Skip to main content
Wellcome Open Research logoLink to Wellcome Open Research
. 2025 May 23;9:657. Originally published 2024 Nov 7. [Version 2] doi: 10.12688/wellcomeopenres.23278.2

Cognitive bias modification for social anxiety: protocol for a living systematic review of human studies and meta-analysis

Jaycee Kennett 1,2,a, Claire Friedrich 1,2, Virginia Chiocchia 3, Simon E Blackwell 4, Toshi Furukawa 5, Per Carlbring 6, Thomy Tonia 3, Ava Homiar 1,2, Simonne Wright 7, Kelvin Opiepie 8, Richardson Mojica 9, Paulina Schenk 10, Susan Michie 10, Janna Hastings 11,12, Hossein Dehdarirad 13, Claire Stansfield 13, James Thomas 13, Jennifer Potts 1,2, Georgia Salanti 3, Andrea Cipriani 1,2
PMCID: PMC12149809  PMID: 40495937

Version Changes

Revised. Amendments from Version 1

This version of the manuscript incorporates revisions based on the feedback from both reviewers. The abstract and the background sections have been re-worded for more clarity and depth on the context of our review. In addition, we have added more detail to our research methods, specifically how we propose to classify nodes for the network meta-analysis as well as giving more context as to the need for a network meta-analysis. We have also clarified how we intend to assess risk of bias in response to reviewer comments.

Abstract

Background

Social anxiety is a heightened fear and discomfort in social situations. Cases of elevated distress and impaired functioning can lead to a clinical diagnosis of social anxiety disorder. Altering cognitive biases associated with social anxiety has been suggested as potentially beneficial; however, little is known about the comparative effectiveness of such interventions. The aim of this living systematic review is to examine the efficacy of cognitive bias modification for reducing social anxiety.

Methods

We will search multiple electronic databases for randomised controlled trials evaluating the efficacy of cognitive bias modification for people diagnosed with social anxiety and people exposed to a social stressor. The primary outcome will be change in social anxiety related symptoms; secondary outcomes will be changes in social functioning and quality of life and adverse events. Study selection, data extraction and risk of bias assessment will be done by at least two reviewers using pre-defined tools. We will synthesise data from people with social anxiety diagnosis and those subjected to a simulated social stressor separately using random effects meta-analyses. Heterogeneity will be evaluated by investigating characteristics of included studies and we will conduct a network meta-analysis in order to compare the efficacy of subtypes of cognitive bias modification for social anxiety disorder. We will appraise the strength of the evidence for each outcome by reviewing the overall association, internal and external validity, and reporting biases. Where data allows, we will triangulate the evidence from both sources with a multidisciplinary group of experts. We will also descriptively report factors reported to mediate cognitive bias modification, The review will begin in living mode and the database search will be rerun every three months to identify potential new evidence. We will co-produce this review with members of a global lived experience advisory board. This protocol was registered on 15.10.2024 (CRD42024601380)..

Keywords: GALENOS, social anxiety, cognitive bias modification

Background and research questions

Background

Anxiety is characterised by symptoms of worry and apprehension, psychological and autonomic arousal ( American Psychiatric Association, 2022). Social anxiety refers to these symptoms that happen specifically in the context of social situations ( Szuhany & Simon, 2022). This may involve everyday encounters such as having a conversation, eating or drinking with others, or performing in front of others ( e.g. giving a speech), and the feelings of anxiousness occur disproportionately to the actual threat posed by the social situation and the surrounding sociocultural context ( Morrison & Heimberg, 2013). These symptoms stem from a concern that the person will be judged negatively by others and may include feelings of humiliation, embarrassment. ( American Psychiatric Association, 2022; World Health Organisation, 2021). If social anxiety is persistent and causes significant impairment it may be diagnosed as social anxiety disorder (SAD). Individuals with SAD have been shown to preferentially attend to negative information about social situations, interpret ambiguous information as threatening, and display a bias towards remembering experiences more negatively. ( Stopa, 2009). Biases affecting which information is attended to are categorised as ‘attentional biases’ whereas biases affecting how information is interpreted is are categorised as interpretation biases ( MacLeod & Mathews, 2012). There is also evidence to suggest that memory biases play a role in SAD ( Krans, de Bree & Bryant, 2014). These cognitive biases are thought to play a causal role in the development of SAD and its persistence ( Heimberg et al., 2010).

Cognitive Bias Modification (CBM) interventions aim to alter these biases via simple repetitive training procedures and by doing so improve symptoms of mental disorders ( Vrijsen et al., 2024). CBM is an umbrella term which includes interventions which specifically aim to alter attentional biases (CBM-A), or interpretation biases (CBM-I). During these interventions, stimuli is typically presented via a computer and the patient completes dot-probe tasks in short sessions, multiple times a week, although CBM paradigms vary significantly ( MacLeod & Mathews, 2012). CBM intervention studies for SAD have shown mixed results; while some randomised controlled trials (RCTs) have found CBM-A to be effective in reducing SAD symptoms ( Amir et al., 2009; Schmidt et al., 2009), other studies have failed to replicate these findings ( Julian et al., 2012). Early claims of promise for attention bias modification were soon followed by criticisms of the evidence base ( e.g., Kruijt & Carlbring, 2018). There is evidence that certain kinds of CBM interventions may be effective in reducing symptoms for a range of disorders such as depression and anxiety ( Fodor et al., 2020), and alcohol addiction ( Boffo et al., 2019). It remains unclear as to which subtypes of CBM are more effective than others for specific disorders. The effects of CBM have also been investigated via altering cognitive biases in healthy volunteers and measuring emotional or physiological responses to a social stressor (e.g. Vassilopoulos et al., 2015). This enables investigation of the link between the modulation of cognitive biases and emotional and physical responses in a situation where social anxiety is induced ( MacLeod & Mathews, 2012).

Cognitive behavioural therapy (CBT) remains the most often recommended first line treatment for SAD ( Pelissolo et al., 2019); CBT consists of 1:1 sessions with a mental health professional, usually once a week for a minimum of six weeks, contrasting with CBM which can be delivered without the guidance of a mental health professional and specifically targets a cognitive bias. However, there are numerous logistical and financial barriers to accessing psychological services such as CBT, particularly in the Global South. As CBM can be delivered digitally and without instruction, it represents a potentially cost-effective intervention that could be used in countries in which access to professional psychological support is limited. As people with SAD are less likely to access in-person mental healthcare due to the nature of their disorder, CBM potentially offers an effective method of providing support to them ( Goetter et al., 2020). Moreover, CBT is now a complex intervention involving different techniques, each having their foundation in behaviourist or cognitive theories, which make their further improvement also complex. By contrast, CBM is more mechanistic and founded in experimental psychology and therefore may lend itself to more straightforward refinement ( Holmes et al., 2018).

A better understanding of the CBM literature on SAD is crucial to improving these interventions, as is disentangling both the effectiveness of different subtypes of CBM and its mechanisms. Understanding more about which subtypes are most effective and by examining the mechanisms by which cognitive biases can be changed most effectively will improve the targeting of interventions and provide a better starting point for future CBM research.

This living systematic review (LSR) will use MacLeod and Mathews’s (2012) definition of cognitive bias, defining it as “a systematic selectivity in information processing that operates to favour one type of information over another" (p2). CBM will be used as an umbrella term for interventions that target any cognitive bias, defining it as directly altering a specific cognitive bias by repeatedly exposing individuals to tasks that promote predetermined patterns of selective processing ( MacLeod & Mathews, 2012). To examine the association between CBM and symptoms of social anxiety, it will draw on two sources of evidence; a) individuals with diagnosed SAD (hereafter referred to as the clinical source of evidence) and b) people without any mental health condition. This will include people who may have elevated levels of social anxiety/sub clinical social anxiety but have not been diagnosed with SAD, and individuals not recruited for studies based on having SAD. These two sources of evidence will each be examined using separate network meta-analyses and will be combined through triangulation. In order to capture updates in this rapidly growing area of research, this LSR will be updated every three months and its findings, underlying data, and code will be open access.

