currently, the Food and Drug Authority approved treatments include cholinesterase inhibitors (ChEIs) such as physostigmine (Eserine®), donezepil (Aricept®) and rivastigmine (Exelon®), from which patients experience typically modest cognitive improvements (Delagarza, 2003) and, most recently, the combined ChEI and nAChR positive allosteric modulator galanthamine (Reminyl®) (Coyle and Kershaw, 2001; Maelicke et al., 2001) (Table 1).

Brenner et al. (1993) suggested that smoking is associated with a lower likelihood of developing AD. In a case–control study, Wang et al. (1997) showed that light smoking may decrease the risk for developing AD whereas heavier daily smoking may actually increase this risk. However, in a review of the literature on the potential neuroprotective effects of smoking in AD, Fratiglioni and Wang (2000) concluded that there is no clear evidence that smoking has a protective effect on the development of AD. Nonetheless, because of the clear involvement of nAChRs in the progression of AD, including the loss of nAChR binding sites in the brains of AD patients, nAChRs are likely to be pharmacological targets for medications development for the treatment of this disease.

In studies of the effects of nicotine on cognitive function in AD, the literature is rapidly expanding (Newhouse et al., 1997). In the domain of learning and memory, which represents an area of substantial cognitive deficit in patients with AD, results of the effects of nicotine have been mixed. Newhouse et al. (1988) demonstrated that i.v. nicotine (0.125, 0.25 and 0.50 μg/kg/min.) produced dose-dependent improvement in intrusion errors on a word recall task in non-smokers with AD, and that maximum effects occurred at 0.25 μg/kg/min, suggesting an ‘inverted-U’ dose–response pattern (Newhouse et al., 1988). By contrast, Jones et al. (1992) found that subcutaneous nicotine injections did not improve verbal or nonverbal short-term memory deficits in patients with AD (Jones et al., 1992). Administration of transdermal nicotine improved performance on a repeated acquisition task in patients with probable AD (Wilson et al., 1995); however, the study by White and Levin (1999) did not support this finding because they found that performance on a letter memory test did not improve with nicotine patch administration, and no difference between placebo and TNP conditions was detected on measures of short-term memory in AD patients (Snaedal et al., 1996). Furthermore, TNP did not improve performance on the Mattis DRS in patients with probable AD (Wilson et al., 1995). Finally, using a within-subjects, placebo-controlled study of three doses of the nAChR channel activator ABT-418 in subjects with moderate AD, Potter et al. (1999) demonstrated that this agent could dose-dependently improve deficits in total recall in a verbal learning task, a seven-item selective reminding task, and in non-verbal tasks such as spatial learning and memory and repeated acquisition. Methodological differences amongst these studies, including dose and route of nicotine administration, may explain these disparate effects of nicotine on learning and memory in AD.

In the domain of attentional processing, patients with mild-to-moderate AD demonstrated a reduced variability of response speed, as well as a reduction of errors of omission on a task of attentional function, after 1 day of exposure to TNP, and this improvement was sustained with chronic exposure to nicotine (White and Levin, 1999). However, Howe and Price (2001) found no effect of TNP on attentional performance in a sample of healthy older subjects at risk for developing dementia. Another study found that i.v. nicotine improved detection performance on the critical flicker fusion test in patients with AD, and improved their ability to discriminate stimuli and their reaction times, suggesting effects of nicotine on visual perceptual and attentional cortical mechanisms (Sahakian et al., 1989). Similarly, work by Jones et al. (1992) further supports this finding, and demonstrated improved perception on the flicker fusion task in patients with AD in response to subcutaneous nicotine administration. Administration of nicotine orally (nicotine polacrilex, 2 mg gum) was shown to shorten mismatch negativity (MMN) latencies in AD patients and, interestingly, these latencies were improved with physostigmine pre-treatment (Engeland et al., 2002), although this same dose of nicotine gum did not alter auditory P300 (Oddball task) event-related potentials in either physostigmine-treated or non-treated AD patients (Knott et al., 2002). Thus, nicotine administration appears to improve some forms of attentional function in AD, but more controlled studies are needed.

Parkinson’s disease

Parkinson’s disease (PD) is a degenerative disease that typically begins between the fourth and seventh decades, and affects 1% of the population over the age of 65 years. This disease is marked by tremor, poverty of voluntary movement, rigidity, and shuffling gait (Victor, 2001). Generally speaking, patients with PD have been found to smoke at lower rates than controls (Baumann et al., 1980) both during the illness and before its diagnosis. It has been observed that there is a significant loss of nicotinic binding sites and dopaminergic neuronal degeneration in the substantia nigra (from which DA cell bodies project to caudate putamen, and is part of the extrapyramidal motor system) in patients with PD (Bosboom et al., 2003). The potential role of nAChRs as targets for treatment of neurochemical changes related to this disease has been raised (Burghaus et al., 2003), including the specific treatment of cognitive dysfunction associated with PD (Newhouse et al., 1997). Initial pre-clinical and epidemiological studies presented competing evidence about the potential neuroprotective effects of nicotine and cigarette smoking in PD (Rajput, 1984; Rajput et al., 1987; Grandinetti et al., 1994). Subsequent well-controlled studies (Gorell et al., 1999) and an extensive review of the literature (Fratiglioni, 2000) have more fully supported the notion that, although the mechanisms are not entirely understood, cigarette smoking provides a neuroprotective effect, albeit undefined, against the development of PD, and this effect cannot be accounted for by sample or study design bias as has been suggested.

There has only been one published study in the literature on the effects of nicotine on cognition in PD (Kelton et al., 2000). In this study, multiple doses of i.v. nicotine (up to 1.25 mg/kg/min.) were infused in n = 15 non-demented patients with early-to-moderate PD (average Hoehn–Yahr stage = 1.77, Mini-Mental State Examination score = 28.6/30), which was followed by transdermal nicotine at doses up to 14 mg/day for up to 2 weeks. These investigators found that acute i.v. nicotine improved several areas of cognitive performance in PD patients, including reaction time, speed of processing and tracking errors, but not in other areas, such as selective attention and semantic retrieval. Transdermal nicotine improved extrapyramidal functions, and this effect was sustained for up to 30 days after patch discontinuation. Consistent with these positive effects on extrapyramidal function, Fagerstrom et al. (1994) presented a case study of two elderly patients diagnosed with PD who, after administration of nicotine gum, demonstrated a reduction in tremor, disorganized thinking, bradykinesia, and increased energy. By contrast, other studies with nicotine administration have shown that neither nicotine gum nor patch significantly altered symptoms of PD (Clemens et al., 1995; Vieregge et al., 2001), and after 12 h of exposure to nicotine via the patch, PD patients showed a worsening of motor performance compared to the placebo condition (Ebersbach et al., 1999). Clearly, further controlled studies of nicotine’s effects on cognitive and motor function in PD are warranted.

Tourette’s disorder

Gilles de la Tourette’s syndrome, commonly known as Tourette’s Disorder (TD), is a typically lifelong neurological disorder characterized by motor and verbal tics of unknown cause. To be classified as TD, motor symptoms must have appeared before the age of 21 years, and the prevalence rates are approximately 0.05% (Simon, 2002). Several lines of evidence suggest the role of nAChRs in mediating Tourette’s-related symptoms. McConville et al. (1991 (1992) studied the effect of TNP in TD patients currently treated with haloperidol, and the combination of TNP and haloperidol significantly reduced tics compared to haloperidol treatment alone. Silver et al. (2001a) extended this finding and confirmed that nicotine gum augments haloperidol treatment more effectively than placebo gum in reducing the symptoms of TD as measured by the Clinical and Parent Global Improvement Scales. Nicotine administered by patch has also been shown to reduce the number of tics in non-smoking patients with TD, as measured by the Yale Global Tic Severity Scale (Dursun and Reveley, 1997). Long-term administration of nicotine leads to nAChR desensitization and upregulation, and thus the use of nicotinic antagonist treatment for motor symptoms of TD has been proposed; however, an 8-week placebo-controlled trial of mecamylamine (up to 7.5 mg/day) was not superior to placebo as a monotherapy for children and adolescents with TD in an 8-week trial (Silver et al., 2001). Nicotinic mechanisms have been studied in an animal pharmacological model of TD, which demonstrated that both acute and chronic nicotine administration reduced a drug-induced head twitch response in mice (Tizabi et al., 2001).

