Abstract
Episodic memory is the ability to recall personally experienced events that are situated within specific spatial contexts. Within a navigation environment, salient objects can function as landmarks, and interactions with them can be encoded as distinct episodic events. However, the nuanced integration of “where” and “what” aspects of objects during episodic memory formation remains poorly understood. In this study, we conduct a comprehensive analysis of neuronal responses along the rat CA1 proximodistal axis to manipulations of environmental cues and object features. Our results demonstrate that the proximal and distal portions of CA1 preferentially represent landmarks and events, respectively. Following object displacement, we identify an event-like population coding within CA1, distinct from the anticipated pattern of landmark representation. These findings elucidate the specific roles that objects may play in spatial navigation and memory encoding, providing novel insights into the selective processing of object-related information within the entorhinal-hippocampal network under varying conditions.
Subject terms: Spatial memory, Hippocampus
Distinct activity patterns along the CA1 transverse axis during object-related event coding, as well as spatial and non-spatial contextual coding, shed light on the selective processing of information within the entorhinal-hippocampal network.
Introduction
The hippocampus is crucial for the formation of episodic memory, representing personally experienced events within specific spatial frameworks1. In rodents, the collective localized firing of hippocampal neurons, known as place cells, generates a cognitive map of the spatial environment, providing a structural basis for linking episodic memories2. Place cells are capable of reorganizing their firing patterns, a process termed remapping, to accommodate environmental changes or novel experiences. This dynamic reorganization facilitates the decorrelation of overlapping neural signals, thereby preventing interference between distinct memories3,4. When objects are introduced into the navigation space, hippocampal place cells undergo substantial changes in activity, including modifications in the size, number, and spatial positioning of their receptive fields5–10. Among these diverse responses, some hippocampal cells fire specifically at the location of an object5, while others fire at a fixed distance and direction from the object, independent of its identity8. These unique behaviors, observed in “landmark-vector cells” and “object cells,” likely reflect adaptations to position shifts in landmark, a distinctive and recognizable reference point critical for spatial representation, and to local object-related events encountered during navigation. This diversity suggests a multifaceted role of salient objects in spatial mapping and memory encoding. However, the complex interplay between landmark and event-related information in the hippocampus complicates the dissection of their individual contributions to the “where” and “what” aspects of episodic memory, with the precise role of objects in these processes still unclear.
The anatomical and functional segregation within the entorhinal-hippocampal network offers critical insights into the complexity of episodic memory formation. The entorhinal cortex (EC) integrates multimodal sensory information and conveys highly processed signals to the hippocampus11. Within the EC, the medial region (MEC) contains several specialized cell types, including grid cells12,13, aperiodic spatial cells14, head direction cells15, border cells16, object-vector cells (OVCs)17, and speed cells18, which collaboratively encode landmark-based distance and directional information, dynamically mapping the spatial location of the animal within the environment19,20. In contrast, the lateral EC (LEC) exhibits minimal spatial selectivity in open fields21,22, with neurons predominantly representing non-spatial sensory aspects of the animal’s experience, such as presence or absence of objects23,24, odors in discrimination tasks25, and passage of time26, providing the hippocampus with a temporal progression of events2. Inputs from the EC are partially segregated along the CA1 transverse axis, where MEC neurons primarily project to the proximal CA1 (pCA1), and LEC neurons preferentially target the distal CA1 (dCA1)27,28. Immediate-early gene expression analyses have indicated distinct mnemonic functions along the CA1 proximodistal axis, with dCA1 preferentially processing non-spatial information, such as time, odor, and objects, across various behavioral tasks, while pCA1 is more engaged in tasks requiring spatial memory29–34. Electrophysiological studies have further demonstrated a stronger spatial modulation in pCA135,36 and a more coherent response to conflicting landmarks in dCA137, as well as coordinated activity between the LEC and dCA1 in odor-associated memory encoding25,38. These findings collectively suggest that the entorhinal-hippocampal network processes landmark and event information with distinct population coding in pCA1 and dCA1, offering a potential resolution to the enigmatic role of objects in self-localization and event integration within episodic memory.
In this study, we aimed to distinguish the “where” and “what” components of local object memory by analyzing place cell activity across the proximodistal CA1 axis as rats (Rattus norvegicus) navigated an open field with or without objects. Our findings revealed a complex population response to both the object features and the environmental changes resulting from their placement. Manipulating the identity and location of one object elicited stronger population responses in dCA1 than in pCA1. This suggests that newly introduced objects were unlikely to be processed as landmarks within the entorhinal-hippocampal network. Instead, transient changes to the object were incorporated into animals’ ongoing exploratory experience and encoded as events. These results highlight the complexity of information integration that occurs during spatial navigation and episodic memory formation.
Results
Object-induced local and remote population responses in CA1
To investigate whether CA1 responses to discrete objects varied along the proximodistal axis, we recorded place cell activity across CA1 subregions (Fig. S1) when two identical familiar objects were introduced to a recording box during trials 2 and 3 of a four-trial experimental paradigm (Fig. S2a, b). Similar to a prior report6, rats spent more time exploring the newly-placed objects in the initial phase of the trial, but the overall time within the object region remained unaltered (Fig. S2c). 1759 CA1 place cells recorded from 14 rats in 65 experiments showed substantial changes in firing patterns, primarily through altered firing rates around the objects (Fig. 1a). To enable cross-animal comparisons, recording positions were normalized for each tetrode, dividing CA1 into three equal-sized bands: pCA1, middle CA1 (mCA1), and dCA1. Notably, dCA1 exhibited the lowest proportion of place cells (Figs. S3 and S4a) but the highest proportion of rate-modified neurons, while mCA1 contained the highest proportion of stable cells (Fig. 1b).
Fig. 1. Object placement induced a complex population response in CA1.
a Representative firing rate distribution maps (rate maps) of neurons recorded from proximal, middle, and distal bands of CA1, showing responses to familiar objects placed in the recording box. Maps for the same cell across four trials are arranged in rows, with animal and cell IDs indicated on the left. Color is scaled to maximum peak rate of four trials. Text above each map indicates the peak rate of that trial. Circles represent objects. Obj−, trial without objects; Obj+, trial with two familiar objects. b Distribution of cells in response to object placement across three CA1 bands. Numbers indicate active place cells in each category. Proportion of cells with firing rate changes: pCA1, 57.94%, mCA1, 50.41%, and dCA1, 60.58%; chi-square test, χ2 = 15.640, P < 0.001, Cramer’s V = 0.094; Holm–Bonferroni post hoc tests, ***P < 0.001. c Heatmaps showing PVC of all putative pyramidal neurons recorded from each CA1 subregion between trials with or without objects (top two rows), as well as changes in PVC between condition pairs (bottom row). Heatmap bins correspond to spatial bins in rate maps. Color scales are indicated on the right of each row. Small circles represent objects, large circles indicate object regions. d Degree of changes in PVC as a function of distance to the objects (mean ± SEM) in each CA1 subregion, with colored dots representing data from individual spatial bins. Two-way ANOVA, distance: F (16, 1131) = 64.191, P < 0.001, η2 = 0.476; band: F (2, 1131) = 24.107, P < 0.001, η2 = 0.041; distance × band: F (32, 1131) = 4.963, P < 0.001, η2 = 0.123. e Boxplots showing degree of changes in PVC between trial pairs, for spatial bins inside and outside the object region, with outliers omitted. Bins inside object region: median (25%–75% percentile), pCA1, 0.199 (0.171–0.228), mCA1, 0.215 (0.174–0.285), and dCA1, 0.185 (0.154–0.239). Bins outside object region: pCA1, 0.096 (0.065–0.150), mCA1, 0.063 (0.026–0.107), dCA1, 0.075 (0.042–0.130). Population responses inside and outside the object region were significantly different: two-way ANOVA: band: F (2, 796) = 4.496, P = 0.011, η2 = 0.011; object region: F (1, 398) = 381.672, P < 0.001, η2 = 0.490; band × object region: F (2, 796) = 28.513, P < 0.001, η2 = 0.067. Holm–Bonferroni post hoc tests for bins outside the object region, ***P ≤ 0.001.
Given that hippocampal place cells collectively signal the presence of objects at a population level10, we analyzed object-related population coding across CA1 subregions using population vector correlation (PVC) to capture composite distribution changes in firing location and rate39. Consistent with prior studies10, trial 4 exhibited a pronounced hysteresis effect (Fig. S2d), prompting the exclusion of these data from analysis to avoid confounding carryover effects. PVC analysis revealed a high correlation between trials 2 and 3 (matching conditions) in all CA1 subregions. As expected, correlations decreased between trials 1 and 2 (before and after object placement), with the most substantial changes in PVC occurring near the objects (Fig. 1c). Additionally, dispersed activity shifts distant from the objects were detected in both pCA1 and dCA1, indicating that these responses may not directly reflect the landmark or sensory features of the objects, but rather environmental changes triggered by object placement. The reduction in PVC gradually diminished with object distance from mCA1, consistent with previous findings10; however, pCA1 and dCA1 showed either sustained or increased PVC for regions distant from the objects (Fig. 1d). Notably, a remote population response to objects was not observed in the CA3 region, which projects heavily to CA1 (Fig. S2e–g), suggesting that direct inputs from the EC may shape this remote response to objects in CA1.
Overall population responses to object introduction differed significantly across CA1 subregions, with the most pronounced PVC reduction observed in pCA1 (Fig. S2d, h). To clarify object-induced activity changes, we defined an “object region” as the area within a 15 cm radius around the surface of each object, following the distance used in a prior study10. While parallel changes in PVC within the object region were observed across CA1 subregions, mCA1 cells displayed minimal activity changes outside the region of interest (ROI) following object placement (Figs. 1e and S2i). Additionally, we noted a stronger response to the right object across all CA1 subregions (Fig. S2j), despite no observed preference in exploration time between the two objects (Fig. S2k). This phenomenon was present when animals were tested in the task for the first time (Fig. S2l). These observations collectively suggest that the preference for the right object is neither behaviorally nor experience-dependent, and the underlying causes remain to be determined.
A similar V-shaped pattern of rate redistribution outside the object region was observed when analyzing only the place cells, and this pattern was not related to the degree of spatial tuning of these cells (Fig. S4b–d). Rats with multi-subregion recordings also exhibited a similar trend (Fig. S4e). Thus, the introduction of familiar objects elicited a profound population response in the CA1 region, integrating multiple object-related information components. Neurons in mCA1 demonstrated a comparable local response to the objects but exhibited the weakest remote population reorganization compared to neurons in both pCA1 and dCA1.