Review objectives

  • To review the evidence of the effects of CBM and its subtypes on social anxiety symptoms in people with social anxiety disorder.

  • To review the evidence of the effects of CBM and its subtypes on social anxiety in people without any mental health condition.

  • To describe the potential mediators of the effects of CBM on the symptoms of social anxiety.

Research questions

  • 1.

    What are the effects of CBM on reducing symptom severity in individuals with SAD?

  • 2.

    What are the effects of CBM on changing social anxiety in individuals not diagnosed with any mental health condition?

  • 3.

    What mediates the effects of CBM on social anxiety?

Methods

Study inclusion and exclusion criteria

The population, intervention, comparator, and outcomes that we will include and exclude for both sources of evidence are detailed in Table 1 and Table 2.

Table 1. Study inclusion and exclusion criteria for source of evidence: individuals with SAD.

Study design We will include:
   •  Randomised controlled trials
   •  Studies published in any language and in any year
Population We will include:
   •  Participants with a clinical diagnosis of social anxiety disorder or above-threshold
symptoms on any standardised scale used to diagnose social anxiety disorder
   •  Participants of any age

We will exclude:
   •  Participants with social anxiety disorder as a secondary diagnosis and who were recruited
based on another mental health diagnosis 
   •  Participants who are undergoing any other psychological treatment during the study
period
Experimental
interventions/exposures
We will include:
   •  An intervention of CBM, structured and designed to alter a cognitive bias related to social
situations. We will accept any number of sessions or any delivery format
   •  Interventions which include multi-component CBM

We will exclude:
   •  CBM as augmentation of a pharmacological or psychological intervention which occur
concurrently
Control interventions/
non-exposures
We will include:
   •  Inactive controls such as a) waitlist control or b) no intervention
   •  Non-specific controls such as a) sham CBM, b) placebo training, c) attention control
condition, d) neutral control condition (i.e. a task that does not involve any CBM, or e)
other interventions that aim to control for aspects of the CBM intervention such as time
spent completing tasks or interaction with the experimental setup, but which are not
intended to affect cognitive biases
   •  Negative controls (where participants are trained towards the opposite cognitive bias to
the intervention)

We will exclude:
   •  Any form of current psychotherapy (defined as a directed psychological intervention
designed to improve distress)
Outcomes Studies will be included irrespective of outcomes reported.

Primary outcome:
   •  Change in social anxiety severity from pre to post intervention

Secondary outcomes:
   •  Acceptability (number of drop-outs due to any reason)
   •  Tolerability (number of drop-outs due to adverse events)
   •  Number of participants reporting any adverse events
   •  Number of participants reporting each specific adverse event. To mitigate variability
in how adverse events are reported we will use the Medical Dictionary for Regulatory
Activities to harmonise and organise terminology related to adverse events
   •  Change in cognitive bias measured as self-report measures or attentional or interpretive
bias tasks
   •  Clinical global impression of improvement
   •  Change in diagnostic status (no longer meeting diagnostic criteria)
   •  Change in general anxiety levels ( e.g. GAD-7, BAI)
   •  Change in general depression levels ( e.g. PHQ-9, BDI-II)
   •  Change in quality of life/measures of subjective wellbeing
   •  Change in symptoms of social functioning

Table 2. Study inclusion and exclusion criteria for the source of evidence: individuals without any mental health condition.

Study design We will include:
   •  Randomised controlled trials
   •  Studies published in any language and in any year
Population We will include:
   •  Participants who have not been recruited based on having a diagnosis of SAD
   •  Participants of any age
   •  Participants exposed to an analogue social stressor during the study (i.e. a task or
situation that aims to induce mild social anxiety)

We will exclude:
   •  Participants recruited for having a specific physical or mental health diagnosis
   •  Participants who are undergoing any other psychological treatment during the study
period
Experimental
interventions/exposures
We will include:
   •  An intervention of CBM, structured and designed to alter a cognitive bias relating to
stressful social situations. We will accept any number of sessions or any delivery format.
   •  Interventions which include multi-component CBM

We will exclude:
   •  CBM as augmentation of a pharmacological or psychological intervention which occurs
concurrently
Control interventions/
non-exposures
We will include:
   •  Inactive controls such as waitlist control or no intervention
   •  Non-specific controls such as a) sham CBM, b) placebo training, c) attention control
condition d) neutral control condition (i.e. a task that does not involve any CBM or e)
other interventions that aim to control for aspects of the CBM intervention such as time
spent completing tasks or interaction with the experimental setup, but which are not
intended to affect cognitive biases
   •  Negative controls (where participants are trained towards the opposite cognitive bias to
the intervention)

We will exclude:
   •  Any form of current psychotherapy (defined as a directed psychological intervention
designed to improve distress)
Outcomes Studies will be included irrespective of outcomes reported. 


  Primary outcomes:  
   •  Change in social anxiety-related symptoms, defined below

  Secondary outcomes:
   •  Acceptability (number of drop-outs due to any reason)
   •  Tolerability (number of drop-outs due to adverse events)
   •  Number of adverse events
   •  Number of participants with a specific adverse event to mitigate variability in how
adverse events are reported we will use the Medical Dictionary for Regulatory Activities to
harmonise and organise terminology related to adverse events
   •  Change in cognitive bias

Study identification

The search strategy will be defined in collaboration with the search team and informed by prior research ( Fodor et al., 2020; Martinelli et al., 2022; Spijker et al., 2023). The ontology team will be informed of the search strategy and will help identify additional search terms where possible and relevant. The resulting search strategy will also inform the scope of the ontology ( Schenk et al., 2024).

The primary approach in developing the search strategy will focus on searching titles, abstracts, keywords and database-controlled vocabulary on the concepts of Cognitive Bias Modification (CBM) and studies with a randomised experimental design. To enhance sensitivity, a range of terms for CBM will be used. In addition, to supplement these terms, terms are used to find research using cognitive bias detection methods within the context of social anxiety.

In this initial iteration, we will search the following databases covering health, psychology and multi-disciplinary research fields: CENTRAL (Cochrane Library), Embase (OVID), MEDLINE (OVID), PsycINFO (OVID), Scopus, Web of Science (SCI, SSCI, ESCI and the related conference and book indexes). LILACS, WHO Index Medicus, the trial registries (ClinicalTrials.gov, ScanMedicine, WHO-ICTRP) will also be searched. Searches will be adapted and simplified where necessary. Dissertation abstracts will be excluded. In addition, reference lists of key relevant reviews known the to review team will be manually checked. See the extended data for the search strategy for Medline (OVID) as an example ( Kennett et al., 2024). This protocol was registered on PROSPERO (CRD42024601380) on 15.10.24.

Study selection

Once the searches have been completed, records will be imported into EPPI-Reviewer ( Thomas et al., 2020) and duplicates will be removed. The titles and abstracts of the identified records will be examined by two members of the review team who will independently mark each one as eligible or ineligible. Disagreements between the two will be resolved through discussion or in consultation with a third reviewer. The full texts of all records deemed eligible will then be retrieved and screened by two members of the review team, disagreements will be resolved in the same way. In the case that a full text of an eligible abstract cannot be located, corresponding authors will be contacted. In the event of no response, a follow up email will be sent one month later. If no response is received, the study will be included, and we will continue to attempt to contact the authors in further iterations of the review. Additional researchers with knowledge of different languages will be recruited if needed to assist with the study selection and data extraction process. The selection process will be recorded and reported in a PRISMA flowchart ( Page et al., 2021a). Excluded studies will be reported along with reasons for exclusion in a 'Characteristics of excluded studies' table.