Affective and anxiety disorders

Increasing attention has been paid to the role of nicotine dependence and smoking in affective disorders, but there has been little study of the effects of nicotine on cognitive performance deficits in these patients (Hammar et al., 2003). Rates of smoking in clinical studies of mental illness are higher than non-psychiatric controls, and exceed 50% in many major disorders such as bipolar disorder, major depression and panic disorder (George and Vessicchio, 2001). Interestingly, in an examination of 1566 female twin pairs from the Virginia Twin Registry, and using a best-fitting bivariate twin model, Kendler et al. (1993) found evidence for shared genetic factors causing the strong association between smoking and major depression, which suggests that there is little evidence for a causal relationship between smoking and major depression. Shytle et al. (2002) and Silver et al. (2001b) have discussed the role of anti-depressant medications in inhibiting nicotine acetylcholine nAChRs, and suggested that antagonism of these receptors contributes to their effectiveness in reducing symptoms of depression (Table 1). In this regard, an open-label study in clinically depressed non-smokers found that transdermal nicotine patch application lead to a dramatic decrease in depressive symptoms within 3 days (Salin-Pascual et al., 1996). It is thought that an increase in depressive symptoms may follow smoking cessation in patients with depression (Niaura et al., 1999; Tsoh et al., 2000; Killen et al., 2003) indicating a potential maintenance role for these receptors in this disorder, and there is also evidence that patients with depression have more difficulty maintaining early abstinence from cigarettes than non-depressed patients (Pomerleau et al., 2001).

Most of the research on nicotine and bipolar disorder has been associational in design, and has documented the higher rates of smoking in bipolar patients compared to controls (Gonzalez-Pinto et al., 1998; Corvin et al., 2001) and examined its association with clinical features of this disorder. For example, it is thought that severity of smoking may predict the severity of psychotic features in this disorder (Corvin et al., 2001).

A link between anxiety disorders and nicotine has also begun to be established. Breslau et al. (1991) showed that nicotine dependence in young adults was more often associated with anxiety disorders than the non-nicotine dependent population (Breslau et al., 1991). Epidemiological studies from the same group later suggested an association between increased daily smoking and risk for first-time occurrence of panic attacks, and that this risk is higher in those actively smoking compared to those who are former smokers (Breslau and Klein, 1999). According to Breslau and Klein (1999), these results also suggest that the presence of panic attacks is not significantly associated with increased risk for the development of smoking and nicotine dependence.

There have been several studies of smoking prevalence, and the association of smoking with psychopathological features, in posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) mostly by investigators at Duke University, and confined to Vietnam-era veterans (Beckham, 1999). In initial studies, Beckham et al. (1995) found that smoking rates in Vietnam-era veterans with PTSD were approximately 60% (Beckham et al., 1995). Furthermore, women with PTSD had higher rates of smoking compared to women with no history of PTSD (Acierno et al., 1996). Beckham et al. (1997) also found that Vietnam-era combat veterans with PTSD had higher rates of smoking (53%) than those without PTSD (45%). PTSD patients reported higher rates of heavy smoking consumption (> 25 cigarettes/day) than non-PTSD veterans (48% versus 28%). In heavy smokers, there were higher levels of total PTSD symptoms and Cluster C (Avoidance and Numbing) and Cluster D (hyper-arousal) symptoms. Because these studies were cross-sectional, it is not clear whether the higher levels of PTSD symptoms were directly attributable to their heavy smoking, or a trait marker unrelated to smoking. Other studies have shown that nicotine withdrawal symptoms appear to be worse in smokers with PTSD in response to trauma-related stimuli compared to those without PTSD (Beckham et al., 1995).

Schizophrenia

Schizophrenia is a chronic psychotic disorder whose essential features include the presence of delusions and hallucinations and thought disorder, in addition to negative symptoms, which include flattening of affect and dysfunction of occupational, social or interpersonal relationships. The link between cigarette smoking and schizophrenia is well-established (Dalack et al., 1998; George et al., 2003) and, recently, there has been evidence to suggest dysregulation of nAChR systems in schizophrenics, including reduced upregulation of high-affinity nAChRs in the schizophrenic brain (Breese et al., 2000) and functional polymorphisms in the promoter region of the low-affinity (α7) nAChR that alter sensory physiology (Leonard et al., 2002).

In terms of studies of the effects of nicotine and smoking on neuropsychological performance in patients with schizophrenia, a human laboratory study in schizophrenic smokers showed that nicotine transdermal patch (0–21 mg/day) could dose-dependently reverse haloperidol-induced working memory, attentional and reaction time impairments (Levin et al., 1996b). Furthermore, transdermal nicotine has been shown to reduce haloperidol-induced bradykinesia and rigidity in schizophrenics compared to a placebo patch (Yang et al., 2002). Accordingly, it has been suggested that one reason for the high rates of smoking and inability to quit in schizophrenic patients is that they derive specific benefits from nicotine self-administration, such as remediation of antipsychotic-induced cognitive impairments and extrapyramidal symptoms (Dalack et al., 1998; George et al., 2003). Interestingly, a recent study by Park et al. (2000) in non-psychiatric smokers found that acute cigarette smoking may impair spatial working memory, but not spatial selective attention. This is in contrast to data from our group in schizophrenic and control smokers suggesting that smoking cessation impairs visuospatial working memory (VSWM) function in schizophrenic patients (George et al., 2002); in healthy control smokers, smoking cessation improves VSWM (George et al., 2002), consistent with the results of Park et al. (2000). These differential effects of nicotine/smoking on VSWM have been supported by recent laboratory studies in our group, and appear to be mediated by stimulation of high-affinity nAChRs (Sacco et al., 2004). Finally, Smith et al. (2002) recently showed that nicotine (versus placebo) nasal spray had modest effects on reversing abstinence-induced impairment of complex reaction time and working memory in patients with schizophrenia. Curiously, in the study by Smith et al. 2002, some of these modest (Cohen’s d < 0.5) effects of cigarette smoking on clinical and cognitive outcome measures could also be improved by smoking denicotinized cigarettes, suggesting that non-nicotine (non-pharmacological) aspects of the process of cigarette smoking may contribute to these pro-cognitive effects of smoking.

There are several lines of evidence to suggest that nicotine and smoking enhance attentional processing in schizophrenic subjects and that, in many cases, this enhancement is preferentially seen in schizophrenic patients (and first-degree relatives) compared to healthy controls (Adler et al., 1998). Several studies (Adler et al., 1993; Leonard et al., 2002) from the University of Colorado group have documented that nicotine and smoking can transiently ameliorate deficits in P50 auditory evoked potentials, and that these effects seem to be mediated by α7-subtype nAChRs, which are enriched in the hippocampus (Stevens et al., 1998; Picciotto et al., 2000; Leonard and Bertrand, 2001). These deficits in P50 gating are closely linked to the α7 receptor locus on chromosome 15q14 (Freedman et al., 1997). Other studies have shown similar effects of nicotine (Olincy et al., 1998; Depatie et al., 2002; Sherr et al., 2002; Avila et al., 2003) and cigarette smoking (Olincy et al., 2003) on other pre-attentional measures, such as the leading saccades of smooth-pursuit eye movements and, most recently, that sustained attentional task performance (% Hit Rate) as assessed by Continuous Performance Test (Depatie et al., 2002; Sacco et al., 2004), and sensorimotor gating, as assessed by pre-pulse inhibition of the acoustic startle response (George et al., 2003), could be selectively enhanced by nicotine and cigarette smoking in schizophrenic versus control subjects.

It is important to address the body of literature on the common genetic foundations of many of these disorders with respect to the specific example of nicotine use and psychiatric comorbidity. Independent of psychiatric diagnosis, in a review of factors influencing initiation of smoking, genetic liability accounted for 50% of the variance in risk of becoming a regular smoker (Tsuang et al., 2001), whereas a meta-analysis of genetic and environmental influences on liability to schizophrenia by Sullivan et al. (2003) estimated high heritability (81%). Kendler et al. (2003) suggest that genetic risk factors principally account for the comorbidity of many common psychiatric and substance use disorders. Thus, although it has not been rigorously studied, the hypothesis that comorbid smoking and major mental illness share common genetic risk factors appears to be likely and warrants further investigation (Kendler et al., 2003).

Dose–response effects of nicotine on cognitive function in human subjects

To date, there have been few studies of the dose–response effects of nicotine on cognitive performance in humans. Only two studies in normal controls (Parrott and Craig, 1992; Foulds et al., 1996) and single studies in patients with AD (Newhouse et al., 1988), PD (Kelton et al., 2000) and schizophrenia (Levin et al., 1996b) have conducted a careful examination of the dose–response effects of nicotine on cognitive performance. Interestingly, in the control studies, nicotine appeared to dose-dependently (either subcutaneously or with nicotine gum) improve sustained attention on the RVIP task (Foulds et al., 1996), but this was not the case on the Stroop task (Parrott and Craig, 1992). In AD patients given i.v. nicotine, there were inverted-U effects of nicotine on a verbal memory task (Newhouse et al., 1988), while in Parkinson’s patients, i.v. nicotine dose-dependently improved psychomotor performance, reaction times and extrapyramidal symptoms (Kelton et al., 2000). Finally, in schizophrenic patients who had haloperidol-induced cognitive deficits, a transdermal nicotine patch (7–21 mg/day) dose-dependently reversed such working memory and attentional deficits (Levin, 1996). Therefore, there may be different dose– response effects of nicotine across different cognitive tasks, but this may also depend on the route of nicotine administration, and these dose–response effects on cognitive function will require further study.