V-shaped population coding of non-geometric contextual cues along the CA1 transverse axis
To assess whether the remote responses to object placement in pCA1 and dCA1 were driven by changes in non-spatial contextual features, which help characterize the environment without shaping its spatial layout40, we recorded CA1 neuronal activity during a color-reversal task, a conventional non-spatial manipulation paradigm of contextual cues involving the navigation of a square box with alternating black or white walls (Fig. S5a, b). A linear decrease in spatial modulation from pCA1 to dCA1 in the open field was observed among 2622 place cells recorded from 20 rats in 178 experiments (Figs. S5 and S6), consistent with earlier findings35,36. Different from the object-placement task, rats did not spend more time exploring the color-reversed walls (distance ≤ 15 cm) after training (Fig. S7a), suggesting a minimal influence of animals’ experience41. As anticipated from prior work39,42,43, rate remapping, characterized by alterations in firing rates within place fields while maintaining consistent firing locations4, was induced in CA1 (Fig. 2a). We first examined population coding of color across CA1 subregions by comparing PVCs between conditions. While population vectors (PVs) correlated strongly between matching conditions in all CA1 subregions, correlations were weaker between black and white wall configurations. Differences in PVC between conditions with reversed wall colors and matching wall colors were significant across subregions (Figs. 2b and S7b). Similar to the remote population response to objects, the activity changes were stronger in spatial bins distant from the walls (Fig. S7c), and again, mCA1 exhibited the smallest change in PVC between condition pairs (Fig. 2b, c).
Fig. 2. V-shaped population response to reversed wall color across CA1 subdivisions.
a Representative rate maps from simultaneously recorded CA1 neurons in two separate color-reversal experiments. Left: pCA1 and mCA1 place cells recorded from rat R0388. Right: mCA1 and dCA1 place cells recorded from rat R0175. Gray lines indicate the trajectories of the animal in each trial. B, black; W, white version of the same box. Note, mCA1 showed a minimum response to the color change. b Heatmaps showing PVC of all putative pyramidal neurons recorded from each CA1 subregion between color conditions (top two rows), and changes in PVC between condition pairs (bottom row) in the color-reversal task. Note, mCA1 exhibited the lowest activity changes. Reduction in PVC is stronger for spatial bins ≥15 cm from arena walls (within the white box), where the color was manipulated. c Degree of changes in PVC between condition pairs, with outliers omitted. Place cells in mCA1 exhibited the least activity change. pCA1: 0.108 (0.085–0.147), mCA1: 0.031 (0.005–0.054), and dCA1: 0.052 (0.022–0.094); Kruskal–Wallis test, H = 541.211, P < 0.001, η2 = 0.450, n = 1200 bins, Holm–Bonferroni post hoc tests, ***P < 0.001. d Distribution of rate-remapped neurons between black and white color conditions across three CA1 bands. Numbers indicate active place cells in each category. Proportion of strongly remapped cells: pCA1, 24.03%, mCA1, 12.58%, and dCA1, 18.69%, chi-square test, χ2 = 41.214, P < 0.001; Cramer’s V = 0.125; Holm–Bonferroni post hoc tests, *P < 0.05, **P < 0.01, ***P < 0.001. e Left: distributions of SC in CA1 subregions between matching (Same) and color-reversed (B vs W) conditions. Horizontal line indicates median, and thick vertical bar indicates interquartile range (IQR, 25%–75% percentile) of each subregion. Two-way repeated-measures ANOVA, color: F (1, 2619) = 262.113, P < 0.001, η2 = 0.091; band: F (2, 2619) = 26.679, P < 0.001, η2 = 0.020; color × band: F (2, 2619) = 28.724, P < 0.001, η2 = 0.021. Right: changes in SC between condition pairs, with outliers omitted. pCA1: 0.032 (−0.003 to 0.106), mCA1: 0.010 (−0.020 to 0.049), and dCA1: 0.025 (−0.011 to 0.091); Kruskal–Wallis test, n = 2622 cells, H = 68.492, P < 0.001, η2 = 0.026; Holm–Bonferroni post hoc tests, *P < 0.05; ***P < 0.001. f Similar to (e) but showing changes in RD across CA1 subdivisions. Left: color: F (1, 2619) = 288.514, P < 0.001, η2 = 0.099; band: F (2, 2619) = 20.373, P < 0.001, η2 = 0.015; color × band: F (2, 2619) = 41.298, P < 0.001, η2 = 0.031. Right: pCA1: 0.012 (−0.005 to 0.140), mCA1: 0.001 (−0.006 to 0.047), and dCA1: 0.001 (−0.010 to 0.048); H = 38.540, P < 0.001, η2 = 0.014; post hoc tests, ***P < 0.001. g Changes in SC between condition pairs as a function of tetrode position. Each cross corresponds to the average data of a single tetrode. Blue curve, linear regression; red curve, quadratic polynomial regression. Explained variances (R2) are indicated with the same color code. pro. proximal end, dis. distal end, p proximal half, d distal half. Proximal half: r = −0.375, P = 0.002, Pearson correlation, n = 63 tetrodes; distal half: r = 0.374, P = 0.007, n = 50 tetrodes. h Similar to G but showing correlations between tetrode position and changes in RD between condition pairs. Proximal half: r = −0.452, P < 0.001; distal half: r = 0.314, P = 0.029.
We next examined rate coding in individual place cells, finding that the proportions of rate-remapped cells between black and white conditions differed across CA1 bands, with mCA1 containing the lowest percentage of rate-remapped neurons (Fig. 2d). Rate remapping was quantified using two metrics: spatial correlation (SC), reflecting the degree of rate redistribution for each cell when firing location remains constant, and rate difference (RD), representing the normalized overall change in firing rate irrespective of the firing location of the cell. SC distributions closely resembled PVC distributions along the CA1 transverse axis, with significant SC reductions due to color change in pCA1 and dCA1 compared to mCA1 (Fig. 2e). Consistent with the observed SC pattern, RD values also differed significantly between condition pairs and across CA1 subregions, with pCA1 and dCA1 showing more pronounced rate redistribution than mCA1 upon wall color reversal (Fig. 2f, left). Notably, pCA1 displayed a significantly higher RD increase between condition pairs (Fig. 2f, right). The minimal rate remapping in mCA1 was not due to individual animal variations, as similar patterns emerged when analyses were restricted to simultaneously recorded pCA1, mCA1, and dCA1 neurons (Fig. S7d), or to animals with multi-subregion recordings (Fig. S7e). Additionally, the V-shaped pattern was not a consequence of varied spatial modulation across subregions, as resampling of place cells with similar spatial tuning measures from each subregion revealed consistent trends along the transverse axis (Fig. S7f–i). A similar outcome was obtained when the weighted RD for individual place fields (RDf) was computed (Fig. S8a–c), suggesting that the diminished rate changes were not caused by the neutralization across multiple place fields.
To visualize population response patterns across the CA1 transverse axis, mean SC and RD values were plotted by tetrode location, revealing a V-shaped distribution from the proximal to distal end of CA1 for both measures. Analyses of proximal and distal tetrodes independently showed significant correlations within each half for SC and RD (Fig. 2g, h). A similar pattern was observed when RDf was plotted as a function of tetrode location (Fig. S8d). To further verify activity change patterns along the CA1 transverse axis, a naïve Bayes decoder was trained on place cell activity from trials with white walls (trials 2 and 3) to predict rat location in a black-walled environment (trial 1). Considering that the decoding accuracy reached 0.8 when 30 cells were included (Fig. S5n), we decoded the three experiments with over 30 simultaneously recorded place cells in both subregions. The decoding accuracy was highest in the mCA1 subregion (Fig. S8e), indicating minimal activity change between the black and white configurations.
To summarize, hippocampal CA1 activity during the color-reversal task demonstrated a V-shaped population response along the transverse axis under non-spatial cue manipulation. Firing rate redistribution was distant from the cue changes (Fig. S7c), and was minimum near the middle of the axis, with progressive increases towards both the proximal and distal ends. Moreover, this pattern of population coding closely resembled the remote response observed in the CA1 region to object placement (Fig. S8f), suggesting that the latter may also be driven by non-spatial environmental changes.
Extensive reorganization of pCA1 activity in response to landmark changes
Given that the MEC preferentially conveys spatial signals through cell types such as grid cells, border cells, and head direction cells, while the LEC neurons predominantly provide temporally varying signals as well as sensory-related signals representing the experiential aspects of an event2,24–26,44, we hypothesized that landmark changes would elicit stronger population responses in pCA1, while event changes would lead to elevated activity alterations in dCA1. To verify this hypothesis, we analyzed population activity along the CA1 transverse axis in two rooms with distinct spatial configurations and distal landmarks (Fig. S9a, b). Similar to the color-reversal task, the rats’ time near the walls remains unchanged in the two-room task (Fig. S9c).
Global remapping, characterized by the engagement of a second population of place cells to represent a distinct spatial map in the hippocampus4, was evident at the individual cell level based on significant changes in the firing location and rate within the recording box in most of the animals (Fig. 3a), in line with previous reports39,42,43. PVCs of 1145 pyramidal neurons recorded from 13 rats in 28 experiments were high for trials within the same room across all subregions but decreased substantially between trials in different rooms (Figs. 3b and S9d). The reduction in PVC was region-dependent, with pCA1 exhibiting the greatest decrease, mCA1 showing a moderate decrease, and dCA1 demonstrating the smallest decrease (Fig. 3c), suggesting that pCA1 was more sensitive to changes in salient landmarks compared to the distal regions of CA1.
Fig. 3. Proximal CA1 was more sensitive to changes in landmarks.
a Simultaneously recorded CA1 neurons in two rats exhibiting global remapping in the two-room task. Left: pCA1 and mCA1 place cells recorded from rat R0172. A Room A, B Room B. Right: mCA1 and dCA1 place cells recorded from rat R0175. Note, redistribution of firing locations between different rooms. b Heatmaps showing PVCs of all putative pyramidal neurons recorded from each subregion in the same room (top row) and between rooms (middle row), and changes in PVC between room pairs (bottom row). c Degree of changes in PVC between condition pairs, with outliers omitted. pCA1: 0.678 (0.609–0.731), mCA1: 0.452 (0.367–0.535), and dCA1: 0.388 (0.334–0.441); Kruskal–Wallis test, H = 679.871, P < 0.001, η2 = 0.566, n = 1200 bins, Holm–Bonferroni post hoc tests, ***P < 0.001. d Left: distribution of SC in CA1 subdivisions in the same room, and in different rooms (Room A vs B). Two-way repeated-measures ANOVA, room: F (1, 758) = 1175.415, P < 0.001, η2 = 0.608; band: F (2, 758) = 13.538, P < 0.001, η2 = 0.034; room × band: F (2, 758) = 27.266, P < 0.001, η2 = 0.067. Right: change in SC between condition pairs, with outliers omitted. pCA1, 0.455 (0.256–0.747), mCA1, 0.354 (0.151–0.527), and dCA1, 0.287 (0.095–0.504); Kruskal–Wallis test, n = 761 cells, H = 41.735, P < 0.001, η2 = 0.055; Holm–Bonferroni post hoc tests, ***P < 0.001. e Pearson correlation between tetrode position and changes in SC across condition pairs (54 tetrodes). Each cross corresponds to the average data of a single tetrode. Blue curve, linear regression; red curve, quadratic polynomial regression. f Scatter plot showing Pearson correlation between the spatial information rate of individual neurons and changes in SC in the two-room task. Each dot represents data from an individual place cell. Neurons located in pCA1, mCA1, and dCA1 are color-coded. Dashed line: linear regression. g Scatter plot showing Pearson correlation between spatial information rate and changes in SC in the two-room task, based on data from 54 tetrodes. Each cross corresponds to the average data of a single tetrode. Linear regression is shown in blue with the Pearson correlation coefficient and P value provided.
To further examine spatial sensitivity at the level of individual place cells, we analyzed population overlap across CA1 subregions, which remained consistent (Fig. S9e, f). The SC of individual place cells, which is sensitive to rate map reorganization, was high for repeated trials within the same room in all CA1 subregions but decreased markedly between rooms (Fig. 3d, left), indicating considerable shifts in firing location across rooms. Between-room SC tended to be higher in mCA1 and dCA1 than in pCA1 (Fig. S9g). The decrease in SC with room changes was most pronounced in pCA1 (Fig. 3d, right) and declined progressively along the proximodistal axis, as reflected by a strong linear correlation between SC decline and tetrode location along CA1 (Fig. 3e). Similar patterns emerged when using RD to assess neuronal activity levels (Fig. S9h, i), as well as in animals with multi-subregion recordings (Fig. S9j). Furthermore, SC variations were significantly correlated with degree of spatial tuning (Figs. 3f, g and S9k, l), implying a significant contribution of direct MEC inputs to landmark coding within pCA1.
Overall, place cell representations were most orthogonalized in pCA1 when salient landmarks were altered, with orthogonality decreasing monotonically along the transverse axis, consistent with previous research37. The enhanced discrimination between spatial representations in pCA1 appears to be a consequence of activity realignment of grid, head direction, and border cells within the MEC16,45.
Enhanced rate coding in dCA1 in response to object-replacement evoked events
Events occurring within the spatial context are specific occurrences or actions that are distinct from the general characteristics of the environment40. To assess whether the dCA1 region exhibited increased sensitivity to events within the environment, we replaced one of the familiar objects with a novel one (Fig. S10a), without altering its location, so that its potential for position marking remains unchanged17. Although the rats did not spend excessive amount of time around the novel object (Fig. S10b), a substantial subset of the 1451 place cells (Fig. S10c) recorded from 14 rats in 54 experiments adjusted their firing rates in response to the novel object (Fig. 4a), with dCA1 exhibiting the highest proportion of cells responsive to object identity (Fig. 4b). To control for potential hysteresis effects observed in trial 4 (Fig. S10d), our analysis focused on firing rate shifts between trials 1 and 2 (object identity switched) and between trials 2 and 3 (object identity unchanged).
Fig. 4. Distal CA1 preferentially represented changes in object identity.
a Representative neurons in proximal, middle, and distal bands of CA1, showing remapping to the novel object. The circle and star represent familiar and novel objects, respectively. Fam, trial with two familiar objects; Nov, trial with the right object replaced with a novel object. b Distribution of neurons in response to the novel object across three CA1 bands. Proportion of cells responsive to object-replacement: pCA1, 37.84%, mCA1, 41.15%, and dCA1, 53.02%; chi-square test, χ2 = 24.602, P < 0.001, Cramer’s V = 0.130; Holm–Bonferroni post hoc tests, ***P < 0.001. c Left: distributions of RD in CA1 subregions between trials with a novel object (Same) and trials before and after object replacement (Obj F vs N). Two-way repeated-measures ANOVA, novelty F (1, 1448) = 58.170, P < 0.001, η2 = 0.039; band: F (2, 1448) = 10.527, P < 0.001, η2 = 0.014; novelty × band: F (2, 1448) = 6.169, P = 0.002, η2 = 0.008. Right: changes in RD between condition pairs, with outliers omitted. pCA1, 0.008 (−0.101 to 0.120), mCA1, 0.036 (−0.064 to 0.162), and dCA1, 0.054 (−0.085 to 0.225); Kruskal–Wallis test, n = 1451 cells, H = 13.604, P = 0.001, η2 = 0.008; Holm–Bonferroni post hoc tests, *P < 0.05; ***P < 0.001. d Pearson correlation between tetrode position and changes in RD between condition pairs (n = 67 tetrodes). Each cross corresponds to the average data of a single tetrode. Blue curve, linear regression; red curve, quadratic polynomial regression. e Heatmaps showing PVCs of all putative pyramidal neurons recorded from each subregion between trials (top two rows), as well as changes in PVC between trial pairs (bottom row) in the object-replacement task. Small circle and star represent familiar and novel objects, respectively; the large circle indicates region of the novel object. f Degree of changes in overall PVC between trial pairs across CA1 bands, with outliers omitted. pCA1, 0.031 (0.006–0.065), mCA1, 0.040 (0.023–0.061), and dCA1, 0.047 (0.014–0.079); Kruskal–Wallis test, H = 19.995, P < 0.001, η2 = 0.015, n = 1200 bins; Holm–Bonferroni post hoc tests, **P ≤ 0.01; ***P ≤ 0.001. g Degree of changes in PVC inside and outside ROI between trial pairs across CA1 bands, with outliers omitted. Bins inside ROI: pCA1, 0.005 (−0.014–0.028), mCA1, 0.034 (0.021–0.046), and dCA1, 0.070 (0.053–0.082). Bins outside ROI: pCA1, 0.035 (0.009–0.069), mCA1, 0.041 (0.023–0.064), and dCA1, 0.044 (0.012–0.078); Population responses inside and outside ROI were nearly significantly different: two-way ANOVA: band: F (2, 796) = 30.240, P < 0.001, η2 = 0.071; ROI: F (1, 398) = 3.681, P = 0.056, η2 = 0.009; band × ROI: F (2, 796) = 17.478, P < 0.001, η2 = 0.042. Holm–Bonferroni post hoc tests across CA1 subregions, *P < 0.05; ***P ≤ 0.001.
Due to minimal changes in activity patterns, no significant regional differences in SC changes were detected across trials in the novel-object task (Fig. S10e). The overall changes in firing rate for individual place cells were marginally but significantly enhanced in trial pairs before and after object replacement, with the degree of enhancement strongly dependent on the location of place cells. Neurons in pCA1 exhibited the lowest increase in RD, followed by mCA1 and dCA1 (Fig. 4c). RD enhancements were significantly correlated with recording positions (Fig. 4d). The linear changes in RD were not related to sample size (Fig. S10f), or overall time spent around the novel object (Fig. S10g), or the degree of spatial tuning of the place cells (Fig. S11a–c). A similar linear increase was also present in animals with multi-subregion recordings (Fig. S11d). Consistent patterns were obtained for RDf scores calculated for individual place fields (Fig. S10h, i).
The ensemble activity of all pyramidal neurons, as measured by PVC, demonstrated a marginal decrease following object replacement, with a significant interaction between novelty and CA1 subregions (Figs. 4e and S10d). Cell assemblies within pCA1 exhibited the smallest response to the novel object (Fig. 4f), while Bayesian decoding showed that context prediction accuracy was higher for cells in mCA1 than in dCA1 (Fig. S10j). Unlike the object-placement task, population activity reorganization in response to the novel object was less pronounced and less localized across CA1 subregions (Fig. 4e). The regional differences in response were primarily attributed to subtle differences in activity changes around the object, with ensemble activity remaining largely stable outside the object region (Figs. 4g and S10k). In addition, CA3 showed a minimal overall but highly localized response to the novel object (Fig. S10l, m), different from those observed in CA1.
In conclusion, place cells in dCA1 displayed greater firing rate redistribution following object replacement, likely due to preferential input from object-identity-responsive cells in the LEC and potentially from the perirhinal cortex (PER)22,23,46,47, while inputs from CA3 may also contribute to this process.
Object-translocation elicited stronger population coding in dCA1
Assuming that the differential encoding of landmarks and events in pCA1 and dCA1 regions remains largely constant during episodic memory formation29–32,37, we next examined whether a relocated familiar object within a known environment would contribute to spatial mapping or be encoded as an event. To test this, we translocated a familiar object to a different quadrant of the recording box (Fig. S12a). If the object served as a landmark, we anticipated increased activity in pCA1; alternatively, if the translocation was processed as an event, a stronger population response would be expected in dCA1. Despite the overall constant discrimination index (DI), there was a transient increase in the discrimination score over the displaced object during the first minute, indicating that the rats had noticed the relocation (Fig. S12b).
A substantial subset of 1486 CA1 place cells recorded from 14 rats in 59 experiments adjusted their firing patterns, with many neurons displaying new firing fields around the relocated object (current-location coding, CLC), or near its original position (original-location coding, OLC), reminiscent of object-trace cells reported in the LEC24 (Fig. 5a). Notably, although the dCA1 region had the lowest proportion of place cells (Fig. S12c), it contained the highest proportion of OLC and CLC neurons, as well as neurons with emergent or disappearing fields outside the region around the relocated object (ROI) (Fig. 5b). A minority of cells maintained consistent firing at a fixed distance and direction relative to the relocated object, with or without trace activity remaining in its initial location after relocation, similar to previously reported OVCs in the MEC17. However, these OVC-like neurons were rare (<2.2% for all subregions) and did not differ across CA1 subregions (Fig. 5b).
Fig. 5. Object translocation induced a stronger population response in dCA1.
a Representative neurons in proximal, middle, and distal bands of CA1, showing responses to object displacement. Circles represent objects. Fam, objects in familiar locations; Reloc, trial with the right object relocated. b Distribution of neurons in response to object translocation across three CA1 bands. Proportion of cells that showed field changes to the translocation: pCA1, 29.52%, mCA1, 36.65%, and dCA1, 54.42%; chi-square test, χ2 = 61.433, P < 0.001, Cramer’s V = 0.203; Holm–Bonferroni post hoc tests, ***P < 0.001. Subgroups: OVC-like neurons, Fisher’s exact test, P = 0.338; other relocation-responsive cells, chi-square test, all P < 0.05. c Left: distribution of SC in CA1 subregions between trials with relocated objects (Same obj location) and trials before and after object relocation (Obj location A vs B). Two-way repeated-measures ANOVA, relocation: F (1, 1483) = 153.986, P < 0.001, η2 = 0.094; band: F (2, 1483) = 21.400, P < 0.001, η2 = 0.028; relocation × band: F (2, 1483) = 4.917, P = 0.007, η2 = 0.007. Right: degree of changes in SC between condition pairs, with outliers omitted. pCA1, 0.024 (−0.026 to 0.121), mCA1, 0.049 (−0.021 to 0.175), and dCA1, 0.069 (−0.027 to 0.233); Kruskal–Wallis test, n = 1486 cells, H = 10.123, P = 0.006, η² = 0.007; Holm–Bonferroni post hoc tests, **P < 0.01. d Pearson correlation between recording position and changes in SC between condition pairs (69 tetrodes). Each cross corresponds to the average data of a single tetrode. Blue curve, linear regression; red curve, quadratic polynomial regression. e Similar to C but showing changes in RD across CA1 subdivisions. Left: relocation: F (1, 1483) = 147.858, P < 0.001, η2 = 0.091; band: F (2, 1483) = 20.702, P < 0.001, η2 = 0.027; relocation × band: F (2, 1483) = 2.609, P = 0.074, η2 = 0.004. Right: pCA1, 0.044 (−0.073 to 0.205), mCA1, 0.057 (−0.057 to 0.221), and dCA1, 0.112 (−0.056 to 0.308); H = 9.346, P = 0.009, η2 = 0.005; post hoc tests, *P < 0.05. f Similar to D but showing correlations between tetrode position and changes in RD between condition pairs. g Heatmaps showing PVCs of all putative pyramidal neurons recorded from each subregion between trials (top two rows), as well as changes in PVC between trial pairs (bottom row) in object-relocation task. Small circles represent objects, large circles indicate the target region. h Degree of changes in overall PVC between trial pairs across CA1 bands, with outliers omitted. pCA1, 0.055 (0.014–0.111), mCA1, 0.068 (0.030–0.127), dCA1, 0.077 (0.043–0.138); Kruskal–Wallis test, H = 44.493, P < 0.001, η2 = 0.035, n = 1200 bins; Holm–Bonferroni post hoc tests, ***P ≤ 0.001. i Degree of changes in PVC inside and outside ROI between trial pairs across CA1 bands, with outliers omitted. Bins inside ROI: pCA1, 0.081 (0.054–0.114), mCA1, 0.212 (0.147–0.269), and dCA1, 0.225 (0.155–0.303). Bins outside ROI: pCA1, 0.043 (0.009–0.106), mCA1, 0.049 (0.026–0.091), and dCA1, 0.063 (0.038–0.101); Population responses inside and outside ROI were significantly different: two-way ANOVA: band: F (2, 796) = 146.927, P < 0.001, η2 = 0.270; ROI: F (1, 398) = 422.712, P < 0.001, η2 = 0.515; band × ROI: F (2, 796) = 99.646, P < 0.001, η2 = 0.200. Holm–Bonferroni post hoc tests across subregions, *P < 0.05; ***P ≤ 0.001.
As anticipated from the distribution of relocation-responsive cell types, the dCA1 region displayed a stronger population response to object relocation compared to its proximal counterpart. The SC of individual place cells was high when objects were in the same location (trials 2 vs 3) but slightly reduced after object relocation (trials 1 vs 2) across all CA1 subregions. The reduction in SC varied significantly by subregion, with the smallest decrease in pCA1 and the largest decrease in dCA1 (Fig. 5c). A significant correlation was observed between SC change and tetrode location (Fig. 5d). The increase in RD between trial pairs mirrored the SC decrease across CA1 subregions and increased progressively from pCA1 to dCA1 (Fig. 5e). A significant correlation was also found between the shift in RD for individual tetrodes and their recording positions (Fig. 5f). These spatially graded changes along the CA1 transverse axis were unlikely to be influenced by overall time spent around the displaced object (Fig. S12d), sample size (Fig. S12e, f), degree of spatial modulation (Fig. S13), or inter-animal variations (Fig. S12g). Consistent findings were observed when RDf scores were calculated for individual place fields (Fig. S12h, i).
Correlations between corresponding PVs across spatial bins displayed similar changes to those observed in SC (Figs. 5g and S12j). Notably, the decline in PVC between trial pairs progressively increased along the CA1 transverse axis, from pCA1 to dCA1 (Fig. 5h). Bayesian decoding of the three experiments, involving over 30 simultaneously recorded effective place cells in both subregions, revealed a marked improvement in position predictions within proximally located CA1 regions, indicating lower population responses (Fig. S12k). Furthermore, significant reductions in PVC were detected for bins within the ROI in the distal two-thirds of CA1, while pCA1 remained minimally affected (Figs. 5i and S12l). Together, the minimal reorganization in pCA1 suggests that the relocated object was not assimilated into the spatial map as a fixed landmark. The pronounced response in dCA1 may reflect encounter events following object dislocation, while other inputs, such as memory traces presented by LEC, may also contribute to this proximodistal difference24.
Distinct inputs shape responses at original and relocated object locations
Given that the stronger dCA1 response to object relocation may be shaped by convergent inputs from different sources, we next examined the contributions of specific neuronal subpopulations and different components to this process by systematically removing each subgroup from the overall population analysis. Excluding OVC-like neurons, which were more responsive to landmarks, led to a graded increase in population response to object relocation (Fig. 6a). The decline in PVC around the relocated object before and after relocation increased progressively from pCA1 to dCA1 (Fig. 6b). Since activity changes at the original and current object locations may be more related to memory traces of past experience and ongoing exploratory experience, respectively, we analyzed the population responses at these positions separately. We found similar trends of increasing activity along the CA1 transverse axis at both locations (Fig. 6c). The elevated response in dCA1, in combination with exploratory behavior at the current object location, suggests the coding of encounter events to the displaced object.
Fig. 6. Distinct contributions of neuron subpopulations during event coding.
a Heatmaps showing PVCs of place cells excluding OVC-like neurons recorded from each subregion between trials (top two rows), as well as changes in PVC between trial pairs (bottom row) in object-relocation task. Small circles represent objects, large circles indicate the target region. b Degree of changes in PVC within ROI between trial pairs across CA1 bands, with outliers omitted. pCA1, 0.075 (0.043–0.110), mCA1, 0.127 (0.081–0.187), dCA1, 0.244 (0.183–0.308); Kruskal–Wallis test, H = 128.394, P < 0.001, η2 = 0.533, n = 240 bins; Holm–Bonferroni post hoc tests, ***P < 0.001. c Degree of changes in PVC at original and current locations between trial pairs across CA1 bands, with outliers omitted. Original location: pCA1, 0.089 (0.058–0.125), mCA1, 0.130 (0.091–0.201), and dCA1, 0.285 (0.229–0.319). Current location: pCA1, 0.069 (0.022–0.090), mCA1, 0.108 (0.069–0.170), and dCA1, 0.198 (0.147–0.291); Population responses at the original and current locations were significantly different: two-way ANOVA: band: F (2, 156) = 182.131, P < 0.001, η2 = 0.700; location: F (1, 78) = 17.137, P < 0.001, η2 = 0.180; band × location: F (2, 156) = 1.187, P = 0.308. Holm–Bonferroni post hoc tests across subregions, ***P < 0.001. d Heatmaps showing changes in PVC for population of place cells without OLC neurons between trial pairs, as well as reductions in PVC change (compared to the bottom panel in (a) after exclusion for CA1 subregions. Circles indicate ROI. e Reductions in PVC at original and current locations after exclusion of OLC neurons across CA1 bands, with outliers omitted. Original location: pCA1, 0.077 (0.038–0.141), mCA1, 0.080 (0.052–0.105), and dCA1, 0.045 (0.020–0.077). Current location: pCA1, 0.008 (0.003–0.021), mCA1, −0.002 ( − 0.010–0.008), and dCA1, −0.001 (−0.008 to 0.002). Two-way ANOVA: band: F (2, 156) = 47.814, P < 0.001, η2 = 0.380; location: F (1, 78) = 121.416, P < 0.001, η2 = 0.609; band × location: F (2, 156) = 16.810, P < 0.001, η2 = 0.177. Holm–Bonferroni post hoc tests at original location, ***P < 0.001. f Normalized PVC change after OLC neuron exclusion at original location across CA1 bands, with outliers omitted. pCA1, 0.901 (0.417–1.000), mCA1, 0.547 (0.416–0.622), and dCA1, 0.163 (0.077–0.245); Kruskal–Wallis test, H = 71.904, P < 0.001, η2 = 0.597, n = 120 bins. Holm–Bonferroni post hoc tests, ***P < 0.001. g–i Similar to (d–f), but shows changes in PVC for population of place cells without CLC neurons (g), reductions in PVC (h). OL: pCA1, −0.0003 (−0.0007 to 0.0002), mCA1, 0.001 (−0.003 to 0.005), and dCA1, 0.011 (−0.001 to 0.025). CL: pCA1, 0.021 (0.010–0.031), mCA1, 0.034 (0.023–0.050), and dCA1, 0.056 (0.033–0.092); Two-way ANOVA: band: F (2, 156) = 44.460, P < 0.001, η² = 0.363; location: F (1, 78) = 150.555, P < 0.001, η² = 0.659; band × location: F (2, 156) = 15.285, P < 0.001, η² = 0.164. Holm–Bonferroni post hoc tests at CL, all P < 0.001, and normalized PVC change at current location (i). pCA1, 0.290 (0.183–0.404), mCA1, 0.389 (0.178–0.523), and dCA1, 0.270 (0.107–0.630); Kruskal–Wallis test, H = 0.658, P = 0.720, n = 120 bins. Holm–Bonferroni post hoc tests, n.s. not significant; *P < 0.05; **P < 0.01, ***P < 0.001.
Further removal of OLC neurons led to significant changes at the original location but not the current location (Fig. 6d, e). Notably, the population response to object removal at original object location was predominantly driven by a small fraction of OLC neurons in pCA1 (3.08% of cells contributed to 94.61% of activity changes), while a larger fraction of OLC neurons in dCA1 (8.08%) contributed considerably less to activity change (17.44%) (Fig. 6f), suggesting the rapid involvement of other neuronal subgroups in representing past experience from pCA1 to dCA1. Conversely, excluding CLC neurons led to only mild reductions in PVC change at the current location (Fig. 6g). Although activity changes showed a graded increase (Fig. 6h), the relative reduction in population response due to CLC neuron exclusion remained low (28.56%–35.37%) and was consistent along the transverse axis (Fig. 6i), implying that neuronal populations coding for the displaced object were relatively stable across CA1. Consider the profound activity change observed at the original location and the weaker response at the current location in CA3 (Fig. S14a, b), the distinct population coding at the original and current object locations in CA1 may be partially shaped by CA3 inputs. Excluding neurons with field changes outside the ROI minimally affected responses at either original or current object location (Fig. S14c, d). Overall, the graded population response in CA1 appears to be shaped by signals from multiple sources, with CA3 inputs strongly affecting responses at the original location, reflecting memory traces, and other inputs influencing responses at the current location, closely related to ongoing events.
Discussion
The integration of salient objects in episodic memory formation remains a subject of ongoing debate. Here, we performed a comprehensive analysis of population-level responses across the CA1 proximodistal axis to evaluate the encoding of newly introduced objects in the recording environment, alongside traditional manipulations of spatial and non-spatial environmental cues. Results revealed that object placement triggered localized firing rate changes near the objects and elicited population-level responses similar to those elicited by non-geometric contextual changes (Figs. 1 and 2). Furthermore, by altering spatial layout and object identity, we validated the segregated encoding of landmarks and events in pCA1 and dCA1 (Figs. 3 and 4). Finally, our final experiment indicated that pCA1 neurons exhibited minimal activity changes in response to object translocation, while more distal neurons along the transverse axis displayed diverse responses to the displaced object (Figs. 5 and 6). Collectively, these findings suggest that changes in object location, especially for objects that are later introduced into a familiar environment, appeared to be processed as transient events rather than a shift of landmarks for navigation.
Our findings that pCA1, which receives strong MEC inputs, exhibited minimum response to object relocation (Fig. 5), challenge the traditional view that object-location memory (OLM) relies predominantly on the MEC over the LEC48. While impairments in OLM have been linked to extensive lesions in both the MEC and LEC49, or complete MEC lesions and inactivation of MEC stellate cells50–52, LEC-specific lesions appear to have no effect on OLM performance53,54. These discrepancies can be explained by the Hebbian plasticity theory, which posits that the formation of a cognitive map with accurate positional information is highly dependent on repeated experience with landmarks55. In standard object-location tasks, brief habituation may not allow for the complete development of a cognitive map, leading to the incorporation of later introduced objects as prominent landmarks. In our study, however, objects were introduced after weeks of intensive training, resulting in event coding tied to a well-established spatial map referencing other established landmarks. An alternative hypothesis is that both objects were encoded as landmarks from the outset, yet the landmark feature of the right object diminished soon after frequent manipulations, as the same animals were exposed to the object-related tasks multiple times. While both hypotheses suggest that landmark coding in the hippocampus is experience-dependent, the MEC is capable of prompting immediate grid cell reorganization following landmark changes, which may stem from modified firing patterns in entorhinal OVCs that convey allocentric vector information about landmarks17,56. How MEC-driven changes are selectively and flexibly reflected in the hippocampus based on past experience remains an area that requires vigorous exploration.
The enhanced population response to object relocation observed in dCA1 likely arises from convergent inputs from multiple brain regions containing different information components. The event-related and memory-dependent activities are evident in the pronounced activity changes in dCA1 observed at the current and original object locations, respectively (Figs. 5 and 6), which may rely heavily on LEC inputs. Subpopulations of LEC neurons are tuned to fire at specific object locations23, which may contribute to event coding at the current object location. In the meantime, object-trace cells within the LEC, which fire exclusively at locations previously occupied by an object, may convey past experiences to dCA124, thus contributing to activity changes at the original location. Furthermore, the PER, which projects to the LEC and dCA1, contains object-responsive cells similar to those found in the LEC27,46,47, may also be involved in this process. Additional object-place signals may also originate from subicular dCA1-projecting neurons, which modulate place cell responses to object displacement57. Recently identified subicular corner cells58 may further contribute to this heterogeneity, although their back-projection to dCA1 remains to be determined.
While the proximal and distal portions of CA1 have been shown to preferentially represent spatial and non-spatial content of episodic memory, respectively29–34, the non-geometric contextual information in the hippocampus is believed to be conveyed primarily from the LEC. This belief is supported by evidence that certain LEC neurons undergo remapping in response to contextual changes in the environment, and lesions in the LEC have been shown to partially impair hippocampal rate coding22,42. However, in the current study, we observed a distinct V-shaped rate remapping pattern in the color-reversal task (Fig. 2), with the strongest population response to non-geometric changes in pCA1. This unexpected finding suggests that non-geometric contextual cues are significantly distinct from other non-spatial elements of episodic memory. While temporal and experiential information is preferentially encoded in the LEC and dCA12,41, non-geometric cues appear to be integrated with the spatial layout of the environment and represented holistically by the MEC and pCA1. Additional support for this notion comes from studies demonstrating that, in addition to changes in spatial layout, MEC grid cells also exhibit reliable rate redistribution in response to non-geometric contextual manipulations59–61.
Based on previous reports and results from the current study, the present conceptual dichotomy of the MEC providing spatial information and the LEC providing non-spatial information to the hippocampus requires revision. It appears that the MEC and LEC do not exclusively, but rather preferentially, process spatial and non-spatial information. In addition to representing location within the spatial environment, the MEC can also sense non-spatial contextual changes59–61. Conversely, in addition to encoding of time and non-spatial sensory details, the LEC also conveys spatial information about egocentric distances and directions to individual objects62. The V-shaped pattern of rate remapping along the CA1 transverse axis may reflect the relative dominance of distinct MEC and LEC inputs. Neurons in pCA1 or dCA1, predominantly receiving inputs from either the MEC or LEC, exhibit pronounced rate remapping, whereas mCA1 neurons, receiving balanced inputs from both the MEC and LEC, display weaker rate remapping due to dynamic interaction between their competing signals.
While the EC significantly influences the transverse organization of CA1 function, topographical projections from the CA3 and CA2 regions can also play a role. However, unlike the entorhinal inputs, the functional gradients within the CA1 region (Fig. 7) do not mirror the patterns observed along the CA3-CA2 axis. Neurons in pCA3 are characterized by sharp and confined place fields, strong rate remapping, and orthogonalized global remapping, whereas neurons in dCA3 and CA2 exhibit reduced spatial modulation and lower sensitivity to environmental changes36,37,43,63. Interestingly, axons from dCA3 and CA2 preferentially innervate pCA1 neurons, which display enhanced spatial content and pronounced contextual responsiveness. In contrast, pCA3 heavily projects to dCA1, which is characterized by less spatial selectivity and reduced cue-sensitivity27,64. This discrepancy between the functional segregation and projection patterns of CA3-CA2 and CA1 suggests counterbalanced roles of CA3-CA2 and EC inputs along the CA1 transverse axis. In the current study, the segregated entorhinal inputs appeared to exert more influence than the CA3 inputs (Figs. 2 and 3). Our findings suggest the need for further investigation into the causal dynamics linking selective upstream inputs to specific downstream representations. Future theoretical and experimental endeavors should aim to elucidate the precise mechanisms that govern these interactions, providing crucial insights into the neural basis of spatial navigation, episodic memory, and the broader cognitive functions of the hippocampus.
Fig. 7. Summary of mnemonic functions and related afferent connections that varied along CA1 transverse axis.
The three distinct patterns of population coding that differ along CA1 proximodistal axis. The medial entorhinal cortex is responsible for processing information related to navigation, such as direction and distance to landmarks, position, borders, and speed. In contrast, the lateral entorhinal cortex supplies information pertinent to ongoing experience, including egocentric object vectors, object identity, memory traces, odors and time.
This study has two primary limitations. First, CA1 neuronal activity was recorded using a hyperdrive with movable tetrodes, constraining our capacity to capture activity from a large population of neurons across CA1 subregions simultaneously in individual rats. This restriction may have introduced inter-animal variability, potentially confounding the interpretation of our results. Second, the complexity in CA1 extends to its radial organization into superficial and deep sublayers, each with distinct characteristics and functions65. Our tetrode-based methodology was unable to differentiate between these sublayers. As such, future studies should incorporate two-photon calcium imaging and multi-shank probe recording to monitor neural activity within specific sublayers. These advanced techniques could provide further insights into laminar contributions to functional heterogeneity along the CA1 transverse axis, leading to a more nuanced understanding of landmark and/or event coding within the hippocampus.
Materials and methods
Subjects
The experiments were conducted at the Kunming Institute of Zoology, Chinese Academy of Sciences (Kunming, China) in compliance with the guidelines for the care and use of laboratory animals. The study received approval from the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee of the Kunming Institute of Zoology (KIZ-IACUC-RE-2021-06-019). We have complied with all relevant ethical regulations for animal use. Data were collected from 24 male Long-Evans rats (3–4 months old, 500–600 g at the time of implantation). Five rats from a previous study43 contributed proximal-to-middle CA1 data. The remaining 19 rats were implanted with a multi-tetrode “hyperdrive” targeting the right hippocampus. The animals were maintained on a reversed 12-h light/dark cycle (lights off from 9.00 a.m. to 9.00 p.m.) under standard laboratory conditions (temperature: 20–23 °C, relative humidity: 40%–60%). Following surgery, the rats were housed individually, and experimental procedures were conducted during the dark phase. During the experimental period, the animals were mildly food restricted to maintain their weight at 85%–90% of free-feeding body weight. The 19 rats and the 5 rats from the previous study were trained and tested under identical protocols, ensuring consistency in experimental design and execution.
Electrode preparation
The multi-tetrode “hyperdrive”, consisting of 18 independently movable tetrodes made of 17 µm polyimide-coated platinum-iridium (90%-10%) wire (California Fine Wires, USA), was prepared as described previously66. The electrode tips were plated to reduce impedance to 150–250 kΩ at 1 kHz (Biomega, Bio-Signal Technologies, China) and sterilized with ethylene oxide (Sanqiang Medical, China) within 48 h before implantation.
Surgery
Rats were anesthetized with isoflurane (air flow: 0.8–1.0 L/min, 0.5–3% isoflurane vol/vol mixed with oxygen), with the concentration adjusted based on physiological monitoring (RWD Life Science, China). Upon induction of anesthesia, meloxicam (2 mg/mL, 1 mg/kg body weight), enrofloxacin (50 mg/mL, 5 mg/kg), and atropine (0.5 mg/mL, 0.1 mg/kg) were administered subcutaneously. The animal’s head was then fixed in a stereotaxic frame (RWD Life Science, China). Local anesthetic (2% lidocaine, 200 μL) was applied under the skin before making the incision. The skull and dura were removed, followed by immediate hyperdrive implantation. Tetrodes were inserted at the cortical surface above the right hippocampus (CA1: anteroposterior (AP) from 3.2 to 5.6 mm, mediolateral (ML) from 1.0 to 4.5 mm relative to bregma; CA3: AP: 4.2–4.9 mm, ML: 2.4–4.0 mm). The hyperdrive was secured to the skull with jewelers’ screws and dental cement. Grounding was achieved by connecting two screws above the cerebellum to the hyperdrive ground, while two reference tetrodes were positioned in an inactive area of the cortex for differential recording.
Electrophysiological recordings
For the first 3–4 weeks post-surgery, the tetrodes were advanced daily until they reached the dorsal CA1 pyramidal cell layer, characterized by large-amplitude theta-modulated complex-spike activity. On the recording day, the electrodes were kept stationary to ensure stable recordings. Broadband neural signals were captured from the hyperdrive using a multichannel data acquisition system (Zeus, Bio-Signal Technologies, China). Unit activity was amplified (×192) and band-pass filtered (300–7500 Hz). Spike waveforms that exceeded the −50 μV threshold were time-stamped and digitized at 30 kHz for 1 ms. Local field potential (LFP) signals, one per tetrode, were recorded in the 0.3–300 Hz frequency band at a sampling rate of 1000 Hz. The animal’s position was monitored in real-time by tracking light-emitting diodes (LEDs) on the headstage at 50 Hz using a Cyclops video tracking system (Bio-Signal Technologies, China).
Behavioral paradigms
Each task consisted of four 10-min exploration trials within the recording box, interspersed with five 5-min rest periods in a flowerpot placed on a pedestal outside the box. The exploration trials were designed in an A-B-B-A configuration, where A and B represented distinct test conditions. To encourage exploration, cookie crumbs were randomly scattered throughout the recording box. One week after surgery, the mildly food-restricted rats were trained daily in the color-reversal task in two separate rooms for ~approximately 3 weeks until they could traverse the entire arena within 10 min in all trials. Once the tetrodes reached the CA1 cell layer, all the animals underwent sequential testing and repeated recording in the color-reversal and two-room tasks for about a week. From the fifth week, a sub-population of 19 rats were first test in the object-placement task on Day 1, followed by either the object-relocation task, or the novel-object task on Day 2, and the remaining object-manipulation task on Day 3. This set of object-related tasks was repeated 3–4 times, with the order of the object-relocation and novel-object tasks counterbalanced. Repeated experiments were included for analysis only when the recording positions were at least 40 μm apart to minimize overlap between cell populations.
Test environments
Recording boxes (100 × 100 cm) consisted of a metal frame and four individually exchangeable plastic walls (black on one side, white on the other; 100 × 50 cm). Cue cards (white cue card for black walls and vice versa; 50 × 30 cm) were placed at the center of one wall. Distal background cues were obscured by curtains encircling the recording box (120 × 120 cm) for the color-reversal and object-related tasks but not for the two-room task. The pedestal with the flowerpot was positioned between the test box and the experimenter, outside the curtains. The recording rooms for the two-room task were similar in size but featured different spatial arrangements.
Color-reversal task
As detailed previously, this task was designed to elicit rate remapping in the hippocampus39,42,43. Rats were tested in the recording box at a fixed location over four trials daily, with trials 1 and 4 conducted with black walls and trials 2 and 3 conducted with white walls. The floor of the recording box was cleaned with 75% ethanol after each trial.
Two-room task
This task was designed to evoke global remapping in the hippocampus39,43. Rats were tested in similar recording boxes with black walls located at fixed locations in two separate recording rooms, with trials 1 and 4 conducted in room A and trials 2 and 3 conducted in room B. The floor of the recording box was cleaned with 75% ethanol after each trial.
Tasks with objects
Object-placement task: Animals were tested in the black recording box at a fixed location over four trials, with two identical familiar objects (glass bottles filled with white stones) placed in quadrants 3 and 4 of the box for trials 2 and 3. Novel-object task: Animals were tested in the black recording box containing two objects over four trials, with the familiar object in quadrant 4 replaced by a novel object in trials 2 and 3. Object-relocation task: Animals were tested in the black recording box containing two familiar objects over four trials, with the object in quadrant 4 relocated to quadrant 1 for trials 2 and 3. For each task, the floor of the recording box and the objects were thoroughly cleaned with 75% ethanol before each trial.
Occupation time and discrimination index
Occupation time (OT) and DI were employed to quantify preference for objects in the object-placement task and for relocated/novel objects in the object-relocation and novel-object tasks. OT was defined as the proportion of time spent around the objects relative to the total time spent in the recording box, and DI was calculated as:
where Tr is the time spent around the relocated/replaced object and Tu is the time spent around the unchanged object. A higher DI value indicates a greater preference for the relocated/novel object.
Histology and recording sites
Following the completion of electrophysiological recordings, the electrodes were maintained in their positions. The rats were subsequently euthanized and perfused with formalin for brain extraction. Frozen brain sections (40 μm thick) were cut on a cryostat (KEDEE, KD-2950, China), and stained using cresyl-violet (Sigma Aldrich, USA, CAS: 10510-54-0, Cat# C5042-10G). the Nissl-stained brain sections were imaged using a bright-field microscope (2×/0.06, 4×/0.13 NA objectives, COIC, UB103i, China), with UOPView software (v2.0). The location of each tetrode tip was then identified and recorded. Hippocampal subregions were delineated based on established criteria27,35. ImageJ (v1.52a, RRID:SCR_00307067) was used to estimate the normalized position along the proximodistal axis of the CA1 region for each recording site. Hand-drawn lines tracing the CA1 pyramidal cell layer were extended from the CA2/CA1 border to the CA1/subiculum border, passing through the recording site on each section. The distance from the CA2/CA1 border to the recording site was measured and normalized by the total length of the CA1 region on the same section. Based on these normalized positions, recording sites were categorized into proximal (pCA1, 0–0.33), middle (mCA1, 0.33–0.67), and distal (dCA1, 0.67–1) subregions.
Spike sorting and cell classification
Spike sorting was manually performed using the MClust graphical cluster-cutting tool (v4.4, A.D. Redish) in Matlab (RRID:SCR_001622). The process involved the analysis of two-dimensional projections of the multidimensional parameter space, consisting of waveform amplitudes, waveform energies, and waveform peak-valley differences. Waveform and autocorrelation functions were also employed as tools for separation and classification. Clustering was applied on the entire dataset of each experiment, with the spikes subsequently divided into four exploration trials.
Putative excitatory cells were distinguished from putative interneurons and bypassing axons based on spike width (peak-to-trough waveform durations greater than 250 μs), average rate (less than 5 Hz), and occasional presence of bursts. Only cells classified as putative excitatory neurons were included in subsequent analyses. Cells recorded repeatedly in the same task were excluded from analyses.
Rate maps and place fields
Position estimates were derived from tracking the LEDs on the headstage connected to the hyperdrive. Data were filtered to include only epochs with instantaneous running speeds between 2.5 and 100 cm/s. The mean firing rate for each cell was calculated by summing the total number of spikes and dividing it by the time spent in that trial. Place field analyses were conducted exclusively for cells with a mean firing rate of 0.10 Hz or higher, defined as active cells. Spatial firing rate distributions (place fields) for each well-isolated neuron were generated by summing the spikes within specific location bins (5 × 5 cm), dividing by the duration of time spent by the rat in that location, and applying Gaussian smoothing centered on each bin, as described previously43. A place field was defined as a contiguous region of at least 225 cm2 (nine or more 5 × 5 cm bins) where the firing rate exceeded 20% of the peak rate and the peak firing rate of the area was 1 Hz or greater. The number of non-overlapping place fields was estimated for each cell.
Neuron subpopulations in the object-relocation task
Neurons classified as OVC-like were characterized by their exclusive activation at a specific distance and direction relative to the displaced object, both before and after its relocation. Furthermore, the center of the receptive field must be situated within a region 20 cm around the object. Neurons designated as original and current location coding cells were identified by the presence or absence of at least one receptive field at the object’s original or current location (within a 20 cm radius), respectively.
Theta modulation
The theta modulation index (TMI) was computed to assess the temporal modulation of firing within the theta frequency range (7–10 Hz) of each cell, as described previously68. A temporal autocorrelogram was constructed by tallying the occurrences of spikes within each 5-ms bin over a range of 0–500 ms from a reference spike at time 0, then dividing by the total trial length to determine the rate of occurrence for each bin. The TMI was calculated as:
where P and T represent the theta modulation peak (mean of bins 100–140 ms) and trough (mean of time bins 50–70 ms), respectively.
Spatial information, coherence, and stability
The spatial information rate in bits per second (SIr) and spatial information content in bits per spike (SIc) were calculated as described in previous research69:
where λi is the mean firing rate of a unit in the ith bin, λ is the overall mean firing rate, and pi is the probability of the animal being in the ith bin (occupancy in the ith bin/total recording time). Pyramidal neurons with SIc scores above the 95th percentile of the shuffled distribution were classified as place cells.
Spatial coherence was estimated as the first-order spatial autocorrelation of the unsmoothed place field map, i.e., mean correlation between the firing rate of each bin and average firing rate in the eight adjacent bins.
Spatial stability was determined by comparing the firing pattern in the first half of each trial to that in the second half. The rate map for each half was smoothed and binned into 5 × 5 cm matrices, and the firing rates in the bins of the two half-trial maps were correlated for each trial.
Spatial correlation (SC)
Firing patterns were compared across trials using an SC procedure. Each map was smoothed and binned into 5 × 5 cm matrices, and the firing rates in the pixels of the two maps were correlated for each cell. SC between two inactive trials (firing rates below 0.1 Hz) was excluded to avoid artifacts. For the two-room task, rate maps were rotated in 90° steps to identify the maximum SC score when compared across rooms.
Rate difference (RD) and rate difference by field (RDf)
The change in overall firing rate across trials was calculated as RD:
where FR1 is the mean firing rate of a cell in one trial and FR2 is the mean firing rate of the same cell in a second trial, with RD = 1 indicating that the cell was active in one trial only and RD = 0 indicating that the firing rates in the two trials were the same.
The RDf was computed to mitigate the neutralization effect across individual place fields from the same cell:
where RDi is the rate difference of the ith place field and wi is the weight of the ith place field estimated from its firing rate. RDf was not computed when changes in firing location occurred.
Population vector correlation (PVC)
Rate vectors were constructed for the entire population of cells recorded under each condition, including those with firing rates below 0.1 Hz and spatial information content below 0.9586 bits/spike. This process involved arranging all rate maps for all cells in an x-y-z stack, with x and y denoting the two spatial dimensions (20 × 20 bins) and z representing the cell-identity index39. For the two-room task, rate maps were rotated analogously to the SC analyses performed for each cell. The distribution of bin rates along the z-axis for a specific x-y location represented the composite population vector for that location. Population vectors from corresponding positions were then correlated across pairs of trials. Normalized PVC change was calculated as:
Shuffling
Shuffling of spatial information was conducted on a cell-by-cell basis, as described previously35. Each CA1 cell recorded from the color-reversal task underwent 100 permutations. In each permutation, the entire sequence of spikes fired by the cell was time-shifted along the animal’s path by a random interval ranging from 20 s to the total trial length minus 20 s. The end of the trial was wrapped back to the beginning to allow for circular displacements. A rate map was generated for each permutation, and the distribution of spatial information values across all permutations for all CA1 cells was determined. A putative excitatory neuron was classified as a place cell if (1) its spatial information content exceeded the 95th percentile of the shuffled distribution (0.9586 bits/spike), and (2) its receptive field(s) fulfilled the criteria for place fields.
For each CA1 band, a control distribution for SC analyses was established using a random permutation procedure. This procedure involved all active maps (mean firing rates higher than 0.10 Hz) from all cells recorded within that band43. For each permutation, two distinct active maps from different cells were randomly selected and correlated in 90° rotation steps to achieve the highest correlation score. In total, 10,000 permutations were performed for each CA1 band. This method preserved the rate maps within each band while eliminating connections between maps. The distribution of random correlations from all 10,000 permutations was then calculated, and the 95th percentile was determined.
Bayesian decoding
A Bayesian decoding algorithm was employed to convert ensemble spiking activity into positional information for each experiment. The decoding process used a 2000 ms sliding time window, advancing by 200 ms with each step. The probability of a rat being at position x, given the number of spikes n from each unit recorded within the time window, was estimated using Bayes’ rule70:
The probabilistic likelihood P(n | x) was calculated using the probability mass function of the Poisson distribution to determine the likelihood of observing a specific number of spikes given the activity pattern of a neuron:
where λ is the expected number of spikes based on the tuning curve and n is the actual observed number of spikes within the given time window. The prior probability of position P(x) was set to the frequency of the animal’s presence in each state within the training data. The normalizing constant P(n) ensured that the posterior probability P(x|n) summed to 1. For visualization and maximum decoding performance estimation, only experiments with over 30 simultaneously recorded place cells in both CA1 bands were considered. Downsampling was applied to equalize cell numbers and repeated three times to determine the mean decoding accuracy value. For position decoding, 90% of the data from the four trials served as the training set, while the remaining 10% was used as the test set. This cross-validation procedure was repeated 10 times (10-fold validation). For context decoding, data from trials 2 and 3 formed the training set, with 90% of trial 1 data serving as the test set. This procedure was also repeated 10 times. Decoding accuracy was expressed as:
Where ŷi and yi are the predicted and true positions of the ith time bin, respectively, and ȳ is the mean position value.
Statistics and reproducibility
Results are presented as medians and interquartile ranges (IQR) due to the non-normal distribution of most data. The Kruskal–Wallis test was used to compare the firing properties of cells across CA1 bands. The Mann–Whitney U test was employed to compare two groups of unrelated measures, while the Wilcoxon signed-rank test and paired t-test were employed to compare two related measures. Regional differences in remapping between two conditions were assessed using two-way repeated-measures ANOVA with a 3 × 2 factor design, considering condition pairs (matching or different) as within-subject factors and CA1 bands as between-subjects factors. Two-way repeated-measures ANOVA was also used when comparing data across recording experiments. The Pearson chi-square test and Fisher’s exact test were applied to compare frequency distributions across groups, and Pearson correlation was calculated to measure the linear correlation between two variables. The binomial test was applied to determine whether the observed frequency differed from the expected frequency. Holm–Bonferroni post hoc tests were used to identify significant differences between pairs. The significance level was set at P < 0.05 for two-tailed tests. All statistical analyses were conducted using SPSS v25 (IBM, USA, RRID:SCR_002865).
Reporting summary
Further information on research design is available in the Nature Portfolio Reporting Summary linked to this article.
Supplementary information
Description of Additional Supplementary Files
Acknowledgements
This research was supported by grants from the Science and Technology Innovation (STI) 2030-Major Projects (2022ZD0205000 to LL), National Natural Science Foundation of China (31970963 to L.L.), CAS “Light of West China” Program (xbzg-zdsys-202404 to L.L.), Yunnan Revitalization Talent Support Program (Yunling Scholar Project to L.L.), Yunnan Province (202305AH340006 to L.L.), and Yunnan Fundamental Research Projects (202101AT070289 to X.C.). The authors express gratitude to Yi-Qing Hui, Yi-Fan Ye, Chang-Lin Shen, Yi-Ling Shi, as well as the staff members of the National Research Facility for Phenotypic & Genetic Analysis of Model Animals (Primate Facility) (https://cstr.cn/31137.02.NPRC), for providing technical support and assistance in data collection and analysis. They also thank Edvard Moser and May-Britt Moser for providing the Behavioral Neurology Toolbox and data from five animals, and Cheng Wang, Xin-Jian Li, Dun Mao, Hai-Bing Xu, and Yong-Gang Yao for their valuable suggestions on the manuscript.
Author contributions
L.L. designed the experiments and analyses; Y.L.D., S.Y.H., X.F.G., J.L.L., C.D., C.J.L., R.Z. and X.C. performed the experiments; Y.L.D., S.Y.H., and L.L. conducted the analyses; Y.M.S. carried out the Bayesian decoding. Y.L.D., X.C. and L.L. wrote the manuscript. All authors contributed to discussion and interpretation of the results. Y.L.D. and S.Y.H. contributed equally to this work.
Peer review
Peer review information
Communications Biology thanks Xiaoxiao Lin and the other, anonymous, reviewer(s) for their contribution to the peer review of this work. Primary Handling Editor: Benjamin Bessieres. [A peer review file is available].
Data availability
All data supporting the findings of this study are available in the Supplementary data file (Supplementary Data 1–3). All other data are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.
Code availability
The custom code supporting this study is available within the Supplementary codes.
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Footnotes
Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
These authors contributed equally: Ya-Li Duan, Shu-Yi Hu.
Supplementary information
The online version contains supplementary material available at 10.1038/s42003-025-08241-2.
References
- 1.Tulving, E. Episodic memory: from mind to brain. Annu Rev. Psychol.53, 1–25 (2002). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Sugar, J. & Moser, M. B. Episodic memory: neuronal codes for what, where, and when. Hippocampus29, 1190–1205 (2019). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Colgin, L. L., Moser, E. I. & Moser, M. B. Understanding memory through hippocampal remapping. Trends Neurosci.31, 469–477 (2008). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Latuske, P., Kornienko, O., Kohler, L. & Allen, K. Hippocampal remapping and its entorhinal origin. Front. Behav. Neurosci.11, 253 (2017). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Rivard, B., Li, Y., Lenck-Santini, P. P., Poucet, B. & Muller, R. U. Representation of objects in space by two classes of hippocampal pyramidal cells. J. Gen. Physiol.124, 9–25 (2004). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Manns, J. R. & Eichenbaum, H. A cognitive map for object memory in the hippocampus. Learn Mem.16, 616–624 (2009). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Burke, S. N. et al. The influence of objects on place field expression and size in distal hippocampal CA1. Hippocampus21, 783–801 (2011). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Deshmukh, S. S. & Knierim, J. J. Influence of local objects on hippocampal representations: Landmark vectors and memory. Hippocampus23, 253–267 (2013). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Vandrey, B., Duncan, S. & Ainge, J. A. Object and object-memory representations across the proximodistal axis of CA1. Hippocampus31, 881–896 (2021). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Nagelhus, A., Andersson, S. O., Cogno, S. G., Moser, E. I. & Moser, M. B. Object-centered population coding in CA1 of the hippocampus. Neuron111, 2091–2104.e14 (2023). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Canto, C. B., Wouterlood, F. G. & Witter, M. P. What does the anatomical organization of the entorhinal cortex tell us?. Neural Plast.2008, 381243 (2008). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Hafting, T., Fyhn, M., Molden, S., Moser, M. B. & Moser, E. I. Microstructure of a spatial map in the entorhinal cortex. Nature436, 801–806 (2005). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Ouchi, A. & Fujisawa, S. Predictive grid coding in the medial entorhinal cortex. Science385, 776–784 (2024). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Fyhn, M., Molden, S., Witter, M. P., Moser, E. I. & Moser, M. B. Spatial representation in the entorhinal cortex. Science305, 1258–1264 (2004). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Sargolini, F. et al. Conjunctive representation of position, direction, and velocity in entorhinal cortex. Science312, 758–762 (2006). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Solstad, T., Boccara, C. N., Kropff, E., Moser, M. B. & Moser, E. I. Representation of geometric borders in the entorhinal cortex. Science322, 1865–1868 (2008). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Hoydal, O. A., Skytoen, E. R., Andersson, S. O., Moser, M. B. & Moser, E. I. Object-vector coding in the medial entorhinal cortex. Nature568, 400–404 (2019). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Kropff, E., Carmichael, J. E., Moser, M. B. & Moser, E. I. Speed cells in the medial entorhinal cortex. Nature523, 419–424 (2015). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Tukker, J. J. et al. Microcircuits for spatial coding in the medial entorhinal cortex. Physiol. Rev.102, 653–688 (2022). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Long, J. L. & Lu, L. Dynamic coding in the hippocampus during navigation. Zool. Res.43, 1023–1025 (2022). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Hargreaves, E. L., Rao, G., Lee, I. & Knierim, J. J. Major dissociation between medial and lateral entorhinal input to dorsal hippocampus. Science308, 1792–1794 (2005). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Huang, X. et al. Distinct spatial maps and multiple object codes in the lateral entorhinal cortex. Neuron111, 3068–3083.e7 (2023). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Deshmukh, S. S. & Knierim, J. J. Representation of non-spatial and spatial information in the lateral entorhinal cortex. Front. Behav. Neurosci.5, 69 (2011). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Tsao, A., Moser, M. B. & Moser, E. I. Traces of experience in the lateral entorhinal cortex. Curr. Biol.23, 399–405 (2013). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Igarashi, K. M., Lu, L., Colgin, L. L., Moser, M. B. & Moser, E. I. Coordination of entorhinal-hippocampal ensemble activity during associative learning. Nature510, 143–147 (2014). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Tsao, A. et al. Integrating time from experience in the lateral entorhinal cortex. Nature561, 57–62 (2018). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Cappaert, N. L., van Strien, N. M. & Witter, M. P. Hippocampal Formation. in The Rat Nervous System (ed. Paxinos, G.) 511–573 (Academic Press, 2015).
- 28.Nilssen, E. S., Doan, T. P., Nigro, M. J., Ohara, S. & Witter, M. P. Neurons and networks in the entorhinal cortex: a reappraisal of the lateral and medial entorhinal subdivisions mediating parallel cortical pathways. Hippocampus29, 1238–1254 (2019). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Nakamura, N. H., Flasbeck, V., Maingret, N., Kitsukawa, T. & Sauvage, M. M. Proximodistal segregation of nonspatial information in CA3: preferential recruitment of a proximal CA3-distal CA1 network in nonspatial recognition memory. J. Neurosci.33, 11506–11514 (2013). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Beer, Z. et al. The memory for time and space differentially engages the proximal and distal parts of the hippocampal subfields CA1 and CA3. PLoS Biol.16, e2006100 (2018). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Flasbeck, V., Atucha, E., Nakamura, N. H., Yoshida, M. & Sauvage, M. M. Spatial information is preferentially processed by the distal part of CA3: implication for memory retrieval. Behav. Brain Res.354, 31–38 (2018). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Ito, H. T. & Schuman, E. M. Functional division of hippocampal area CA1 via modulatory gating of entorhinal cortical inputs. Hippocampus22, 372–387 (2012). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Nakazawa, Y., Pevzner, A., Tanaka, K. Z. & Wiltgen, B. J. Memory retrieval along the proximodistal axis of CA1. Hippocampus26, 1140–1148 (2016). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Hartzell, A. L. et al. Transcription of the immediate-early gene Arc in CA1 of the hippocampus reveals activity differences along the proximodistal axis that are attenuated by advanced age. J. Neurosci.33, 3424–3433 (2013). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Henriksen, E. J. et al. Spatial representation along the proximodistal axis of CA1. Neuron68, 127–137 (2010). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Oliva, A., Fernandez-Ruiz, A., Buzsaki, G. & Berenyi, A. Spatial coding and physiological properties of hippocampal neurons in the Cornu Ammonis subregions. Hippocampus26, 1593–1607 (2016). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Deshmukh, S. S. Distal CA1 maintains a more coherent spatial representation than proximal CA1 when local and global cues conflict. J. Neurosci.41, 9767–9781 (2021). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Li, Y. et al. A distinct entorhinal cortex to hippocampal CA1 direct circuit for olfactory associative learning. Nat. Neurosci.20, 559–570 (2017). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Leutgeb, S. et al. Independent codes for spatial and episodic memory in hippocampal neuronal ensembles. Science309, 619–623 (2005). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Marks, W. D., Yokose, J., Kitamura, T. & Ogawa, S. K. Neuronal ensembles organize activity to generate contextual memory. Front. Behav. Neurosci.16, 805132 (2022). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 41.Knierim, J. J., Neunuebel, J. P. & Deshmukh, S. S. Functional correlates of the lateral and medial entorhinal cortex: objects, path integration and local-global reference frames. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci.369, 20130369 (2014). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Lu, L. et al. Impaired hippocampal rate coding after lesions of the lateral entorhinal cortex. Nat. Neurosci.16, 1085–1093 (2013). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Lu, L., Igarashi, K. M., Witter, M. P., Moser, E. I. & Moser, M. B. Topography of place maps along the CA3-to-CA2 axis of the hippocampus. Neuron87, 1078–1092 (2015). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Rowland, D. C., Roudi, Y., Moser, M. B. & Moser, E. I. Ten years of grid cells. Annu. Rev. Neurosci.39, 19–40 (2016). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Fyhn, M., Hafting, T., Treves, A., Moser, M. B. & Moser, E. I. Hippocampal remapping and grid realignment in entorhinal cortex. Nature446, 190–194 (2007). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Burke, S. N. et al. Representation of three-dimensional objects by the rat perirhinal cortex. Hippocampus22, 2032–2044 (2012). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Deshmukh, S. S., Johnson, J. L. & Knierim, J. J. Perirhinal cortex represents nonspatial, but not spatial, information in rats foraging in the presence of objects: comparison with lateral entorhinal cortex. Hippocampus22, 2045–2058 (2012). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Aggleton, J. P. & Nelson, A. J. D. Distributed interactive brain circuits for object-in-place memory: a place for time?. Brain Neurosci. Adv.4, 2398212820933471 (2020). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Parron, C. & Save, E. Comparison of the effects of entorhinal and retrosplenial cortical lesions on habituation, reaction to spatial and non-spatial changes during object exploration in the rat. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem.82, 1–11 (2004). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Rodo, C., Sargolini, F. & Save, E. Processing of spatial and non-spatial information in rats with lesions of the medial and lateral entorhinal cortex: environmental complexity matters. Behav. Brain Res.320, 200–209 (2017). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Van Cauter, T. et al. Distinct roles of medial and lateral entorhinal cortex in spatial cognition. Cereb. Cortex23, 451–459 (2013). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Tennant, S. A. et al. Stellate cells in the medial entorhinal cortex are required for spatial learning. Cell Rep.22, 1313–1324 (2018). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Wilson, D. I. et al. Lateral entorhinal cortex is critical for novel object-context recognition. Hippocampus23, 352–366 (2013). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Wilson, D. I., Watanabe, S., Milner, H. & Ainge, J. A. Lateral entorhinal cortex is necessary for associative but not nonassociative recognition memory. Hippocampus23, 1280–1290 (2013). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Ocko, S. A., Hardcastle, K., Giocomo, L. M. & Ganguli, S. Emergent elasticity in the neural code for space. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA115, E11798–e11806 (2018). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Wen, J. H., Sorscher, B., Aery Jones, E. A., Ganguli, S. & Giocomo, L. M. One-shot entorhinal maps enable flexible navigation in novel environments. Nature635, 943–950 (2024). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 57.Sun, Y. et al. CA1-projecting subiculum neurons facilitate object-place learning. Nat. Neurosci.22, 1857–1870 (2019). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Sun, Y., Nitz, D. A., Xu, X. & Giocomo, L. M. Subicular neurons encode concave and convex geometries. Nature627, 821–829 (2024). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Diehl, G. W., Hon, O. J., Leutgeb, S. & Leutgeb, J. K. Grid and nongrid cells in medial entorhinal cortex represent spatial location and environmental features with complementary coding schemes. Neuron94, 83–92 e86 (2017). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Ismakov, R., Barak, O., Jeffery, K. & Derdikman, D. Grid cells encode local positional information. Curr. Biol.27, 2337–2343 e2333 (2017). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Marozzi, E., Ginzberg, L. L., Alenda, A. & Jeffery, K. J. Purely translational realignment in grid cell firing patterns following nonmetric context change. Cereb. Cortex25, 4619–4627 (2015). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Wang, C. et al. Egocentric coding of external items in the lateral entorhinal cortex. Science362, 945–949 (2018). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Lee, H., Wang, C., Deshmukh, S. S. & Knierim, J. J. Neural population evidence of functional heterogeneity along the CA3 transverse axis: pattern completion versus pattern separation. Neuron87, 1093–1105 (2015). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Sun, Y., Nitz, D. A., Holmes, T. C. & Xu, X. Opposing and complementary topographic connectivity gradients revealed by quantitative analysis of canonical and noncanonical hippocampal CA1 inputs. eNeuro5, ENEURO.0322-17.2018 (2018). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 65.Soltesz, I. & Losonczy, A. CA1 pyramidal cell diversity enabling parallel information processing in the hippocampus. Nat. Neurosci.21, 484–493 (2018). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Lu, L., Popeney, B., Dickman, J. D. & Angelaki, D. E. Construction of an improved multi-tetrode hyperdrive for large-scale neural recording in behaving rats. J. Vis. Exp. 57388 10.3791/57388 (2018). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 67.Schneider, C. A., Rasband, W. S. & Eliceiri, K. W. NIH Image to ImageJ: 25 years of image analysis. Nat. Methods9, 671–675 (2012). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Cacucci, F., Lever, C., Wills, T. J., Burgess, N. & O’Keefe, J. Theta-modulated place-by-direction cells in the hippocampal formation in the rat. J. Neurosci.24, 8265–8277 (2004). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Skaggs, W. E., McNaughton, B. L., Wilson, M. A. & Barnes, C. A. Theta phase precession in hippocampal neuronal populations and the compression of temporal sequences. Hippocampus6, 149–172 (1996). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Glaser, J. I. et al. Machine learning for neural decoding. eNeuro7, ENEURO.0506-19.2020 (2020). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Description of Additional Supplementary Files
Data Availability Statement
All data supporting the findings of this study are available in the Supplementary data file (Supplementary Data 1–3). All other data are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.
The custom code supporting this study is available within the Supplementary codes.