Outcomes and prioritisation

Source of evidence: participants with SAD

Primary outcomes

Our primary outcome of interest will be the change in social anxiety symptom severity from pre- to post-intervention, measured with validated clinician-rated or self-reported measures ( e.g. Liebowitz Social Anxiety Scale (LSAS, Liebowitz, 1987).

Secondary outcomes

  • We will seek data for the acceptability of CBM (measured by the number of total dropouts and the number of dropouts due to adverse events),

  • We will seek data on the tolerability of CBM (dropouts due to adverse events), as well as details of the reasons given for dropouts due to any reason and details of any adverse events reported to occur in CBM studies.

  • We will also seek to examine data on change in cognitive bias following the intervention, either self-reported or measured on attentional or interpretive tasks.

  • Additionally, we will include quality of life as a secondary outcome as individuals with social anxiety often report poor quality of life ( Dryman et al., 2016). This was prioritised by our Global Lived Experience Advisory Board (GLEAB), as an important element of social anxiety to consider.

  • We will also seek data on clinical global impression of improvement, change in participants diagnostic status (whether, following treatment, they were judged not to have SAD) and levels of depression and levels of general anxiety.

Source of evidence: participants without any mental health condition

Primary outcomes

The primary outcome for the non-clinical source of evidence will be change in social anxiety related symptoms via observer or self-reported scales. The most recent versions of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-5-TR) and the International Classification of Diseases (ICD-11) characterises social anxiety disorder as feelings of fear, beliefs that one’s actions will be negatively evaluated by others, and avoidance of social interactions ( American Psychiatric Association, 2022; World Health Organisation, 2021). In their systematic review of social anxiety measures, Wong et al. (2016) echo the importance of these domains, in addition to reporting that social anxiety measures also seek to capture physiological elements, feelings of a racing heart, increased perspiration, and faster breathing. As research on populations without any mental health conditions is unlikely to have utilised clinical social anxiety scales, to capture symptoms related to social anxiety, this systematic review will seek data from measures that are intended to capture the following areas:

  • Avoidance behaviours

  • Feelings of fear

  • Belief in future negative evaluation

  • Heart rate variability

  • Blood pressure

  • Skin conductance

Where it is unclear whether a particular measure relates to social anxiety-related symptoms, content experts will be consulted. However, where a nonclinical study does report and use measures pertaining to social anxiety symptom severity, we will prioritise these.

Secondary outcomes

For the evidence on individuals not diagnosed with any mental health condition, the secondary outcomes will be the same as for the SAD source of evidence, detailed above although we will not seek data on global clinical impression improvement, quality of life and subjective wellbeing, general anxiety or depression levels, or changes in social functioning.

Prioritisation of effect measures

It is anticipated that measures used and reported across trials will vary, where a trial reports more than one measure of the same outcome we will follow a predefined hierarchy for selecting the measure, as follows:

  • Where a trial reports a measure as their primary outcome we will prioritise this;

  • If they do not clarify a primary outcome, we will select the one which is not self-reported;

  • If all measures are self-reported, we will select the one with best psychometric properties;

  • If they have similar psychometric properties, we will select the one which has most commonly been used in other studies including in our review.

For the primary outcomes (social anxiety symptom severity) we will publish a list of all measures that data was extracted for and rationale for prioritising each measure that will be included in the analyses

Mediators

To address research question three; examining potential mediators influencing effects of CBM on social anxiety, we will extract data on mediators in the included studies as a secondary outcome and describe these narratively.

Data extraction

Data will be extracted using EPPI-Reviewer ( Thomas et al., 2020). Before extraction begins, the ontology team will review the data extraction form to identify relevant ontology categorisations to support data extraction. The form will be piloted and if subsequent revisions are required, these will be made prior to beginning data extraction. Two reviewers will independently extract data using the final extraction form and any differences will be reconciled through discussion or involvement of a third reviewer. We will extract:

  • Study duration (in months)

  • Year of study completion

  • Date of publication

  • Conflicts of interest

  • Population characteristics: number of participants randomised (total, per arm), age, gender as reported in study, ethnicity, medical diagnoses and treatment information

  • Intervention characteristics: number of sessions, setting where the intervention takes place, how the intervention is delivered, duration of intervention, stimulus type, and type of cognitive bias targeted

  • Comparator: whether the comparator is a placebo or inactive control, if the comparator is a non-specific or negative control, we will extract the same level of detail as for the intervention

  • Outcomes

Extraction of outcome data

For continuous outcomes, data will be extracted as means and standard deviations at baseline and post-intervention timepoints, if available, or as change from baseline. If standard deviation is not reported, it will be calculated manually from standard error. If standard error is also missing, it will be calculated from reported test statistics, confidence intervals (CIs) or alternative reported distribution ( e.g. median/range). If a study uses more than one outcome measure (continuous and dichotomous), we will prioritise the continuous outcome.

Where studies report number of events ( e.g. dropouts or number of participants reporting adverse events) we will extract the numbers reported. Where studies report these as percentages, we will convert these to absolute numbers. Any participant who is randomised but does not complete the final assessment in the study will be considered a dropout due to any reason.

When there is missing outcome data, we will examine trial registries and clinical study reports. If this is not possible, we will contact the authors and if there is no reply, we will send one follow up email one month later. Where studies have used methods that account for missing data, we will give preference to mixed models of repeated measurement (MMRM) and multiple imputations, followed by last observation carried forward (LOCF) and observed cases. If a study reports both completer and imputed analyses, we will prioritise imputed data. In the case of a study reporting multiple time points, we will prioritise the data reported at the end of the intervention. If this is not available, we will extract the next closest post-intervention timepoint.

Data extraction for mediators

Where mediators are reported in the included studies ( e.g. levels of attention) we will extract the mediator, the relevant outcome, the measure used to examine the mediating effect and report the association found using the exact statements in the study regardless of statistical significance. Data will be extracted on any mediator. Previously reported mediators of CBM are:

Risk of bias

The Risk of Bias 2 (RoB 2) tool will be used to assess risk of bias for randomised controlled trials for each outcome ( Sterne et al., 2019). The overall risk of bias will be graded as: low risk, if no domains are rated high risk and no more than one domain is rated some concerns; high risk, if any domain is rated high risk; and some concerns, if more than one domain is rated as having some concerns and no domains are rated high risk. The risk of bias for all outcomes of all studies will be assessed by two reviewers and any disagreements settled by discussion or by a third reviewer. The risk of bias assessments for each outcome will inform the certainty of the evidence, and presented in the summary of evidence table, as well as in traffic light plots.

Data analysis and synthesis

If different scales are used to measure the same outcome, we will standardise the direction for the outcome using score inversion. For example, this may occur when the Situational Confidence Questionnaire ( Annis & Graham, 1988) is used to measure social anxiety, where a higher score indicates improvement in social anxiety symptoms. If data from this scale were to be extracted in this review, the direction of scores would be standardised to have the same direction as the a priori selected outcome: social anxiety symptom severity, where higher scores indicate greater symptom severity.