Conclusions and recommendations for further study

Taken together, there is emerging evidence to suggest that nicotine may enhance cognitive performance in humans, although such cognitive enhancement may only be observed in patients with neuropsychiatric disorders who exhibit defined neuropsychological and psychophysiological deficits that are intrinsic to their illness. A review of the available evidence of nicotinic effects on cognition in healthy human subjects (Table 2) suggests that, with the exception of positive gains in general psychomotor performance, there is little, if any, evidence to support nicotinic effects on most domains of cognitive performance. By contrast, there appears to be stronger evidence for nicotinic modulation of cognitive dysfunction in several neuropsychiatric disorders (Table 3), including ADHD, AD, PD and schizophrenia. For other neuropsychiatric disorders, nicotinic enhancement of cognitive function is less clear, although there is evidence that nicotine may improve certain clinical symptoms associated with these disorders (e.g. motor tics in TD, mood symptoms in affective disorders), and the role for nAChR stimulation in remediating clinical and cognitive dysfunction in these disorders will require further study.

To date, several pre-clinical (e.g. rodent and primate) studies suggest positive effects of nicotine on working memory and attentional performance. However, further study of the effects of nicotine on neurocognitive deficits in animal models of neuropsychiatric disorders (e.g. induced by pharmacological agents, transgenic strains of mice, anatomical lesions) would be useful, particularly because such studies, in the light of their translational nature, would help to define basic mechanisms and support further hypothesis testing, which could have important therapeutic implications. The use of well-defined animal models of cognition that have analogues in humans, such as the radial arm maze (working memory), the five-choice serial reaction time test (attention), and sensory gating/pre-attentional measures, such as event-related potentials (e.g. N40) and pre-pulse inhibition, coupled with standardized methods for administering nicotine, both acutely and chronically (to model the situation in tobacco users), is highly recommended. Furthermore, more controlled studies of the direct administration of nicotine or nicotinic agents on cognitive deficits in neuropsychiatric disorders are required. This would include an evaluation of nicotinic agent dose–response effects, which would be important in the light of the propensity of nAChRs to desensitize and upregulate, especially because there is some evidence for nicotine administration leading to inverted-U dose–response effects on some cognitive measures (Newhouse et al., 1988). Furthermore, additional systematic study of the effects of nicotinic on various domains of cognitive dysfunction (e.g. attention, verbal and non-verbal memory, working memory and executive function) is suggested, especially because stimulation of different nAChR subtypes may lead to differential effects on various types of cognitive performance. Because cigarette smoking status (and nicotine withdrawal) may be a significant confounding variable in the assessment of the pro-cognitive effects of nicotine, these clinical ‘proof of principal’ studies will have to control carefully for smoking status and nicotine deprivation effects, which appear to be clinically significant. The ethics of administering nicotine to healthy non-smokers (including never smokers) and non-smoking patients with neuropsychiatric disorders also needs further consideration. In most cases, the abuse liability of nicotine administered by nicotine replacement therapies (other than cigarette smoking) is negligible (Henningfield and Keenan, 1993; West et al., 2000), and relevant regulatory agencies (e.g. government funding agencies, institutional review boards) will need to be appropriately educated about these issues because careful implementation of critical safeguards and safety measures will ensure that the true therapeutic effects of nicotine and other nicotinic agents in various neuropsychiatric disease entities will be safely ascertained, and then translated to clinical treatment in a timely manner. Nonetheless, it appears highly likely that several novel nAChR-based pharmacological treatments for cognitive dysfunction in neuropsychiatric disorders could become available within the next 5–10 years.

Acknowledgments

This work was supported in part by National Institutes of Health grants R01-DA-14039, R01-DA-13672 and K02-DA-16611 (T.P.G.), the National Alliance for Research on Schizophrenia and Depression (T.P.G.) and The Patrick and Catherine Weldon Donaghue Medical Research Foundation (T.P.G.).

Contributor Information

Kristi A. Sacco, Program for Research in Smokers with Mental Illness (PRISM), Division of Substance Abuse, Department of Psychiatry, Yale University School of Medicine, New Haven, CT, USA..

Katie L. Bannon, Program for Research in Smokers with Mental Illness (PRISM), Division of Substance Abuse, Department of Psychiatry, Yale University School of Medicine, New Haven, CT, USA.

Tony P. George, Program for Research in Smokers with Mental Illness (PRISM), Division of Substance Abuse, Department of Psychiatry, Yale University School of Medicine, New Haven, CT, USA.

