Skip to main content
F1000Research logoLink to F1000Research
. 2024 Oct 30;10:1295. Originally published 2021 Dec 20. [Version 5] doi: 10.12688/f1000research.74504.5

Healing through faith: Meeting a chaplain coupled with biblical readings could produce lymphocyte changes that correlate with brain activity (HEALING study)

András Béres 1,2,a, Miklós Emri 3,4, Csaba Aranyi 3, Dániel Fajtai 4, Ferenc Nagy 2, Péter Szabó 2, Pál Bödecs 5, Edit Hörcsik 2, Éva Perpékné Papp 2,5, Ferenc Tomanek 2,6, Márta Kuti 2,5, Ágnes Petőfalviné 2, Hajnalka Kisdeákné 2, Gergely Bíró 4, Dániel Kovács 4, Bettina Bakos 4, Eszter Vinczen 4, Eszter Gál 4, Renáta Sillinger 4, Zoltán Szalai 4, Antal Szilágyi 7, Marianna Kiss-Merki 7, György Nagyéri 7, Judit Fodor 5, Tamás Németh 1, Erzsébet Papp 2, Imre Repa 2,4
PMCID: PMC12149807  PMID: 40496603

Version Changes

Revised. Amendments from Version 4

We thank the Reviewers for the thoughtful suggestions and insights. The manuscript has benefited from these insightful suggestions. The manuscript has been rechecked and the necessary changes have been made in accordance with the Reviewers’ suggestions. Our detailed responses to the Reviewers can be found at  the end of the article. The manuscript has been revised to highlight its contribution to the relevant literature. The Introduction is amended with a reference to research on religion and spirituality as factors affecting health. Non-significant results have been moved to the Discussion.   Following repeated revisions, the manuscript also underwent rigorous editing. In addition to grammar and format checking, the entire manuscript was repeatedly revised to form a consistent whole. Some paragraphs have been slightly re-structured to ensure they are not too long, to further enhance the flow of the text, and to allow the reader to follow our points more easily. 

Abstract

Background

Faith and belief systems impact the emotional as well as immunological states of believers in ways that we are just beginning to understand. However, the clinical implications of prior studies are limited.

The aim of the HEALING (Hospital-based Ecumenical and Linguistic Immuno-NeuroloGic) study is to examine immunological and neurological changes in hospitalized patients after meeting with a chaplain coupled with the study of biblical readings.

Methods

Hospitalized patients were pre-screened to identify those who were most in need of a spiritual intervention. A passage from the Bible was read to them during a meeting with the chaplain at bedside (n = 20) or in the chapel (n = 18). No meeting occurred in the randomized control group (n = 19). Blood samples were obtained 30 min prior and 60 min after the meeting to measure white blood cell (WBC) count, interferon-gamma (IFN-γ), immunoglobulin M (IgM), IgA, IgG, and complement 3 (C3). A subgroup of the visited patients was subjected to functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), during which they listened to an audiotape of readings of the same biblical passage (n = 21).

Results

Immunological changes were not significant. Conversely, a significant (p fwe = 0.003) correlation was observed between lymphocyte changes and activation of the angular gyrus (left BA39) during fMRI, a brain area involved in word recognition.

Conclusions

This article contributes to the relevant literature by helping to create a realistic picture of the possibilities of neuroimmune modulation in clinical practice. Compared to healthy volunteers, the extent of short-term neuroimmunomodulation becomes narrower in a clinical setting. Although limited by the sample size and cohort study design, the findings suggest that the depth of psycho-immunological changes could depend on the degree to which the chaplain’s main message is understood.

Keywords: faith, hospitalization, psychoneuroimmunology, theology, fMRI


Abbreviations

BA

Brodmann area

fMRI

functional magnetic resonance imaging

NK cells

natural killer cells

NSAID

nonsteroidal anti-inflammatory drug

For other abbreviations, see Table 1.
Hungarian translation - Magyar fordítás: See Extended data.

Table 1. Psychological factors and immune-hematological parameters assessed in the HEALING study.

Psychological factors Immune-hematological parameters
Based on the patient’s answers to the HEALING “before” questionnaire:
  • Patient’s personal belief on how she/he got sick (BI1 and BI2)
  • Patient’s desire for the visit (BII1, BII4)
  • Patient’s satisfaction with her/his life (BI3, BI4, BI5, BI6)
  • Patient’s self-assessed burden caused by hospitalization (BI7, BIV)
  • Patient’s personal belief on the way she/he will be healing (BII2, BII3)
  • Patient’s personal faith (BII1)
  • Patient’s religious practice (BIII2)
Perceived stress scale 14 items (PSS-14)
Based on the patient’s or the chaplain’s answers to the HEALING “after” questionnaire:
  • Patient’s/Chaplain’s overall satisfaction with the visit (AI and PI, respectively)
  • Patient’s assessment on the intimacy of the encounter (AII)
  • Chaplain’s assessment on the patient’s openness (PII)
  • Patient’s assessment on the sincerity of the encounter (AIII)
  • Chaplain’s assessment on the extent she could connect to the patient (PIII)
  • Patient’s assessment on the trustworthiness of the chaplain (AIV)
  • Patient’s/Chaplain’s assessment on the emotionally turbulent, roiling effect of the encounter (AV, PIV)
  • Patient’s/Chaplain’s assessment on the emotional depth of the encounter (AVI, PV)
Abbreviations refer to the question number in the corresponding HEALING questionnaire
  • White blood cell (WBC) count
  • Neutrophils Changes in:
    • neutrophil count (dNeut)
    • percentage of neutrophils (dNeut%)
  • Eosinophils Changes in:
    • eosinophil count (dEo)
    • percentage of eosinophils (dEo%)
  • Basophils Changes in:
    • basophil count (dBas)
    • percentage of basophils (dBas%)
  • Monocytes Changes in:
    • monocyte count (dMono)
    • percentage of monocytes (dMono%)
  • Lymphocytes Changes in:
    • lymphocyte count (dLy-abs)
    • percentage of lymphocytes (dLy%)
    • percentage of large granulated lymphocytes (dLGL%)
    • percentage of small condensed lymphocytes (dSmallLy%)
    • percentage of activated lymphocytes (dMiddleLy%)
  • Lymphocyte/neutrophil ratio Change in: (dLy/Neut)
  • Platelet count (Plt)
  • Interferon-gamma level (IFNγ)
  • Immunoglobulin M level (IgM)
  • Immunoglobulin A level (IgA)
  • Immunoglobulin G level (IgG)
  • Complement C3 level (C3)
  • Partial thromboplastin time (PTT)
  • International normalized ratio (INR)

Introduction

Following Selye’s description of how stress modulates the immune system, 1 numerous studies have shown a direct and complex relationship between acute, 2 5 chronic stress and the immune system, 6 9 with evidence revealing the long-term effects of early life stress on the immune response. 10 12 Far less research has examined the modulating effect of positive emotions on immunity. Berk et al. reported signs of immune stimulation in healthy adults after watching humorous videos. They displayed a 60-min humorous video to two groups, each consisting of 10 healthy adult men. They observed an increase in immunoglobulin levels (IgM, IgA, and IgG), activated T-cells (the total number of T cells as well as the Th-, Tc- and naive T cells separately), the proportion of lymphocyte subgroups, and peripheral IFNγ level. In six men, they observed that when NK-cell activity increased, it was statistically significant (IgM: p < 0.09; IgA: p < 0.01; IgG: p < 0.02; T-cell number: p < 0.01; IFN-γ-level: p = 0.02; NK-cell activity: p < 0.01). Most of the changes peaked 30 min and 90 min after the intervention began and were still measurable the next day, 12 h afterward. 13 Takahashi et al. showed a 75-min humorous video to 21 healthy adult volunteers and noted a significant ( p < 0.05) increase in NK-cell activity. 14 Following these early findings, we posited that the short-term effects of positive emotions would primarily be manifested in immune stimulation (as seen in an increased number or activity of immune parameters), as opposed to the negative effect of chronic stress, which causes immune suppression. As further research has evolved, a more nuanced picture has developed.

In 2001, Bittman et al. held approximately 60-min group therapy sessions involving percussion music for 30 adults and reported a significant increase in NK cell activity ( p = 0.055). However, such an increase did not occur after every session—only after composite drumming, and there was no measurable change in IL-2 and IFN levels. This was one of the first studies to draw attention to the importance of patient selection and the quality of interventions. 15

Later, Bennett et al. suggested that the extent of immunological changes related to interventions intended to elicit positive emotions may largely depend on how the emotions are subjectively perceived. After showing a humorous video to 16 healthy women, they did not find significant changes in NK-cell activity compared to the control group, except when they included the level of cheerfulness in their calculations ( p = 0.037). They measured this by counting the number of pre-established metacommunicative signals (from simply smiling to laughing out loud), which indicated the amount of mirthful laughter elicited among the participants (the Humor Response Scale). 16

Delving further into the phenomenon of positive emotions affecting the immune system, we broadened the scope of the study to include sick people, that is, participants whose immune systems were not intact when the study began. This is especially challenging because certain conditions are associated with immunosuppression while others are linked to chronic inflammation, underlining the importance of context when assessing which immunological changes can be considered positive. In 2001, Burns et al. conducted a music therapy session for 29 adults at a cancer help center using both recorded and live music and observed an increase in secretory IgA levels in both cases. 17 In 2005, Matsuzaki et al. showed a 60-min live session of Rakugo (a traditional Japanese tale) to 41 adults with rheumatoid arthritis and observed significant changes in their IL-6, TNF-α, IL-4, IL1Ra-citokine, and cytokine receptor antagonist levels ( p < 0.05) within 10 min of finishing the story. Changes in pro- and anti-inflammatory markers differed and could be related to the severity of the disease. 18

In 2007, Hayashi et al. showed a 60-min humorous video to six men and four women with type 2 diabetes and observed a significant change in postprandial glucose levels as well as an increased expression of many genes, including those that regulate NK cell activity. 19 Similar studies have traced how positive experiences can trigger immunological changes in a chain reaction down to the genetic code, creating “molecular signatures” related to mind–body interventions. 20

Most of the basic studies cited above were conducted with healthy adults in non-clinical conditions using non-personal tools (humorous videos) in the context of group sessions; hence, their clinical relevance is limited. As such, we aimed to evaluate the potential immunological effects of personal interventions intended to elicit positive emotions at the bedside of hospitalized patients. In the daily routines of hospital wards, this involves artists visiting sick children and chaplains visiting adults. Previously, the SHoRT (Smiling Hospital Research Team) study examined the immunological impact of positive emotions spurred by the positive experiences of sick children treated in a hospital. 21 In the HEALING (Hospital-based Ecumenical and Linguistic Immuno-NeuroloGic) study described below, we tried to elicit a positive emotional effect by meeting with a chaplain in a pre-selected adult population. With the current study being representative of a new direction in clinical psycho-neuro-immunological (PNI) research, 22 , 23 we decided to record the psychological and immunological changes related to the intervention as well as employ functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) to detect neurological events.

Intended to elicit positive emotions to alleviate the burden of being hospitalized and facilitate healing among patients, chaplains perform a spiritual function by representing the religious institutions from which they originate. Hence, the effects of their work on patients contain rich religious undertones.

Research on religion and spirituality as therapeutic factors is a scientific field in its own right. There is now a general consensus that these experiences are beneficial for health, including physical aspects. 24 At the same time, pioneering researchers in the field early drew attention to the fact that due to the nature of the spiritual phenomena, they cannot be researched in exactly the same way as in other fields, “as simply another garden-variety topic for sophisticated analysis.” 25 In addition to characteristics that depend on cultural context, 26 the historical period, crises in particular, 27 and to a certain extent age (interestingly emphasizing the role of a time window in adolescence, 28 midlife, 29 and old age 30 ), the mere orientation of the spiritual experience towards transcendence raises more fundamental, epistemological questions concerning the limits of how far scientific knowledge can or should progress in this direction at all. 31

Research on how religious experiences impact health—especially physical health—is a developing field. Prior studies have only explored the long-term impacts of religious life on the immune system 32 34 or relevant brain areas, 35 38 while neurological events have been examined in isolation from other physical changes. Moreover, the concept of religious practice occurring in a hospital setting is a sensitive issue, posing many practical difficulties (e.g., “God at the bedside”). 39

The above studies with healthy volunteers indicate (especially with regard to the modest statistical p values measured, even among healthy volunteers) that producing changes in immune parameters through spiritual means is not an easy task. Taking into account financial and logistical limitations as well, we aimed to determine whether—with arrangements similar to prior studies using the above sample sizes—we could measure relevant immunological changes that significantly influence patient recovery in a clinical setting. With the HEALING study, we aimed to examine whether general PNI patterns could emerge from a single spiritual encounter within a clinical environment or whether changes measured in previous studies were lost in the sea of other factors affecting the immune system.

Based on the abovementioned literature, one could expect to detect at least subtle immunological changes following a visit by a chaplain and activation of brain areas such as the medial frontal gyrus, lateral middle frontal gyrus, angular gyrus, and supramarginal gyrus (similar to the areas activated by meditation) 35 in the current study, which involved patients listening to and subsequently recognizing a sacred text (i.e., a biblical reading). With regard to the correlations linking immune parameters with fMRI recordings, it is reasonable to expect a large number of type I errors given the study design, which was prepared to relate the relatively limited database of immune parameters to the robust database of all brain activities recorded. We attempted to address this challenge by comparing our findings with those of previous studies.

Our trial is registered at www.ClinicalTrials.gov (Identifier: NCT04112121, Registration date: October 2, 2019).

Methods

Study design

We used a randomized, parallel, open-labeled, controlled clinical trial design:

  • 1.
    The effect of biblical readings on immunological parameters
    • HEALING I. Measuring the effect of biblical readings at the bedside: The core measurement occurred at the first meeting with the chaplain, coupled with biblical readings by the patient’s bed. We tried to evaluate if the spiritual intervention of “acceptance of the Word” elicited immunological as well as psychological changes, as evaluated by laboratory measurements and questionnaires. (In Christianity, listening to a passage from the Bible is considered a way of encountering God. Thus, in Christian terminology, the phrase “Word of God” refers either to a specific biblical passage, to the Bible in general, or directly to God. 40 Certain terms or phrases used in this article that refer to faith/the Bible will be placed in quotation marks.) The first blood sample was collected 30 min before the visit; the second blood sample was drawn 120 min after the first sample. We evaluated 20 patients.
    • HEALING II. Measuring the effect of biblical readings at the hospital chapel: For this segment, the biblical readings took place in the hospital chapel in small groups. We included 18 patients. The same biblical passage used in the previous setting was used for reading.
  • 2.

    The effect of biblical readings on fMRI activity

We recruited patients from the previous two measurements for this portion of the study based on their mobility and fMRI availability. During this portion, patients listened to biblical readings again (the same passages they first heard during one of the past two measurements). Passages alternated between a control text and a period of silence. We focused on whether any of the immunological or psychological parameters that appeared to change after the first listening session correlated with changes in fMRI activity.

For a graphic overview of the study design, see Figure 1.

Figure 1. HEALING study design.

Figure 1.

1. and 2.: blood samples.

We screened a total of 351 patients for eligibility and randomized 60. We excluded data from three patients because their medical condition required acute use of oral non-steroidal anti-inflammatory drugs (NSAIDs)/metamizole sodium. We analyzed results for a total of 20 patients in the bedside group (with an enrollment rate of up to two patients per week, i.e., HEALING I), 18 patients in the chapel group (with an enrollment rate of three to five patients per week, i.e., HEALING II), and 19 patients in the control group. We analyzed data from 57 patients in total. Of these, 22 underwent fMRI. We reported a technical failure in analyzing fMRI data from one patient (the first patient in the chapel group), although the event did not recur. We observed no other exclusions or loss of data after randomization.

We obtained measurements in the chapel (n = 18) in five independent groups (each group: minimum = 3 patients; maximum = 5 patients). In three of the five groups (11 patients), we gave the patients the option to receive communion: the Catholic Eucharist or the Reformed Lord’s Supper. Five of the 11 patients opted to receive the Catholic Eucharist (four patients) or the Reformed Lord’s Supper (one patient).

Patients were recruited from September 10, 2015, to January 2, 2017; the nature of the interventions did not require follow-up after January 2, 2017. No changes were observed in the trial outcomes or methods after the study commenced. The study was terminated when the number of planned enrollments was reached. We identified no harmful or unintended effects in the patients during or after the study.

For an overview of enrollment, see the CONSORT Flow Diagram of the HEALING study (cf. Figure 1 in the Extended data).

The protocol was approved by the Hungarian Medical Research Council’s Committee for Research Ethics (approval #7245-1/2014/EKU [55./2014], authorization #SOR/074/00130-4/2014) and the internal ethical boards of our institutions (IG/02013-003/2015; 270/2015). We obtained written informed consent from all participants and conducted the study in accordance with the principles of the Declaration of Helsinki.

Enrollment

The inclusion criteria were adult age (>18 years), hospitalization, ability to communicate verbally, alertness, orientation, no signs of psychosis in their medical history, and willingness to participate in the study after giving written informed consent. The hospital’s infectious disease and nephrology wards were involved in the recruitment process. We proposed enrolling all patients satisfying the above criteria. Mobility was an inclusion criterion for events in the chapel and fMRI measurements. Due to the limited availability of chaplains, not all eligible patients were able to participate. The decision was based on the patient’s degree of need and willingness as assessed by and at the discretion of the chaplain using a quick stratification scoring system designed to address the practical needs of this study (the HEAL Score, see Appendix 1, Extended data). The HEAL Score (which consists of the sum of four scales ranging from 1 to 4 each) is based on the hospital chaplain’s/hospital staff’s intuition of the neediest patients (“H4”), the patients who self-reported the highest need for visits (“E4”), the least religious patients (“A4”), and the patients with the most serious illness (“L4”). Exclusion criteria were the inability to communicate verbally, psychotic state (as reported by the physician responsible for the patient), altered mental state, unwillingness to participate, active and treated malignant disease, steroids, use of NSAIDs, or use of metamizole sodium, since these could have influenced the measured immunological parameters.

Random assignment was based on the availability of a chaplain on the day of the measurement rather than chance allocation of all patients willing to be visited; thus, the results of the control group were not biased by disappointments or frustrations caused by the cancellation/postponement of an anticipated visit. As such, the atmosphere in the control group reflected the genuine psychological environment of a common day at the hospital undisturbed by ordinary events. The randomized control group consisted of patients who knew the goal of the measurement but were explicitly asked to help with their participation in the control group; that is, they knew they were controls and that they were not going to meet the chaplain (upon request, the encounter could be scheduled for a later occasion).

The investigator arranged for enrollment. To minimize allocation bias, covariate-adaptive, blocked, stratified randomization was used: Block size was fixed to 19 (±1) enrolled patients for each group, with a 1:1 allocation ratio. For the control group, we enrolled patients whose diagnoses and number of days in the hospital were similar to those of the intervention groups to ensure a good balance of participant characteristics since the intervention groups were saturated.

Intervention and measurements

Personal encounters with the chaplain, listening to a biblical passage, and psycho-immunological measurements

Both groups listened to the same passage (Isaiah 40, 27–31, Figure 2). In the group where patients were visited at bedside, the chaplain provided personalized, supportive spiritual therapy in which, after listening to the patient’s current situation and reflecting on it, she tried to integrate a predetermined, encouraging verse into the conversation. In the group that met in the hospital chapel, the chaplain delivered the same short sermon to all participants at the same time, which was based on the same biblical verse. Among the five groups in the chapel, communion was offered to three: the Eucharist for Catholics and the Lord’s Supper for members of the Reformed Church. We asked the patients and chaplains to complete a questionnaire designed for the study (the HEALING questionnaire, preliminary pilot testing; see Appendix 2, Extended data). For the groups in the chapel, we used the validated score of the Perceived Stress Scale, which has 14 items (PSS-14). 41 We collected blood samples 30 min before and 60 min after the encounter due to some anticipated differences in the length of the visits, which lasted about 30 min each. The time interval between the two samples was fixed at 120 min. We supplemented the lab measurements with microscopic examination of the blood smears 42 44 ( Figures 4, 5, and 6), measurements of IFNγ- (HEALING I) or immunoglobulin M-, A-, G-, and C3-levels (enzyme-linked immunosorbent assays), and a few blood clotting factors (partial thromboplastin time [PTT] and international normalized ratio [INR]) (HEALING II).

Figure 2. Read aloud to all patients in the bedside or chapel groups, and during subsequent fMRI-s.

Figure 2.

Passage listened to by patients at bedside individually or in groups at the chapel as well as on audiotape during the fMRIs.

Isaiah 40:27-31. New King James Version (NKJV).

In this study, patients heard readings from the chaplain in their native language, Hungarian.

Figure 4. Activated lymphocytes: Microscopic examination of blood smear in the HEALING study.

Figure 4.

Figure 5. Large granular lymphocyte (LGL): Microscopic examination of blood smear in the HEALING study.

Figure 5.

Figure 6. Small condensed lymphocyte: Microscopic examination of blood smear in the HEALING study.

Figure 6.

For an overview of the measured psychological factors, immunological parameters, and corresponding abbreviations, see Table 1.

For nominal values, we used non-parametric, associative tests (i.e., Kolmogorov–Smirnov). Regarding immunological parameters, we used a normality test followed by a parametric, paired-samples t-test. For all parameters measured in the study, we performed a network analysis employing two different methods: a Bayesian analyzer developed at the University of Technology and Economics (BME) in the Faculty of Electrical Engineering and Informatics in Budapest, 45 and the R package IsingFit (R code used: IsingFit [data, family = “binomial,” AND = TRUE, gamma = 0.1, plot = TRUE, progressbar = TRUE, lowerbound.lambda = NA, vsize = 10]), 46 followed by correlation analysis. We divided the commonly used p-value (0.05) by the number of comparisons analyzed (Bonferroni correction).

Biblical passages read repeatedly on audiotape and fMRI measurements

Subsequently, we performed fMRI examinations, which were contingent upon the availability of fMRI and the capability of the patients to be mobilized. After providing written informed consent, the patients receiving fMRI were comforted to prevent any possible anxiety related to the measuring environment (narrowness and loudness often pose a challenge for patients). They were given instructions on the process and then laid down on the fMRI equipment, where they could hear the passage read aloud on an audiotape. They were allowed to stop the examination at any time by pressing a button.

To identify the regions of the brain that the current measurement could impact, we conducted the fMRI examinations using a block-design technique in three functional states:

  • 1.

    In the active biblical passage (“a”) block, patients could listen to the same passage they had heard in their hospital bed or in the hospital chapel. They heard the passage read by the chaplain once again in their native language (Hungarian) with a modern translation ( Figure 2).

  • 2.

    In the scientific control (“s”) block, the stimulus was a scientific text from an audiotape, also read by the chaplain ( Figure 3). Although this text was intelligible, it contained many difficult scientific words and complex grammatical structures in Hungarian (a Finno-Ugric language), which posed an intellectual challenge for patients.

  • 3.

    For a reference state, we introduced a block of patients exposed to silence (“c”).

Figure 3. Control text read to all participants during the fMRIs in the HEALING Study.

Figure 3.

Control text listened to on audiotape by the patients (either coming from the bedside or the control groups) who underwent fMRIs.

“Innate (non-specific) immune system” (extract). From: Szalka A, Timár L. Infektológia [Infectology]. Budapest: Medicina; 2005.

Patients heard the text read by the chaplain in their native language, Hungarian, in the study.

The fMRI examinations were performed using a 1.5T Siemens Magnetom Avanto MR scanner (Syngo software versionVB17/A, Siemens Medical Solutions, Erlangen, Germany); for the timing of the stimulation and synchronization of data collection, we used the software Nordic Aktiva v1.1. (Nordic Neurolab, Bergen, Norway). For all enrolled patients, we obtained a structural 3D T1-weighted axial MP-RAGE recording (TE = 4.73 ms, TR = 1540 ms, TI = 800 ms, flip angle = 15°, slice-thickness 0.8 mm, 0.9 × 0.9 × 0.9 mm voxel-size) with a 3-s repetition time. We obtained a blood oxygenation level-dependent (BOLD) recording sequence (T2* gradient echo, TR = 3000 ms, TE = 42 ms, flip angle = 90°, interleaved 4 mm axial slice thickness, 3.6 × 3.6-pixel size) with 145 components. During the fMRI measurements, the stimulation always started with a 15-s block of silence, followed by seven sections of activation blocks for 60 s each. The latter constituted a 30-s active and a 30-s control section or silence. During the measurements, we performed the “block design” type stimulation in a “c → as → ac → as → ac → as → ac → as” sequence order for all patients.

In the first phase of processing the fMRI image database, we assigned the T1-weighted structural image transformation into the MNI152 atlas space using the FSL 5.0 and ANTS 1.9 programs. 47 , 48 Using the segmentation algorithm of the FreeSurfer 5.0 software package 49 in native space, we created the brain-T1 pictures, which only contained the images originating from the surface of the brain. Using the brain-T1 pictures, we transformed the motion-corrected fMRI picture sequences into the T1-picture corresponding to the person in question and then transformed it into the MNI152 atlas space. 47 , 48 Finally, for all fMRI sequences, after eliminating the first four recordings containing the T1-effect, we applied 8-mm isotropic Gaussian filtering. We used SPM12 software 50 to perform statistical analyses for the created fMRI picture database at the individual and population levels. While processing the individual fMRI image sequences using the a-c, a-s, and s-c contrasts, we generated statistical image databases (contrasting pictures) showing the differences between the effects of the various stimuli, which we employed in the population-level analysis to statistically characterize the effect of the stimuli. Finally, we examined the differences in BOLD-answers linked to the active and control audio stimuli and their correlation with concrete clinical data corresponding to each patient. In the SPM analysis, to compare the statistical differences between the a-c, c-a, a-s, s-a, c-s, and s-c activities, we sorted the activation clusters containing a minimum of 100 voxels, with a Student-t = 3.58 threshold corresponding to the non-corrected p < 0.001 value from the SPM {T} pictures. To characterize them, we then used the MNI152 spatial coordinates of the cluster maximum, the maximal t-value, the corresponding family-wise error rate (FWER)-corrected probability (peak-level inference), the size of the cluster, and the FWE-corrected probability of cluster occurrence (cluster-level inference). 50

Results

The enrollment period lasted for one year. In total, we analyzed 57 patients. Their median age was 64 years (HEALING I), 65 years (HEALING II), and 66 years (control). The only criterion when enrolling patients for the control group was that the patient’s age, type of disease, and days of treatment did not show considerable differences compared with the intervention groups. Due to the small number of patients willing to participate, we needed to use statistical stratification with constraints (cf. Table 1, Extended data).

Part 1: Psycho-immunological changes measured after the meeting with the chaplain

As for whether they believed in God, most patients (HEALING I: 65%, HEALING II: 77.8%) answered positively. Only 25–38.9% of the patients said they actively practice their faith. A total of 35% and 44.4% of the patients reported cathartic or a very positive experience at the end of the measurement (a maximal rating of 5/5 to Question “AI” in the HEALING “after” questionnaire, i.e., the patient’s overall satisfaction with the visit, as reported on a single-item rating scale); 60% and 66.7% reported the visits to be deeply emotional (a minimum rating of 4/5 to Question “AVI” in the HEALING “after” questionnaire, i.e., the patient’s assessment of the emotional depth of the encounter, as reported on a single-item rating scale).

Changes in immunological parameters were not statistically significant after the Bonferroni correction. While we checked all possible correlations between the psychological and immunological parameters, no significant correlation emerged (see the visualization of all correlations in Figure 5, emphasizing the stronger psycho-immunological correlations in Figure 6; Extended data).

Part 2: Results of the fMRI measurement

We obtained evaluable fMRIs from a total of 21 patients in the second part of the study.

The comparison of the “active biblical reading (a)” block with the “reference silence (c)” block showed significant ( p < 0.001) activation in the right and left BA 22 (Wernicke’s) and the right and left BA41 (primary auditory) areas only . The comparison of the “scientific control (s)” block with the “reference silence (c)” block revealed significant ( p < 0.001) activation in the same areas only. The comparison of the “active biblical reading (a)” block with the “scientific control (s)” block during fMRI did not show any demarcated brain area that would have significantly different activity among all patients (Figures 2–4, Extended data).

After that, we performed several subgroup analyses to explore the correlations between the psychological factors or the immunological parameters and the changes in fMRI activity between the “active biblical reading (a)” and “scientific control (s)” blocks. We categorized the subgroups of patients according to the extent of the change that we measured in the psychological factors—i.e., the chaplain’s overall satisfaction with the visit [“PI”] and the patient’s overall satisfaction with the visit and assessment of the emotional depth of the encounter [“AI,” “AVI”], as reported on the single-item rating scales corresponding to the HEALING “after” questionnaire—or the immunological parameters. The latter entails changes in the lymphocyte count (dLy-abs), the percentage of lymphocytes (dLy%), and the lymphocyte/neutrophil ratio (dLy/Neut) (as measured in the laboratory), along with changes in the percentage of LGLs (dLGL%) (as measured with microscopic examination of blood smears). These subgroups contained approximately similar (half and half) numbers of patients.

Among the dLy-abs subgroups, there was a tendency toward a change in fMRI activity. We observed a weak difference in activity in the area of the left BA39 between the subgroup of patients who showed an increase in lymphocyte count and those who exhibited no relevant increase in lymphocyte count ( p = 0.393). No other psychological or immunological pairs of subgroups displayed any differences with regard to changes in fMRI activity.

After that, we aimed to correlate the dLy parameters with the areas showing changes in fMRI activity (still for the changes in fMRI activity between the “active biblical reading (a)” and the “scientific control (s)” blocks only). In Figure 7, the darker brain areas indicate a stronger correlation of brain activity with the number of lymphocytes. An inverse linear correlation emerged ( p = 0.019 with dLy%, p = 0.003 with dLy-abs) between the change in activity of the left BA39 and the change in lymphocyte count ( Figures 7 and 8; p = 0.003, r = -0.9584). Finally, we performed the correlation analysis for the change in fMRI activity between the “active biblical reading (a)” and “scientific control (s)” periods and the dNeut, dLy/Neut, AI, AVI, and PI factors or parameters, but we observed no significant correlation.

Figure 7. dLy-abs vs. a-s fMRI in the left BA39.

Figure 7.

Change in lymphocyte count (“dLy”) versus the difference between activation during the active biblical reading “a” and the scientific control “s” block (“response”) during fMRI in the left BA39 .

Figure 8. dLy vs. fMRI in the left BA39.

Figure 8.

Change in lymphocyte count (“dLy”) versus the difference between activation during the active biblical reading “a” and the scientific control “s” block (“response”) during fMRI in the left BA39 ( p = 0.003, r = -0.9584).

Accounting for correlation and subgroup analyses, we examined only 12 factors or parameters; hence, the significance level was 0.05/12 = 0.00417. The correlation between the left BA39 area and change in lymphocyte count remained significant after Bonferroni correction.

Discussion

Neurological viewpoint

The most easily implementable technique—entailing the least possibility of error in the laboratory measurements—the automated lymphocyte count showed a significant correlation with the fMRI outcomes. Unintended bias did not affect this part of the study’s randomization process because the fMRIs were performed in an auto-control setting for the visited patients only. However, as Lieberman and Cunningham pointed out, whenever fMRI is used as a principal tool in a research setting, the number of measurements obtained during each examination is excessive from a statistical perspective. Even using the most conservative p-values cannot rule out the possibility of type I errors. This makes the outcomes of every fMRI study meaningful only after comparing it with past studies and verifying it with subsequent research. 51 We discuss a plausible interpretation of the results, but further investigation is needed to confirm them.

Since we could not rule out the potential effects of magnetic resonance on immunological parameters from the fMRI examination, we intentionally separated the fMRI and immunological measurements. We laid out the “block-design” setting we used based on a meta-analysis of 48 studies, 52 including Leff et al. 53 and Beaucousin et al., 54 who described different activation patterns in the fMRIs of patients exposed to passages read aloud that featured different emotional content.

The comparison of the “a”-“c” and “s”-“c” periods revealed a significant change in fMRI activity in the primary auditory areas, whereas we detected no such difference when comparing “a” and “s.” This proves that the measurement was physiologically trustworthy: the patients heard the biblical passages and the control text.

Since we intentionally left ample opportunity for spontaneity and subjectivity, it is not surprising in hindsight that no distinct brain area emerged when comparing the “a” and “s” periods. As stated above, spontaneous meetings with a person of faith are the most subjective, the most diverse, and hence the least generalizable part of the spectrum of spiritual experiences. We would have observed general differences in brain areas between participants hearing a religious recitation and a neutral/scientific recitation if the intervention group had been composed of participants highly trained in meditation (such as Buddhist monks). The participants of the HEALING study were ordinary people from various religious backgrounds, which probably resulted in more subjective differences in the way they perceived the sacred texts read aloud to them.

We could not unanimously detect any general differences among all enrolled patients between the biblical and control blocks; therefore, hearing a religious recitation did not affect the brain differently than a neutral/scientific recitation. However, a significant correlation emerged between lymphocyte count and brain activity when subtracting the scientific control block from the biblical block. This outcome echoes previous psycho-immunological results mentioned in the introduction, especially those of Bennett et al., 16 who detected no general immunological changes following the presentation of a humorous video to healthy volunteers but observed a significant correlation with changes in immune parameters when they found a way to quantify how the patients subjectively viewed the video. They employed a humor response rating scale that corresponds to the fMRI results in our study, thus representing different tools used for the same purpose: to measure individual differences in how interventions are perceived.

Contrary to our expectations, although the database of immune parameters was many times scarcer than the enormous database of whole brain activity, remarkably, we did not obtain obvious false positive results. When comparing the “a” and “s” periods, the lack of generally distinguishable areas of activation reduced the chance of deriving type I errors related to dense activation patterns. Only a single correlation emerged, suggesting that it was reasonable not to automatically consider this result a spurious effect. The biblical passages constituted the sole fixed, generalizable element of the visits; consistent with this, the only brain area that correlated with any of the measured immune parameters was involved in understanding these passages.

The biblical passage used is an ancient (more than two-and-a-half thousand years old) text with plain words and simple syntax; it is thought to have originally been intended to offer comfort. In contrast, the control text contained many words that would be challenging for the layman to easily grasp; moreover, it had long sentences that were difficult to follow and several words with potential fear-inducing connotations. Thus, the results of the correlation analysis imply that the change in lymphocyte count is related to the patients’ subjectively perceived content of the biblical passage as opposed to the control text. The fact that the changes in lymphocyte count were not significant (despite some widening in the confidence intervals) indicates that these changes were subject to limitations by the circumstances and disease. However, the correlation of these changes with the fMRIs suggests that even when biblical passages do not seem to have a physiological effect, they could have an ordering or arranging effect along a specific guiding principle based on key parameters such as lymphocyte count. In other words, although the effect size of the lymphocyte changes was too small to produce significant changes under the limited sample size of this study, the amplitude of the changes appeared to vary with the evolution of a single parameter: activation of the left angular gyrus.

According to neurosynth.org, previous scientific publications related to the region in the 2-mm area of the -52, -56, and 26 clusters have linked this area to the tactile and manual reconstruction of shape recognition, the learning of words, emotional speech, and the encoding of belief systems in neural pathways and their connection with ethical decision-making. 55 61 The only brain area that showed a correlation with any of the immune parameters measured in this study—the left BA39 (gyrus angularis)—was contralaterally the same as that activated during meditation or recitation of Buddhist scripture. 35 Although the depth of comprehension could only denote cognitive understanding, due to the core nature of the phenomenon observed and the study design, it could refer to broader spiritual experiences, which in turn contain cognitive and emotional components and are not (or only partially) subject to conscious influence.

Psycho-immunological viewpoint

Contrary to our expectations, grounded in a preliminary scientific review of the literature relating faith to psycho-neuro-immunology, 62 no significant psycho-immunological results emerged. We assume one reason for this is that the study could have been underpowered regarding its sample size, given the small size of the immunological changes observed. Another reason could be that most patients—although truly grateful for being visited—were not motivated to meet with a chaplain to have a transformative experience. If we turn our attention from mass healing to the personal accounts of healing found in the New Testament—such as those of a man with leprosy (Mt 8, 2–4), a sick woman who was bleeding (Lk 8, 43–48), and the blind (Mk 10, 46–52)—the healed seem to share at least a few of the following traits:

  • Their conditions were critical, often compounded by remorse and loneliness.

  • The sick themselves or a close relative strongly wanted them to be healed.

  • Jesus seemed like the ultimate hope, a last chance, as seen in the following:
    • What do you want me to do for you?” (Mk 10,51).
    • “Your faith saved you” (Mk 5,34).
    • “If I can do something? Everything is possible for the one who believes” (Mk 9,23); see Grün et Robben. 63

The people healed in the biblical accounts describe their encounters with Jesus as transformative. However, statistically speaking, the psychological effect was of much lower importance for the patients enrolled in the present study. Despite the thorough enrollment process, only 40% of the patients in the bedside group and 5.5% of patients in the chapel group were given an “H4” Score (cf. HEAL Score above). This indicates that even among the “chosen” in the pre-screened group of patients whom we evaluated with the HEAL Score, the chaplain was certain that only a tiny fraction of patients needed to be visited. The PSS-14 scores revealed that the patients’ average level of stress in the month preceding measurement was not significantly high (the PSS-14 had a mean value of 27/56), and their responses to the Healing Questionnaire showed that they did not perceive the psychological or physical wounds incurred in their daily lives to be deep (see tables 2, 3, and 4, Extended data).

As for whether the patients felt responsible for their own chronic ailments, blaming oneself was the only psychological factor that displayed a correlation (albeit a weak one) with any immunological parameter (see the answer “BI2_b,” i.e., “It is my fault that I became sick; I blame myself …”). This was one of the rarest answers among the patients (10% to 22% of the patients in the bedside [HEALING I] and chapel [HEALING II] groups, respectively). In other words, by the time the chaplain arrived for a visit, most patients were not experiencing personal difficulties and did not view the visit as a life-altering event. Patients were open to the visit but were mostly concerned with feeling exhausted and needing help during their hospitalization.

In order to verify finer deviations, future research needs to clarify that although these changes were not statistically significant after the Bonferroni correction, some trend-like deviations (consistent with prior literature) appeared, suggesting the study may have been underpowered in terms of sample size. Indeed, changes in lymphocyte count appeared to exhibit trend-like deviations in both groups (at bedside and in the chapel; Figures 9 and 10). Also, for future research, the personal, one-on-one meetings (not the group sessions) consistently produced an insignificant but considerable small effect size in lymphocyte changes across the measurements. The effect size for dLy-abs was d = 0.23 at the bedside (HEALING I, individual) but d = 0.09 in the chapel (HEALING II, group). The effect size for dLy-% was d = 0.28, p = 0.08 in terms of laboratory testing, and d = 0.31 with p = 0.053 for microscopic examination of blood smears at bedside (see the p-values of immune system changes in tables 2, 3, 4, Extended data).

Figure 9. Changes in lymphocyte count in the HEALING I (bedside) and HEALING II (chapel) studies.

Figure 9.

The columns indicate the number of patients. 1 = dark brown: lymphocyte count (Ly-abs) and percentage (Ly %) decreased; 2 = blue: Ly-abs and Ly % did not change in the same direction; 3 = light brown: Ly-abs and Ly % increased. Taking the control group as the baseline condition (center), the blue arrows suggest two possibilities for eliciting change: intervention at the bedside (the arrow to the left) or intervention in the chapel (the arrow to the right). Yellow arrows denote possible trends.

Figure 10. Number of patients with a lymphocyte count decreased, with no change, and increased.

Figure 10.

In case a meeting took place, the numbers above the columns indicate the average of subjective scores (from 1 to 5) by which the artists (SHoRT) or the chaplain (HEALING) evaluated the encounter.

In Figure 10, the columns are grouped in threes; in each group, from left to right, the height of the column shows the number of children with a decrease, no change, and an increase in lymphocyte count, respectively. At the top of the columns, the patients’ visitors’ subjective assessment of the visits is shown on a 5-point scale (higher scores denote more successful visits; at the time of the assessment, the visitors did not know the results of the laboratory measurements). As the figure demonstrates, although the measured immune changes were too minimal to be distinguishable from other clinical effects, the results from the bedside encounters revealed a striking similarity with those of the ShoRT study, in which Smiling Hospital artists visited children from whom blood samples were drawn in a non-painful way through branules (SHoRT). 21 Once again, more successful visits generated more marked changes in lymphocyte count.

Figure 9 draws attention to another interesting observation. The columns indicate the number of patients, with the color of the columns referring to the direction of the change in lymphocyte count (1 = dark brown: the lymphocyte count (Ly-abs) and percentage (Ly %) decreased; 2 = blue: Ly-abs and Ly % did not change in the same direction; 3 = light brown: Ly-abs and Ly % increased). Taking the control group as the baseline condition (center), the blue arrows suggest two possibilities for eliciting change: intervention at bedside (the arrow to the left) or intervention in the chapel (the arrow to the right). The yellow arrows imply possible trends; it appears as if the closer the patients got (physically and psychologically) to the chapel—especially regarding the results of the group receiving communion (without exception in this subgroup, we recorded a decline in lymphocyte count, which is considered a sign of stress in the PNI literature; see Figure 9)—the growing inconsistency in lymphocyte changes seemed to reveal a sense of fascination and admiration mixed with tones of fear, as reflected by the psychological/religious term tremendum (see Rudolf Otto’s terminology of the “numinous” and mysterium tremendum et fascinans to describe holy experiences). 64 While the primary purpose of the intervention was to elicit positive emotions, we observed that the same biblical passage could trigger a wide range of thoughts and feelings and that different facets of the same emotional pattern could be amplified, including fear of God, which is a recurrent motif for biblical encounters between God and humans. For instance, in the Book of Exodus, God says to Moses: “I will make all my goodness pass before thee … Thou canst not see my face: for there shall no man see me and live.” (Exodus 33, 19-20, KJV). More measurements are required to map the main emotional themes at play during meetings with a chaplain and clarify the statistical significance of these observations.

Limitations

Our study has several limitations.

First, the small sample size does not allow for the extrapolation of far-reaching conclusions; however, the sample size reaches or exceeds that of previous PNI studies 13 17 and is standard in basic fMRI research. 35 37

Second, the patients were treated for various illnesses while immersed in a clinical environment. However, our goal was to determine whether meetings with a chaplain could elicit patterns marked enough to overwrite the heterogeneity of diagnoses. The diversity of the patients’ psychological and somatic conditions was managed by an encounter-centered study design, and the time interval between both data collections was relatively small (2 h, following the previously mentioned literature’s ranges for time intervals) 13 , 16 , 18 compared to the length of the patient’s stay in the hospital (ranging from several days to weeks). The difference between the control and intervention groups did not affect the main, significant outcome found in the fMRI examinations since we performed that part of the study within an auto-control setting; that is, only patients previously visited by the chaplain received an fMRI, and we did not compare their results to those of the non-visited group. Instead, the patients who underwent fMRI examinations while listening to the biblical passage read aloud were also exposed to the scientific text read aloud as well as their own reference states of silence that served as controls.

Finally, the limitations of all portions of the current study on spirituality are related to general concerns about their reproducibility. The study design of the measurements addressed this issue by placing strong emphasis on assuring that the encounters remained as authentic as possible for the intervention groups (e.g., leaving ample opportunity for the chaplain’s spontaneity) and by reproducing the genuine atmosphere of a usual day in the hospital for the control group (open randomization based on the availability of a chaplain instead of chance allocation, which could lead the patients to become disappointed).

The term “spiritual experience” refers to a vast range of collective and personal experiences, and its neural aspects (the “neural correlates of spirituality”) seem to depend heavily on the study design that is used to record them (cf. “Mystical experience in the lab”). 65 As Andersen et al. stated, “while studying powerful mystical experiences in believers in a controlled environment would enable researchers to identify, isolate, and analyze the central aspects of the phenomenon, we need an experimental setting with the power to elicit spontaneous mystical experiences.” In line with this, the spatial delimitability of the spiritual experiences’ neural imprint appears to depend on whether the nature of the experience itself is based on the more objective (e.g., monks reciting Buddhist scripture) 35 or more subjective (e.g., nuns recalling the most intense mystical experience they have ever had) 66 end of the spectrum. While an abundance of relevant literature elaborates on what happens with the “self” during meditation, 67 mindfulness, 68 or prayer, 36 we intentionally investigated a different area of spiritual experience by conceptualizing it in terms of meeting with a significant other. We assert that the spiritual experience is fundamentally not (or not merely) a different state of mind but the opportunity to transcend one’s “self” through meeting a significant other (in the case of this study, a chaplain). Although we cannot rule out additional secondary effects of meditation or prayer when a chaplain visits a patient (at times, patients are occasionally allowed to pray or meditate on a biblical passage), we did not focus much on the impacts of these adjunct elements but instead on the involuntary influences of an interactive, direct meeting with a person of faith.

In the spiritual care approach, the hospital chaplain facilitates the relationship with the transcendent Other. Hypothetically speaking, let us assume that, through scientific examination of the spiritual experience, it may be understood as both an intra-personal and inter-personal event between the individual and the significant, transcendent Other. Let us also assume that measurement tools of the natural sciences (since they are immanent to the natural world) might not be suitable for measuring the transcendent Other. As such, the scientific approach is limited in terms of providing an appropriate form of measurement and may detract from the authentic nature of spiritual encounters. When using the scientific approach by itself, we may be content with simply measuring (and even then, only to a limited extent) the impact of an inter-personal encounter on the individual, the intensity of one’s experience with the transcendent Other, the subjectively perceived presence of the transcendent Other, etc. without grasping or exercising control over the transcendent Other.

When examining the impact of a religious intervention on the self, we generally consider it necessary to ensure spontaneity (by allowing the chaplain’s intuition to prevail), authenticity (in the present measurement, using the original biblical passage), and personalization (with the chaplain’s personalized bedside meeting).

The main methodological challenge of this study is that genuine meetings with chaplains in hospitals always contain a spontaneous (“inspired”) element. Right from their initiation, these authentic meetings are always based on the chaplain’s intuition (which includes a range of elements, from which patient she visits to the advice she gives). This spontaneous aspect has a major impact on the entire meeting and, in turn, any PNI imprint (if there is any). The ample opportunity left for the chaplain’s inspiration reflects the original setting of biblical times in which the “Word of God” was first said when such meetings happened as a spontaneous act of the participants. This is how Jesus met people. Had we removed this spontaneous element, we would have prevented the occurrence of the phenomenon for which we were trying to estimate the potential PNI imprint. The question that arose was whether meetings with a chaplain could elicit immunological changes in patients if these meetings were allowed to be as authentic and spontaneous as they once were in biblical times. This methodological decision to leave the spontaneous aspect in did not affect the reproducibility of the results.

We suggest replicating the findings by proposing to schedule a meeting for every patient available in a ward on the day of taking measurements, by establishing a rough order for the visits (the “HEAL Score”, cf. above) to help manage the time available (but always letting the chaplain ultimately decide which patients to visit), and asking the chaplain to use the same biblical passage in every spontaneous, personalized meeting.

Journalist Paul Salopek trekked 21,000 miles (33.780 km) on foot to retrace the pathways of the first humans who migrated out of Africa and spread around the globe. He posited that one must devote time to cultivating interpersonal ties to comprehend the human phenomenon fully; he later created the non-profit “ Out of Eden Walk.” 69 Considering his “slow journalism” concept as a model, we advocate for “slow science” to ensure a gradual pace for methodology. Together with the ecumenical atmosphere of the study, the consciously slow pace of enrollment reinforced the authenticity of the encounters. For example, for two patients who were difficult to transport, we brought them to their actual beds by elevator to the hospital chapel, located on one floor beneath the Infectious Disease Ward. This experience evoked the biblical scene of the four men who brought their paralytic friend down from the roof, lowering him with cords, to help him reach Jesus through the crowd (Mk 2,4).

Conclusions and clinical relevance

Our results contribute to the relevant literature by helping to paint a realistic picture of the extent of short-term immune changes that can be induced by spiritual means in the clinical environment. The results indicate that, compared to healthy volunteers, at least when it comes to the immune parameters measured in the daily routine, the possibilities of neuroimmune modulation in the clinical environment become narrower.

Our results point to a brain area whose activity changes along with lymphocyte count while meeting with a chaplain, coupled with listening to biblical passages in a hospital setting (for an excursus on theological perspectives, see below). Provided they accept that visits cannot be forced, nor that the outcome of the meetings is fully anticipated, physicians inviting a chaplain to meet with their patients should consider these visits as an additional factor that could exert an influence on the patients’ lymphocyte count and hence alter their recovery from the disease, depending on the depth at which the chaplain’s core message is understood.

Excursus: Theological perspectives

We caution against interpreting our results solely in terms of healing that is accelerated by an intimate religious experience. Our measurements touch upon the religious sensibilities raised by the topic. We must elaborate on them from a theological angle to avoid potential distortions in interpreting our findings. The results raise deep questions: Why did we not measure more immunological changes if the “Word of God” is believed to heal people? If lymphocyte count is correlated with the part of the brain involved in understanding words, does that mean that those who try to understand biblical passages better will have more healing lymphocytes? These are theological questions. As such, the spiritual and religious nature of the phenomenon observed requires integrating the theological viewpoint for careful interpretation of the results.

For centuries in the European tradition, theology—originally closely associated with the septem artes liberales (the seven liberal arts)—has been the chosen branch of academic studies as far as the Bible is concerned. More recently, modern research methods have been embraced, especially by the reformed Christian theologians of the 20 th and 21 st centuries, rendering it a school of thought that only differs from the methodology of any branch of scientific cognition by focusing on the “Word of God” instead of the natural world. 40 This legitimizes examining how our findings relate to the relevant theological literature in the discourse of a study involving religion, especially in the case of using a biblical passage.

When exploring the effect of the Word of God on humans and trying to correlate it with human parameters, dialectical theologians immediately emphasize the inherent asymmetry in the relationship between God and people. As Tillich asserted, we cannot control God with our will: “The experience of spiritual presence does something that the human soul in itself cannot.” 70 In light of this, it is important to underline that we cannot claim that the immunological and neurological changes observed in our measurements were due to each patient’s own efforts; rather, such changes are traces of experiences that happened to the patients. Another important theological statement is that the content of the biblical message as a whole does not hold promise for immediate physical healing. According to the Bible, Jesus’ healings occurred rarely, which implies that they had a signal value. Jesus did not heal anything himself (cf. the account of the healing at Lake Bethesda, Jn 5, 1-9), and at a critical moment, he accepted his own death. There are instances where Jesus’ Gospel has been theorized to give strength to overcome disease and to endure it. Nevertheless, a correlation between God and healing exists in theology . Tillich wrote: “The answers emerging from the event of revelation are comprehensible only if they are in correlation with the questions of our whole existence, the existential question.” 70 Tillich used correlation mainly with respect to the dialogue between Christian messages and contemporary society (i.e., culture). This study describes how correlation can be employed in dialogue between the natural sciences, the social sciences, and theology. In Christianity, encounters between God and humans occur within a human context, including while listening to a biblical passage (i.e., listening to the “Word of God”). 40 In this way, a meeting with a chaplain could trigger the recollection of a genuine, primordial experience of meeting with the transcendent. This phenomenon could express ancient (maybe even pre-Christian) patterns buried in our collective subconscious, bringing up ancient, genuine, instinctive reactions according to Jung’s concept of the working mechanism of “archetypes,” i.e., of inherited inner patterns buried in the collective subconscious. 71 The simultaneous changes in the nervous and immune systems that we recorded during repeated instances of listening to the Bible suggest that, in addition to psychological effects, general biological patterns could also be activated during meetings between God and humans. In line with this concept, beyond personal stories of healing, the Bible’s Synoptic contains numerous accounts of mass healing (i.e., Mt 4, 24.8, 16.12, 15.14, 14.15, 30.19, 2.21, 14; Mk 6, 56; Lk 4, 40.9, 11), giving the impression that meeting with Jesus produced general healing effects in people.

We can solve the above contradiction—i.e., that the Word of God may be healing while its main message sometimes does not lead to healing—if we place the question and our results in the context of modern theologians’ evolutionary theory. It states that religiousness and a life of faith have developmental aspects; we can, therefore, view them as having an evolutionary dimension with a direction of growth, at times including the notion of suffering through the process (see Whitehead 72 and “process theology”). As such, religiousness and a life of faith are not opposed to the natural sciences’ concept of evolution.

According to Pannenberg, “At the end of the [19th] century and in the first half of the 20th, sadly, Christian churches and theologians could not recognize that the teaching of evolution offers an unprecedented possibility to theology in regard to the possibility of its relationship with modern science. The fight against Darwinism was one of the mistakes resulting in the most serious consequences throughout the history of theology’s relationship with the sciences.” 73 In a small way, our findings can support this position and shed light on the fact that some form of religious belief could indefatigably spread and survive despite the lack of significant and noticeable biological effects. If, depending on the depth of spiritual understanding, a religious experience could influence lymphocyte count, then it might contribute to the prevention of or recovery from disease, hence providing an evolutionary advantage (in the sense that the natural sciences use this term). However, from a theologian’s perspective, the goal of evolution is not survival or accommodation in the narrow sense of the word. Teilhard de Chardin wrote, “As early as in St. Paul and St. John we read that to create, to fulfill and to purify the world is, for God, to unify it by uniting it organically with himself.” Moreover, God is “from this point of vantage in the heart of [the] matter, assuming the control and leadership of what we now call evolution.” 74 He stated that “through human socialization, whose specific effect is to involute upon itself the whole bundle of reflexive scales and fibers of the earth, it is the very axis of the cosmic vortex of interiorization which is pursuing its course.” 74 Pannenberg often alluded to God’s appearance in world history in the manifestation of Jesus as an event that can be considered portent, anticipating the future. 75 In this sense, healings are of signal value because they hint at a precursory picture of a harmonious God-human relationship, including all its psychical and physical aspects. Pannenberg wrote, if, “[like Teilhard de Chardin,] we can consider life’s evolution as the process of the creation of life forms of increasing complexity and at the same time becoming increasingly introspective, then we can also state that in the succession of different forms of life by creatures, is expressed the increase in the shareholding of the divine spirit, of life’s spirit.” 73 , 74

Faith is an evolutionary advantage in the theological sense because evolution aims to increase the shareholding of the divine spirit. 74 This is the endpoint at which the results of our measurements converge.

Data availability

Underlying data

Figshare: Healing Study Beres et al. - Part 1: Psychological and Immunological Changes after meeting a Chaplain coupled with Biblical Readings among Hospitalized Patients, https://doi.org/10.6084/m9.figshare.16750384.v1. 76

Figshare: Healing Study Beres et al. - Part 2: fMRI Changes after meeting a Chaplain coupled with Biblical Readings and Recalling the Visits with Audiotapes among Hospitalized Patients, https://doi.org/10.6084/m9.figshare.16751851.v1. 77

Extended data

Figshare: HEALING Study Beres et al. - F1000Research - extended data, https://doi.org/10.6084/m9.figshare.17029715.v3. 78

Reporting guidelines

Figshare: CONSORT checklist for “Healing through faith: meeting a chaplain coupled with biblical readings could produce lymphocyte changes that correlate with brain activity (HEALING study)”, https://doi.org/10.6084/m9.figshare.17029715.v3. 78

Data are available under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International license (CC-BY 4.0).

Acknowledgments

We thank all the patients who participated in this study.

We thank András Falus, MD, PhD, DSc, for his invaluable help in making this study possible.

We thank Péter Antal, PhD, and Bence Bruncsics for their invaluable assistance with the statistical analysis of Part 1 (psycho-immunological changes measured after the experience of the meeting) of this study.

We thank Tamás Szelestei, MD, PhD, and Josef Fuisz, ThD, for their suggestions on the final version of this article.

For English consultation, we would like to express our profound gratitude to Tricia Garrison, creative collaborator in the “Happiness Helps Healing” initiative (version 0), to the Editorial at F1000Research (version 1) and the Taylor&Francis-Editage Editing Service (version 5).

We thank Andrea Varga, MPharm, the mother of Dr. Béres, for taking the risk of substantially sponsoring the study.

In addition, the Kaposi Mór Teaching Hospital pays tribute to the 500 th anniversary of the Reformation, which helped create a healthy dialogue between theology and the natural sciences.

Funding Statement

The study was supported by the Foundation for the Kaposi Mór Hospital (PhD grant).

The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.

[version 5; peer review: 1 approved

References

  • 1. Selye J: Életünk és a stressz. [Our life and stress]. Budapest: Akadémiai;1978. [Google Scholar]
  • 2. Weisse CS, Pato CN, McAllister CG, et al. : Differential effects of controllable and uncontrollable acute stress on lymphocyte proliferation and leukocyte percentages in humans. Brain Behav. Immun. 1990;4(4):339–351. 10.1016/0889-1591(90)90037-Q [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3. Sieber WJ, Rodin J, Larson L, et al. : Modulation of human natural killer cell activity by exposure to uncontrollable stress. Brain Behav. Immun. 1992;6(2):141–156. 10.1016/0889-1591(92)90014-F [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4. Kondo H, Morimoto K: Effects of mental arithmetic stress on blood cell counts and the immune system. Environ. Health Prev. Med. 1996;1(2):76–79. 10.1007/BF02931194 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5. Breen MS, et al. : Acute psychological stress induces short-term variable immune response. Brain Behav. Immun. 2016;53:172–182. 10.1016/j.bbi.2015.10.008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6. Segerstrom SC, Miller GE: Psychological stress and the human immune system: a meta-analytic study of 30 years of inquiry. Psychol. Bull. 2004;130(4):601–630. 10.1037/0033-2909.130.4.601 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7. Maydych V, et al. : Impact of chronic and acute academic stress on lymphocyte subsets and monocyte function. PLoS One. 2017;12(11):e0188108. 10.1371/journal.pone.0188108 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8. Godoy LD, et al. : A comprehensive overview on stress neurobiology: basic concepts and clinical implications. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 2018;12:127. 10.3389/fnbeh.2018.00127 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9. Talarowska ME, et al. : Immune to happiness–inflammatory process indicators and depressive personality traits. Arch. Med. Sci. 2019;15:1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10. Danese A, Lewis SJ: Psychoneuroimmunology of early-life stress: the hidden wounds of childhood trauma?. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2017;42(1):99–114. 10.1038/npp.2016.198 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11. Kuhlman KR, et al. : Developmental psychoneuroendocrine and psychoneuroimmune pathways from childhood adversity to disease. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2017;80:166–184. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.05.020 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12. Menke A, et al. : Childhood trauma dependent anxious depression sensitizes HPA axis function. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2018;98:22–29. 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2018.07.025 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13. Berk LS, Felten DL, Tan SA, et al. : Modulation of neuroimmune parameters during the eustress of humor-associated mirthful laughter. Altern. Ther. Health Med. 2001;7(2):62–72. 74–76. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14. Takahashi K, Iwase M, Yamashita K, et al. : The elevation of natural killer cell activity induced by laughter in a crossover designed study. Int. J. Mol. Med. 2001;8(6):645–650. 10.3892/ijmm.8.6.645 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15. Bittman BB, Berk LS, Felten DL, et al. : Composite effects of group drumming music therapy on modulation of neuroendocrine-immune parameters in normal subjects. Altern. Ther. Health. Med. 2001;7(1):38–47. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16. Bennett MP, Zeller JM, Rosenberg L, et al. : The effect of mirthful laughter on stress and natural killer cell activity. Altern. Ther. Health Med. 2003;9(2):38–45. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17. Burns SJ, Harbuz MS, Hucklebridge F, et al. : A pilot study into the therapeutic effects of music therapy at a cancer help center. Altern. Ther. Health. Med. 2001;7(1):48–56. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18. Matsuzaki T, Nakajima A, Ishigami S, et al. : Mirthful laughter differentially affects serum pro- and anti-inflammatory cytokine levels depending on the level of disease activity in patients with rheumatoid arthritis. Rheumatology (Oxford). 2006;45(2):182–186. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19. Hayashi T, Tsujii S, Iburi T, et al. : Laughter up-regulates the genes related to NK cell activity in diabetes. Biomed. Res. 2007;28(6):281–285. 10.2220/biomedres.28.281 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20. Buric I, Farias M, Jong J, et al. : What is the molecular signature of mind–body interventions? A systematic review of gene expression changes induced by meditation and related practices. Front. Immunol. 2017;8:670. 10.3389/fimmu.2017.00670 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21. Béres A, Lelovics ZS, Antal P, et al. : “A mosoly is gyógyít”? Beteg gyermekek immunválasza is változhat a Mosolygó Kórház Alapítvány művészeinek látogatásakor. [“Does happiness help healing?” Immune response of hospitalized children may change during visits of the Smiling Hospital Foundation’s Artists]. Orv. Hetil. 2011;152(43):1739–1744. 10.1556/OH.2011.29228 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22. Eisenberger NI, Taylor SE, Gable SL, et al. : Neural pathways link social support to attenuated neuroendocrine stress responses. NeuroImage. 2007;35(4):1601–1612. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.01.038 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23. Slavich GM, Way BM, Eisenberger NI, et al. : Neural sensitivity to social rejection is associated with inflammatory responses to social stress. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A. 2010;107(33):14817–14822. 10.1073/pnas.1009164107 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24. Koenig HG: Religion, spirituality, and health: the research and clinical implications. ISRN Psychiatry. 2012;2012: 278730. Published 2012 Dec 16. 10.5402/2012/278730 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25. Levin J, Meador K: Healing to all their flesh: Jewish and Christian perspectives on spirituality, theology, and health. Templeton Press;2012. [Google Scholar]
  • 26. Love P, Moore M, Warburton J: Nurturing spiritual well-being among older people in Australia: Drawing on Indigenous and non-Indigenous way of knowing. Australas J. Ageing. 2017;36(3):179–185. 10.1111/ajag.12284 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27. Giannoulis K, Giannouli V: Religiosity and spirituality in the era of the COVID-19 pandemic: An overview of exploring emotional parameters. ENCEPHALOS. 2021;58:24–28. [Google Scholar]
  • 28. Miller L: Spiritual awakening and depression in adolescents: a unified pathway or “two sides of the same coin”. Bull Menninger Clin. 2013;77(4):332–348. 10.1521/bumc.2013.77.4.332 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29. Anderson MR, Wickramaratne P, Svob C, et al. : Religiosity and Depression at Midlife: A Prospective Study. Religions (Basel). 2021;12(1):28. 10.3390/rel12010028 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30. Love P, Moore M, Warburton J: Nurturing spiritual well-being among older people in Australia: Drawing on Indigenous and non-Indigenous way of knowing. Australas J Ageing. 2017;36(3):179–185. 10.1111/ajag.12284 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31. Andrade C, Radhakrishnan R: Prayer and healing: A medical and scientific perspective on randomized controlled trials. Indian J. Psychiatry. 2009;51(4):247–253. 10.4103/0019-5545.58288 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32. Lissoni P, Messina G, Parolini D, et al. : A spiritual approach in the treatment of cancer: relation between faith score and response to chemotherapy in advanced non-small cell lung cancer patients. In Vivo. 2008;22(5):577–581. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33. Woods TE, Antoni MH, Ironson GH, et al. : Religiosity is associated with affective and immune status in symptomatic HIV-infected gay men. J. Psychosom. Res. 1999;46:165–176. 10.1016/S0022-3999(98)00078-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34. Kopp M, Székely A, Skrabski Á: Vallásosság és egészség az átalakuló társadalomban. Mentálhigiéné és Pszichoszomatika. 2004;5(2):103–125. 10.1556/Mental.5.2004.2.1 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 35. Shimomura T, Fujiki M, Akiyoshi J, et al. : Functional brain mapping during recitation of Buddhist scriptures and repetition of the Namu Amida Butsu: a study in experienced Japanese monks. Turk. Neurosurg. 2008 Apr;18(2):134–141. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36. Schjoedt U, Stødkilde-Jørgensen H, Geertz AW, et al. : Highly religious participants recruit areas of social cognition in personal prayer. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 2009 Jun;4(2):199–207. Epub 2009 Feb 25. 10.1093/scan/nsn050 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37. Silveira S, Bao Y, Wang L, et al. : Does a bishop pray when he prays? And does his brain distinguish between different religions? Psych J. 2015 Dec;4(4):199–207. 10.1002/pchj.116 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38. Baldwin PR, Velasquez K, Koenig HG, et al. : Neural correlates of healing prayers, depression and traumatic memories: A preliminary study. Complement. Ther. Med. 2016;27:123–129. 10.1016/j.ctim.2016.07.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39. Groopman J: God at the bedside. N. Engl. J. Med. 2004;350(12):1176–1178. 10.1056/NEJMp038206 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40. McGrath AE: Christian Theology: An Introduction. Oxford: Blackwell Publishers;1994. Chapter 6. [Google Scholar]
  • 41. Cohen S, Kamarck T, Mermelstein R: A global measure of perceived stress. J. Health Soc. Behav. 1983;24(4):385–396. 10.2307/2136404 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42. Dacie, Lewis: Practical Haematology. Amsterdam: Elsevier;2011. Chapter 4, Chapter 5. [Google Scholar]
  • 43. Hoffbrand AV, Moss PAH, Pettit JE: Essential Heamatology. Oxford: Blackwell Publishing;2006. [Google Scholar]
  • 44. Cooper MA, Fehniger TA, Caligiuri MA: The biology of human natural killer-cell subsets. Trends Immunol. 2001;22(11):633–640. 10.1016/S1471-4906(01)02060-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45. Antal P, et al. : A Bayesian view of challenges in feature selection: feature aggregation, multiple targets, redundancy and interaction. New Challenges for Feature Selection in Data Mining and Knowledge Discovery. 2008.
  • 46. Van Borkulo CD, Borsboom D, Epskamp S, et al. : A new method for constructing networks from binary data. Sci. Rep. 2014;4:5918. 10.1038/srep05918 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47. Jenkinson M, Bannister P, Brady JM, et al. : Improved Optimisation for the Robust and Accurate Linear Registration and Motion Correction of Brain Images. NeuroImage. 2002;17(2):825–841. 10.1006/nimg.2002.1132 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48. Avants BB, Epstein CL, Grossman M, et al. : Symmetric diffeomorphic image registration with cross-correlation: evaluating automated labeling of elderly and neurodegenerative brain. Med. Image Anal. 2008 Feb;12(1):26–41. 10.1016/j.media.2007.06.004 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49. Fischl B, Salat DH, Busa E, et al. : Whole brain segmentation: automated labeling of neuroanatomical structures in the human brain. Neuron. 2002;33:341–355. 10.1016/S0896-6273(02)00569-X [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50. Friston KJ, Holmes AP, Worsley KJ, et al. : Statistical Parametric Maps in Functional Imaging: A General Linear Approach. Hum. Brain Mapp. 1995;2:189–210. [Google Scholar]
  • 51. Lieberman MD, Cunningham WA: Type I and Type II error concerns in fMRI research: re-balancing the scale. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. December 2009;4(4):423–428. 10.1093/scan/nsp052 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52. Buhle JT, Silvers JA, Wager TD, et al. : Cognitive reappraisal of emotion: A meta-analysis of human neuroimaging studies. Cereb. Cortex. 2013 Jun 13;24:2981–2990. 10.1093/cercor/bht154 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53. Leff AP, Iverson P, Schofield TM, et al. : Vowel-specific mismatch responses in the anterior superior temporal gyrus: an fMRI study. Cortex. 2009 Apr;45(4):517–526. Epub 2008 Feb 7. 10.1016/j.cortex.2007.10.008 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54. Beaucousin V, Lacheret A, Turbelin MR, et al. : FMRI study of emotional speech comprehension. Cereb. Cortex. 2007 Feb;17(2):339–352. Epub 2006 Mar 8. 10.1093/cercor/bhj151 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55. Yarkoni T: Studies reporting activation within 2 mm of (-52, -56, 26). Neurosynth. Reference Source [Google Scholar]
  • 56. Jäncke L, Kleinschmidt A, Mirzazade S, et al. : The role of the inferior parietal cortex in linking the tactile perception and manual construction of object shapes. Cereb. Cortex. 2001 Feb;11(2):114–121. 10.1093/cercor/11.2.114 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57. Woodruff CC, Johnson JD, Uncapher MR, et al. : Content-specificity of the neural correlates of recollection. Neuropsychologia. 2005;43(7):1022–1032. Epub 2005 Jan 24. 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2004.10.013 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58. Young L, Saxe R: The neural basis of belief encoding and integration in moral judgment. NeuroImage. 2008 May 1;40(4):1912–1920. Epub 2008 Feb 14. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.01.057 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59. Cooper EA, Hasson U, Small SL: Interpretation-mediated changes in neural activity during language comprehension. NeuroImage. 2011 Apr 1;55(3):1314–1323.Epub 2011 Jan 11. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.01.003 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60. Drolet M, Schubotz RI, Fischer J: Explicit authenticity and stimulus features interact to modulate BOLD response induced by emotional speech. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 2013 Jun;13(2):318–329. 10.3758/s13415-013-0151-0 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61. Ferreira RA, Göbel SM, Hymers M, et al. : The neural correlates of semantic richness: evidence from an fMRI study of word learning. Brain Lang. 2015 Apr;143:69–80. 10.1016/j.bandl.2015.02.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62. Koenig HG, Cohen HJ: The Link between Religion and Health: Psychoneuroimmunology and the Faith Factor. Oxford: Oxford University Press; Jan 17,2002. 10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195143607.001.0001 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 63. Grün A, Robben MM: Finde deine Lebensspur. Die Wunden der Kindheit heilen. –Spirituelle Impulse. Verlag Herder GmbH: Freiburg;2001.
  • 64. Otto R: The Idea of the Holy. Oxford: Oxford University Press;1923. [Google Scholar]
  • 65. Andersen M, Schjoedt U, Nielbo KL, et al. : Mystical Experience in the Lab. Method Theory Study Relig. 2014;26(3):217–245. 10.1163/15700682-12341323 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 66. Beauregard M, Paquette V: Neural correlates of a mystical experience in Carmelite nuns. Neurosci. Lett. 2006;405(3):186–190. 10.1016/j.neulet.2006.06.060 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67. Newberg A, Alavi A, Baime M, et al. : The measurement of regional cerebral blood flow during the complex cognitive task of meditation: a preliminary SPECT study. Psychiatry Res. 2001;106:113–122. 10.1016/s0925-4927(01)00074-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68. Vago DR, Silbersweig DA: Self-awareness, self-regulation, and self-transcendence (S-ART): a framework for understanding the neurobiological mechanisms of mindfulness. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2012 Oct;6(6):296. 10.3389/fnhum.2012.00296 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69. Salopek P: To Walk the World. National Geographic Magazine; December2013. Reference Source [Google Scholar]
  • 70. Tillich P: Rendszeres teológia [Systematic Theology] . István S, translator. Budapest: Osiris;2003. [Google Scholar]
  • 71. Jung CG: The Archetypes and the Collective Unconscious Princeton: Princeton University Press;1959. [Google Scholar]
  • 72. Whitehead AN: Process and Reality New York: The Free Press;1978:347–348.351. [Google Scholar]
  • 73. Pannenberg W: Rendszeres teológia II. kötet [Systematic Theology, Volume 2]. Görföl T, translator. Budapest: Osiris;2006; pp.39.99. [Google Scholar]
  • 74. Chardin PT: The Phenomenon of Man . New York: Harper Perennial Modern Thought;2008; pp.293–294.302, 306. [Google Scholar]
  • 75. Pannenberg W: Systematic Theology. Michigan: Wm. B. Eerdmans Publishing Co.;1997; Volume3: p.335. (Chapter 13, III., § 2., “The Lord’s Supper and Christian Worship”) [Google Scholar]
  • 76. Béres A, Szabó P, Petőfalviné Á: Healing Study Beres et al. - Part 1: Psychological and Immunological Changes after meeting a Chaplain coupled with Biblical Readings among Hospitalized Patients. figshare.[Dataset].2021. 10.6084/m9.figshare.16750384.v1 [DOI]
  • 77. Béres A, Emri M, Aranyi C: Healing Study Beres et al. - Part 2: fMRI Changes after meeting a Chaplain coupled with Biblical Readings and Recalling the Visits with Audiotapes among Hospitalized Patients. figshare.[Dataset].2021. 10.6084/m9.figshare.16751851.v1 [DOI]
  • 78. Béres A, Emri M, Aranyi C, et al. : HEALING Study Beres et al. - F1000Research - extended data. figshare.[Dataset].2021. 10.6084/m9.figshare.17029715.v3 [DOI]
F1000Res. 2025 Jun 9. doi: 10.5256/f1000research.173734.r386606

Reviewer response for version 5

Kathy B Hoppe 1

This article examines the impact of a religious intervention on patients served in acute medical care who received visits from a church chaplain and Bible reading together. In blood draws and, in a smaller group, fMRI scans were performed. There was no central immunological effect, but changes in lymphocytes were linked to changes in angular gyrus brain activity, which is involved in word recognition. The research emphasizes the individualized and subjective character of spiritual care in individuals and how chaplains provide the support people need. It also examines the relationship between spirituality, the immune system, and the brain and potential health benefits of spiritual experiences in addition to the need for future research in this area.

While the researchers performed a sound study, there are several posits offered that may be overly confident or speculative. On page 15, the researchers state, "...remarkably, we did not obtain obvious false positive results." This might be overly confident considering the small sample size. This statement should acknowledge the possibility that Type I errors may still be present. In addition, on page 17, the following statement, "...the growing inconsistency in lymphocyte changes seemed to reveal a sense of fascination and admiration mixed with tones of fear," may be overly speculative based on immunological data alone. State this more tentatively. One final note is related to page 19, which states, "...could exert an influence on the patients' lymphocyte count and hence alter their recovery from the disease, depending on the depth at which the chaplain's core message is understood." This may infer a direct causal link between cognitive understanding and physiological changes. This may oversimplify the study's findings. 

The article addresses a theological framework, which is helpful. However, it remains unclear how such ideology relates to the interpretation of scientific results. A point of clarification would be helpful. Finally, the final statement on page 20, "Faith is an evolutionary advantage..." is a broad leap from the results of this study, which showed one single brain area. Focus more on how theology may help interpret the observations.

Otherwise, this study is remarkably interesting and further replication is encouraged.

Is the work clearly and accurately presented and does it cite the current literature?

Yes

If applicable, is the statistical analysis and its interpretation appropriate?

I cannot comment. A qualified statistician is required.

Are all the source data underlying the results available to ensure full reproducibility?

Yes

Is the study design appropriate and is the work technically sound?

Yes

Are the conclusions drawn adequately supported by the results?

Partly

Are sufficient details of methods and analysis provided to allow replication by others?

Yes

Reviewer Expertise:

trauma, secondary trauma, burnout, spirituality, theology

I confirm that I have read this submission and believe that I have an appropriate level of expertise to confirm that it is of an acceptable scientific standard, however I have significant reservations, as outlined above.

F1000Res. 2024 Oct 31. doi: 10.5256/f1000research.173734.r336511

Reviewer response for version 5

Philip R Baldwin 1

The authors attended to all my earlier comments very graciously. I recommend the work for indexing.

Is the work clearly and accurately presented and does it cite the current literature?

Yes

If applicable, is the statistical analysis and its interpretation appropriate?

Partly

Are all the source data underlying the results available to ensure full reproducibility?

Yes

Is the study design appropriate and is the work technically sound?

Partly

Are the conclusions drawn adequately supported by the results?

Partly

Are sufficient details of methods and analysis provided to allow replication by others?

Yes

Reviewer Expertise:

Applied math, functional mri, structural biology

I confirm that I have read this submission and believe that I have an appropriate level of expertise to confirm that it is of an acceptable scientific standard.

F1000Res. 2024 Sep 27. doi: 10.5256/f1000research.135237.r318896

Reviewer response for version 4

Philip R Baldwin 1

I think the work is important and I would like to encourage this line of inquiry as much as possible. I have been involved with a similar study

Neural correlates of healing prayers, depression and traumatic memories: A preliminary study" from August 2016.

I appreciate your feedback and understand the importance of adhering to rigorous scientific standards. I believe it would be beneficial to ensure that only significant results are quoted in the article. After a thorough revision to align with these guidelines, I would be happy to accept the changes.

For example, there should be no sentence in the opening summary of results like:

" Results: Lymphocyte counts increased more often after the more successful visits, but the immunological changes were not significant. "

All such statements should be moved to the discussion. It is not what ordinarily scientists would consider a result.

Is the work clearly and accurately presented and does it cite the current literature?

Yes

If applicable, is the statistical analysis and its interpretation appropriate?

Partly

Are all the source data underlying the results available to ensure full reproducibility?

Yes

Is the study design appropriate and is the work technically sound?

Partly

Are the conclusions drawn adequately supported by the results?

Partly

Are sufficient details of methods and analysis provided to allow replication by others?

Yes

Reviewer Expertise:

Applied math, functional mri, structural biology

I confirm that I have read this submission and believe that I have an appropriate level of expertise to state that I do not consider it to be of an acceptable scientific standard, for reasons outlined above.

F1000Res. 2024 Oct 24.
András Béres 1

Responses to Philip R. Baldwin (Reviewer#3) 

Thank you Philip R. Baldwin for undertaking the review of this clinical study, the significant outcome of which is the correlation of the fMRI measurements (left BA39) with an immune parameter (lymphocyte count).  

We are honored to now have a psycho-endocrinologist and two neuro-researchers review our article regarding the impact of discoveries in psycho-neuro-immunology (PNI) in clinical practice. Additionally, we are especially pleased to learn that the reviewers reflect the diverse representation of gender and geographic location that is advocated for by the F1000Research open publishing platform hosting this article.  

Please find below our responses to your comments.

“I believe it would be beneficial to ensure that only significant results are quoted in the article. After a thorough revision to align with these guidelines, I would be happy to accept the changes. 

For example, there should be no sentence in the opening summary of results like the following: ‘Results: Lymphocyte counts increased more often after the more successful visits, but the immunological changes were not significant.’ 

All such statements should be moved to the discussion. This is not what scientists would ordinarily consider to be a result.” 

Response: We are particularly pleased that you considered the presentation of the literature to be well-founded, the supporting data to be credible, and the presentation of the method to be reproducible. We trust that thanks to our detailed corrections, you will now consider the interpretation and conclusions to be acceptable. 

The non-significant findings are important from the standpoint of estimating the extent of neuro-immunomodulation in a clinical setting, but we have moved these results (and especially emerging trends) to the Discussion. The trends may be interesting to the reader because they resonate with previous studies (suggesting that the sample size of our study was underpowered for the measurement of immune changes), but the most important finding is that in a clinical environment, the possibilities of inducing psycho-immunological effects become narrower. 

At the same time, the fMRI-immune correlation gives us hope that even in this situation, burdened with limitations, the mechanisms that can be relied upon for recovery will remain.

F1000Res. 2024 Sep 12. doi: 10.5256/f1000research.135237.r322888

Reviewer response for version 4

Vaitsa Giannouli 1

This interesting article presents findings of a research on the immunological and neurological changes in hospitalized patients after meeting a chaplain coupled with biblical readings. One of the points that authors need to justify in the Methodology is the sample size. How was it estimated? The generalizability of the findings is under question due the small sample size of the three groups. In the Introduction (first paragraph) authors do not need to report statistics from other studies. In addition, in the Introduction authors need to add a separate section focusing on religion and spirituality as therapeutic factors in older patients and also discuss their role in different cultural contexts (e.g. for a relevant review which explains these two different concepts: http://www.encephalos.gr/pdf/58-2-02e.pdf). For relevant articles on the positive effects of these variables authors can find information in the following recently published articles (doi: 10.1007/978-3-030-32637-1_8

doi: 10.1007/978-3-031-31986-0_35). Please provide more details regarding the intervention (so researchers can replicate this study). Please describe in more detail the recruitment procedure. Please describe the figures in the main text as they are difficult to follow. One more point to consider for a major revision is the structure of the Discussion (please avoid starting with limitations), but present first the main findings and clinical applications and then the limitations.

Is the work clearly and accurately presented and does it cite the current literature?

Partly

If applicable, is the statistical analysis and its interpretation appropriate?

Partly

Are all the source data underlying the results available to ensure full reproducibility?

Partly

Is the study design appropriate and is the work technically sound?

Partly

Are the conclusions drawn adequately supported by the results?

Partly

Are sufficient details of methods and analysis provided to allow replication by others?

Partly

Reviewer Expertise:

Psychology and endocrine diseases

I confirm that I have read this submission and believe that I have an appropriate level of expertise to confirm that it is of an acceptable scientific standard, however I have significant reservations, as outlined above.

References

  • 1. : Subjective Quality of Life, Religiousness, and Spiritual Experience in Greek Orthodox Christians: Data from Healthy Aging and Patients with Cardiovascular Disease. Adv Exp Med Biol .2020;1196: 10.1007/978-3-030-32637-1_8 85-91 10.1007/978-3-030-32637-1_8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2. : Better Understand to Better Predict Subjective Well-Being Among Older Greeks in COVID-19 Era: Depression, Anxiety, Attitudes Towards eHealth, Religiousness, Spiritual Experience, and Cognition. Adv Exp Med Biol .2023;1425: 10.1007/978-3-031-31986-0_35 359-364 10.1007/978-3-031-31986-0_35 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
F1000Res. 2024 Oct 24.
András Béres 1

Response to  Vaitsa Giannouli (Reviewer#2)

Thank you, Vaitsa Giannouli for joining the open peer review process for our article.

We are honored to now have a psycho-endocrinologist and two neuro-researchers review our article regarding the impact of discoveries in psycho-neuro-immunology (PNI) in clinical practice. Additionally, we are especially pleased to learn that the reviewers reflect the diverse representation of gender and geographic location that is advocated for by the F1000Research open publishing platform hosting this article.

Please find below our detailed responses to your comments.

“One of the points that authors need to justify in the Methodology is the sample size. How was it estimated? The generalizability of the findings is under question due the small sample size of the three groups.” 

Response: We have now clearly indicated in the Introduction that the number of cases was principally limited by financial constraints. We were forced to limit our goals to the number of cases reaching that of the pilot studies conducted with healthy participants, which is addressed in the Introduction. 

We have also outlined the scope of conclusions that can realistically be drawn from the test results. Whether the observed changes would have been significant in relation to the same immune parameters with a larger number of cases remains an open question; however, owing to the same number of cases as in previous research, we can establish that compared to studies conducted with healthy participants, the magnitude and possibility of neuro-immunomodulation in the clinic is more limited.

“In the Introduction (first paragraph) authors do not need to report statistics from other studies.”  

Response: Based on the above, we consider it important to retain the brief references to the sample sizes of the pilot studies that we mention in the Introduction.  

As for the p-values, we consider it important to mention them as we now emphasize in the Introduction that influencing the immune system with psychological factors is already difficult, even in the case of healthy volunteers (as shown by several p-values ​​close to the limit value).

“In addition, in the Introduction, authors need to add a separate section focusing on religion and spirituality as therapeutic factors in older patients and also discuss their role in different cultural contexts.”  

Response: We have now amended the Introduction with a reference to religion and spirituality as therapeutic factors affecting health.  

It was interesting to read that quality of life-life satisfaction, religiosity, and spirituality were not strongly interconnected in your Greek Orthodox Christian sample (DOI:  10.1007/978-3-030-32637-1_8); this underscores how difficult it is to truly enforce the confirmed positive effects of spirituality on the entire body in the whirlwind of real life beyond clichés. Our study shows the same in the immersive experience of a hospital setting. 

This is also why we consider it appropriate and forward-thinking to incorporate theology into science by investigating the relationship between religion and health (as outlined in the expanded excursus: DOI:  10.1007/s10943-023-01888-3). Theology paves the way for understanding measured outcomes by demonstrating how the religious approach reinterprets the overall concept of health, which ultimately affects the choices and lifestyles of religious people. Christian theology is a striking example of this, where choosing a fuller life also implies taking up the cross, sometimes prompting choices that are in stark contrast to Western ideas of comfort.

“Please provide more details regarding the intervention (so researchers can replicate this study).”  

Response: We have now explained the intervention in more detail under "Intervention and measurements."

“Please describe in more detail the recruitment procedure.” 

Response:  We have now explained the recruitment procedure in more detail under "Enrollments,” with particular regard to the more detailed description of the HEAL Score. The HEAL Score template form can be downloaded from the Extended data (Appendix S1), as indicated in the article. 

According to our approach, the religious experience, due to its nature, cannot be examined in precisely the same way as other psychological experiences; even the elicitability of the experience and its measurability are questionable. If we assume that a transcendent spiritual experience can be considered an encounter between the individual and the transcendent Other, then the scientific investigation of this experience also has to take into account that the experience may be understood as both an inter-personal and intra-personal event. 

In this approach, the research design of natural scientific inquiry into a spiritual experience should focus on preserving the authentic nature of the encounter as much as possible.  When examining a religious experience, we consider it essential to ensure authenticity, personalization, and spontaneity, which we now further emphasize in the Discussion. 

“Please describe the figures in the main text as they are difficult to follow.”  

Response: Thank you for pointing this out; we have now included an explanation of the figures in the text.

“One more point to consider for a major revision is the structure of the Discussion (please avoid starting with limitations), but present first the main findings and clinical applications and then the limitations.” 

Response: Following Reviewer#3’s suggestion, the revised version focuses on the study’s more significant results; we have placed the non-significant results and limitations at the end of the Discussion.

We have concluded by addressing the issue of clinical applicability in order to summarize the overall picture emerging from the pattern of significant and non-significant findings: In a clinical setting, the possibilities of neuro-immunomodulation are narrower.

F1000Res. 2022 Feb 21. doi: 10.5256/f1000research.78267.r120035

Reviewer response for version 1

Andrew B Newberg 1

Regarding the article entitled, “Healing through faith: meeting a chaplain coupled with biblical readings could produce lymphocyte changes that correlate with brain activity (HEALING study)”, overall, this study is potentially quite interesting and would be of interest. However, the lack of positive results and the confusing presentation with heavy reliance on religious and theological discussion substantially limits the value of this article from a scientific perspective.

Introduction

The introduction needs to clearly state what the authors’ hypotheses were. Without this, it is difficult to understand what they are trying to accomplish in their study. Do they think that lymphocyte counts will increase or decrease? What correlations are they expecting? What parts of the brain are they anticipating will be involved.

They should consider elaborating on studies of mindfulness and other meditation practices on immune function.

Methods

The authors should clarify how subjects were allocated since they were not truly randomized, but allocated by convenience.

The authors state: The decision was based on the patient’s degree of need and willingness, as assessed by, and at the discretion of, the chaplain…

What does this sentence mean? Is the chaplain deciding who is included in the study? This seems highly problematic.

The authors state: The randomized control group consisted of patients who knew the goal of the measurement but were explicitly asked to help with their participating in the control group, i.e., they knew they weren’t going to meet the chaplain (by request, the encounter could be scheduled for a later occasion). Does this mean that they knew they were controls or just that they weren’t meeting with the chaplain?

The authors state blood collections occurred 30 minutes prior to and 60 minutes after the spiritual intervention. Can they provide an explanation as to why this particular timing? Is this based upon any data regarding the speed with which these serum markers can change?

Regarding the passages read to the subjects, is it possible that the Isiah chapter is something that the participants know well? Would it have been better to have read from some other passage of the bible such as a listing of names which has a potential neutral effect rather than something that is medically oriented?

Part I Results

This comes across as a very confusing section. The tables and figures are very confusing and in the end, it seems that no results were significant. Perhaps this section can be substantially altered to state that there were no significant findings. If there was a trend towards significance in any of the values, perhaps the authors can use this information to provide an effect size that might be useful for developing future studies. But otherwise, it does not seem appropriate to include tables and complex figures on data that is non-significant.

Part II Results:

Perhaps this was not clear, but were there differences between the active prayer block and the control block or just between prayer and silence? This might be particularly relevant and interesting in that hearing a religious recitation affects the brain differently than a neutral/scientific recitation. This might also be more relevant in terms of other physiological and psychospiritual measures. Otherwise, as with the section above, the authors should really only show the significant results and can delete Figure 11.

In the text, the authors state: After that, we aimed to correlate the dLy parameters with the areas showing change in the fMRI activity. An invert linear correlation emerged (p = 0.019 with dLy%, p = 0.003 with dLy-abs) between the change in activity of the left BA39 area and the change in lymphocyte counts

Can they clarify that this refers to the active minus silence condition? Again though, perhaps it would be more interesting to see correlations between the active and scientific control?

Discussion:

The first few sentences of the Discussion do not seem to be relevant to the main discussion and should be deleted:

Taking Salopek’s “slow journalism” concept as a model [he makes a 21,000 mile-long journey (33.780 km) by foot, to retrace the pathways of our ancestors, the first humans who migrated from Africa and journeyed around the Earth - he posits that one must devote time to inter-personal connections in order to fully comprehend the human phenomenon - “Out of Eden Walk,” 35 ], we consciously stood for the concept of “slow science” for a slow pace of methodology. 

In the Discussion, the authors refer to the auto-control setting. Can the authors clarify what they mean by this?

It is not clear why the SHoRT trial is being used for comparison since this was a study of children. It seems that the design is completely different as well focusing on artistic components rather than spiritual. This should either be deleted or expanded more to clarify why this is an appropriate study for comparison.

The authors state that the intervention was designed to elicit positive emotions, but may have resulted in other emotions as well. The authors should review articles on positive emotions and immune function. In addition, there should be greater clarification of what other emotions were elicited. Did they acquire data for this?

Can the authors clarify the following passage? What do they mean by a “guiding principle”?

however, the correlation of these changes with the fMRI-s indicated that even when biblical readings did not appear to have a physiological effect, they could have an ordering, arranging effect, along some specific guiding principle, on a key parameter like the lymphocyte counts.

It is appreciated that the authors try to connect the angular gyrus to spiritual aspects of the experience, but this needs to be clarified in the Introduction as well. On the other hand, the authors state: Thus, the brain area involved in the correlation described above (gyrus angularis) could also be linked to the “Aha! moment”. Why is there suddenly discussion about an Aha moment when this has not been discussed before and there is no mention of it in the methods or results? This should probably be deleted.

Can the authors clarify why there were few significant findings? Perhaps it would be better to discuss the meaning of the lack of significant findings.

The entire last part of the Discussion beginning with the paragraph that starts: “When one is examining the effect of the Word of God on man,…” seems to be speculation, theology, and not based on the scientific results presented. This whole section should be deleted.

While the goal of the authors is appreciated, it seems that the limitations of the article described above are substantial.

Is the work clearly and accurately presented and does it cite the current literature?

Partly

If applicable, is the statistical analysis and its interpretation appropriate?

No

Are all the source data underlying the results available to ensure full reproducibility?

Partly

Is the study design appropriate and is the work technically sound?

Partly

Are the conclusions drawn adequately supported by the results?

No

Are sufficient details of methods and analysis provided to allow replication by others?

Partly

Reviewer Expertise:

Neuroimaging, meditation

I confirm that I have read this submission and believe that I have an appropriate level of expertise to state that I do not consider it to be of an acceptable scientific standard, for reasons outlined above.

F1000Res. 2022 Feb 21.
András Béres 1

First of all, thank you Andrew Newberg for accepting our invitation and becoming the first to openly peer-review this paper. Thank you for going through the article so thoroughly with us.  

This is to confirm that yes, it is the correlation between change in lymphocytes, and difference in activation between the active and the scientific control block that was significant (not between the active and the silence block; the abbreviation "s" refers to the "scientific text" 's block).

This is also to clarify that main purpose of the study was to measure the potential immunological effects of personal interventions that are primarily intended to elicit positive emotions at the bedside, among patients lying in the hospital. In the everyday practice of a hospital, this practically translates to visits by artists for the child-, and by chaplains for the adult population. This explains why we can refer to these two, seemingly different interventions on the same page in this study. "SHoRT" Study examined the effect of artists among sick children, "HEALING" Study examined the effect of a chaplain among sick adults.   

The methodological challenge was that genuine meetings with chaplains in hospitals always contain a spontaneous ("inspired") element, the very creation of such authentic meetings is always based on the chaplain's intuition, and this spontaneous element has a major effect on the whole meeting - and hence on any immunological imprint, would there be any. This spontaneity and the large room left for the chaplain's inspiration reflects the original setting of the biblical times in which the "Word of God" was firstly said, when such meetings happened as a spontaneous act of the participants - i.e., this is how Jesus met people. Had we taken this spontaneous element out, we would have suffocated the very phenomenon of which we were trying to estimate the potential immunological imprint. 

This methodological decision to leave the spontaneous element in, does not affect the reproducibility of the study - to the contrary, by proposing the possibility of the meeting for EVERY patient available on a ward on the day of the measurement, by establishing a rough order for the visits ("HEAL Score") to help manage the time available, but by always leaving to the chaplain the final choice of the patient she intends to visit, and only asking the chaplain to include the same biblical passage in every spontaneous meeting: these are the ways this study proposes to be reproducible anytime and anywhere in the world. 

The question raised was whether meetings with a chaplain can elicit immunological changes in patients, IF these meetings are left as authentical, as spontaneous as they originally are.      

It is the determination of a concrete group of patients among which to choose, the instruction for the chaplain to remain spontaneous and the permanence of the concrete biblical passage read that are fixed. 

In contrast to the previous studies on praying and meditation, the main phenomenon that this study was focused on was the effect of a spiritual meeting - a meeting with the chaplain and through her, with the "Word of God". Thus, the phenomenon in focus in this study was not praying, but meeting. 

It is the religious sensibilities raised by the topic touched by this measurement, and to avoid any potential distorsions in the interpretation of the results that we found, that we felt obliged to include the theological viewpoint in the discussion of the results. I.e. the reader - believer or atheist - could later ask: WHY have we not measured more immunological changes, if the "Word of God" is believed to heal? And, if lymphocyte counts are correlated with an area of the brain involved in understanding words, does that mean that those who TRY to understand the biblical passage better, will have more healing lymphocytes? These are theological questions.         

Thank you for being with us on this journey.  

We hope that these clarifications will prove to be meaningful for you. We believe these are also the main topics the readers will possibly face while summing the article up in themselves. 

Based on the Editorial Team's suggestion, we will be waiting for additional peer review reports before starting on the article revisions, in which we will address all the issues raised in full detail. We hope these improvements will render the article acceptable for you.

F1000Res. 2022 Mar 17.
András Béres 1

We are now publishing a revised version (v3) of our article, in order to address your report in full detail. 

To this we would add that the Isaiah chapter was expected to be less known by the participants, since it is seldom quoted and most of the enrolled patients were not actively practicing their faith; the proposition raised in your report, to use a different control as neutral, and to read from some other passage of the Bible such as a listing of names, is very interesting.

A potentially surprising twist to this idea is that such a list of names, as neutral as it seems to be from a grammatical point of view, is not neutral from a spiritual point of view. Indeed, central in Judaism and in Christian piety is the assumption that the  whole Bible - and not just parts of it - is sacred, the emotionally exuberant and the less spectacular verses alike. Interestingly, then, even the listing of names could hold a deeper meaning in this context: in line with Hellinger's theory stating that our "self" cannot be conceived as an individual being, but our subconscious is deeply rooted in the soul of our family, that of our ancestors, in the wider sense of the whole family of mankind (1, 2), the recitation of ancient names is portent of deep memories, thus can be evocative, and has the potential to move the deepest layers of the collective subconscious.  

The interesting question is whether these potential effects can be measured with fMRI, and whether this additional spiritual content would only be perceived by the religious participants, or some unintended imprints would also be detectable among the atheists. What do you think? 

In the current measurement, the usage of a text without any spiritual content as control seemed more appropriate, to maximally differentiate it from the biblical reading used in the active block.

The comparison of biblical passages with different emotional content during fMRI, as well as the more sensitive measurement of the immunological parameters (by means of flow-cytometry for example, a tool that we could not afford due to financial constraints) are lines along which further research could develop - but this already paves the way for a series of completely new measurements.    

References

(1) Hellinger B:  Rachel Weeping for Her Children: Family Constellations in Israel. Heidelberg, Germany: Carl-Auer-International; 2003.

(2) Hellinger B:  Peace begins in the soul: Family constellations in the service of reconciliation (C. Beaumont, Trans.). Heidelberg, Germany: Carl-Auer-Systeme Verlag; 2003.

F1000Res. 2022 Jun 17.
András Béres 1

Dear Andrew B. Newberg,

We are now publishing a further revised version of the article "Healing through faith: meeting a chaplain coupled with biblical readings could produce lymphocyte changes that correlate with brain activity (HEALING study)”, in order to fully address the concerns you articulated regarding Version1.

Introduction

Based on your advice, we have now clarified what our hypotheses were, what we were expecting to find in regards of changes in the immune response and the involvement of which brain areas could be anticipated. The truth is we intentionally started this clinical trial with as few preconceptions as possible, to remain open to any significant correlation that could emerge between the parameters we could afford to measure in a clinical setting – some of which are traditionally considered to correspond to the body, some others to the soul. Our measurement is a pilot study in this respect.  

Methods

We have further clarified why the subjects were not randomized by chance, but by convenience i.e., depending on the availability of the chaplain on the days of the measurement (when the laboratory staff was available for the extra measurements and when the fMRI equipment could be used for the study). Allocating by chance would not have been a difficult option technically but could have resulted in significant distortions in the supposed psycho-immunological effects.  Indeed, by the time the patients were classified as eligible and randomizable in this study, they represented this tiny portion of patients who were truly in need of a visit by the chaplain. The cancellation/postponement of the visit could have resulted in significant stress, disappointments, by which means the measurement method would have disproportionately influenced the parameters measured in the control group. 

In the intervention group, it was the chaplain who decided whom she wanted to visit among the patients who were randomizable. In the Discussion, we explain in detail why we opted for this methodological decision – the main goal was not to interfere with the genuine, authentic character of the chaplain-patient relationship that we were aiming to understand, and that always contains such a spontaneous element.   

The timing of the blood collections (30 minutes prior, and 60 minutes after the meeting) is now explained, it was based on the similar intervals used by previous, basic psycho-immunological studies that served as our starting point and that are mentioned in the Introduction (especially Berk et al., Bennett et al., Matsuzaki et al.). 

We believe the Isaiah chapter we used is, although not an entirely neglected, but a rarely used passage in religious practice, moreover, most of the patients enrolled were not actively practicing their faith. As to the usage of another biblical passage such as a listing of names for neutral control, we refer you back to our response in Version3 – even “neutral” passages can be portent of a “numinous” component. The contrast between a sacred passage intended to offer comfort, and a medical text with potentially fearful connotations was more marked, and therefore helped bring out the potential differences better.     

Part I Results

Based on your suggestions, we have now significantly simplified this section and moved all the figures and tables containing null-results to the Extended data, so as not to overwhelm the reader with data that is non-significant. Also based on your suggestions we added data on effect size when there seemed to be a trend towards significance that might help developing further studies. 

Part II Results

We have unambiguously clarified the meaning of the “a”, “s” and “c” abbreviations.

There were no general differences that could be unanimously detected among all the patients, between the active prayer block and the control block, so hearing a religious recitation did not affect the brain differently than a neutral/scientific recitation, unanimously for all the patients enrolled in this study – but the main finding of the study is that a significant correlation between lymphocytes and brain activity emerged with the active minus scientific control block. This finding totally echoed previous psycho-immunological findings and is now highlighted in the Discussion.

We hope your assessment of the statistical analysis being not appropriate was related to the confusing presentation of the null results, and that this issue is now fixed. The statistical tools used for interpretation, especially for fMRI evaluation are detailed in the Methods section and the Results part now is now limited to the precise description of the positive findings with some careful allusion to possible trends. We added the r value for the main correlation found.    

Discussion

We left the reference to Salopek’s “Out of Eden Walk” in the Discussion but put it to the end of the first part, to show that this reference is meant to clarify the methodological decisions of the authors – a constant effort to produce authentic encounters at the bedside or in the chapel, even at the cost of a very slow pace of enrollment. We further illustrate this concept with the example of two patients whom we enrolled by proactively trying to overcome the special difficulties raised by their disabilities. Only a very personified study design could allow for this.

Also, the term” auto-control” setting is now clarified in the text.

It is particularly important that patients’ individual results are first compared to their own baseline values (difference in their own activation patterns during intervention and silence; difference in their own lymphocyte counts), in the context of a study using a cohort design, with many different patients enrolled.    

The SHoRT trial is used for comparison in the Discussion because either the artistic interventions for hospitalized children or the spiritual interventions for hospitalized adults were originally implemented in clinical practice with the implicit intent to elicit positive emotions among patients, to alleviate the burden caused by the disease. This is now explained in the Introduction, and based on your suggestion, the revised Introduction has been considerably enriched with additional key articles on positive emotions and immune function. These additional references help understand why the current study needed to put so much emphasis on proper patient selection, authenticity of the interventions; they also put the current study’s sample size, measured immune parameters, obtained p values, direction of change (increase or decrease) in perspective, as compared with similar basic studies.

The revised Introduction also points towards a more nuanced approach to the visits by a person of faith, with a highlight on the religious undertones, and the revised Discussion also elaborates on that the intervention originally designed to elicit positive emotions, may have resulted in other emotions as well. The lymphocyte changes in the subgroup who received communion gives a striking example. The „Healing questionnaire” designed to record rough psychological aspects of the meetings, like emotional depth, perceived turmoil, correspondence with the chaplain as assessed by the participants… shows lines for further research, but the amount and length of questionnaires needed to be considerably restrained in the current setting, so as not to overwhelm the patients with documents to be completed - that could have interfered with the meetings themselves. As the Discussion acknowledges, the different emotional aspects of the spiritual experience in hospitals will need further research.            

The concept of a “guiding principle” having an arranging effect on the lymphocyte counts is now clarified in the Discussion.

Based on your suggestion, we clarified in the Introduction how the angular gyrus could be involved in the spiritual experience, so that its appearance in the Discussion does not come across as a totally unexpected finding. Also based on your suggestion we deleted the allusion to the ‘Aha moment’ from the Discussion (correlations are possible but seem too remote to be dealt in detail in the Discussion of this study). 

Finally, although we keep the final part of the Discussion for the theological perspectives, we’d like to highlight that this section is called “Excursus”. We think it is always interesting to see how empiric observations relate to the way theology thinks about God. This is not meant as a substitute to replace the scientific discussion, but a way to complement it.   

We hope that with these significant revisions in the Discussion, your assessment regarding the conclusions as not having been drawn as adequately supported by the results is now changed.

We are honored by your accepting to review this study. We honestly appreciate your reviewing it so thoroughly and hope our revisions have sufficiently addressed your concerns so that you now find the article acceptable from a scientific perspective.

Associated Data

    This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

    Data Citations

    1. Béres A, Szabó P, Petőfalviné Á: Healing Study Beres et al. - Part 1: Psychological and Immunological Changes after meeting a Chaplain coupled with Biblical Readings among Hospitalized Patients. figshare.[Dataset].2021. 10.6084/m9.figshare.16750384.v1 [DOI]
    2. Béres A, Emri M, Aranyi C: Healing Study Beres et al. - Part 2: fMRI Changes after meeting a Chaplain coupled with Biblical Readings and Recalling the Visits with Audiotapes among Hospitalized Patients. figshare.[Dataset].2021. 10.6084/m9.figshare.16751851.v1 [DOI]
    3. Béres A, Emri M, Aranyi C, et al. : HEALING Study Beres et al. - F1000Research - extended data. figshare.[Dataset].2021. 10.6084/m9.figshare.17029715.v3 [DOI]

    Data Availability Statement

    Underlying data

    Figshare: Healing Study Beres et al. - Part 1: Psychological and Immunological Changes after meeting a Chaplain coupled with Biblical Readings among Hospitalized Patients, https://doi.org/10.6084/m9.figshare.16750384.v1. 76

    Figshare: Healing Study Beres et al. - Part 2: fMRI Changes after meeting a Chaplain coupled with Biblical Readings and Recalling the Visits with Audiotapes among Hospitalized Patients, https://doi.org/10.6084/m9.figshare.16751851.v1. 77

    Extended data

    Figshare: HEALING Study Beres et al. - F1000Research - extended data, https://doi.org/10.6084/m9.figshare.17029715.v3. 78

    Reporting guidelines

    Figshare: CONSORT checklist for “Healing through faith: meeting a chaplain coupled with biblical readings could produce lymphocyte changes that correlate with brain activity (HEALING study)”, https://doi.org/10.6084/m9.figshare.17029715.v3. 78

    Data are available under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International license (CC-BY 4.0).


    Articles from F1000Research are provided here courtesy of F1000 Research Ltd

    RESOURCES