To report the relative effects of CBM for continuous outcomes, we will calculate mean difference if the included studies have all used the same scale. If the included studies use different scales, we will calculate standardised mean difference (SMD). For dichotomous outcomes such as adverse events, odds ratio (OR) will be used as a measure of effect. 95% confidence intervals will be reported for all outcomes.

Pairwise meta-analysis

We will perform a quantitative synthesis via a pairwise meta-analysis using a frequentist random effects model for all CBM interventions versus control conditions. Heterogeneity will be assessed by visual inspection of forest plots and presented by the 95% prediction intervals. Restricted Maximum Likelihood (REML) will be used to estimate the variance in heterogeneity τ, and 95% confidence intervals will be corrected using a Hartung Knapp correction should more than five studies contribute to the analysis.

Network meta-analysis

NMA allows for the indirect comparison of interventions which may not have been tested head-to-head and allows the ranking of interventions against one another. We expect there to be significant heterogeneity within the control conditions used in CBM trials, particularly among sham conditions ( Fodor et al., 2020). However, previous reviews have been critiqued for making the assumption that all sham conditions, despite involving different components, could be adequately lumped as one control group ( Blackwell, 2020). CBM trials often utilise sham conditions that attempt to match the intervention group as closely as possible, however qualitative differences between these conditions may pose a problem when combining them for pairwise meta-analysis ( Blackwell et al., 2017). If enough data for the same outcome is available, we will conduct a quantitative synthesis using a random effects network meta-analysis (NMA) model, separating the different control conditions and the different CBM approaches. We will check that clinical and methodological characteristics that could act as effect modifiers (see investigation of heterogeneity below) are distributed similarly across comparisons to ensure that the assumption of transitivity is not violated.

We will assume a common heterogeneity parameter (τ 2) across treatment comparisons in the NMA random effects model. Relative treatment effects will be reported in forest plots and league tables, with treatments ordered according to ranking based on the surface under the cumulative ranking curve (SUCRA) ( Salanti et al., 2011). Data analysis will be conducted in R statistical software using the packages meta ( Balduzzi et al., 2019), netmeta ( Balduzzi et al., 2023), and NMA ( Noma et al., 2024).

We have specified a list of CBM interventions that we expect will from nodes in the NMA based on previous reviews (see Table 3).

Table 3. List of expected interventions.

Abbreviation Intervention
IBM / CBM-I Interpretive Bias Modification
ABM / CBM-A Attention Bias Modification
MBM Memory Bias modification
CBM-MC Multi-Component CBM
CBM-App Cognitive Bias Modification- Appraisal
AIM Attention Interpretations Modification
VRET Virtual Reality Exposure Therapy

However, as there are often novel adaptations of CBM paradigms (see Martinelli et al., 2022), we expect this list will not be exhaustive. If a novel variant of CBM is reported, the research team will adjudicate the extent to which CBM variants can be lumped into nodes or be a stand-alone node, considering the methodological and clinical implications of doing so. An example of a novel variant of CBM is in the study by Rohrbacher et al. (2014) where standard CBM-I was compared to a variant of CBM-I which included participants taking part in an image generation task as part of the intervention. This adjudication will be done after data have been collected, but before data have been analysed and no researchers involved in data extraction will be involved in the adjudication. When important uncertainty or disagreement persist among the research team, we will decide on the primary model before we conduct the meta-analyses and use the remaining models as sensitivity analyses, before we conduct the meta-analyses. Where disagreement persists, we will highlight this in our final review.

Similarly, we have specified a list of control interventions that we expect to find and that will form different nodes in our network (see Table 4).

Table 4. List of expected control conditions.

Control condition
Opposite ABM
Opposite CBMI
Sham
Waitlist
Treatment as usual

To ensure the network is both methodologically valid and clinically relevant, we will determine to what extent heterogenous control interventions should be lumped via the adjudication process described above, after collection of data but without knowledge of the outcome. If a NMA is conducted, we will assess inconsistency in the network using both local and global methods ( Salanti, 2012), respectively the SIDE (separating-indirect-evidence-from-direct-evidence) test and the design-by-treatment interaction test ( Dias et al., 2010; Higgins et al., 2012)

Investigation of heterogeneity

We will investigate potential sources of heterogeneity and inconsistency by exploring the following effect modifiers in subgroup analyses:

  • At least one of the authors has allegiance bias (is involved in the CBM intervention that is tested)

  • Delivery mode of CBM (in person/online/via mobile)

  • Number of sessions (a posteriori categorisation)

  • Cognitive bias targeted; interpretive, attentional, or memory bias

Sensitivity analysis

We will perform a sensitivity analysis for the primary outcome excluding studies with high risk of bias. We will also use sensitivity analysis, limiting the clinical group to individuals diagnosed with SAD by a medical professional, not just those with above threshold symptom scores.

Synthesis of mediators

The details of any mediators of CBM on social anxiety symptoms in the included studies will be descriptively reported.

Reporting bias

Reporting bias for the pairwise meta-analysis will be assessed using the Risk of Bias due to Missing Evidence tool ( Page et al., 2021b). Should an NMA be conducted, we will assess reporting bias using the Risk of Bias due to Missing Evidence in Network meta-analysis (ROB-MEN) tool ( Chiocchia et al., 2021). We will consider small study outcomes effects such as publication bias by visually inspecting contour enhanced funnel plots ( Peters et al., 2008).

Summary of the evidence

We will assess the certainty of the evidence for each outcome for each source of evidence (individuals with and without SAD ). Summary of Evidence (SoE) tables will be produced for each outcome for each source of evidence with outcomes as rows and domains of confidence as columns (see Table 5 and Table 6). The confidence in the evidence will be assessed by a single reviewer and checked by a second reviewer. Disagreements will be resolved by discussion or involvement of a third reviewer. Should a pairwise meta-analysis and an NMA be conducted, we will prioritise the pairwise meta-analysis.

Table 5. Example summary of Evidence (SoE) table for the source of evidence: individuals diagnosed with SAD.

Outcome Source of
evidence
Timepoint Summary
of the
association
Bias due to study
limitations
Bias due to
reporting bias
Bias due to
indirectness:
Bias due to
other reasons
The
effects of
CBM on
outcome
X
Studies on
individuals
diagnosed
with SAD
End of
study
Number of
studies, total
number of
participants.

Numerical
summary of
meta-analysis
with point
estimate, 95%
confidence
and prediction
intervals.
Number of studies
rated low, some
concerns, and
high in RoB 2 tool,
expected direction
of bias ( e.g. over- or
underestimation of
the true effect).

Assessment of the
internal validity of
the findings using
the sensitivity
analysis using only
studies with low
risk of bias.
Assessment of
the impact of
reporting bias
on the size and
direction of the
bias based on
findings using
the ROB-ME
tool ( Page et al., 2021b)
Assessment of
the potential
impact of bias
and its direction
on the magnitude
and size of the
effect due to the
included studies
not capturing
the intended
population,
intervention,
comparator, and
outcome.
Assessment of
the impact of any
other sources
of bias. We will
follow a thorough
review process to
minimise other
sources of bias.

Table 6. Example summary of Evidence (SoE) table for the source of evidence: individuals not diagnosed with any mental health condition.

Outcome Source of
evidence
Timepoint Summary
of the
association
Bias due to
study limitations
Bias due to
reporting bias
Bias due to
indirectness:
Bias due to
other reasons
The
effects of
CBM on
outcome
X
Studies on
individuals
without
any mental
health
condition
end of
study
Number of
studies, total
number of
participants.

Numerical
summary of
meta-analysis
with point
estimate, 95%
confidence
and prediction
intervals.
Number of studies
rated low, some
concerns, and
high in RoB 2
tool, expected
direction of bias
( e.g. over- or
underestimation
of the true effect).

Assessment of the
internal validity of
the findings using
the sensitivity
analysis using only
studies with low
risk of bias.
Assessment of
the impact of
reporting bias
on the size and
direction of the
effect based on
findings using
the ROB-ME
tool ( Page et al., 2021b)
Assessment of
the potential
impact of bias
and its direction
on the magnitude
and size of the
effect due to the
included studies
not capturing
the intended
population,
intervention,
comparator, and
outcome.
Assessment of
the impact of any
other sources
of bias. We will
follow a thorough
review process to
minimise other
sources of bias.

Triangulation of the evidence from living systematic reviews

It has been argued that SAD can be distinguished from feelings of nervousness and shyness in social situations neurobiologically ( Liu et al., 2015), cognitively ( Lavoie et al., 2014), and in terms of quality of life ( Stein & Kean, 2000). As such, we will consider the two sources of evidence in this living systematic review– people diagnosed with SAD and those without a mental health condition –sufficiently different to potentially warrant triangulation methods to consider their results together. Where there is sufficient evidence for one outcome from both sources and the sources’ respective errors and biases are deemed to be unrelated, we will discuss these outcomes in a triangulation meeting. This will include members of the review team, methodological experts, and experts in CBM and SAD. The SoE tables from both sources of evidence will be used in the triangulation meeting to draw an overall conclusion on the effect of CBM on social anxiety, based on the direction of the results and the strength of the evidence.

Updating the systematic review and ending the living mode of the review

Updating the living systematic review

We will conduct searches every three months to identify new evidence that is relevant to the review; the number of databases may be reduced for the update searches, if some databases do not provide a unique yield of relevant references in the first search. As we find new evidence, the authors of the last iteration of the review will assess the evidence and based on its potential to substantially change the overall findings, we will incorporate it into the review. We will report all updates using the versioning system of F1000 (Wellcome Open Research), highlight the new evidence added, and specify when each update has been performed. After the conclusion of the initial version of the systematic review, the methods will be reconsidered to judge their suitability and efficiency in practice. If deemed appropriate, the methodological approaches of data extraction and synthesis will be adapted accordingly, and any changes will be documented. We will also investigate the use of machine learning (including large language models) for automating some of the study selection (and possibly data extraction, should technology advance sufficiently) tasks once the review is in living mode. We will do this by testing and training prompts to enable a large language model (GPT-4o) to correctly identify relevant and irrelevant studies. We will do this in EPPI-reviewer in a similar way to Shemilt, Noel-Storr, and Thomas et al. (2022) and will soon publish a paper on our training of the model on a separate LSR. The code will be open source and the data will be put on the Open Science Framework. Where we can demonstrate that we can save manual effort without compromising the reliability of the review, we will use these new technologies to make our overall workflows more efficient. If deemed appropriate by the review team, inclusion and exclusion criteria will be adapted as necessary with changes being reported on our versioning system. Any deviations from this protocol will be reported with justification.

Ending the living mode of the review

At each triangulation meeting, the decision to continue the living mode of the review will be discussed, and for which outcomes it should continue. If it is decided that it is unlikely that new evidence changing the results of the review will be published and that the research aims of the review have been fulfilled, the review will exit living mode. If there is not enough evidence to call for a triangulation meeting, the review will continue in living mode with its planned searches.

Co-production aspects

This protocol has been written in collaboration with people with lived experience. The question was selected by members of GALENOS’ Global Lived Experience Advisory Board (GLEAB) during a prioritisation exercise, in which several questions were ranked against a criterion that was decided upon by the group. Two members of the GLEAB are part of the author team for this review and have contributed to the writing of this protocol, including: the formulation of the research aims, the outcomes of importance, and the design of the review.

The lead author was provided with guidance on how to effectively include people with lived experience in their research, while members of the GLEAB were provided with training on systematic reviews.

Considering the complexity and multidimensionality of the review topic, we will establish a schedule of regular team meetings and foster effective communication within the GALENOS project. The primary objective of these initiatives is to facilitate a shared understanding, promote the transferability of knowledge, encourage the exchange of ideas and perspectives, and identify the distinct needs of various stakeholders. By implementing these measures, we aim to create an environment where all stakeholders have equal standing and can actively contribute to the collaborative production of the review.

Dissemination of information

We plan to publish the review on the GALENOS website and on Wellcome Open Research. A plain language summary will accompany the review. We will use social media outlets to publicise the results and will write blog posts that will be available on the GALENOS website. We will also include the results in the quarterly Research Roundup newsletter that MQ issues. We hope to present GALENOS at the World Congress of Biological Psychiatry as well as other conferences.

Study status

The study status at the date of submission 15.20.2024 is reported below.

Preliminary searches

Started, not completed

Piloting the study selection process

Not started

Piloting the study selection process

Not started

Full searches

Not started

Full screening of search results against eligibility criteria data extraction

Not started

Risk of bias or quality assessment

Not started

Data synthesis

Not started

Ethics and consent

Ethical approval and consent were not required.

Funding Statement

We are funded by Wellcome, a global charitable foundation.

The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.

[version 2; peer review: 2 approved, 1 approved with reservations]

Data and software availability

Underlying data

No data are associated with this article.

Extended data

OSF: Cognitive bias modification for social anxiety: protocol for a living systematic review of human studies and meta-analysis. https://doi.org/10.17605/OSF.IO/2W69Z ( Kennett et al., 2024).

Reporting guidelines

OSF: PRISMA-P checklist for ‘Cognitive bias modification for social anxiety: protocol for a living systematic review of human studies and meta-analysis’. https://doi.org/10.17605/OSF.IO/2W69Z ( Kennett et al., 2024).

Data are available under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International license (CC-BY 4.0).

Software availability

We will use EPPI-Reviewer for study selection and data extraction ( Thomas et al., 2020, available at EPPI-Reviewer: systematic review software).

CRediT author statement

Jaycee Kennett: Conceptualisation, Methodology, Writing – Original Draft, Project Administration

Claire Friedrich: Conceptualisation, Methodology, Writing – Original Draft

Virginia Chiocchia: Methodology, Writing – Review and Editing, Supervision

Simon E. Blackwell: Conceptualisation, Writing – Review and Editing

Toshi Furukawa: Conceptualisation, Methodology, Writing – Review and Editing, Supervision, Funding Acquisition

Per Carlbring: Conceptualisation, Writing – Review and Editing

Thomy Tonia: Writing – Review and Editing

Ava Homiar: Writing – Review and Editing

Simonne Wright – Writing – Review and Editing

Kelvin Opiepie: Writing – Review and Editing

Richardson Mojica: Writing – Review and Editing

Paulina Schenk: Writing – Review and Editing

Susan Michie: Writing – Review and Editing

Janna Hastings: Writing – Review and Editing

Hossein Dehdarirad: Methodology, Writing – Review and Editing

Claire Stansfield: Methodology, Writing – Review and Editing

James Thomas: Methodology, Writing – Review and Editing

Jen Potts: Writing – Review and Editing, Project Administration

Georgia Salanti: Conceptualisation, Methodology, Writing – Review and Editing, Supervision, Funding Acquisition

Andrea Cipriani: Conceptualisation, Methodology, Writing – Review and Editing, Supervision, Funding Acquisition

References

  1. American Psychiatric Association: Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders 5th edition, Text Revision (DSM-5-TR).American Psychiatric Association Publishing, VA, USA,2022. 10.1176/appi.books.9780890425787 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  2. Amir N, Beard C, Taylor CT, et al. : Attention training in individuals with Generalized Social Phobia: a randomized controlled trial. J Consult Clin Psychol. 2009;77(5):961–973. 10.1037/a0016685 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Annis H, Graham JM: Situational Confidence Questionnaire (SCQ): user's guide.Addiction Research Foundation,1988. Reference Source [Google Scholar]
  4. Balduzzi S, Rücker G, Nikolakopoulou A, et al. : netmeta: an R package for network meta-analysis using frequentist methods. J Stat Softw. 2023;106(2):1–40. 10.18637/jss.v106.i02 37138589 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  5. Balduzzi S, Rücker G, Schwarzer G: How to perform a meta-analysis with R: a practical tutorial. Evid Based Ment Health. 2019;22(4):153–160. 10.1136/ebmental-2019-300117 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Beadel JR, Smyth FL, Teachman BA: Change processes during Cognitive Bias Modification for obsessive compulsive beliefs. Cognitive Ther Res. 2014;38(2):103–119. 10.1007/s10608-013-9576-6 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  7. Blackwell SE: Clinical efficacy of Cognitive Bias Modification interventions. Lancet Psychiatry. 2020;7(6):465–467. 10.1016/S2215-0366(20)30170-X [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Blackwell SE, Woud ML, MacLeod C: A question of control? Examining the role of control conditions in experimental psychopathology using the example of Cognitive Bias Modification research. Span J Psychol. 2017;20:E54. 10.1017/sjp.2017.41 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Boffo M, Zerhouni O, Gronau QF, et al. : Cognitive Bias Modification for behavior change in alcohol and smoking addiction: Bayesian meta-analysis of individual participant data. Neuropsychol Rev. 2019;29(1):52–78. 10.1007/s11065-018-9386-4 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Chiocchia V, Nikolakopoulou A, Higgins JPT, et al. : ROB-MEN: a tool to assess Risk Of Bias due to Missing Evidence in Network meta-analysis. BMC Med. 2021;19(1): 304. 10.1186/s12916-021-02166-3 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Dias S, Welton NJ, Caldwell DM, et al. : Checking consistency in mixed treatment comparison meta-analysis. Stat Med. 2010;29(7–8):932–944. 10.1002/sim.3767 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Dryman MT, Gardner S, Weeks JW, et al. : Social anxiety disorder and quality of life: how fears of negative and positive evaluation relate to specific domains of life satisfaction. J Anxiety Disord. 2016;38:1–8. 10.1016/j.janxdis.2015.12.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Fodor LA, Georgescu R, Cuijpers P, et al. : Efficacy of Cognitive Bias Modification interventions in anxiety and depressive disorders: a systematic review and network meta-analysis. Lancet Psychiatry. 2020;7(6):506–514. 10.1016/S2215-0366(20)30130-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Goetter EM, Frumkin MR, Palitz SA, et al. : Barriers to mental health treatment among individuals with social anxiety disorder and generalized anxiety disorder. Psychol Serv. 2020;17(1):5–12. 10.1037/ser0000254 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Heimberg RG, Brozovich FA, Rapee RM: A cognitive behavioral model of social anxiety disorder: update and extension.In: Social Anxiety.Elsevier,2010;395–422. 10.1016/B978-0-12-375096-9.00015-8 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  16. Higgins JPT, Jackson D, Barrett JK, et al. : Consistency and inconsistency in network meta-analysis: concepts and models for multi-arm studies. Res Synth Methods. 2012;3(2):98–110. 10.1002/jrsm.1044 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Holmes EA, Ghaderi A, Harmer CJ, et al. : The Lancet Psychiatry Commission on psychological treatments research in tomorrow's science. Lancet Psychiatry. 2018;5(3):237–286. 10.1016/S2215-0366(17)30513-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Julian K, Beard C, Schmidt NB, et al. : Attention training to reduce attention bias and social stressor reactivity: an attempt to replicate and extend previous findings. Behav Res Ther. 2012;50(5):350–358. 10.1016/j.brat.2012.02.015 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Kennett J, Friedrich C, Chiocchia V, et al. : Cognitive Bias Modification for social anxiety: protocol for a living systematic review of human studies and meta-analysis.[Data].2024. 10.17605/OSF.IO/2W69Z [DOI]
  20. Krans J, de Bree J, Bryant RA: Autobiographical memory bias in social anxiety. Memory. 2014;22(8):890–897. 10.1080/09658211.2013.844261 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Kruijt AW, Carlbring P: Processing confusing procedures in the recent re-analysis of a Cognitive Bias Modification meta-analysis. Br J Psychiatry. 2018;212(4):246. 10.1192/bjp.2018.41 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Lavoie MA, Battaglia M, Achim AM, et al. : A meta-analysis and scoping review of social cognition performance in social phobia, posttraumatic stress disorder and other anxiety disorders. J Anxiety Disord. 2014;28(2):169–177. 10.1016/j.janxdis.2013.09.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Liebowitz MR: Social phobia. Mod Probl Pharmacopsychiatry. 1987;22:141–73. 10.1159/000414022 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Liu F, Guo W, Fouche JP, et al. : Multivariate classification of social anxiety disorder using whole brain functional connectivity. Brain Struct Funct. 2015;220(1):101–115. 10.1007/s00429-013-0641-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. MacLeod C, Mathews A: Cognitive Bias Modification approaches to anxiety. Annu Rev Clin Psychol. 2012;8(1):189–217. 10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-032511-143052 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Martinelli A, Grüll J, Baum C: Attention and Interpretation cognitive bias change: a systematic review and meta-analysis of bias modification paradigms. Behav Res Ther. 2022;157: 104180. 10.1016/j.brat.2022.104180 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Morrison AS, Heimberg RG: Social anxiety and social anxiety disorder. Annu Rev Clin Psychol. 2013;9(1):249–274. 10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-050212-185631 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Nieto I, Vazquez C: Disentangling the mediating role of modifying interpretation bias on emotional distress using a novel Cognitive Bias Modification program. J Anxiety Disord. 2021;83: 102459. 10.1016/j.janxdis.2021.102459 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Noma H, Maruo K, Tanaka S, et al. : NMA: Network Meta-Analysis based on multivariate meta-analysis models. R package version 1.4–3,2024. Reference Source
  30. Page MJ, McKenzie JE, Bossuyt PM, et al. : The PRISMA 2020 statement: an updated guideline for reporting systematic reviews. BMJ. 2021a;372:n71. 10.1136/bmj.n71 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Page MJ, Sterne JAC, Boutron I, et al. : Risk Of Bias due to Missing Evidence (ROB-ME): a new tool for assessing risk of non-reporting biases in evidence syntheses.2021b; (accessed 01 September 2024). Reference Source
  32. Pelissolo A, Abou Kassm S, Delhay L: Therapeutic strategies for social anxiety disorder: where are we now? Expert Rev Neurother. 2019;19(12):1179–1189. 10.1080/14737175.2019.1666713 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Peters JL, Sutton AJ, Jones DR, et al. : Contour-enhanced meta-analysis funnel plots help distinguish publication bias from other causes of asymmetry. J Clin Epidemiol. 2008;61(10):991–6. 10.1016/j.jclinepi.2007.11.010 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Price RB, Wallace M, Kuckertz JM, et al. : Pooled patient-level meta-analysis of children and adults completing a computer-based anxiety intervention targeting attentional bias. Clin Psychol Rev. 2016;50:37–49. 10.1016/j.cpr.2016.09.009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Rohrbacher H, Blackwell SE, Holmes EA, et al. : Optimizing the ingredients for imagery-based interpretation bias modification for depressed mood: is self-generation more effective than imagination alone? J Affect Disord. 2014;152–154:212–218. 10.1016/j.jad.2013.09.013 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Salanti G: Indirect and mixed-treatment comparison, network, or multiple-treatments meta-analysis: many names, many benefits, many concerns for the next generation evidence synthesis tool. Res Synth Methods. 2012;3(2):80–97. 10.1002/jrsm.1037 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Salanti G, Ades AE, Ioannidis JPA: Graphical methods and numerical summaries for presenting results from Multiple-Treatment Meta-analysis: an overview and tutorial. J Clin Epidemiol. 2011;64(2):163–171. 10.1016/j.jclinepi.2010.03.016 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Salemink E, van den Hout M, Kindt M: How does Cognitive Bias Modification affect anxiety? Mediation analyses and experimental data. Behav Cogn Psychother. 2010;38(1):59–66. 10.1017/S1352465809990543 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Schenk PM, Hastings J, Santilli M, et al. : Towards an ontology of mental health: protocol for developing an ontology to structure and integrate evidence regarding anxiety, depression and psychosis [version 2; peer review: 1 approved, 3 approved with reservations]. Wellcome Open Res. 2024;9:40. 10.12688/wellcomeopenres.20701.2 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Schmidt NB, Richey JA, Buckner JD, et al. : Attention training for generalized social anxiety disorder. J Abnorm Psychol. 2009;118(1):5–14. 10.1037/a0013643 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Shemilt I, Noel-Storr A, Thomas J, et al. : Machine learning reduced workload for the cochrane COVID-19 study register: development and evaluation of the cochrane COVID-19 study classifier. Syst Rev. 2022;11(1):15. 10.1186/s13643-021-01880-6 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Spijker R, Dinnes J, Glanville J, et al. : Searching for and selecting studies. Cochrane Handbook for Systematic Reviews of Diagnostic Test Accuracy. 2023;97–129. 10.1002/9781119756194.ch6 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  43. Stein MB, Kean YM: Disability and quality of life in social phobia: epidemiologic findings. Am J Psychiatry. 2000;157(10):1606–1613. 10.1176/appi.ajp.157.10.1606 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Sterne JAC, Savović J, Page MJ, et al. : RoB 2: a revised tool for assessing risk of bias in randomised trials. BMJ. 2019;366: l4898. 10.1136/bmj.l4898 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Stopa L: Why is the self important in understanding and treating social phobia? Cogn Behav Ther. 2009;38(Suppl 1):48–54. 10.1080/16506070902980737 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Szuhany KL, Simon NM: Anxiety disorders: a review. JAMA. 2022;328(24):2431–2445. 10.1001/jama.2022.22744 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Thomas J, Graziosi S, Brunton J, et al. : EPPI-Reviewer: advanced software for systematic reviews, maps and evidence synthesis.EPPI-Centre, UCL Social Research Institute, University College London,2020. https://eppi.ioe.ac.uk/cms/default.aspx?tabid=1913
  48. Vassilopoulos SP, Brouzos A, Andreou E: A multi-session attribution modification program for children with aggressive behaviour: changes in attributions, emotional reaction estimates, and self-reported aggression. Behav Cogn Psychother. 2015;43(5):538–548. 10.1017/S1352465814000149 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Vrijsen JN, Grafton B, Koster EHW, et al. : Towards implementation of Cognitive Bias Modification in mental health care: state of the science, best practices, and ways forward. Behav Res Ther. 2024;179: 104557. 10.1016/j.brat.2024.104557 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. WHO: ICD-11 Clinical Descriptions and Diagnostic guidelines for mental and behavioural disorders.Geneva: World Health Organisation,2021. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Wong QJJ, Gregory B, McLellan LF: A review of scales to measure social anxiety disorder in clinical and epidemiological studies. Curr Psychiatry Rep. 2016;18(4): 38. 10.1007/s11920-016-0677-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Wellcome Open Res. 2025 Jun 9. doi: 10.21956/wellcomeopenres.26799.r124418

Reviewer response for version 2

Yujia Peng 1

The current study protocol presented an exciting direction focusing on cognitive bias modification for social anxiety (SA). The aim of this living systematic review is to examine the efficacy of cognitive bias modification for reducing SA.The current study protocol presented an exciting direction focusing on cognitive bias modification for social anxiety (SA). The aim of this living systematic review is to examine the efficacy of cognitive bias modification for reducing SA.

The study design is overall clear and feasible. The theoretical contribution is relatively clear. As previous reviewers mentioned, a few questions remain to be answered:

 

  1. Living systematic review, can the authors clearly define this term? And explain how does a living systematic review benefit the field? The feasibility and necessity should be more clearly addressed.

  2. Is conducting reviews and updates every 3 months too frequent? Would an interval of 6-12 months be more appropriate?

  3. It is recommended to provide more details about the CBM intervention measures, such as stimulus presentation methods, number of training sessions, duration, etc., to help readers better understand its advantages over CBT.

  4. The method of categorizing CBM interventions into 7 subcategories requires explanation and definition. It would be preferable to allocate the corresponding studies to their respective subcategories.

Is the study design appropriate for the research question?

Yes

Is the rationale for, and objectives of, the study clearly described?

Yes

Are sufficient details of the methods provided to allow replication by others?

Yes

Are the datasets clearly presented in a useable and accessible format?

Not applicable

Reviewer Expertise:

computational psychiatry, depression and anxiety, neuroimaging, social cognition

I confirm that I have read this submission and believe that I have an appropriate level of expertise to confirm that it is of an acceptable scientific standard.

Wellcome Open Res. 2025 Jun 3. doi: 10.21956/wellcomeopenres.26799.r124066

Reviewer response for version 2

Kate Clauss 1

The authors were responsive to feedback and the methods are clearer as a result. I do think it will be challenging to update the living review every 3 months, but that schedule could be modified as needed. Looking forward to seeing the authors bring data repositories to the CBM space.

Is the study design appropriate for the research question?

Yes

Is the rationale for, and objectives of, the study clearly described?

Partly

Are sufficient details of the methods provided to allow replication by others?

Yes

Are the datasets clearly presented in a useable and accessible format?

No

Reviewer Expertise:

My research focuses on cognitive dysfunction in PTSD and related conditions with an emphasis on attentional bias and cognitive bias modification. I also work on several large study-level data repositories (i.e., PTSD Repository, Suicide Prevention Trials Database), which are similar to the living review in the proposed study.

I confirm that I have read this submission and believe that I have an appropriate level of expertise to confirm that it is of an acceptable scientific standard.

Wellcome Open Res. 2024 Dec 12. doi: 10.21956/wellcomeopenres.25657.r110652

Reviewer response for version 1

Kate Clauss 1

Thank you for the opportunity to review: “Cognitive bias modification for social anxiety: protocol for a living systematic review of human studies and meta-analysis.” I think this is an exciting addition to the field of CBM. Below are several suggestions to strengthen the description of the project and the utility of the living review.

ABSTRACT

  • I believe the second background and method section is a version of the abstract for a lay reader. Could this be labelled as such?

  • In the introduction, there is considerable emphasis on the different subtypes of CBM and which would be most appropriate for social anxiety, but this is not mentioned in the abstract. Please make these consistent.

INTRODUCTION

  • Could the authors define interpretation and attentional biases? There are examples in the first paragraph, but these are not clearly labelled. This would provide more context for the second line in paragraph two where they introduce CBM-A and CBM-I, which are designed to target attentional and interpretation biases.

  • Given that most cognitive models also suggest that memory biases contribute to psychopathology, it maybe also be useful to reference this in the intro. Particularly, given that Memory Bias modification is one of the types of CBM the authors intend to use and memory bias is being investigated as a source of heterogeneity.

  • Additional information on what CBM interventions are, would be beneficial. For example, describing the format (i.e., presenting stimuli via the computer), number of trials, duration of a training session, and typical number of sessions would provide the reader with more context. Particularly, given that the next paragraph speaks to the advantages of CBM over CBT.

  • The first sentence in paragraph three was misleading. I thought the paragraph would be about whether CBM-A vs. CBM-I was more beneficial for social anxiety. Please reword.

  • In paragraph 4, the authors note that an important next step is understanding which subtypes are most effective and which mechanisms underlie changes in cognitive biases; however, mechanisms were not mentioned in the abstract as one of the primary outcomes for the systematic review/meta-analysis – this would be helpful to include. Similarly, the differential effect of the subtypes of CBM is not included in the review objectives or research questions, which was surprising after the emphasis on this in the intro.

  • I think the fifth paragraph would be more appropriate at the end of paragraph two where evidence for the efficacy of CBM is discussed.

  • The benefits of a living systematic review could be more clearly delineated. This is an exciting next step for our field and having such a resource could greatly increase the speed with which future meta-analyses on CBM in social anxiety are conducted. Will the living review be available to other researchers after a period?

  • The types of CBMs the authors intend to include in the meta-analysis (Table 3) are actually quite a bit more diverse than we are led to believe in the intro (i.e., only CBM-A and CBM-I are discussed). Will each be analyzed separately, or grouped into categories (e.g., CBM-A, CBM-I, CBM-memory, etc.)?

METHOD

  • I noticed that the authors plan to include studies published in any language. Is there a plan for translation or recruiting study staff who speak the language to double code these studies?

  • Additional intervention variables might be interesting (e.g., stimulus type [words, faces], whether the stimuli were personalized for the participant, number of trials, number of sessions)

  • Will the authors calculate effect sizes for CBM on social anxiety at follow-up?

  • Updating the review every 3 months may be challenging (i.e., conducting a search, exporting the articles identified, abstract review, full text, data abstraction, and risk of bias is a lot to accomplish in 3 months). Perhaps bi-annually would be more effective, while still keeping the review current?

  • Our team has found that our risk of bias rating varies based on the outcome measure. For studies with multiple continuous outcomes (e.g., clinician rated and self-report) will multiple risk of bias ratings be calculated? Will clinician-administered outcomes be prioritized?

RESULTS

  • Please define REML (Pairwise meta-analysis section).

  • Whether in the results or intro/method, I think it would be useful to describe what Network Meta-Analysis accomplishes (i.e., allows us to compare interventions for which there may not be head-to-head trials, if they have been tested against similar comparators)

  • I am surprised that the authors are choosing to conduct pairwise meta-analysis across all CBM interventions as there is considerable heterogeneity in these interventions and the proposed intervention target for each.

  • Will publication bias be evaluated?

  • How will the authors group/categorize the difference types of CBMs for use in the network meta-analysis?

  • Applying machine learning to study selection is a big undertaking – one that the authors mention very briefly but do not describe adequately to understand the methods they mean to use.

Overall, I believe the authors have a unique opportunity to provide a valuable resource to the field of CBM, yet there is no mention of making the systematic review available to other study teams (perhaps after an embargo period) or disseminating the proposed machine learning tools. Applying FAIR data principles to the proposed study could greatly increase its impact.

Is the study design appropriate for the research question?

Yes

Is the rationale for, and objectives of, the study clearly described?

Partly

Are sufficient details of the methods provided to allow replication by others?

Yes

Are the datasets clearly presented in a useable and accessible format?

No

Reviewer Expertise:

My research focuses on cognitive dysfunction in PTSD and related conditions with an emphasis on attentional bias and cognitive bias modification. I also work on several large study-level data repositories (i.e., PTSD Repository, Suicide Prevention Trials Database), which are similar to the living review in the proposed study.

I confirm that I have read this submission and believe that I have an appropriate level of expertise to confirm that it is of an acceptable scientific standard, however I have significant reservations, as outlined above.

Wellcome Open Res. 2024 Dec 9. doi: 10.21956/wellcomeopenres.25657.r110655

Reviewer response for version 1

David M Clark 1

Cognitive bias modification is a field that is somewhat controversial due to several failures to replicate beneficial effects of various cognitive bias interventions on social anxiety.

A comprehensive meta-analysis could potentially provide clarity. The proposed meta-analysis has many strengths including: 1) looking separately at studies with people who have a diagnosis of social anxiety disorder (SAD) and those who score high on relevant scales without necessarily being diagnosed and 2) breaking down the diverse category of cognitive bias modification into potentially more informative subcategories.

It is suggested the study will be a "living review" with new literature searches conducted every three months and analyses being repeated when new studies become available, I could not find any justification for the every three months feature. Is this really a field where so many studies are being conducted that a re-analysis has to be done every three months? Wouldn't a repeat after 12 months be adequate? Also is it feasible to run complex procedures like a network meta-analysis every 3 months?

There are some other aspects of the design that can be questioned.

1) Cognitive bias interventions will be divided into 7 sub-categories. How are these sub-categories defined and how will the investigators check that they have allocated a study to the correct sub-category?  Will they share their category definitions with the authors of included studies and check with the authors that the type of intervention in their study has been correctly identified? This a major current issue with  meta-analytic teams are often being criticised for their classification of interventions and a lack of transparency about the process.

2) Selection of outcome measures. There is likely to be considerable variation in the measures of social anxiety used in different studies. I am not convinced that the authors' way of dealing with this is optimal. Instead of the priority list that they propose, one might take an average of  standardised scores on all appropriately validated measures of social anxiety. 

3) Several important aspects of trial design aren't mentioned and so may not be taken into account. Nowadays it is generally agreed that the design of a trial (including the proposed primary outcome measures and analytic procedures ) should be pre-registered. I could not see evidence that this crucial variable will be taken into account. 

4) Comparison with other active psychological therapies. There are a number of well-validated psychological therapies for SAD that are in widespread use. If the review is to change clinical practice it will need to also look at how various forms of CBM compare with existing properly specified alternatives. This doesn't seem to be included in the scope (lumping all alternatives together as TAU is not acceptable)

Is the study design appropriate for the research question?

Partly

Is the rationale for, and objectives of, the study clearly described?

Yes

Are sufficient details of the methods provided to allow replication by others?

Partly

Are the datasets clearly presented in a useable and accessible format?

Yes

Reviewer Expertise:

Treatment of social anxiety disorder

I confirm that I have read this submission and believe that I have an appropriate level of expertise to confirm that it is of an acceptable scientific standard, however I have significant reservations, as outlined above.

Associated Data

    This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

    Data Citations

    1. Kennett J, Friedrich C, Chiocchia V, et al. : Cognitive Bias Modification for social anxiety: protocol for a living systematic review of human studies and meta-analysis.[Data].2024. 10.17605/OSF.IO/2W69Z [DOI]

    Data Availability Statement

    Underlying data

    No data are associated with this article.

    Extended data

    OSF: Cognitive bias modification for social anxiety: protocol for a living systematic review of human studies and meta-analysis. https://doi.org/10.17605/OSF.IO/2W69Z ( Kennett et al., 2024).

    Reporting guidelines

    OSF: PRISMA-P checklist for ‘Cognitive bias modification for social anxiety: protocol for a living systematic review of human studies and meta-analysis’. https://doi.org/10.17605/OSF.IO/2W69Z ( Kennett et al., 2024).

    Data are available under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International license (CC-BY 4.0).


    Articles from Wellcome Open Research are provided here courtesy of The Wellcome Trust

    RESOURCES