References

  1. Acierno RA, Kilpatrick DG, Resnick HS, Saunders BE, Best C. Violent assault, posttraumatic stress disorder, and depression. Risk factors for cigarette use among adult women. Behav Modificat. 1996;20:363–384. doi: 10.1177/01454455960204001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Adler LE, Hoffer LD, Wiser A, Freedman R. Normalization of auditory physiology by cigarette smoking in schizophrenic patients. Am J Psychiatry. 1993;150:1856–1861. doi: 10.1176/ajp.150.12.1856. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Adler LE, Hoffer LD, Waldo M, Harris JG, Griffth J, Stevens K, Flach K, Nagamoto H, Bickford P, Leonard S, Freedman R. Schizophrenia, sensory gating and nicotinic receptors. Schizophr Bull. 1998;24:189–202. doi: 10.1093/oxfordjournals.schbul.a033320. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Arnsten A. Through the looking glass: differential noradrenergic modulation of prefrontal cortical function. Neural Plast. 2000;7:133–146. doi: 10.1155/NP.2000.133. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Avery EE, Baker LD, Asthana S. Potential role of muscarinic agonists in Alzheimer’s disease. Drugs Aging. 1997;11:450–459. doi: 10.2165/00002512-199711060-00004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Avila MT, Sherr JD, Hong E, Myers CS, Thaker G. Effects of nicotine on leading saccades during smooth pursuit eye movements in smokers and nonsmokers with schizophrenia. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2003;28:2184–2191. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1300265. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Baranano DE, Ferris CD, Snyder S. Atypical neural messengers. Trends Neurosci. 2001;24:99–106. doi: 10.1016/s0166-2236(00)01716-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Barkley RA, Fischer M, Smallish L, Fletcher K. The persistence of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder into young adulthood as a function of reporting source and definition of disorder. J Abnorm Psychol. 2002;111:279–289. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Bartus RT, Flicker C, Dean RL, Pontecorvo M, Figueiredo JC, Fisher S. Selective memory loss following nucleus basalis lesions: long term behavioral recovery despite persistent cholinergic deficiencies. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 1985;23:125–135. doi: 10.1016/0091-3057(85)90139-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Baumann RJ, Jameson HD, McKean HE, Haack DG, Weisberg L. Cigarette smoking and Parkinson disease: comparison of cases with matched neighbors. Neurology. 1980;30:839–943. doi: 10.1212/wnl.30.8.839. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Beckham J. Smoking and anxiety in combat veterans with chronic posttraumatic stress disorder: a review. J Psychoact Drugs. 1999;31:103–110. doi: 10.1080/02791072.1999.10471731. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Beckham JC, Lytle BL, Vrana SR, Hertzberg MA, Feldman ME, Shipley R. Smoking withdrawal symptoms in response to trauma-related stimuli among Vietnam combat veterans with posttraumatic stress. Addict Behav. 1995a;20:1–9. doi: 10.1016/0306-4603(95)00038-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Beckham JC, Roodman AA, Shipley RH, Hertzberg MA, Cunha GH, Kudler HS, Levin ED, Rose JE, Fairbank J. Smoking in Vietnam combat veterans with posttraumatic stress disorder. J Traumat Stress. 1995b;8:461–472. doi: 10.1007/BF02102970. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Beckham JC, Kirby AC, Feldman ME, Hertzberg MA, Moore SD, Crawford AL, Davidson J, Fairbank J. Prevalence and correlates of heavy smoking in Vietnam veterans with chronic posttraumatic stress disorder. Addict Behav. 1997;22:637–647. doi: 10.1016/s0306-4603(96)00071-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Bell SL, Taylor RC, Singleton EG, Henningfield JE, Heishman SJ. Smoking after nicotine deprivation enhances cognitive performance and decreases tobacco craving in drug abusers. Nicotine Tobacco Res. 1999;1:45–52. doi: 10.1080/14622299050011141. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Bertrand D, Bertrand S, Ballivet M. Pharmacological properties of the homomeric alpha-7 receptor. Neurosci Lett. 1992;146:87–90. doi: 10.1016/0304-3940(92)90179-b. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Bosboom JL, Stoffers D, Wolters ECh. The role of acetylcholine and dopamine in dementia and psychosis in Parkinson’s disease. J Neural Transm . 2003;65(Suppl):185–195. doi: 10.1007/978-3-7091-0643-3_11. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Breese CR, Marks MJ, Logel J, Adams CE, Sullivan B, Collins AC, Leonard S. Effects of smoking history on [3H]nicotine binding in human post-mortem brain. J Pharmacol Exp Ther. 1997;282:7–13. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Breese CR, Lee MJ, Adams CE, Sullivan B, Logel J, Gillen KM, Marks MJ, Collins AC, Leonard S. Abnormal regulation of high affinity nicotinic receptors in subjects with schizophrenia. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2000;23:351–364. doi: 10.1016/S0893-133X(00)00121-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Brenner DE, Kukull WA, van Belle G, Bowen JD, McCormick WC, Teri L, Larson E. Relationship between cigarette smoking and Alzheimer’s disease in a population-based case-control study. Neurology. 1993;43:293–300. doi: 10.1212/wnl.43.2.293. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Breslau N, Klein D. Smoking and panic attacks. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1999;56:1141–1147. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.56.12.1141. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Breslau N, Kilbey M, Andreski P. Nicotine dependence, major depression, and anxiety in young adults. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1991;48:1069–1074. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1991.01810360033005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Burghaus L, Schutz U, Krempel U, Lindstrom J, Schroder H. Loss of nicotinic acetylcholine receptor subunits alpha4 and alpha7 in the cerebral cortex of Parkinson patients. Parkinsonism Relat Disord. 2003;9:243–246. doi: 10.1016/s1353-8020(03)00028-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Clemens P, Baron JA, Coffey D, Reeves A. The short-term effect of nicotine chewing gum in patients with Parkinson’s disease. Psychopharmacology. 1995;117:253–256. doi: 10.1007/BF02245195. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Conners C K (1995) The Continuous Performance Test. Multi-Health Systems, Toronto
  26. Conners CK, Levin ED, Sparrow E, HInton SC, Erhardt D, Meck WH, Rose JE, March J. Nicotine and attention in Adult Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) Psychopharmacol Bull. 1996;32:67–73. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Corvin A, O’Mahony E, O’Regan M, Comerford C, O’Connell R, Craddock N, Gill M. Cigarette smoking and psychotic symptoms in bipolar affective disorder. Br J Psychiatry. 2001;179:35–38. doi: 10.1192/bjp.179.1.35. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Court J, Martin-Ruiz C, Piggott M, Spurden D, Griffiths M, Perry E. Nicotinic receptor abnormalities in Alzheimer’s Disease. Soc Biol Psychiatry. 2001;49:175–184. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(00)01116-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Coyle J, Kershaw P. Galanthamine, a cholinesterase inhibitor that allosterically modulates nicotinic receptors: effects on the course of Alzheimer’s disease. Biol Psychiatry. 2001;49:289–299. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(00)01101-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Dalack GW, Healy DJ, Meador-Woodruff J. Nicotine dependence and schizophrenia: clinical phenomenon and laboratory findings. Am J Psychiatry. 1998;155:1490–1501. doi: 10.1176/ajp.155.11.1490. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Davranche K, Audiffren M. Effects of a low dose of transdermal nicotine on information processing. Nicotine Tobacco Res. 2002;4:275–285. doi: 10.1080/14622200210141635. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Delagarza V. Pharmacologic treatment of Alzheimer’s Disease: an update. Am Family Phys. 2003;68:1365–1372. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Della Casa V, Hofer I, Weiner I, Feldon J. The effects of smoking on acoustic prepulse inhibition in healthy men and women. Psychopharmacol. 1998;137:362–368. doi: 10.1007/s002130050631. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Della Casa V, Hofer I, Weiner I, Feldon J. Effects of smoking status and schizotypy on latent inhibition. J Psychopharmacol. 1999;13:45–57. doi: 10.1177/026988119901300106. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Depatie L, Driscoll GA, Holahan A-L, Atkinson V, Thavundayil JX, Ying Kin NN, Samarthji L. Nicotine and behavioral markers of risk for schizophrenia: a double-blind, placebo-controlled, cross-over study. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2002;27:1056–1070. doi: 10.1016/S0893-133X(02)00372-X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Donnelly Roberts DL, Puttfarcken PS, Kuntzweiler TA, Briggs CA, Anderson DJ, Campbell JE, Piattoni-Kaplan M, McKenna DG, Wasciak JT, Holladay MW, Williams M, Arneric S. ABT-594: a novel, orally effective analgesic acting via neuronal nicotinic acetylcholine receptors: I. In vitro characterization. J Pharmacol Exp Ther. 1998;285:777–786. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Duncan E, Madonick S, Chakravorthy S, Parwani A, Szilagyi S, Efferen T, Gonzenbach S, Angrist B, Rotrosen J. Effects of smoking on acoustic startle and prepulse inhibition in humans. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2001;156:266–272. doi: 10.1007/s002130100719. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Dursun SM, Reveley M. Differential effects of trandermal nicotine on microstructured analyses of tics in Tourette’s syndrome: an open study. Psychol Med. 1997;27:483–487. doi: 10.1017/s0033291796003984. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Eaton JB, Peng J-H, Schroeder KM, George AA, Fryer JD, Krishnan C, Buhlman L, Kuo Y-P, Steinlein P, Lukas R. Characterization of human alpha-4 beta-2 nicotinic acetylcholine receptors stably and heterologously expressed in native nicotinic receptor-null SH-EP1 human epithelial cells. Mol Pharmacol. 2003;64:1283–1294. doi: 10.1124/mol.64.6.1283. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Ebersbach G, Stock M, Muller J, Wenning G, Wissel J, Poewe W. Worsening of motor performance in patients with Parkinson’s disease following transdermal nicotine administration. Movement Disord. 1999;14:1011–1013. doi: 10.1002/1531-8257(199911)14:6<1011::aid-mds1016>3.0.co;2-f. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Eisenberg T, Griffiths RR, Stitzer M. Mecamylamine does not precipitate withdrawal in cigarette smokers. Psychopharmacology. 1996;127:328–336. doi: 10.1007/s002130050094. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Engeland C, Mahoney C, Mohr E, Ilivitsky v, Knott V. Acute nicotine effects on auditory sensory memory in tacrine-treated and non-treated patients with Alzheimer’s disease. An event-related potential study. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 2002;72:457–464. doi: 10.1016/s0091-3057(02)00711-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Ernst M, Heishman SJ, Spurgeon L, London E. Smoking history and nicotine effects on cognitive performance. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2001a;25:313–319. doi: 10.1016/S0893-133X(01)00257-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Ernst M, Matochik JA, Heishman SJ, Van Horn JD, Jons PH, Henningfield JE, London E. Effect of nicotine on brain activation during performance of a working memory task. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2001b;98:4728–4733. doi: 10.1073/pnas.061369098. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Fagerstrom KO, Pomerleau O, Giordani B, Stelson F. Nicotine may relieve symptoms of Parkinson’s disease. Psychopharmacology. 1994;116:117–119. doi: 10.1007/BF02244882. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. First M B (1994) Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders, 4th edn (DSM-IV). American Psychiatric Association, Washington DC
  47. Foulds J, Stapleton J, Swettenham J, Bell N, McSorley K, Russell M. Cognitive performance effects of subcutaneous nicotine in smokers and never-smokers. Psychopharmacology. 1996;127:31–38. doi: 10.1007/BF02805972. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Fowler JS, Volkow ND, Wang G-J, Pappas N, Logan J, MacGregor R. Inhibition of monoamine oxidase B in the brains of smokers. Nature. 1996a;379:733–736. doi: 10.1038/379733a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Fowler JS, Volkow ND, Wang G-J, Pappas N, Logan J, Shea C. Brain monoamine oxidase A: inhibition by cigarette smoke. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 1996b;93:14065–14069. doi: 10.1073/pnas.93.24.14065. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Fratiglioni L, Wang H. Smoking and Parkinson’s and Alzheimer’s disease: review of epidemiological studies. Behav Brain Res. 2000;113:117–120. doi: 10.1016/s0166-4328(00)00206-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Freedman R, Coon H, Myles-Worsley M, Orr-Urtreger A, Olincy A, Davis A, Polymeropoulos M, Holik J, Hopkins J, Rosenthal J, Waldo MC, Reimherr F, Wender P, Yaw J, Young DA, Breese CR, Adams C, Patterson D, Adler LE, Kruglyak L, Leonard S, Byerley W. Linkage of a neurophysiological deficit in schizophrenia to a chromosome 15 locus. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 1997;94:587–592. doi: 10.1073/pnas.94.2.587. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Fryer JD, Lukas R. Antidepressants noncompetitively inhibit nicotinic acetylcholine receptor function. J Neurochem. 1999a;72:1117–1124. doi: 10.1046/j.1471-4159.1999.0721117.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Fryer JD, Lukas R. Noncompetitive functional inhibition at diverse, human nicotinic acetylcholine receptor subtypes by bupropion, phencyclidine, and ibogaine. J Pharmacol Exp Ther. 1999b;288:88–92. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Fung YK, Schmid MJ, Ander TM, Lau Y. Effects of nicotine withdrawal on central dopaminergic systems. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 1996;53:635–640. doi: 10.1016/0091-3057(95)02063-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Gentry CL, Lukas R. Regulation of nicotinic acetylcholine receptor numbers and function by chronic nicotine exposure. Curr Drugs Targets CNS Neurol Dis. 2002;1:359–385. doi: 10.2174/1568007023339184. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. George TP, O’Malley S. Current pharmacological treatments for nicotine dependence. Trends Pharmacol Sci. 2004;25:42–48. doi: 10.1016/j.tips.2003.11.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. George TP, Vessicchio J. Nicotine addiction and schizophrenia. Psychiatr Times. 2001;18:39–42. [Google Scholar]
  58. George TP, Verrico CD, Roth R. Effects of repeated nicotine pre-treatment on mesoprefrontal dopaminergic and behavioral responses to acute footshock stress. Brain Res. 1998;801:36–49. doi: 10.1016/s0006-8993(98)00537-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. George TP, Verrico CD, Picciotto MR, Roth R. Nicotinic modulation of mesoprefrontal dopamine systems: pharmacologic and neuroanatomic characterization. J Pharmacol Exp Ther. 2000d;295:58–66. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. George TP, Vessicchio JC, Termine A, Sahady DM, Head CA, Pepper WT, Kosten TR, Wexler B. Effects of smoking abstinence on visuospatial working memory function in schizophrenia. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2002;26:75–85. doi: 10.1016/S0893-133X(01)00296-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. George T P, Termine A, Dudas M M, Seyal A A, Ludhi T, Bannon K, Vessicchio J C, Madonick S H, Sacco K A (2003a) Differential attentional modulation of the acoustic startle response by cigarete smoking in schizophrenics versus controls: role of nicotinic receptors. American College of Neuropsychopharmacology, San Juan, Puerto Rico
  62. George T P, Vessicchio J C, Termine A (2003b) Nicotine and Tobacco Use in Schizophrenia. In Meyer J M, Nasrallah H A (eds), Medical illness in schizophrenia. American Psychiatric Press, Inc., Washington DC
  63. Ghatan PH, Ingvar M, Eriksson L, Stone-Elander S, Serrander M, Ekberg K, Wahren J. Cerebral effects of nicotine during cognition in smokers and non-smokers. Psychopharmacology. 1998;136:179–189. doi: 10.1007/s002130050554. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Gitelman D. Attention and its disorders. Br Med Bull. 2003;65:21–34. doi: 10.1093/bmb/65.1.21. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Goldman-Rakic P. The physiological approach: functional architecture of working memory and disordered cognition in schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry. 1999;46:650–661. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(99)00130-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Gonzalez-Pinto A, Gutierrez M, Ezcurra J, Aizpuru F, Mosquera F, Lopez P, de Leon J. Tobacco smoking and bipolar disorder. J Clin Psychiatry. 1998;59:225–228. doi: 10.4088/jcp.v59n0503. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Gorell JM, Rybicki BA, Johnson CC, Peterson E. Smoking and Parkinson’s disease. A dose–response relationship. Neurology. 1999;52:115–119. doi: 10.1212/wnl.52.1.115. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Grandinetti A, Morens DM, Reed D, MacEachern D. Prospective study of cigarette smoking and the risk of developing idiopathic Parkinson’s disease. Am J Epidemiol. 1994;139:1129–1138. doi: 10.1093/oxfordjournals.aje.a116960. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Green M. What are the functional consequences of neurocognitive deficits in schizophrenia?’. Am J Psychiatry. 1996;153:321–330. doi: 10.1176/ajp.153.3.321. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Green MF, Marshall BD, Wirshing WC, Ames D, Marder SR, McGurk SR, Kern RS, Mintz J. Does risperidone improve verbal working memory in schizophrenia?’. Am J Psychiatry. 1997;154:799–804. doi: 10.1176/ajp.154.6.799. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Gross TM, Jarvik ME, Rosenblatt M. Nicotine abstinence produces content-specific Stroop interference. Psychopharmacology. 1993;110:333–336. doi: 10.1007/BF02251289. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Hahn B, Shoiab M, Stolerman I. Nicotine-induced enhancement of attention in the five-choice serial reaction time task: the influence of task demands. Psychopharmacology. 2003;162:129–137. doi: 10.1007/s00213-002-1005-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Hammar A, Lund A, Hugdahl K. Long-lasting cognitive impairment in unipolar major depression: a 6-month follow-up study. Psychiatry Res. 2003;118:189–196. doi: 10.1016/s0165-1781(03)00075-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Harvey SC, Maddox FN, Luetje C. Multiple determinants of dihydro-beta-erythroidine sensitivity on rat neuronal nicotinic receptor alpha subunits. J Neurochem. 1996;67:1953–1959. doi: 10.1046/j.1471-4159.1996.67051953.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Hatsukami DK, Fletcher L, Morgan S, Keenan R, Amble P. The effects of varying cigarette deprivation duration on cognitive and performance tasks. J Subst Abuse. 1989;1:407–416. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Heishman SJ. Behavioral and cognitive effects of smoking: relationship to nicotine addiction. Nicotine Tobacco Res . 1999;1(Suppl 2):S143–S147. doi: 10.1080/14622299050011971. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Heishman SJ, Henningfield J. Tolernance to repeated nicotine administration on performance, subjective, and physiological responses in nonsmokers. Psychopharmacology. 2000;152:321–333. doi: 10.1007/s002130000541. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Henningfield JE, Keenan R. Nicotine delivery kinetics and abuse liability. J Consult Clin Psychol. 1993;61:743–750. doi: 10.1037//0022-006x.61.5.743. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Herzig KE, Callaway E, Halliday R, Naylor H, Benowitz N. Effects of cotinine on information processing in non-smokers. Psychopharmacology. 1998;135:127–132. doi: 10.1007/s002130050493. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. Hildebrand BE, Nomikos GG, Panagis G, Hertel P, Schilstrom B, Svensson T. Reduced dopamine output in the nucleus accumbens but not in the medial prefrontal cortex in rats displaying a mecamylamine-precipitated nicotine withdrawal syndrome. Brain Res. 1998;779:214–225. doi: 10.1016/s0006-8993(97)01135-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. Howe MN, Price I. Effects of transdermal nicotine on learning, memory, verbal fluency, concentration, and general health in a healthy sample at risk for dementia. Int Psychogeriatr. 2001;13:465–475. doi: 10.1017/s1041610201007888. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  82. Hutchison KE, Niaura R, Swift R. The effects of smoking high nicotine cigarettes on pre-pulse inhibition, startle latency, and subjective responses. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2000;150:244–252. doi: 10.1007/s002130000399. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  83. Hyman BT, Damasio H, Damasio AR, Van Hoesen G. Alzheimer’s disease. Ann Rev Public Health. 1989;10:115–140. doi: 10.1146/annurev.pu.10.050189.000555. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  84. Jones GM, Sahakian BJ, Levy R, Warbirtpm DM, Gray J. Effects of acute subcutaneous nicotine on attention, information processing and short-term memory in Alzheimer’s disease. Psychopharmacology. 1992;108:485–494. doi: 10.1007/BF02247426. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  85. Kelton MC, Kahn MJ, Conrath CL, Newhouse P. The effects of nicotine on Parkinson’s disease. Brain Cognit. 2000;43:274–282. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  86. Kendler KS, Neale NC, MacLean CJ, Heath AC, Eaves LJ, Kessler R. Smoking and major depression: a causal analysis. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1993;50:36–43. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1993.01820130038007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  87. Kendler KS, Prescott CA, Myers J, Neale M. The structure of genetic and environmental risk factors for common psychiatric and substance use disorders in men and women. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2003;60:929–937. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.60.9.929. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  88. Killen JD, Fortmann SP, Schatzberg A, Hayward C, Varady A. Onset of major depression during treatment for nicotine dependence. Addictive Behav. 2003;28:461–470. doi: 10.1016/s0306-4603(01)00266-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  89. Knott V, Bosman M, Mahoney C, Ilivitsky V, Quirt K. Transdermal nicotine: single dose effects on mood, EEG, performance, and event-related potentials. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 1999;63:253–261. doi: 10.1016/s0091-3057(99)00006-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  90. Knott V, Mohr E, Mahoney C, Engeland C, Ilivitsky V. Effects of acute nicotine administration on cognitive event-related potentials in tacrine-treated and non-treated patients with Alzheimer’s disease. Neuropsychobiology. 2002;45:156–160. doi: 10.1159/000054957. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  91. Kumari V, Checkly SA, Gray J. Effects of cigarette smoking on prepulse inhibtion of the acoustic startle reflex in healthy male smokers. Psychopharmacology. 1996;128:54–60. doi: 10.1007/s002130050109. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  92. Kumari V, Gray J. Smoking withdrawal, nicotine dependence and prepulse inhibition of the acoustic startle reflex. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 1999;141:11–15. doi: 10.1007/s002130050800. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  93. Kumari V, Cotter PA, Checkley SA, Gray J. Effect of acute subcutaneous nicotine on prepulse inhibition of the acoustic startle reflex in healthy male non-smokers. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 1997;132:389–395. doi: 10.1007/s002130050360. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  94. Lasser K, Boyd JW, Woolhander S, Himmelstein DU, McCormick D, Bor D. Smoking and mental illness: a population-based prevalence study. JAMA. 2000;284:2606–2610. doi: 10.1001/jama.284.20.2606. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  95. Lawrence NS, Ross T. Cognitive mechanisms of nicotine on visual attention. Neuron. 2002;36:539–548. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(02)01004-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  96. Le Houzezec J, Halliday R, Benowitz NL, Callaway E, Naylor H, Herzig K. A low dose of subcutaneous nicotine improves information processing in non-smokers. Psychopharmacology. 1994;114:628–634. doi: 10.1007/BF02244994. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  97. Leonard S, Bertrand D. Neuronal nicotinic receptors: from structure to function. Nicotine Tobacco Res. 2001;3:203–223. doi: 10.1080/14622200110050213. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  98. Leonard S, Gault J, Hopkins J, Logel J, Viazon R, Short M, Drebing C, Berger R, Venn D, Sirota P, Zerbe G, Olincy A, Ross RG, Adler LE, Freedman R. Promoter variants in the alpha-7 nicotinic acetylcholine receptor subunit gene are associated with an inhibitory deficit found in schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2002;59:1085–1096. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.59.12.1085. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  99. Levin E. Nicotinic systems and cognitive function. Psychopharmacology. 1992;108:417–431. doi: 10.1007/BF02247415. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  100. Levin E. Nicotinic receptor subtypes and cognitive function. J Neurobiol. 2002;53:633–640. doi: 10.1002/neu.10151. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  101. Levin ED, Simon B. Nicotinic acetylcholine involvement in cognitive function in animals. Psychopharmacology. 1998;138:217–230. doi: 10.1007/s002130050667. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  102. Levin ED, Torry D. Acute and chronic nicotine effects on working memory in aged rats. Psychopharmacology. 1996;123:88–97. doi: 10.1007/BF02246285. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  103. Levin ED, Conners CK, Sparrow E, Hinton SC, Erhardt D, Meck WH, Rose JE, March J. Nicotine effects on adults with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Psychopharmacology. 1996a;123:55–63. doi: 10.1007/BF02246281. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  104. Levin ED, Wilson W, Rose J, McEvoy J. Nicotine–haloperidol interactions and cognitive performance in schizophrenics. Neuropsychopharmacology. 1996b;15:429–436. doi: 10.1016/S0893-133X(96)00018-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  105. Levin ED, Conners CK, Silva D, Hinton SC, Meck WH, March J, Rose J. Transdermal nicotine effects on attention. Psychopharmacology. 1998;140:135–141. doi: 10.1007/s002130050750. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  106. Levin ED, Christopher NC, Weaver T, Moore J, Brucato F. Ventral hippocampal ibotenic acid lesions block chronic nicotine-induced spatial working memory improvement in rats. Cognit Brain Res. 1999;7:405–410. doi: 10.1016/s0926-6410(98)00044-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  107. Levin ED, Conners CK, Silva D, Canu W, March J. Effects of chronic nicotine and methylphenidate in adults with attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol. 2001;9:83–90. doi: 10.1037/1064-1297.9.1.83. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  108. Lezak M D (1995) Neuropsychological assessment. Oxford University Press, New York, NY
  109. Littleton J. Receptor upregulation as a unitary mechanism for drug tolerance and physical dependence – not quite as simple as it seemed! Addiction. 2001;96:87–101. doi: 10.1046/j.1360-0443.2001.961877.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  110. Maelicke A, Samochocki M, Jostock R, Fehrenbacher A, Ludwig J, Albuquerque EX, Zerlin M. Allosteric sensitization of nicotinic receptors by galanthamine, a new treatment strategy for Alzheimer’s disease. Biol Psychiatry. 2001;49:279–288. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(00)01109-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  111. Mancuso G, Warburton DM, Melen M, Sherwood V, Tirelli E. Selective effects of nicotine on attentional processes. Psychopharmacology. 1999;146:199–204. doi: 10.1007/s002130051107. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  112. Mansvelder HD, McGehee D. Cellular and synaptic mechanisms of nicotine addiction. J Neurobiol. 2002;53:606–617. doi: 10.1002/neu.10148. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  113. Marshall D. Presynaptic nicotinic modulation of dopamine release in the three ascending pathways studied by in vivo microdialysis: comparison of naive and chronic nicotine-treated animals. J Neurochem. 1997;68:1511–1519. doi: 10.1046/j.1471-4159.1997.68041511.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  114. McConville BJ, Fogelson MH, Norman AB, Klykylo WM, Manderscheid PZ, Parker KW, Sanberg P. Nicotine potentiation of haloperidol in reducing tic frequency in Tourette’s disorder. Am J Psychiatry. 1991;148:793–794. doi: 10.1176/ajp.148.6.793. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  115. McConville BJ, Sanberg PR, Fogelson H, King J, Cirino P, Parker KW, Norman A. The effects of nicotine plus haloperidol compared to nicotine only and placebo nicotine only in reducing tic severity and frequency in Tourette’s Disorder. Biol Psychiatry. 1992;31:832–840. doi: 10.1016/0006-3223(92)90315-q. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  116. Min SK, Moon I, Ko RW, Shin HS. Effects of transdermal nicotine on attention and memory in healthy elderly non-smokers. Psychopharmacology. 2001;159:83–88. doi: 10.1007/s002130100899. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  117. Murphy FC, Klein R. The effects of nicotine on spatial and non-spatial expectancies in a covert orienting task. Neuropsychologia. 1998;36:1103–1114. doi: 10.1016/s0028-3932(98)00012-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  118. Newhouse PA, Potter A, Corwin J, Lenox R. Acute nicotinic blockade produces cognitive impairment in normal humans. Psychopharmacology. 1992;108:480–484. doi: 10.1007/BF02247425. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  119. Newhouse PA, Potter A, Corwin J, Lenox R. Age-related effects of the nicotinic antagonist mecamylamine on cognition and behavior. Neuropsychopharmacology. 1994;10:93–107. doi: 10.1038/npp.1994.11. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  120. Newhouse PA, Potter A, Levin E. Nicotinic system involvement in Alzheimer’s and Parkinson’s diseases. Implications for therapeutics. Drugs Aging. 1997;11:206–228. doi: 10.2165/00002512-199711030-00005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  121. Newhouse PA, Sunderland T, Tariot PN, Blumhardt CL, Weingartner H, Mellow A, Murphy D. Intravenous nicotine in Alzheimer’s disease: a pilot study. Psychopharmacology. 1988;95:171–175. doi: 10.1007/BF00174504. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  122. Niaura R, Britt DM, Borrelli B, Shadel WG, Abrams DB, Goldstein M. History and symptoms of depression among smokers during a self-initiated quit attempt. Nicotine Tobacco Res. 1999;1:251–279. doi: 10.1080/14622299050011371. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  123. Olausson P, Jentsch JD, Taylor JR. Repeated nicotine exposure enhances reward-related learning in rats. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2003;28:1264–1271. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1300173. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  124. Olincy A, Ross RG, Young DA, Roath M, Freedman R. Improvement in smooth pursuit eye movements after cigarette smoking in schizophrenic patients. Neuropsychopharmacology. 1998;18:175–185. doi: 10.1016/S0893-133X(97)00095-X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  125. Olincy A, Johnson LL, Ross R. Differential effects of cigarette smoking on performance of a smooth pursuit and a saccadic eye movement task in schizophrenia. Psychiatry Res. 2003;117:223–236. doi: 10.1016/s0165-1781(03)00022-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  126. Park S, Knopnick C, McGurk S, Meltzer H. Nicotine impairs spatial sorking memory while leaving spatial attention intact. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2000;22:200–209. doi: 10.1016/S0893-133X(99)00098-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  127. Parker MJ, Beck A, Luetje C. Neuronal nicotinic receptor beta2 and beta4 subunits confer large differences in agonist binding affinity. Mol Pharmacol. 1998;54:1132–1139. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  128. Parrott A. Cigarette-derived nicotine is not a medicine. World J Biol Psychiatry. 2003;4:49–55. doi: 10.3109/15622970309167951. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  129. Parrott AC, Craig D. Cigarette smoking and nicotine gum (0, 2 and 4 mg): effects upon four visual attention tasks. Neuropsychobiology. 1992;25:34–43. doi: 10.1159/000118807. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  130. Pasquier F. Early diagnosis of dementia: neuropsychology. J Neurol. 1999;246:6–15. doi: 10.1007/s004150050299. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  131. Perkins K. Chronic tolerance to nicotine in humans and its relationship to tobacco dependence. Nicotine Tobacco Res. 2002;4:405–422. doi: 10.1080/1462220021000018425. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  132. Perkins KA, Grobe JE, Fonte C, Goettler J, Caggiula AR, Reynolds WA, Stiler RL, Scierka A, Jacob R. Chronic and acute tolerance to subjective, behavioral, and cardiovascular effects of nicotine in humans. J Pharmacol Exp Ther. 1994;270:628–638. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  133. Petrie RX, Deary I. Smoking and human information processing. Psychopharmacology. 1989;99:393–396. doi: 10.1007/BF00445565. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  134. Picciotto M. Nicotine as a modulator of behavior: beyond the inverted U. Trends Pharmacol Sci. 2003;23:494–499. doi: 10.1016/S0165-6147(03)00230-X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  135. Picciotto MR, Caldarone BJ, King SL, Zachariou V. Nicotinic receptors in the brain: links between molecular biology and behavior. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2000;22:451–465. doi: 10.1016/S0893-133X(99)00146-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  136. Pineda JA, Herrera C, Kang C, Sandler A. Effects of cigarette smoking and 12-h abstention on working memory during a serial-probe recognition task. Psychopharmacology. 1998;139:311–321. doi: 10.1007/s002130050722. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  137. Pomerleau CS, Teuscher F, Goeters S, Pomerleau O. Effects of nicotine abstinence and menstrual phase on task performance. Addict Behav. 1994;19:357–362. doi: 10.1016/0306-4603(94)90058-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  138. Pomerleau CS, Brouwer RJ, Pomerleau O. Emergence of depression during early abstinence in depressed and non-depressed women smokers. J Addict Dis. 2001;20:73–80. doi: 10.1300/J069v20n01_07. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  139. Pomerleau O, Downet KK, Stelson FW, Pomerleau CS. Cigarette smoking in adult patients diagnosed with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. J Subst Abuse. 1995;7:373–378. doi: 10.1016/0899-3289(95)90030-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  140. Potter A, Corwin J, Lang J, Piasecki M, Lenox R, Newhouse P. Acute effects of the selective cholinergic channel activator (nicotinic agonist) ABT-418 in Alzheimer’s disease. Psychopharmacology. 1999;142:334–342. doi: 10.1007/s002130050897. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  141. Powell J, Tait S, Lessiter J. Cigarette smoking and attention to signals of reward and threat in the Stroop paradigm. Addiction. 2002;97:1163–1170. doi: 10.1046/j.1360-0443.2002.00117.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  142. Provost SC, Woodward R. Effects of nicotine gum on repeated administration of the Stroop Test. Psychopharmacology. 1991;104:536–540. doi: 10.1007/BF02245662. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  143. Rajput A. Epidemiology of Parkinson’s disease. Can J Neurol Sci. 1984;11:156–159. doi: 10.1017/s0317167100046321. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  144. Rajput A, Offord KP, Beard CM, Kurland LT. A case-control study of smoking habits, dementia, and other illnesses in idiopathic Parkinson’s disease. Neurology. 1987;37:226–232. doi: 10.1212/wnl.37.2.226. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  145. Ralph-Williams R, Lehmann-Masten V, Otero-Corchon V, Low MJ, Geyer M. Differential effects of direct and indirect dopamine agonists on prepulse inhibition: a study in D1 and D2 receptor knock-out mice. J Neurosci. 2002;22:9604–9611. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.22-21-09604.2002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  146. Robbins T. Chemical neuromodulation of frontal-executive functions in humans and other animals. Exp Brain Res. 2000;133:130–138. doi: 10.1007/s002210000407. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  147. Rocca WA, Amaducci LA, Schoenberg B. Epidemiology of clinically diagnosed Alzheimer’s disease. Annal Neurol. 1986;19:415–424. doi: 10.1002/ana.410190502. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  148. Rommelspacher H, Meier-Henco M, Smolka M, Kloft C. The levels of norharman are high enough after smoking to affect monoamine oxidase B in platelets. Eur J Pharmacol. 2002;441:115–125. doi: 10.1016/s0014-2999(02)01452-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  149. Rusted JM, Caufield D, King L, Goode A. Moving out of the laboratory: does nicotine improve everyday attention?’. Behav Pharmacol. 2000;11:621–629. doi: 10.1097/00008877-200011000-00009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  150. Sacco K A, Termine A, Seyal A A, Dudas M M, Vessicchio J C, Krishnan-Sarin S, Jatlow P I, Wexler B E, George T P (2004) Nicotinic recepter mechanisms and neuropsychological functioning in schizophrenia: effects on spatial working memory and attention. Arch Gen Psychiatry, in press [DOI] [PubMed]
  151. Sahakian B, Jones G, Levy R, Gray J, Warburton D. The effects of nicotine on attention, information processing, and short-term memory in patients with dementia of the Alzheimer type. Br J Psychiatry. 1989;154:797–800. doi: 10.1192/bjp.154.6.797. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  152. Sakurai Y, Kanazawa I. Acute effects of cigarettes in non-deprived smokers on memory, calculation and executive functions. Hum Psychopharmacol. 2002;17:369–373. doi: 10.1002/hup.424. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  153. Salin-Pascual RJ, Rosas M, Jimenez-Genchi A, Rivera-Meza BL, Delgado-Parra V. Antidepressant effect of transdermal nicotine patches in non-smoking patients with major depression. J Clin Psychiatry. 1996;57:387–389. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  154. Salmon DP, Thomas RG, Pay MM, Booth A, Hofstetter CR, Thal LJ, Katzman R. Alzheimer’s disease can be accurately diagnosed in very mildly impaired individuals. Neurology. 2002;59:1022–1028. doi: 10.1212/wnl.59.7.1022. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  155. Samochocki M, Hoffle A, Fehrenbacher A, Jostock R, Ludwig J, Christner C, Radina M, Zerlin M, Ullmer C, Pereira E, Lubbert H, Albuquerque EX, Maelicke A. Galanathamine is an allosteric potentiating ligand of neuronal nicotinic but not muscarinic acetylcholine receptors. J Pharmacol Exp Ther. 2003;305:1024–1036. doi: 10.1124/jpet.102.045773. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  156. Schneider JS, Tinker JP, Menzaghi F, Lloyd GK. The subtype-selective nicotine acetylcholine receptor agonist SIB-1553 A improves both attention and memory components of a spatial working memory task in chronic low dose 1-methyl-4-phenyl-1 2,3,6-tetrahydropyridine-treated monkeys. J Pharmacol Exp Therap. 2003;306:401–406. doi: 10.1124/jpet.103.051912. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  157. Sherr JD, Myers C, Avila MT, Elliott A, Blaxton TA, Thaker G. The effects of nicotine on specific eye tracking measures in schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry. 2002;52:721–728. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(02)01342-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  158. Shiffman S, Zetter-Segal M, Kassel J, Paty J, Benowitz NL, O’Brien G. Nicotine elimination and tolerance in non-dependent cigarette smokers. Psychopharmacology. 1992;109:449–456. doi: 10.1007/BF02247722. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  159. Shytle RD, Silver AA, Lukas RJ, Newman MB, Sheehan DV, Sanberg P. Nicotinic acetylcholine receptors as targets for antidepressants. Mol Psychiatry. 2002a;6:525–535. doi: 10.1038/sj.mp.4001035. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  160. Shytle RD, Silver AA, Wilkinson BJ, Sanberg P. A pilot controlled trial of transdermal nicotine in the treatment of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. World J Biol Psychiatry. 2002b;3:150–155. doi: 10.3109/15622970209150616. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  161. Silver AA, Shytle RD, Philipp MK, Wilkinson BJ, McConville B, Sanberg P. Transdermal nicotine and haloperidol in Tourette’s disorder: a double-blind placebo-controlled study. J Clin Psychiatry. 2001a;62:707–714. doi: 10.4088/jcp.v62n0908. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  162. Silver AA, Shytle RD, Sheehan KH, Sheehan DV, Ramos A, Sanberg P. Multicenter, double-blind, placebo-controlled study of mecamylamine monotherapy for Tourette’s disorder. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2001b;40:1103–1110. doi: 10.1097/00004583-200109000-00020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  163. Simon R (2002) Clinical neurology. McGraw Hill, New York
  164. Smith RC, Singh A, Infante M, Khandat A, Kloos A. Effects of cigarette smoking and nicotine nasal spray on psychiatric symptoms and cognition in schizophrenia. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2002;27:479–497. doi: 10.1016/S0893-133X(02)00324-X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  165. Snaedal J, Jonsson JE, Gylfadottir G. The effects of nicotine in dermal plaster on cognitive functions in patients with Alzheimer’s Disease. Dementia. 1996;7:47–52. doi: 10.1159/000106852. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  166. Snyder FR, Davis FC, Henningfield J. The tobacco withdrawal syndrome: performance decrements assessed on a computerized test battery. Drug Alcohol Depend. 1989;23:259–266. doi: 10.1016/0376-8716(89)90090-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  167. Spencer T, Biederman J. Non-stimulant treatment for attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. J Atten Disord. 2002;6(Suppl):S109–S119. doi: 10.1177/070674370200601s13. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  168. Spilich G. Cognitive benefits of nicotine: fact or fiction? Addiction. 1994;89:141–142. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.1994.tb00869.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  169. Srivastava ED, Russell M, Feyerabend C, Masterson JG, Rhodes J. Sensitivity and tolerance to nicotine in smokers and nonsmokers. Psychopharmacology. 1991;105:63–68. doi: 10.1007/BF02316865. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  170. Stein EA, Pankiewicz J, Harsch HH, Cho JK, Fuller SA, Hoffman RG, Hawkins M, Rao SM, Bandettini PA, Bloom A. Nicotine-induced limbic cortical activation in the human brain: a functional MRI study. Am J Psychiatry. 1998;155:1009–1015. doi: 10.1176/ajp.155.8.1009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  171. Stevens KE, Kem WR, Mahnir VM, Freedman R. Selective alpha-7-nicotinic agonists normalize inhibition of auditory response in DBA mice. Psychopharmacology. 1998;136:320–327. doi: 10.1007/s002130050573. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  172. Stolerman IP, Mizra NR, Hahn B, Shoaib M. Nicotine in an animal model of attention. Eur J Pharmacol. 2000;393:147–154. doi: 10.1016/s0014-2999(99)00886-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  173. Sullivan PF, Kendler KS, Neale M. Schizophrenia as a complex trait. Evidenc from a meta-analysis of twin studies. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2003;60:1187–1192. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.60.12.1187. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  174. Suter TW, Buzzi R, Woodson PP, Battig K. Psychophysiological correlates of conflict solving and cigarette smoking. Activas Nervosa Superior. 1983;25:261–272. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  175. Swerdlow NR, Caine SB, Braff DL, Geyer M. The neural substrates of sensorimotor gating of the startle reflex: a review of recent findings and their implications. J Psychopharmacol. 1992;6:176–190. doi: 10.1177/026988119200600210. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  176. Terry A, Buccafusco J. The cholinergic hypothesis of age and Alzheimer’s disease-related cognitive deficits: recent challenges and their implications for novel drug development. J Pharmacol Exp Ther. 2003;306:821–827. doi: 10.1124/jpet.102.041616. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  177. Tizabi Y, Russell LT, Johnson M, Darmant N. Nicotine attenuates DOI-induced head-twitch response in mice: implications for Tourette syndrome. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry. 2001;25:1445–1457. doi: 10.1016/s0278-5846(01)00194-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  178. Tsoh JY, Humfleet GL, Munoz RF, Reus V, I, Hartz DT, Hall S. Development of major depression after treatment for smoking cessation. Am J Psychiatry. 2000;157:368–374. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.157.3.368. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  179. Tsuang MT, Bar JL, Harley RM, Lyons M. The Harvard twin study of substance abuse: what we have learned. Harv Rev Psychiatry. 2001;9:267–279. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  180. Vezina P, Blanc G, Glowinski J, Tassin JP. Nicotine and morphine differentially activate brain dopamine in prefrontal cortical and subcortical terminal fields: effects of acute and repeated injections. JPET. 1992;261:484–490. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  181. Victor M (2001) Adams and Victor’s principles of neurology, McGraw Hill, New York
  182. Vieregge A, Sieberer M, Jacobs H, Hagenah JM, Vieregge P. Transdermal nicotine in PD: a randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled study. Neurology. 2001;57:1032–1035. doi: 10.1212/wnl.57.6.1032. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  183. Wang PN, Wang SJ, Hong CJ, Liu TT, Fuh JL, Chi CW, Liu CY, Liu H. Risk factors for Alzheimer’s disease: a case-control study. Neuroepidemiology. 1997;16:234–240. doi: 10.1159/000109692. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  184. Ward KD, Garvery AJ, Bliss RE, Sparrow D, Young JB, Landsverg L. Changes in urinary catecholamine excretion after smoking cessation. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 1991;40:937–940. doi: 10.1016/0091-3057(91)90109-f. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  185. Wecker NS, Kramer JH, Wisniewski A, Delis DC, Kaplan E. Age effects on executive ability. Neuropsychology. 2000;14:409–414. doi: 10.1037//0894-4105.14.3.409. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  186. Weiss M, Murray C. Assessment and management of attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder in adults. Can Med Assoc J. 2003;168:715–722. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  187. Wesnes K, Revell A. The separate and combined effects of scopolamine and nicotine on human information processing. Psychopharmacology. 1984;84:5–11. doi: 10.1007/BF00432014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  188. West R, Hajek P, Foulds J, Nilsson F, May S, Meadows A. A comparison of the abuse liability and dependence potential of nicotine patch, gum, spray and inhaler. Psychopharmacology. 2000;149:198–202. doi: 10.1007/s002130000382. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  189. West RJ, Russell MAH, Jarvis MJ, Pizzey T, Kadam B. Urinary adrenaline concentrations during 1- days of smoking abstinence. Psychopharmacology. 1984;84:141–142. doi: 10.1007/BF00432045. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  190. White HK, Levin E. Four-week nicotine skin patch treatment effects on cognitive performance in Alzheimer’s disease. Psychopharmacology. 1999;143:158–165. doi: 10.1007/s002130050931. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  191. Wilens TE, Biederman J, Spencer TJ, Bostic J, Prince J, Monuteaux MC, Soriano J, Fine C, Abrams A, Rater M, Pulisner D. A pilot controlled clinical trial of ABT-418, a cholinergic agonist, in the treatment of adults with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Am J Psychiatry. 1999;156:1931–37. doi: 10.1176/ajp.156.12.1931. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  192. Wilson AL, Langley LK, Monley J, Bauer T, Rottunda S, McFalls E, Kovera C, McCarten J. Nicotine patches in Alzheimer’s Disease: pilot study on learning memory and safety. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 1995;51:509–514. doi: 10.1016/0091-3057(95)00043-v. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  193. Wooltorton JR, Pidoplichko V, I, Broide RS, Dani J. Differential desensitization and distribution of nicotinic acetylcholine receptor subtypes in midbrain dopamine areas. J Neurosci. 2003;23:3176–3185. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-08-03176.2003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  194. Yang YK, Nelson L, Kamaraju L, Wilson W, McEvoy J. Nicotine decreases bradykinesia-rigidity in haloperidol-treated patients with schizophrenia. Neuropsychpharmacology. 2002;27:684–686. doi: 10.1016/S0893-133X(02)00325-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  195. Zhao L, Kuo Y-P, George AA, Peng J-H, Purandare MS, Schroeder KM, Lukas RJ, Wu J. Functional properties of homomeric, human alpha-7 nicotinic acetylcholine receptors heterologously expressed in the SH-EP1 human epithelial cell line. J Pharmacol Exp Ther. 2003;305:1132–1141. doi: 10.1124/jpet.103.048777. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  196. Zhou F-M, Wilson C, Dani J. Muscarinic and nicotinic cholinergic mechanisms in the mesostriatal dopamine systems. The Neuroscientist. 2003;9:23–36. doi: 10.1177/1073858402239588. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  197. Zoli M, Moretti M, Zanardi A, McIntosh JM, Clementi F, Gotti C. Identification of the nicotinic receptor subtypes expressed on dopaminergic terminals in rat striatum. J Neurosci. 2002;22:8785–8789. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.22-20-08785.2002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES