Skip to main content
PLOS One logoLink to PLOS One
. 2025 Jun 27;20(6):e0326347. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0326347

Analysing fast food consumption behaviour patterns: The case of Jordan

Noura Abu Asab 1,*, Randah Barakat 2
Editor: António Raposo3
PMCID: PMC12204562  PMID: 40577369

Abstract

This study aimed to examine the factors influencing fast food consumption among a Jordanian population by utilizing the extended theory of planned behavior as its theoretical framework. The research employed theory-related scales, income, BMI, ability to consume (intention), indirect intention, and a retrospective measure of fast-food consumption within a cross-sectional sample of 408 university students aged 18–23 years. Structural equation modeling was used to analyze the determinants of consumption. The findings indicated that the self-reported intention measure failed to mediate the attitude-behavior relationship. However, the use of projective intention (indirect) measures revealed that personal factors such as attitude, subjective norms, and self-identification significantly explained over 50% of the variation in the intention to consume fast food, suggesting that participants often underestimated their intention to consume fast food. Furthermore, the study identified available personal funds, perceived and actual behavioral control, and BMI as significant external predictors of fast-food consumption. The originality of this study lies in its contribution to understanding the growing preference for fast food in the Middle East, particularly in Jordan. It introduces a theoretical model that employs projection-based assessments to implicitly gauge intentions and explores the relationship between income, BMI, and consumption behavior. The practical implications of these findings underscore the importance of key psychosocial elements in developing and implementing preventive strategies aimed at promoting healthy eating behaviors among university students.

1. Introduction

Fast-food consumption has become increasingly prevalent among students and young adults, reflecting a significant shift in lifestyle and dietary habits. This trend can be attributed to several factors, including the influence of Western food cultures portrayed in the media, internet marketing and advertising [1], the rise of multinational food chains in the 1990s [2] and rapid urbanization, which has altered living patterns [35].

Additionally, factors such as increased income, hectic lifestyles, the convenience of home delivery with food safety assurance, and the emergence of new brands offering various specialties have contributed to the rise in fast-food consumption. These elements influence young adults’ eating habits, particularly during periods of significant independence in food planning, shopping, and preparation [6], potentially leading to poor nutritional choices [7].

Fast-food companies have also capitalized on changing demographics and customer psychographics by employing various advertising methods that primarily target children and young adults. Unfortunately, the proliferation and density of fast-food outlets are linked to the global obesity epidemic [812], with fast-food consumption potentially contributing to rising obesity rates [13].

Recently, there has been a notable shift in the dietary habits of individuals in the Middle East, particularly Jordan. This shift has resulted in an increase in the consumption of unhealthy fats, sugars, and processed carbohydrates, exacerbated by the proliferation of American food franchises. According to official data from various institutions, a significant proportion of Jordanians exhibit unhealthy eating habits, with approximately 84% of the population affected [14]. Moreover, the prevalence of overweight individuals increased from 60.6% to 69.6% between 2000 and 2016, according to the World Health Organization [15]. Consequently, there are growing concerns regarding high BMI, dietary hazards, elevated systolic blood pressure, and other chronic diseases in Jordan [16]. Therefore, it is crucial to identify the behavioral and economic factors associated with increased fast-food consumption.

This study is grounded in the assumption that fast-food consumption among young adults in Jordan is influenced by a combination of behavioral intentions, perceived norms, personal identity, and financial and physiological factors. While existing research has explored dietary behavior broadly, few studies have examined these drivers using an extended Theory of Planned Behavior (TPB) in Middle Eastern contexts. The focus on university students reflects a critical life stage where individuals gain autonomy over their food choices, making them particularly vulnerable to unhealthy dietary habits. The University of Jordan, one of the country’s largest and most demographically diverse institutions, offers an appropriate setting for this investigation.

The primary objective of this study is to examine how the extended TPB constructs, along with income (personal funds), BMI, and fear of negative evaluation, shape both intention and actual fast-food consumption behavior. A key methodological contribution lies in the use of a projection-based measure of intention to reduce self-report bias.

By targeting a relatively underexplored population and integrating extended behavioral predictors, this study contributes to the literature by offering a context-specific and theory-informed model that can inform culturally tailored health promotion strategies in Jordan and similar settings.

The article is structured as follows: Section Two presents the theoretical background, Section Three outlines the research methodology, ethical consideration and research design, Section Four displays the results obtained, Section Five discusses the results, and Section Six concludes.

2. Theoretical background

The Theory of Reasoned Action (TRA), proposed by [17], holds significant relevance in the realm of food behavior. This theory posits that behavior is guided by intention, which is shaped by an individual’s attitude towards the behavior and subjective norms. Specifically, the TRA suggests that normative beliefs and willingness determine subjective norms, while behavioral values impact attitudes towards the behavior. Building on this, Ajzen [18] introduced the concept of perceived behavioral control to the TRA, leading to the development of the TPB [19]. The TPB asserts that most deliberate behavior is rational and guided by individuals’ objectives, with intentions mediating the relationship between attitudes and behavior [20].

The TPB further posits that specific factors directly impact behavioral intentions, which in turn affect behavior [21]. Attitudes towards a behavior are shaped by an individual’s beliefs about the consequences and their perception of these consequences. Additionally, subjective norms are formed by an individual’s belief about what others expect from them and their motivation to fulfil these expectations. These perceptions can be divided into descriptive norms, describing how others around an individual behave, and injunctive norms, referring to the anticipated or perceived likelihood of approval or disapproval from a specific individual or group [22]. Parents play a crucial role in introducing their children to new foods, establishing dietary standards, and ensuring an adequate food supply at home. As individuals age, peers become increasingly important in various social aspects [2325].

A crucial aspect of the TPB is “perceived behavioral control” (PBC), which measures an individual’s confidence in executing a specific behavior, influenced by factors such as opportunities, resources, skills, and the impact of beliefs. The TPB model also recognizes ABC, which refers to external circumstances influencing behavior but not immediately controllable by the individual. In consumer behavior, the perceived value of a product is defined as the difference between the perceived and expected benefits to the consumer [26]. This concept involves a trade-off between what the consumer gives up and what they receive in exchange for purchasing a product [27] and is characterized as preferential, perceptual, and cognitive-affective in nature [28]. Numerous studies have demonstrated that a product’s perceived value significantly influences consumers’ purchasing decisions (e.g., [2931]). Dodds and Monroe [32] argued that consumers are more likely to purchase a product they perceive as valuable, combining transaction and acquisition utility, making it an essential factor influencing consumer purchasing intentions [33]. Despite this, high prices can be a significant barrier to purchasing food products, especially for low-income individuals. High prices reduce consumers’ ability to buy a product, causing uncertainty and difficulty in making purchase decisions [34]. Various studies confirm that pricing is a key consideration for many customers when making purchasing decisions [35].

The concept of purchase intention is widely recognized in consumer behavior, with researchers such as [3640] emphasizing its importance in predicting the likelihood of a specific purchase. Numerous studies have demonstrated that the TPB can explain a considerable portion of the variation in intention and behavior [41]. In recent years, this theory has been extensively applied in the literature on food consumption, offering valuable insights into the factors influencing consumption behavior.

Research has dedicated significant effort to understanding individual differences in factors that may impact, enhance, or diminish the variables included in the TPB. Consequently, Dunn et al. [20] proposed additional constructs encompassing other factors previously suggested to affect food consumption behaviors. This includes the inclination to identify as a “healthy eater,” as indicated by [42], which possess the potential to equal cognitive responses. Studies on the latter suggest that individuals often prioritize short-term benefits, represented by affective actions such as taste and convenience, over potential long-term costs, represented by cumulative health risks associated with food choices. Those aware of the long-term health risks would disregard the immediate reward of taste and convenience from fast food intake, as proposed by [43,44]. Conversely, individuals who give little consideration to long-term effects may opt for short-term rewards. Increasing consumer awareness of food origins, spurred by recent food scarcity, environmental concerns, animal welfare, and illicit practices, has led to growing demand for organic, vegetarian, and additive-free food [45].

Another relevant factor is the Fear of Negative Evaluation (FNE), where individuals are concerned about others’ opinions of them [4649]. This phenomenon can manifest in various contexts, including food consumption [20,50]. Additionally, research indicates that individuals from low-income or lower educational backgrounds are typically exposed to environments with a high prevalence of high-energy meals [5153]. Studies also indicate that preparing healthy food at home is more cost-effective than consuming unhealthy food at fast food restaurants [54]. However, some researchers argue that healthy meals are more expensive than unhealthy ones, leading lower-income individuals to spend less time on unhealthy foods [23].

While some studies have found that the Theory of Planned Behavior does not adequately explain behavior towards fast food consumption, many researchers argue that it is essential in explaining food consumption behaviors [23,45,5559]. The present study aimed to investigate the determinants of fast-food consumption among students at the University of Jordan, using the extended TPB and incorporating income (personal funds) and BMI measures as predictors for consumption behavior.

3. Study design, methodology, and ethical consideration

The research design for this study was a cross-sectional survey. This design allowed for the examination of relationships between variables as proposed by the TPB and the extended model. Students were selected as the population for this study due to their unique dietary habits and susceptibility to fast-food consumption, making them an ideal group for understanding the determinants of such behavior.

The sample size for the study was determined based on the population of students at the University of Jordan and the need for sufficient statistical power to detect meaningful effects. A sample size of 408 students was deemed appropriate, ensuring a representative sample while allowing for robust statistical analysis. The respondents were selected using stratified random sampling to ensure diverse representation across different schools and academic levels. The University of Jordan was selected due to its large student population, academic diversity, and geographic centrality, which make it a strong proxy for broader student populations in the country. While it may not represent all Jordanian students, its heterogeneity offers a valuable cross-section for initial analysis. The observed gender imbalance (75% female) is consistent with national enrollment patterns, particularly in the social sciences and humanities.

The student population at the University of Jordan was divided into strata based on two primary criteria: School and academic level. To ensure that each stratum is proportionally represented in the sample, the number of students selected from each school and academic level was proportional to the size of the respective strata in the overall student population. Within each stratum, students were randomly selected. This ensured that the selection process was unbiased and each student within the stratum had an equal chance of being selected.

Selected students were approached to participate in the study. They were provided with the survey questionnaire and informed about the purpose of the study, ensuring voluntary participation and informed consent. Data collection involved a structured questionnaire, divided into two sections. The first section focused on demographic factors and income thresholds, while the second addressed behavioral questions based on the TPB, including attitudes towards behavior, subjective norms, perceived behavioral control, and actual behavioral control. Similar to [20] study, additional factors such as belief strength, consideration of future consequences, and self-identification were included, accounting for possible interaction effects between these factors.

Structural Equation Modeling (SEM), which includes confirmatory factor analysis and path analysis, was conducted to simultaneously analyze multivariate causal relationships based on the cross-sectional survey. The survey incorporated variables related to students’ available income (financial access or flexibility – students’ available personal funds, including family support, stipends, scholarships, and part-time work), height, and weight (to calculate BMI). Table 1 presents the comparison of the demographic characteristics of the groups.

Table 1. Participants’ characteristics and types of food consumed.

Variable Variance levels Frequency (%)
Gender Male 100(24.5)
Female 308(75.5)
living with Family 391(95.8)
Study/work colleagues 2(0.5)
Alone 15(3.7)
Income <100 213(52.2)
100-199 93(22.8)
200-299 42(10.3)
300-350 19(4.7)
351-450 10(2.5)
>450 31(7.6)
BMI Underweight (less than 18.5) 26(6.4)
Normal (between 18.5 and 24.9) 274(67.2)
Overweight (between 25 and 29.9) 84(20.6)
Obese (more than 29.9) 24(5.9)
Which of the following foods do you consume the most? Shawarma and crispy chicken 249(60.9)
Fried foods (Potatoes, Chicken, Falafel, Donut) 217(53.1)
Processed meat (Burger, Sausage) 125(30.6)
Fast -prepared food 117(28.6)
Pastries of all kinds 97(23.7)
All types 77(18.8)

The data were analyzed using SPSS 27 and AMOS 26 software. Table 1 summarizes the mean responses for all variables. The Pearson correlation test was used to assess the relationship between the TPB elements. Bootstrapping was employed to correct for non-normality. The TPB’s internal consistency was tested using Cronbach’s alpha, although its application is limited to unidimensional construct items and its sensitivity varies with the length of the questionnaire per construct [60]. Constructs with fewer than 10 questions exhibiting a Cronbach’s alpha value over 0.5 indicated good reliability [61].

Different criteria were used to assess model fit, including the relative chi-square index, Comparative Fit Index (CFI) [0 (poor fit), 1 (very good fit)], which compares the absolute fit of the specified model to the absolute fit of the independent model, Adjusted Goodness of Fit (AGFI) [0 (no fit), 1 (perfect fit)], and Root Mean Square Error of Approximation (RMSEA) [<0.05 (good model fit); < 0.10 (marginally good model fit)]. “Due to the sensitivity of the Chi-square test to large sample sizes, the relative chi-square index (χ2/df)- Chi-square divided by degrees of freedom- is employed. Value ≤ 3 is considered acceptable-fit [62], while value ≤ 5 indicates a reasonable fit [63]. RMSEA values between 0,08 and 0,1 are considered borderline, values ranging from 0,05 to 0,08 are considered acceptable. MacCallum et al. [64] proposed that an RMSEA value of.05 -.08 is satisfactory. PGFI value of 0,5 or greater indicates an acceptable fit. AGFI value of 0,80 or greater indicates an acceptable fit [65]. Kline [62] suggests that values between 0,90 and 0,95 are acceptable fit and Values above 0,95 are excellent fit. RMR ≤ 0.07 = acceptable fit [66]. A value CFI ≥ 0,90 indicates a perfect fit [67]”.

The questionnaire used in this investigation was authorized by the University of Jordan’s Deanship of Scientific Research under approval decision number (37–2023). Before distributing the questionnaire, the objectives of the study were clearly communicated to the students to ensure they understood the purpose and nature of the research. Informed consent was obtained by requiring participants to indicate their approval by checking a box labeled “I approve to participate.” The online questionnaire was administered in the presence of researchers to provide participants the opportunity to ask questions and receive clarification as needed. Students were recruited from the University of Jordan between 22/10/2023 and 18/12/2023.

A total of 408 participants provided feedback for the survey, resulting in a response rate of 93% of a total of 439 students. The majority of the participants (75%) were female. The findings revealed that almost all the respondents (96%) resided with their families and were unmarried. Regarding their monthly available income amounts, which serve as a measure of student’s financial access or flexibility, over half (50%) reported receiving less than $140, while around one-quarter (25%) stated earning between $140 and $280 per month. Approximately 8% disclosed receiving more than USD 630 per month.

The data were collected from 408 observations gathered from students who completed a Likert-scale survey consisting of response options ranging from “strongly disagree” to “strongly agree.” The scale also included “disagree,” “neutral,” and “agree” as response options. As a measure of actual behavior, students were requested to identify the frequency category for each food item in terms of their normal intake, with the scale ranging from “never or less than once-twice per month” to “one or more times per day.”

The questionnaire presented several fast-food options for participants to select from, including burgers (similar to McDonalds), hot dogs (similar to Hot Dog of Thrones), French Fries (similar to McDonalds), Fried Chicken (similar to KFC), Falafel (similar to Abu Jbara), Donut/Arabic Sweets (similar to Crispy crème/Habibah), Ready to cook meals (similar to Maggi soups, Indomie), and Shawarma (similar to Aldayaa). Participants were given the freedom to choose one option, all options together, or multiple choices that they preferred. According to the responses of the students, most of the fast food consumed was Shawarma or Fried Chicken (61%), followed by fried food (53%) and Burgers (30.6%).

The survey also inquired about the monthly income of the students, which ranged from less than 100 JD (140$) to over 450 JD (634$). In this study, the term “income” refers to students’ available personal funds, which may include financial support from family, university scholarships, part-time jobs, or stipends. This measure captures the disposable amount of money accessible for personal spending, rather than earned income or full financial independence. While not a perfect proxy for economic autonomy, it serves as a contextual variable reflecting the financial flexibility that may enable or constrain fast-food consumption. The BMI of each student was calculated based on the height and weight information provided in the questionnaire. Despite the absence of any significant demographic differences among the respondents, individuals with a higher income were more likely to be overweight.

Two measures were employed to ascertain self-reported fast-food consumption intentions. The first measure asked participants to evaluate, on a scale of 1–5, the likelihood of consuming fast food regularly over the following month based on their lifestyle and taste preferences. The second measure assessed readiness to consume fast food by inquiring about their inclination to do so within the next month, using a scale ranging from 1 to 5. The internal consistency reliability of these two questions was determined using the Cronbach’s alpha coefficient, which yielded a value of 0.79, indicating good consistency. Additionally, an index comprising six facilitating constructs regarding perceptions of fast-food consumption and attention to health and environmental consequences was constructed. The key predictors of intention were mainly derived from the extended TPB.

In addition, the questionnaire was developed in line with the theoretical framework of the TPB, based on previous studies such as [20]. Rather than adopting a fully standardized instrument, the items were adapted to reflect the cultural context, dietary habits, and linguistic norms of Jordanian university students. These adaptations aimed to improve the clarity, relevance, and interpretability of the questions. The instrument was pilot tested and revised accordingly, and internal consistency, measured using Cronbach’s alpha, showed acceptable to strong reliability across constructs (α = 0.60–0.86). While not based on a single validated tool, this approach ensured theoretical consistency while enhancing contextual appropriateness; a strategy frequently employed in the TPB-based behavioral studies conducted in diverse settings.

3.1 Construct of the TPB

The principal factors that predicted intention and behavior, namely, attitude, subjective norms, perceived behavioral control, and actual behavioral control, are outlined below.

3.1.1 Attitude.

Thirteen items were used to assess the cognitive and affective attitudes. Participants were asked to rate their cognitive attitude by providing their opinions about fast food using the sentence “I think, eating fast food is...” followed by five adjectives, which were rated on a 5-point Likert scale. The adjectives were beneficial, quick, convenient, unpleasant, and cheap, and their scores were combined to form a composite index for this type of attitude. Affective attitudes were measured using nine adjectives presented in the question “I feel that eating fast food is...” followed by nine adjectives rated on a 5-point Likert scale, including happy, guilty-free, energetic, gratified, worried-free, enthusiastic, enticed, and capable. The scores from these responses were averaged to form a composite index that reflected affective attitudes. Both cognitive and affective attitudes were equally weighted in the calculation of the overall assessment variable, which represented the overall attitude toward fast-food consumption. The reliability of this variable was determined using Cronbach’s alpha, which was 0.80. The alpha value for cognitive attitude was 0.65, while the alpha value for affective attitude was 0.75.

3.1.2 Subjective norms (SN).

Four items were used to assess normative beliefs, evaluating both injunctive norms (the beliefs of others) and descriptive norms (the actions of others). Each item is rated on a 5-point Likert scale. The two injunctive items comprised statements such as “Individuals important to me deem it necessary to consume fast food on a regular basis” and “My close-knit circle anticipates that I frequently consume fast food.” The two descriptive items were “Those in my inner circle frequently indulged in fast foods” and “Individuals whose opinions carry weight with me, such as university professors or social media influencers, frequently consume fast food.”“. To derive an overall assessment of Social Norms, the composite variable combined the scores from both scales. Cronbach’s alpha, with a value of 0.65, served as a marginally reliable indicator of the consistency of the SN scale.

3.1.3 Perceived behavioral control (PBC).

Four items were used to assess perceptions of control and self-efficacy. Perception of control was evaluated using two 5-point Likert scale items. The first item assessed was “I could control the number of times I will consume fast food in the upcoming month,” while the second item was “The frequency at which I will eat fast food within the next month is primarily under my control.” Self-efficacy was measured using two items rated on a 5-point Likert scale. These items were “I cannot avoid eating fast food regularly over the next month” and “If I choose to, I could prevent myself from consuming fast food regularly over the next month.”.

To create an overall measure of PBC, the scores from both the self-efficacy and behavioral control scales were combined, resulting in a composite variable for PBC. The Cronbach’s alpha for this scale was 0.60.

3.1.4 Actual behavioural control (ABC).

To evaluate the limitations of life or factors that affect an individual’s ability to control fast- food consumption, two items were introduced. These were: ‘Due to my business or illness-disability, I am unable to prepare food for myself “and ‘As there are numerous fast-food outlets near me, I find it more convenient not to cook my own meals’. Additionally, ‘My financial ability allows me to eat fast food regularly’ was also assessed. The evaluation was conducted using a 5-point Likert scale. However, no evaluation of internal consistency was carried out because these two items were not necessarily correlated with each other [20].

3.1.5 Brief fear of negative evaluation (FNE) scale.

The four-item measure developed by [46] was evaluated using a 5-point Likert scale. The items included in the measure were: “Concerned about others’ opinions of me,” “Fearful of rejection,” “Worried about the impression I make,” and “Anxious about saying or doing something wrong.” The internal consistency of the measure was demonstrated by a high Cronbach’s alpha value of 0.86.

3.1.6 Self-identification scale.

The implementation of the 4-item scale, proposed by [42], was conducted to assess an individual’s level of self-identification as a healthy eater. This was done using a 5-point Likert scale, with the four items included: I consider myself a healthy eater, I am concerned with eating healthily, I care about the impact of food choice on my health, and ‘I enjoy eating.’ Additionally, An additional item was added to consider the environmental consequences associated with food consumption, which was phrased as follows: “I consider how my dietary choices affect the environment.” The internal consistency of the scale was evaluated using Cronbach’s alpha, which yielded a value of.70. “Consideration of future consequences index was dropped as its Cronbach’s alpha was low due to low number of constructs included.”

3.1.7 Interaction effects.

To examine the potential influence of a subjective norm-FNE interaction, centered values for both variables were computed using a method by subtracting the individual scores from the mean score [20,68]. This involved creating a new interaction variable, denoted as FNE SN, by multiplying the centered scores.

The following hypotheses have been tested considering the theoretical and empirical debates in the literature:

Hypothesis 1a (H1a): Affective attitudes are positively related to behavioral intention to consume fast food.

Hypothesis 1b (H1b): Cognitive attitudes are positively related to behavioral intention to consume fast food.

Hypothesis 1c (H1c): Cognitive attitudes are stronger predictors of the behavioral intention to consume fast food than affective attitudes.

Hypothesis 1d (H1d): Affective attitudes are stronger predictors of the behavior to consume fast food than affective attitudes.

Hypothesis 2a (H2a): Injunctive norms are positively related to behavioral intention to consume fast food.

Hypothesis 2b (H2b): Descriptive norms are positively related to behavioral intention to consume fast food.

Hypothesis 2c (H2c): The injunctive and the descriptive norms are equally strong predictors of the behavioral intention to consume fast food.

Hypothesis 3a (H3a): Perception of control is negatively related to the behavior to consume fast food.

Hypothesis 3b (H3b): Self-efficacy is negatively related to the intention to consume fast food. Hypothesis 3c (H3c): Perception of control is a stronger predictor of the intention to consume fast food than self-efficacy.

Hypothesis 4 (H4): Self-identification as a healthy eater is negatively related to the behavioral intention to consume fast food.

Hypothesis 4 (H4): Income is positively related to the behavior to consume fast food.

Hypothesis 4 (H4): BMI is positively related to the behavior to consume fast food.

The hypotheses developed above are grounded in an extended conceptualization of the TPB, which incorporates both traditional constructs (attitude, subjective norms, and perceived behavioral control) and additional factors such as self-identification, fear of negative evaluation, and external predictors like income and BMI. This theoretical model positions projective intention, rather than self-reported intention, as the key mediator between psychosocial predictors and fast-food consumption behavior. Fig 1 presents the conceptual framework used in this study, illustrating the extended TPB and external predictors. The model also features a projection-based measure of intention, which replaces direct self-reports to reduce social desirability bias. The arrows in the Figure represent both hypothesized and empirically supported pathways, showing how each variable is expected to influence intention and behavior.

Fig 1. Conceptual framework of the extended Theory of Planned Behavior (TPB) used in the study.

Fig 1

4. Results

According to the research sample, 27% of individuals consumed fast food on a weekly basis, whereas 26% consumed fast food two to three times per month. The majority of students (approximately 65%) experienced positive emotions, such as happiness, excitement, and attraction towards fast food consumption, which is perceived as time-saving and effortless. However, an overwhelming majority (87%) did not consider fast food cheap. Notably, most participants believed that their good health status allowed them to consume fast food without any potential risks. A summary of the demographic characteristics of the students in this study and the types of fast food they consume is presented in Table 1. Descriptive statistics, including Cronbach’s alpha, the mean score, and standard deviation (SD), are presented in Table 2.

Table 2. Descriptive statistics; Cronbach’s α, mean score, and standard deviation (SD).

Construct items Mean (SD)
Cognitive Attitude (α = 0.65)
 I think that fast food is not good for your health. 3.77 (1.035)
 I think that fast food saves time. 3.69 (1.051)
 I think that fast food is easy and convenient. 3.69 (1.055)
 I think that fast food is fun. 3.88 (1.043)
 I think that fast food is cheap. 3.64 (0.964)
Affective Attitude (α = 0.75)
 I feel happy when eating fast food. 3.84(0.972)
 I don’t feel guilty when eating fast food. 3.08(1.276)
 I feel energized when eating fast food. 2.47(1.166)
 I feel full when eating fast food. 4.25 (0.919)
 I don’t feel anxious when eating fast food. 3.09(1.325)
 I feel excited when eating fast food. 3.62(1.190)
 I feel attracted to eating fast food. 3.79(1.100)
 I feel that my health qualifies me to eat fast food. 3.43(1.281)
Subjective Norms (α = 0.65)
Injunctive Norms
 Most people who are important to me think that I should eat fast food regularly. 2.69(1.191)
 People close to me expect me to eat fast food regularly. 2.86(1.061)
Descriptive Norms
 People close to me eat fast food regularly. 2.84(1.001)
 The people in my life whose opinions I value eat fast food regularly. 2.79(1.010)
Perceived Control (α = 0.60)
Perceived Behavioral Control
 I have the power to control the number of times I will eat fast food during the next month. 3.73(1.004)
 The number of times I will eat fast food during the next month is up to my decision at the time. 3.82(0.849)
Self-efficiency
 It is impossible for me to refrain from eating fast food during the next month. 3.03(1.200)
 I can avoid eating fast food regularly during the next month if you want 3.74(0.936)
Actual behavioral control
 I can’t stop myself from eating fast food Because I can’t prepare healthy food. 3.56(1.184)
 I cannot prevent myself from eating fast food due to being in places that encourage me to buy fast food. 2.81(1.126)
Fear of Negative Evaluation (α = 0.86)
 I worry about what others might think of me. 2.53(1.136)
 I worry about not being accepted by others. 2.36(1.173)
 I worry about what kind of impression I make on others. 2.82(1.176)
 I worry that I will say or do the wrong things or that others will notice my shortcomings. 2.74(1.230)
Self-Identification (α = 0.70)
 I see myself as someone who practices healthy eating habits. 2.85(0.883)
 I see myself as someone who is concerned about the health consequences of the foods I eat. 3.25(0.973)
 I see myself as someone who is concerned about the environmental consequences of the foods I eat. 3.07(0.958)
 I see myself as someone who enjoys eating. 3.92(0.892)
 I see myself as Someone interested in eating healthy. 3.18(1.012)
Intention (α = 0.79)
 Based on my taste preferences and current lifestyle:
 I will likely eat fast food regularly over the next month. 2.72(1.118)
 I will probably eat fast food regularly over the next month. 2.75(1.119)
 My financial ability qualifies me not to abstain from eating fast food.

The bivariate correlations among self-reported intentions, constructed intentions, attitudes, subjective norms, PBC (excluding ABC), self-identification, and behavior related to fast-food intake by Jordanian students are presented in Table 3. The TPB variables were significantly associated with an increased desire to consume fast food, except for PBC and ABC (P < 0.001). In contrast, fast-food consumption was negatively correlated with PBC and ABC. No significant correlations were found between income or BMI and intention to engage in this behavior. The study revealed a positive relationship (r = 0.23, P < 0.001) between financial access and frequency of fast-food intake, as well as a direct association (r = 0.24, P < 0.001) between BMI and the amount of fast food consumption.

Table 3. Pearson correlation results.

Behavior Intension Income BMI Cognitive Attitude Affective Attitude
Behavior 1 .103* .232** .241** .198** .156**
Intension .103* 1 0.038 0.017 .126* 0.04
Income .232** 0.038 1 .209** 0.028 0.033
BMI .241** 0.017 .209** 1 −0.003 −.115*
Cognitive Attitude .198** .126* 0.028 −0.003 1 .502**
Affective Attitude .156** 0.04 0.033 −.115* .502** 1
Inductive Norms .134** .313** 0.053 −0.034 .251** .263**
Descriptive Norms 0.028 .192** −0.007 0.028 0.01 0.001
Subjective Norms .111* .331** 0.034 −0.008 .185** .188**
Perceived Behavioural Control −.307** 0.039 −.098* 0.036 −.119* −.179**
Actual control −.325** −.105* −.165** −0.012 −.270** −.247**
Inductive Norms Descriptive Norms Subjective Norms Perceived Behavioral Control Actual control Self-Identification
Behavior .134** 0.028 .111* −.307** −.325** −.225**
Intension .313** .192** .331** 0.039 −.105* .126*
Income 0.053 −0.007 0.034 −.098* −.165** 0.004
BMI −0.034 0.028 −0.008 0.036 −0.012 −0.05
Cognitive Attitude .251** 0.01 .185** −.119* −.270** −0.093
Affective Attitude .263** 0.001 .188** −.179** −.247** −.116*
Inductive Norms 1 .218** .836** −.127* −.159** 0.058
Descriptive Norms .218** 1 .718** −0.044 −0.016 0.079
Subjective Norms .836** .718** 1 −.115* −.122* 0.086
Perceived Behavioral Control −.127* −0.044 −.115* 1 .347** .281**
Actual control −.159** −0.016 −.122* .347** 1 .106*

Note: This table presents the Pearson correlation coefficients among key psychological, behavioral, and socioeconomic variables related to fast-food consumption behavior. Values marked with asterisks indicate statistical significance:

*

p < 0.10,

**

p < 0.05,

***

p < 0.01.

The Ajzen’s TBP model, depicted in Fig 2, failed to effectively explain either the intention to consume or behavior towards fast-food consumption. Despite various modifications, classical theory does not meet all the necessary criteria for a good model fit. Due to its poor fit and space limitations, results were left unreported but revealed inconsistencies between the data collected and the basic TPB theory, which aligns with findings from previous studies, such as [20,61]. Therefore, the extended model includes various factors, such as fear of negative evaluation, self-identification, and interactions between multiple variables, along with the level of income (available income) and BMI. These additional variables were added to the TPB model. However, values with low factor loadings (< 0.5) were removed, along with any non-significant predictive variables, to modify the model accordingly for a better fit based on goodness-of-fit measures.

Fig 2. Path analysis of the classical TPB model, adapted from [18].

Fig 2

Fig 3 illustrates the extended model, which integrates additional factors identified in the literature. Two external variables, income and BMI, were added as direct predictors of behavior. The model also specifies correlations between affective attitude, cognitive attitude, PBC, ABC, income, and BMI, represented by double-headed arrows. Interestingly, it was found that PBC was not significantly associated with intention but had a significant association with actual behavior. Notably, most PBC questions focused on respondents’ perception of their ability to monitor their fast-food consumption frequency rather than their intention to consume fast food. Recent studies have proposed modifications and advancements in the theory, suggesting that PBC plays a crucial role as an intermediary factor between intention and behavior [69].

Fig 3. Path analysis of the extended TPB model predicting fast-food consumption behavior.

Fig 3

The study revealed that cognitive attitudes, descriptive norms, and self-identification, in combination, accounted for 7% of the variation in the intention to consume fast food. Affective attitudes were deemed insignificant and did not directly affect intention. Cognitive attitude demonstrated a strong direct association with consumption intention (β = 0.211, P < 0.005), whereas affective attitude had a positive influence on behavior rather than intention (β = 0.130, P < 0.045).

According to the study’s findings, subjective norms were consistently found to have a negative impact on the accuracy of the model and were insignificant in all model adjustments. To improve the predictive ability of the model, descriptive norms were added, resulting in a significant impact of 0.234 on consumption intention (P < 0.000). This suggests that while students may enjoy fast food, their decisions are more influenced by practical considerations rather than emotional ones.

The perceived behavioral control scale demonstrated a direct relationship between behavior and self-control/self-efficacy, which led to lowered intentions towards consumption, with a negative coefficient of −0.37. On the other hand, self-identification had a positive effect on intention (β = 0.204, P < 0.009), while actual control proved to be a significant predictor of behavior towards consumption (β = −0.264, P < 0.000).

The university student behavior model was expanded to include income and BMI as possible influencing factors, and the results indicated a positive impact on the model, with estimates of 0.09 and 0.36 for these variables, respectively. Moreover, students’ intentions were positively correlated with their own behavior and accounted for approximately 6% of the variance (P < 0.07).

All covariances were significant at a level less than or equal to 1%. Additionally, it is worth noting that the results revealed a positive association between income levels and BMI in the study sample. The results of the model’s fit were deemed satisfactory, and the relative chi-square test yielded significant findings: x2/df ≈ 3.67, GFI = .95, AGFI = .91, CFI = .8, RMSEA = .08, PGFI = 0.56, RMR = 0.05.

The research revealed that students’ intentions served as a moderately significant predictor of their behavior, accounting for 6% of the variance (P < 0.07). Fig 3 shows the extended TPB model, which indicates that both intention and behavior were predicted by the model at 6% and 22%, respectively. However, these percentages are considered low in comparison to other studies that have reported values ranging from 20–50% when it comes to accurately predicting consumption intention. The findings are reported in Table 4.

Table 4. Summary of Multiple Regression.

Construct Unstandardized Coefficients Unstandardized Coefficient t-value P-Value R 2
β S.E β
Panel (A) Summary of Multiple Regression: Effect of Intension, Actual control, Affective Attitude, Perceived Behavioral Control, Income, BMI on behavior
Intension 0.078 0.045 0.077 1.75 0.08 0.22
Actual control −0.264 0.063 −0.196 −4.188 ***
Income 0.086 0.031 0.125 2.761 ***
BMI 0.366 0.07 0.233 5.206 ***
Affective Attitude 0.13 0.065 0.088 2.004 0.045
Perceived Behavioral Control −0.365 0.075 −0.225 −4.842 ***
Panel (b) Summary of Multiple Regression: Effect of Descriptive Norms, Cognitive Attitude, Self-Identification on intension
Descriptive Norms 0.227 0.063 0.174 3.626 *** 0.07
Cognitive Attitude 0.206 0.076 0.131 2.722 ***
Self-Identification −0.21 0.079 0.127 2.65 ***
Panel (c) Summary of covariances: intension
Affective < -- > Cognitive 0.228 0.025 ***
PBC < -- > ABC 0.165 0.026 ***
Income < -- > ABC −0.148 0.053 ***
Income < -- > BMI 0.204 0.05 ***

It appears that the self-reported data used to evaluate intention-to-behavior were underestimated. As a result, an indirect assessment of intention measurement was constructed to accurately determine actual consumption intentions while mitigating any potential social desirability bias (see, for example, [70,71]). “In accordance with [20], participants systematically underestimated their consumption of fast food. Moreover, their responses regarding the frequency of consumption were significantly lower than their actual consumption. Two methods were employed to assess their behavior: retrospective and prospective. The results indicated that the prospective measure, which entailed recording the actual consumption in a daily diary, yielded more accurate outcomes. This finding implies that individuals may tend to underestimate dietary risks while overestimating their intake of healthy foods [72,73].” Thus, the revised projective measure of intention comprises six factors that enable us to indirectly gauge individuals’ propensity to consume fast food. The modified construct assesses students’ perception of their emotional and cognitive ability to eat fast food, their ability to pay for fast food meals, recognition of the risk associated with consumption, lack of guilt and worry after consumption, and concern for health and environmental repercussions stemming from the behavior.

The self-reported and structured projective measures were compared using paired-sample t-tests. The results demonstrated that participants underestimated their fast-food consumption intention, as their self-reported intention consumption (M = 2.70, SD = 1.00) was significantly lower than that of the projective measure (M = 3.20, SD = 0.565; t = 7.52, p < .001).

The extended TPB model, which includes the new construct of intention (projective) illustrated in Fig 4, was more effective in explaining behavioral variations than the previous model. The newly used construct revealed that intention had a positive and statistically significant influence (0.21; p < 0.001) on behavior and explained 22% of its variation. Notably, attitude, descriptive norms, and self-identification accounted for 54% of the variation in the intention to consume fast food. “FNE did not contribute substantially to the variation in either intention or behavior, or its interactive effect with Subjective Norms. Consequently, it was omitted from the final model. This conclusion aligns with the results reported by [20]”.

Fig 4. Path analysis of the extended TPB model using projective (indirect) intention to predict fast-food consumption behavior.

Fig 4

The diagram presents standardized beta coefficients for direct paths. Double-headed arrows indicate correlations among PBC, ABC, income, and BMI. All relationships depicted are statistically significant.

This study found that the intention to eat fast food was determined by attitude (with a direct effect), subjective norms (with a direct effect), and self-identification (indirect effect). The results showed that attitude was the primary predictor of fast-food consumption intentions, followed by self-identification and descriptive norms.

As expected, identifying a healthy eater was associated with a decrease in the intention to consume fast food (β = −0.42, P < 0.000). Both cognitive and affective attitudes were found to have the greatest impact on explaining intention (β = 0.623, P < 0.000), while descriptive norms had a negative effect. The study further revealed that social influence plays an important role in fast-food consumption; strong subjective norms lead to a decreased perception of control overeating fast-food.

Similar to previous models, PBC and ABC had a significant negative impact on behavior. The study found that actual control was strongly linked to behavior (with a β coefficient of −0.264 and a P value less than zero). The findings suggest strong correlations between IBM-income, PBC-actual control, and income-actual control. Specifically, a positive correlation exists between IBM-income, and a negative correlation between income and actual control. These associations are highly significant. The results are summarized in Table 5. This model provided a good fit with a significant relative chi-square test (x2/df = 3.67, GFI = .96, CFI = .87, RMSEA = .08).

Table 5. Summary of Multiple Regression.

Construct Unstandardized Coefficients Unstandardized Coefficient t-value P-Value R 2
β S.E β
Panel (A) Summary of Multiple Regression: Effect of Intension, Actual control, Perceived Behavioral Control, Income, BMI on behavior
Intension2(indirect) 0.211 0.083 0.111 2.547 0.01 0.22
Actual control −0.290 0.063 −0.215 −4.591 ***
Income 0.084 0.031 0.121 2.694 ***
BMI 0.369 0.07 0.235 5.257 ***
Perceived Behavioral Control −0.349 0.075 −0.215 −4.591 ***
Panel (b) Summary of Multiple Regression: Effect of Attitude, Descriptive Norms, Self-Identification on the Intension (Indirect).
Attitude 0.623 0.038 0.554 16.451 *** 0.54
Descriptive Norms −0.068 0.024 −0.097 −2.88 ***
Self-Identification −0.419 0.03 −0.471 −13.973 ***
Panel (c) Summary of covariances: intension (indirect)
Income < -- > BMI 0.204 0.05 ***
PBC < -- > ABC 0.165 0.026 ***
Income < -- > ABC −0.148 0.053 ***

5. Discussion

This study aimed to explore the determinants of fast-food consumption among university students in Jordan using an extended version of the Theory of Planned Behavior. The findings provide valuable insights into the factors influencing fast-food consumption behaviors and offer significant implications for future research and practical applications.

The results highlight the critical role of cognitive attitudes in predicting the intention to consume fast food. Students’ beliefs about the convenience, taste, and enjoyment of fast food were found to significantly shape their consumption behaviors. This underscores the need to target these beliefs in interventions aimed at promoting healthier eating habits. Additionally, the influence of subjective norms and self-identification on fast-food consumption intentions suggests that social-based interventions could be effective. Interestingly, this study found that descriptive norms were more significant predictors of fast-food consumption intention than injunctive norms. This suggests that students are more influenced by what they observe their peers doing (e.g., eating fast food frequently) than by whether those behaviors are socially approved or disapproved. This finding aligns with recent extensions of the TPB, which emphasize that descriptive norms can be powerful [74] in environments where behaviors are highly visible and socially embedded, such as university campuses [75]. The distinction is important, as it suggests that public health interventions may be more effective when they target perceived norms of behavior, rather than solely focusing on approval or moral messaging. Thus, encouraging healthy eating behaviors within peer groups and strengthening students’ health-conscious identities could help reduce unhealthy eating habits.

Building on these psychosocial determinants, demographic and physiological factors also emerged as significant predictors. The study revealed a positive correlation between higher income (personal funds), BMI, and frequent fast-food consumption. This indicates that financial flexibility and weight-related factors significantly influence dietary choices. Addressing the role of financial flexibility and providing affordable healthy food options could mitigate the impact of fast-food consumption, particularly among higher-income students. While this finding appears to contradict findings from high-income countries, where lower income is generally associated with higher obesity due to limited access to nutritious food [76], this relationship may not hold in the Jordanian context. In fact, local evidence suggests the opposite. A study by [77] found that Jordanian school children from higher-income families were more likely to be obese, and [78] showed that university students with higher daily pocket money had significantly greater odds of frequent fast-food consumption and elevated BMI. These findings suggest that in Jordan, greater financial flexibility may facilitate access to calorie-dense convenience foods, particularly among youth, reinforcing a positive rather than negative relationship between income and obesity.

While income may shape access to fast food, individual-level determinants remain crucial. In this regard, perceived and actual behavioral control emerged as significant predictors of consumption behavior. Students with higher perceived control over their eating habits and actual control over their food environment were less likely to consume fast food. Enhancing students’ self-efficacy and providing supportive environments can effectively reduce fast-food intake. These findings suggest that dietary choices are shaped by a complex interplay of factors, including economic access, BMI, social identity, and environmental cues. While higher available income and BMI were associated with more frequent fast-food consumption in this sample, these variables do not operate in isolation. Psychological drivers such as stress and convenience, along with social influences like peer eating habits and food marketing, also contribute meaningfully to dietary behavior. Accordingly, addressing fast-food consumption patterns among university students may require more than just improving access to affordable healthy foods; it also demands strategies that consider social dynamics, perceived norms, and the broader food environment that students navigate daily.

Although this study did not examine directly whether students consume fast food alone or with others, it is likely that such behavior often serves a social function. Fast-food outlets may act as informal gathering spaces for students to socialize, relax, or connect with peers. In such cases, food choices may be shaped less by individual preference and more by group dynamics and perceived social norms. Merely emphasizing the health risks of fast food may therefore be insufficient to change behavior. Effective interventions should also address the social motivations behind fast-food consumption, for example, by promoting healthy options in communal settings, reframing peer norms, and embedding behavioral cues in group dining environments. Future research should investigate the social contexts of fast-food consumption in more depth, such as whether students eat alone or with peers, and how social norms and group dynamics influence their dietary decisions.

Beyond these social influences, the findings also revealed a psychological discrepancy between students’ reported intentions and their underlying behavioral tendencies. Specifically, students tended to underestimate their intention to consume fast food, suggesting a potential gap between what they believe or wish to report and their actual inclinations. This discrepancy underscores the need for more accurate and less biased measurement tools, such as projection-based assessments, to better capture true behavioral intent. Relying solely on self-reported data for attitudes, norms, and consumption behavior may introduce social desirability bias and misreporting. Future studies should consider incorporating objective measures and observational data to validate and triangulate self-reported findings.

However, it is essential to acknowledge the study’s limitations. The cross-sectional nature of the study limits the ability to establish causal relationships between the variables. Longitudinal studies are needed to better understand the temporal dynamics of fast-food consumption behaviors. The study sample was limited to university students in Jordan, which may not be representative of the broader population. Replicating the study in different settings and among diverse demographic groups would enhance the generalizability of the findings. Furthermore, the study focused on specific predictors within the TPB framework and excluded other potential factors such as emotional eating, stress levels, and cultural influences. Future research is needed to consider a more comprehensive approach to capture the multifaceted nature of fast-food consumption behaviors.

In conclusion, the findings from this study contribute to the understanding of fast-food consumption behaviors among young adults in Jordan. By identifying key psychosocial and economic determinants, the study offers valuable insights for designing effective interventions to promote healthier eating habits. Additionally, by revealing why fast-food consumption is rising among students, the findings lay the groundwork for developing targeted strategies that address both psychosocial and structural influences. Future research should build on these findings, addressing the limitations and exploring additional factors to develop a more holistic understanding of fast-food consumption behaviors.

6. Conclusions

This study set out to explore the key behavioral and contextual factors that shape fast-food consumption among university students in Jordan. To better understand what drives these choices, we applied an extended version of the Theory of Planned Behavior, incorporating additional variables such BMI and income (personal funds). Through two separate factor analyses, we examined how elements like attitudes, social norms, perceived and actual control, and self-identification relate to students’ intentions to consume fast food, and how those intentions translate into actual eating behavior.

The study found that students often underestimated their intention to consume fast food. When a structured projective measurement was applied, a much stronger and statistically significant link emerged between intention and actual behavior. One of the key findings was that intention plays an important role in shaping consumption, explaining 22% of the variation in fast-food intake. The results also reinforced the relevance of belief-based factors within the Theory of Planned Behavior: attitudes, subjective norms, and self-identification together accounted for 53% of the variation in intention. Among these, attitudes, particularly emotional responses, had the strongest influence on students’ intentions, highlighting the central role of affect in motivating fast-food consumption. The impact of marketing strategies and visible peer behaviors likely contributed to these affective and normative influences, making fast food appear more socially acceptable, convenient, and rewarding. These insights suggest that promoting healthier food options on campus, while also addressing the social and marketing cues that shape eating norms, could help reduce students’ reliance on fast food. Strengthening a healthy-eater identity may further shift attitudes and reduce intention to consume unhealthy food.

The study also found that both perceived and actual behavioral control were significantly linked to lower fast-food consumption, suggesting that students who feel more in control of their eating habits are less likely to rely on fast food. In addition, a notable association emerged between higher income, higher BMI, and more frequent fast-food intake. This indicates that fast-food consumption is not limited to lower-income groups, but is also common among students with greater financial means. These findings underscore the need to explore how both financial limitations and increased affordability can shape unhealthy eating patterns. Designing targeted interventions that consider both psychological drivers and financial flexibility could help reduce fast-food consumption among students. Future research should explore strategies that shift students’ motivation and perception, especially in cultural contexts like Jordan, where fast food is becoming increasingly normalized.

Supporting information

S1 Dataset. Minimal data set containing coded variables and values used in all statistical analyses, including those used to generate figures and regression models.

(XLSX)

pone.0326347.s001.xlsx (136.4KB, xlsx)
S1 Data. De-identified Arabic-language questionnaire responses collected during the study.

Provided for reference and contextual transparency.

(XLSX)

pone.0326347.s002.xlsx (98.2KB, xlsx)

Data Availability

All relevant data are within the paper and its Supporting Information files.

Funding Statement

The author(s) received no specific funding for this work.

References

  • 1.Haven A. Consumer behavior and the growth of the fast food industry in a small emerging country. IJAME. 2015;4:180–9. [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Popkin BM, Reardon T. Obesity and the food system transformation in Latin America. Obes Rev. 2018;19(8):1028–64. doi: 10.1111/obr.12694 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Tengiz ZM, Oraman Y. The evaluation of the students’ attitudes and behavior towards fast food consumption. Foodbalt 2019: 13th Baltic Conference on Food Science and Technology: Food Nutrition, Well-Being. Latvia Univ Life Sciences & Technologies; 2019. [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Huang TT, Harris KJ, Lee RE, Nazir N, Born W, Kaur H. Assessing overweight, obesity, diet and physical activity in college students. J Am Coll Health. 2003;52(2):83–6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Pereira MA, Kartashov AI, Ebbeling CB, Van Horn L, Slattery ML, Jacobs DR Jr, et al. Fast-food habits, weight gain, and insulin resistance (the CARDIA study): 15-year prospective analysis. Lancet. 2005;365(9453):36–42. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(04)17663-0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Affenito SG, Franko DL, Striegel-Moore RH, Thompson D. Behavioral determinants of obesity: research findings and policy implications. J Obes. 2012;2012:150732. doi: 10.1155/2012/150732 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Phan TA, Mai PH. Determinants impacting consumers’ purchase intention: the case of fast food in Vietnam. Int J Mark Stud. 2016;8(5):56–68. [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Mylona EK, Shehadeh F, Fleury E, Kalligeros M, Mylonakis E. Neighborhood-level analysis on the impact of accessibility to fast food and open green spaces on the prevalence of obesity. Am J Med. 2020;133(3):340-346.e1. doi: 10.1016/j.amjmed.2019.08.024 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Fraser LK, Edwards KL. The association between the geography of fast food outlets and childhood obesity rates in Leeds, UK. Health Place. 2010;16(6):1124–8. doi: 10.1016/j.healthplace.2010.07.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.De Vogli R, Kouvonen A, Gimeno D. Globesization: ecological evidence on the relationship between fast food outlets and obesity among 26 advanced economies. Food Public Health. 2015:10–7. [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Kruger DJ, Greenberg E, Murphy JB, DiFazio LA, Youra KR. Local concentration of fast-food outlets is associated with poor nutrition and obesity. Am J Health Promot. 2014;28(5):340–3. doi: 10.4278/ajhp.111201-QUAN-437 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Rummo PE, Wu E, McDermott ZT, Schwartz AE, Elbel B. Relationship between retail food outlets near public schools and adolescent obesity in New York City. Health Place. 2020;65:102408. doi: 10.1016/j.healthplace.2020.102408 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Jia P, Luo M, Li Y, Zheng J-S, Xiao Q, Luo J. Fast-food restaurant, unhealthy eating, and childhood obesity: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Obes Rev. 2021;22 Suppl 1(Suppl 1):e12944. doi: 10.1111/obr.12944 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.United Nations Jordan. Policy brief: healthy diets for all in Jordan. 2021.
  • 15.World Health Organization. Regional Office for the Eastern Mediterranean. Summary report on the Eastern Mediterranean Region intercountry dialogue on the WHO Acceleration Plan to STOP Obesity Amman, Jordan, 18-20 October 2022. World Health Organization. Regional Office for the Eastern Mediterranean. [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Murray CJ, Aravkin AY, Zheng P, Abbafati C, Abbas KM, Abbasi-Kangevari M, et al. Global burden of 87 risk factors in 204 countries and territories, 1990–2019: a systematic analysis for the Global Burden of Disease Study 2019. Lancet. 2020;396(10258):1223–49. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Fishbein M, Ajzen I. Belief, attitude, intention, and behavior: an introduction to theory and research. Penn State University Press; 1977. [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Ajzen I. The theory of planned behavior. University of Massachusetts at Amherst; 1991. [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Ebadi L, Rakhshanderou S, Ghaffari M. Determinants of fast food consumption among students of Tehran: application of planned behavior theory. Int J Pediatr. 2018;6(10):8307–16. [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Dunn KI, Mohr P, Wilson CJ, Wittert GA. Determinants of fast-food consumption. An application of the theory of planned behaviour. Appetite. 2011;57(2):349–57. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2011.06.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Ajzen I. Attitudes, personality and behavior. Milton Keynes: Open University Press; 1988. [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Ajzen I. The theory of planned behavior: frequently asked questions. Hum Behav Emerg Technol. 2020;2(4):314–24. [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Deliens T, Clarys P, De Bourdeaudhuij I, Deforche B. Determinants of eating behaviour in university students: a qualitative study using focus group discussions. BMC Public Health. 2014;14:53. doi: 10.1186/1471-2458-14-53 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Risti KN, Pamungkasari EP, Suminah S. Relationship of peer influence and family eating habits on healthy food choices in overweight adolescents in Surakarta. MGI. 2021;16(2):124. doi: 10.20473/mgi.v16i2.124-129 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Contento IR, Williams SS, Michela JL, Franklin AB. Understanding the food choice process of adolescents in the context of family and friends. J Adolesc Health. 2006;38(5):575–82. doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2005.05.025 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Miller ZD. The enduring use of the theory of planned behavior. Hum Dimens Wildl. 2017;22(6):583–90. doi: 10.1080/10871209.2017.1347967 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Leszinski R, Marn MV. Setting value, not price. Ind Launderer. 1997;48:51. [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Sánchez-Fernández R, Iniesta-Bonillo MÁ. The concept of perceived value: a systematic review of the research. Mark Theory. 2007;7(4):427–51. doi: 10.1177/1470593107083165 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Luo B, Li L, Sun Y. Understanding the influence of consumers’ perceived value on energy-saving products purchase intention. Front Psychol. 2022;12:640376. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.640376 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Yu S, Lee J. The effects of consumers’ perceived values on intention to purchase upcycled products. Sustainability. 2019;11(4):1034. [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Yue T, Liu J, Long R, Chen H, Li Q, Liu H, et al. Effects of perceived value on green consumption intention based on double-entry mental accounting: taking energy-efficient appliance purchase as an example. Environ Sci Pollut Res Int. 2021;28(6):7236–48. doi: 10.1007/s11356-020-11027-0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Dodds WB, Monroe KB. The effect of brand and price information on subjective product evaluations. Adv Consum Res. 1985;12(1). [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Thaler R. Mental accounting and consumer choice. Mark Sci. 1985;4(3):199–214. doi: 10.1287/mksc.4.3.199 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Tarkiainen A, Sundqvist S. Subjective norms, attitudes and intentions of Finnish consumers in buying organic food. Br Food J. 2005;107(11):808–22. doi: 10.1108/00070700510629760 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Saleki ZS, Saleki SM. The main factors influencing purchase behaviour of organic products in Malaysia. IJCRB. 2012;4(1):98–116. [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Schiffman LG, Kanuk LL. Consumer behavior. Wisconsin: Prentice Hall International, Inc. Sherman, SP; 2000. [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Qiao Y, Yin X, Xing G. Impact of perceived product value on customer-based brand equity: marx’s theory - value-based perspective. Front Psychol. 2022;13:931064. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.931064 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Saeed M, Ahmed A, Boğan E, Abul O, Qammar A. Buying green: the role of green customer-based brand equity in shaping customers’ green buying behavior. Environ Dev Sustain. 2023:1–17. [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Febriana H, Cuandra F. Analyze of customer based brand equity on customer loyalty in fast food restaurant at Batam city. Enrich J Manag. 2022;12(5):3956–64. [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Fungai M. Factors influencing customer repurchase intention in the fast food industry: a case study of Innscor-Mutare, Zimbabwe. Bus Soc Sci J. 2017;2(1):113–33. [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Armitage CJ, Conner M. Efficacy of the theory of planned behaviour: a meta-analytic review. Br J Soc Psychol. 2001;40(Pt 4):471–99. doi: 10.1348/014466601164939 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Armitage CJ, Conner M. Distinguishing perceptions of control from self‐efficacy: predicting consumption of a low‐fat diet using the theory of planned behavior. J Appl Soc Psychol. 1999;29(1):72–90. [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Strathman A, Gleicher F, Boninger DS, Edwards CS. The consideration of future consequences: weighing immediate and distant outcomes of behavior. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1994;66(4):742. [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Dunn KI, Mohr PB, Wilson CJ, Wittert GA. Beliefs about fast food in Australia: a qualitative analysis. Appetite. 2008;51(2):331–4. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2008.03.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Anand R. A study of determinants impacting consumers food choice with reference to the fast food consumption in India. Soc Bus Rev. 2011;6(2):176–87. doi: 10.1108/17465681111143993 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Leary MR. A brief version of the Fear of Negative Evaluation Scale. Pers Soc Psychol Bull. 1983;9(3):371–5. [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Winton EC, Clark DM, Edelmann RJ. Social anxiety, fear of negative evaluation and the detection of negative emotion in others. Behav Res Ther. 1995;33(2):193–6. doi: 10.1016/0005-7967(94)e0019-f [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Kocovski NL, Endler NS. Social anxiety, self‐regulation, and fear of negative evaluation. Eur J Pers. 2000;14(4):347–58. [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Rodebaugh TL, Woods CM, Thissen DM, Heimberg RG, Chambless DL, Rapee RM. More information from fewer questions: the factor structure and item properties of the original and brief fear of negative evaluation scale. Psychol Assess. 2004;16(2):169–81. doi: 10.1037/1040-3590.16.2.169 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Khan SR, Naqvi I. Self-criticism and fear of negative evaluation among university students with and without obesity. Pak J Psychol Res. 2016;31(2):509–30. [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Reidpath DD, Burns C, Garrard J, Mahoney M, Townsend M. An ecological study of the relationship between social and environmental determinants of obesity. Health Place. 2002;8(2):141–5. doi: 10.1016/s1353-8292(01)00028-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Burgoine T, Forouhi NG, Griffin SJ, Brage S, Wareham NJ, Monsivais P. Does neighborhood fast-food outlet exposure amplify inequalities in diet and obesity? A cross-sectional study. Am J Clin Nutr. 2016;103(6):1540–7. doi: 10.3945/ajcn.115.128132 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Burgoine T, Sarkar C, Webster CJ, Monsivais P. Examining the interaction of fast-food outlet exposure and income on diet and obesity: evidence from 51,361 UK Biobank participants. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act. 2018;15(1):71. doi: 10.1186/s12966-018-0699-8 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Mackay S. Measuring the cost and affordability of healthier and less healthy foods, meals and diets, Doctoral dissertation. ResearchSpace@ Auckland; 2018. [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Akbay C, Tiryaki GY, Gul A. Consumer characteristics influencing fast food consumption in Turkey. Food Control. 2007;18(8):904–13. [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Sharifirad G, Yarmohammadi P, Azadbakht L, Morowatisharifabad MA, Hassanzadeh A. Determinants of fast food consumption among Iranian high school students based on planned behavior theory. J Obes. 2013;2013:147589. doi: 10.1155/2013/147589 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Nardi VAM, Jardim WC, Ladeira W, Santini F. Predicting food choice: a meta-analysis based on the theory of planned behavior. Br Food J. 2019;121(10):2250–64. [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Coşkun A, Yetkin Özbük RM. What influences consumer food waste behavior in restaurants? An application of the extended theory of planned behavior. Waste Manag. 2020;117:170–8. doi: 10.1016/j.wasman.2020.08.011 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Bîlbîie A, Druică E, Dumitrescu R, Aducovschi D, Sakizlian R, Sakizlian M. Determinants of fast-food consumption in romania: an application of the theory of planned behavior. Foods. 2021;10(8):1877. doi: 10.3390/foods10081877 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Tavakol M, Dennick R. Making sense of Cronbach’s alpha. Int J Med Educ. 2011;2:53–5. doi: 10.5116/ijme.4dfb.8dfd [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Sajjad M, Bhatti A, Hill B, Al-Omari B. Using the theory of planned behavior to predict factors influencing fast-food consumption among college students. BMC Public Health. 2023;23(1):987. doi: 10.1186/s12889-023-15923-1 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Kline RB. Principles and practice of structural equation modeling. Guilford publications; 2023. [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Marsh HW, Hocevar D. Application of confirmatory factor analysis to the study of self-concept: first-and higher order factor models and their invariance across groups. Psychol Bull. 1985;97(3):562. [Google Scholar]
  • 64.MacCallum RC, Browne MW, Sugawara HM. Power analysis and determination of sample size for covariance structure modeling. Psychol Methods. 1996;1(2):130. [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Hu LT, Bentler PM. Fit indices in covariance structure modeling: sensitivity to underparameterized model misspecification. Psychol Methods. 1998;3(4):424. [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Steiger JH. Understanding the limitations of global fit assessment in structural equation modeling. Pers Individ Differ. 2007;42(5):893–8. [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Fan X, Thompson B, Wang L. Effects of sample size, estimation methods, and model specification on structural equation modeling fit indexes. Struct Equ Modeling. 1999;6(1):56–83. [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Gourlan M, Boiché J, Takito M, Fregeac B, Cousson-Gélie F. Predicting physical activity among children: investigating interaction effects in the theory of planned behavior. Res Q Exerc Sport. 2018;89(4):490–7. doi: 10.1080/02701367.2018.1517236 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Bosnjak M, Ajzen I, Schmidt P. The theory of planned behavior: selected recent advances and applications. Eur J Psychol. 2020;16(3):352–6. doi: 10.5964/ejop.v16i3.3107 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Fisher RJ. Social desirability bias and the validity of indirect questioning. J Consum Res. 1993;20(2):303. doi: 10.1086/209351 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Caputo A. Social desirability bias in self-reported well-being measures: evidence from an online survey. Univ Psychol. 2017;16(2):245–55. [Google Scholar]
  • 72.Lechner L, Brug J, De Vries H. Misconceptions of fruit and vegetable consumption: differences between objective and subjective estimation of intake. J Nutr Educ. 1997;29(6):313–20. [Google Scholar]
  • 73.Lawrence H. Dietary perceptions in people with type 2 diabetes: accuracy of estimated dietary intake and perception of healthy and unhealthy foods. Doctoral dissertation. University of Otago; 2016. [Google Scholar]
  • 74.Rivis A, Sheeran P. Descriptive norms as an additional predictor in the theory of planned behaviour: a meta-analysis. Curr Psychol. 2003;22(3):218–33. doi: 10.1007/s12144-003-1018-2 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 75.Cialdini RB, Kallgren CA, Reno RR. A focus theory of normative conduct: a theoretical refinement and reevaluation of the role of norms in human behavior. Adv Exp Soc Psychol. 1991;24:201–34. [Google Scholar]
  • 76.Burgoine T, Sarkar C, Webster CJ, Monsivais P. Examining the interaction of fast-food outlet exposure and income on diet and obesity: evidence from 51,361 UK Biobank participants. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act. 2018;15(1):71. doi: 10.1186/s12966-018-0699-8 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 77.Khader Y, Irshaidat O, Khasawneh M, Amarin Z, Alomari M, Batieha A. Overweight and obesity among school children in Jordan: prevalence and associated factors. Matern Child Health J. 2009;13(3):424–31. doi: 10.1007/s10995-008-0362-0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 78.Khatatbeh M, Momani W, Altaany Z, Al Saad R, Al Bourah AR, Melhem O, et al. Mediterranean fast food: a leading cause of hypercholesterolemia among university students in northern Jordan. Iran J Public Health. 2022;51(4):779–87. doi: 10.18502/ijph.v51i4.9238 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Decision Letter 0

António Raposo

Dear Dr. Abu Asab,

Thank you for submitting your manuscript to PLOS ONE. After careful consideration, we feel that it has merit but does not fully meet PLOS ONE’s publication criteria as it currently stands. Therefore, we invite you to submit a revised version of the manuscript that addresses the points raised during the review process.

Please submit your revised manuscript by Jun 01 2025 11:59PM. If you will need more time than this to complete your revisions, please reply to this message or contact the journal office at plosone@plos.org . When you're ready to submit your revision, log on to https://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/ and select the 'Submissions Needing Revision' folder to locate your manuscript file.

  • A rebuttal letter that responds to each point raised by the academic editor and reviewer(s). You should upload this letter as a separate file labeled 'Response to Reviewers'.

  • A marked-up copy of your manuscript that highlights changes made to the original version. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Revised Manuscript with Track Changes'.

  • An unmarked version of your revised paper without tracked changes. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Manuscript'.

If you would like to make changes to your financial disclosure, please include your updated statement in your cover letter. Guidelines for resubmitting your figure files are available below the reviewer comments at the end of this letter.

If applicable, we recommend that you deposit your laboratory protocols in protocols.io to enhance the reproducibility of your results. Protocols.io assigns your protocol its own identifier (DOI) so that it can be cited independently in the future. For instructions see: https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/submission-guidelines#loc-laboratory-protocols . Additionally, PLOS ONE offers an option for publishing peer-reviewed Lab Protocol articles, which describe protocols hosted on protocols.io. Read more information on sharing protocols at https://plos.org/protocols?utm_medium=editorial-email&utm_source=authorletters&utm_campaign=protocols .

We look forward to receiving your revised manuscript.

Kind regards,

António Raposo

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Journal Requirements:

When submitting your revision, we need you to address these additional requirements.

1. Please ensure that your manuscript meets PLOS ONE's style requirements, including those for file naming. The PLOS ONE style templates can be found at

https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=wjVg/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_main_body.pdf and

https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=ba62/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_title_authors_affiliations.pdf

2. Thank you for stating the following in your Competing Interests section:

“The authors have declared that no competing interests exist.The authors certify that they have NO affiliations with or involvement in any organization or entity with any financial interest (such as honoraria; educational grants; participation in speakers’ bureaus; membership, employment, consultancies, stock ownership, or other equity interest; and expert testimony or patent-licensing arrangements), or non-financial interest (such as personal or professional relationships, affiliations, knowledge or beliefs) in the subject matter or materials discussed in this manuscript.”

Please complete your Competing Interests on the online submission form to state any Competing Interests. If you have no competing interests, please state "The authors have declared that no competing interests exist.", as detailed online in our guide for authors at http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/submit-now

This information should be included in your cover letter; we will change the online submission form on your behalf.

3. In the online submission form, you indicated that [The data underlying the results presented in the study are available from the authors upon request. Due to ethical considerations, the data cannot be shared publicly to protect the confidentiality and privacy of the participants involved. Interested researchers can contact the corresponding author to obtain access to the data, provided they agree to comply with the ethical guidelines and data protection protocols established by the study's review board.].

All PLOS journals now require all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript to be freely available to other researchers, either 1. In a public repository, 2. Within the manuscript itself, or 3. Uploaded as supplementary information.

This policy applies to all data except where public deposition would breach compliance with the protocol approved by your research ethics board. If your data cannot be made publicly available for ethical or legal reasons (e.g., public availability would compromise patient privacy), please explain your reasons on resubmission and your exemption request will be escalated for approval.

4. We note that you have indicated that there are restrictions to data sharing for this study. PLOS only allows data to be available upon request if there are legal or ethical restrictions on sharing data publicly. For more information on unacceptable data access restrictions, please see http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/data-availability#loc-unacceptable-data-access-restrictions.

Before we proceed with your manuscript, please address the following prompts:

a) If there are ethical or legal restrictions on sharing a de-identified data set, please explain them in detail (e.g., data contain potentially identifying or sensitive patient information, data are owned by a third-party organization, etc.) and who has imposed them (e.g., a Research Ethics Committee or Institutional Review Board, etc.). Please also provide contact information for a data access committee, ethics committee, or other institutional body to which data requests may be sent.

b) If there are no restrictions, please upload the minimal anonymized data set necessary to replicate your study findings to a stable, public repository and provide us with the relevant URLs, DOIs, or accession numbers. For a list of recommended repositories, please see

https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/recommended-repositories. You also have the option of uploading the data as Supporting Information files, but we would recommend depositing data directly to a data repository if possible.

We will update your Data Availability statement on your behalf to reflect the information you provide.

5. Your ethics statement should only appear in the Methods section of your manuscript. If your ethics statement is written in any section besides the Methods, please delete it from any other section.

[Note: HTML markup is below. Please do not edit.]

Reviewers' comments:

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Author

1. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Partly

Reviewer #3: No

**********

2. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously? -->?>

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

Reviewer #3: Yes

**********

3. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available??>

The PLOS Data policy

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

Reviewer #3: Yes

**********

4. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English??>

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: No

Reviewer #3: Yes

**********

Reviewer #1: Dear Author,

Thank you for your valuable work. Below, I have provided some insights to help improve the

paper:

1.

Methods Section:

I recommend including a graph that illustrates the key variable of interest, based on the

theoretical framework, in the Methods section. This would enhance the readers'

understanding of your approach and improve the overall readability of the paper.

I noticed that you defined the variable using the question format described in the paper cited

at reference 20. I kindly invite you to elaborate on why a validated questionnaire was not

utilized to investigate this area. Providing such insights would strengthen the clarity and

robustness of your methodological approach.

2.

Figures 1 and 2: Consider redesigning Figures 1 and 2 to make them more visually appealing

and clearer. A more polished design could increase their impact and effectiveness in

conveying the intended message.

3.

Discussion Section: You stated, "Moreover, the study revealed a positive correlation between

higher income, BMI, and frequent fast-food consumption." However, this finding contrasts

with the existing literature, which generally suggests that higher BMI is associated with lower

income. I suggest including references that establish this general trend, as well as studies

specific to Jordan, to explore whether your findings reflect regional nuances.

4.

Income Variable: Since your study focuses on the student population, the use of "income" as

a variable requires clarification. In this context, "income" likely refers to family income, which

may not accurately represent students’ financial independence or spending habits.

Furthermore, students often manage money that they do not directly earn, which limits the

variable's relevance. I suggest reconsidering the role of income in your analysis or reframing

its significance in your discussion.

5.

Economic Status and BMI: The expression, "This indicates that economic status and weight

related factors significantly influence dietary choices," could be improved to reflect a more

nuanced interpretation. Additionally, the sentence, "Addressing these economic barriers and

providing affordable healthy food options could mitigate the impact of fast-food

consumption, particularly among higher-income students," should consider broader social

factors.

6.

Social Context of Fast-Food Consumption: Investigate whether fast-food consumption

among students serves as a form of socialization or if students visit fast-food outlets alone.

Understanding these social dynamics is crucial for devising strategies to reduce fast-food

consumption. Merely emphasizing the health risks of fast food may not suffice to change

behavior. Instead, addressing the social motivations behind fast-food visits could provide

more effective interventions.

I hope these suggestions help improve your manuscript.

Reviewer #2: Dear Editor in Chief

Plos ONE

The paper: Analysing Fast Food Consumption Behaviour Patterns: The Case of Jordan is a good study and a worthy research topic. It is quiet relevant to Plos One topics. However, the article cannot be considered for publication in Plos One as it is. It needs a strong revision, as there are serious readability problems and methodological issues that the author(s) must manage effectively. I offer some questions and comments with the hope of helping s/he/them improve their work. I wish the research team the very best of luck as they continue work in this domain.

Overall evaluation.

It is a worthy research topic and a interesting study but the paper needs a strong revision.

I hope that the comments are fair and constructive. All Best!

Comment 1) Introduction” section needs revision. The author(s) must underline the initial assumptions of the paper and the originality of the paper – contribution to knowledge. I would suggest considering the following five (5) basic elements in the section of introduction as subheadings of Introduction:

a) Research aim:

b) Initial assumptions of the paper

c) Reasoning for the focus of the paper,

d) Research objectives,

e) Originality of the paper and contribution to knowledge.

The author (s) tried to write based on above, but unfortunately is so poor.

In the last paragraph of the Introduction, the authors should clearly mention the weaker point of former works (identification of the gaps) and describe the novelties of the current investigation to justify the paper deserves to be published in food marketing journals. For example, it is not explained about stage one at section aim in the last paragraph of Introduction. Section one is missing.

What is TPB in section Introduction. Mention the complete form of abbreviation when it is included at the first time.

Comment 2) you have to add Background to your paper.

Comment 3) Method and results Section needs revision. The description of this is tedious and it’s so hard for reader to understand. It is mentioned 408 participants in section abstract. However it is written 400 in this section. There are discrepancies in these parts. Correct it.

Comment 4) Research results: It is so long and it is needed to shorten. It is like a report. There are lots of hypothesis that is not required at this part. In section “Theorical…”, it is mention some number as reference instead of the name of authors like [17], [18], [32], [36,37,38,39..].

Comment 5) Discussion section. Author(s) must explain what is the different of his study with others and if they are same, why they are similar? Comparison must be with papers after 2013. It needs to expand the importance of findings. No study is included at this part. It is like a report.

Comment 6) Conclusion is so poor and so long. Shorten and present the main finding of study.

Comment 7) it is important to review the article’ writing. There are some typos throughout the text.

Reviewer #3: This article aims to investigate the factors influencing fast food consumption among university students in Jordan. However, it does not clearly explain why this particular group needs to be focused on or the reasons behind this choice. Additionally, the title and abstract should be revised to reflect that this study is on university students. the literature emphasizes the Theory of Planned Behavior (TPB), yet there is a lack of literature review relating specifically to the content of this study.

Moreover, the sample is entirely drawn from the University of Jordan. The rationale for selecting this institution as the sole source of participants is not provided. Can this university truly represent the entire university student population of Jordan? Furthermore, 75% of the sample are female students. Does this proportion align with the actual gender distribution among university students in Jordan? How can the authors demonstrate that this constitutes a valid and representative sample?

**********

what does this mean? ). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy

Reviewer #1: No

Reviewer #2: No

Reviewer #3: No

**********

[NOTE: If reviewer comments were submitted as an attachment file, they will be attached to this email and accessible via the submission site. Please log into your account, locate the manuscript record, and check for the action link "View Attachments". If this link does not appear, there are no attachment files.]

While revising your submission, please upload your figure files to the Preflight Analysis and Conversion Engine (PACE) digital diagnostic tool, https://pacev2.apexcovantage.com/ . PACE helps ensure that figures meet PLOS requirements. To use PACE, you must first register as a user. Registration is free. Then, login and navigate to the UPLOAD tab, where you will find detailed instructions on how to use the tool. If you encounter any issues or have any questions when using PACE, please email PLOS at figures@plos.org . Please note that Supporting Information files do not need this step.

Attachment

Submitted filename: Review_Fast Food Jordan.pdf

pone.0326347.s003.pdf (51.3KB, pdf)
PLoS One. 2025 Jun 27;20(6):e0326347. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0326347.r002

Author response to Decision Letter 1


12 May 2025

Response to Reviewer Comments

Manuscript: Analysing Fast Food Consumption Behaviour Patterns: The Case of Jordan

• Comment 1: Use of Non-Validated Questionnaire

We thank the reviewer for this important observation. While validated instruments offer consistency, we deliberately tailored our questionnaire to align with the behavioral, linguistic, and cultural context of Jordanian university students. The instrument retained the core structure of the Theory of Planned Behavior, adapting the extended theory constructs used by Dunn et al. (2011) to enhance relevance and clarity for the target population.

The adapted questionnaire was pilot-tested, revised based on feedback, and assessed for internal consistency (Cronbach’s alpha: 0.60–0.86). This approach is consistent with prior Theory of Planned Behavior-based research in non-Western settings.

To support this methodological framework, we have also added a graph in the Methods section (now Figure 1) that visually illustrates the study’s conceptual framework, based on the extended Theory of Planned Behavior, and key variables of interest. This addition improves reader comprehension of the theoretical structure guiding our analysis.

• Comment 2: Figures 1 and 2 - Clarity and Design

We appreciate the suggestion. Figures 1 and 2 have been redesigned and renamed as Figures 2 and 3, while a new Figure 1 was added in the Methods section to illustrate the full conceptual framework. The revised figures use clearer layoutsand improved labeling to enhance readability and impact.

• Comment 3: BMI- Income

Thank you for this insightful comment. We now reference Burgoine et al. (2018) to reflect global trends showing a negative relationship between income and BMI in high-income settings. To contextualize our contrasting result, we included Jordan-specific studies (Khader et al., 2009; Khatatbeh et al., 2022) showing that students with higher income tend to consume more fast food and exhibit higher BMI levels, likely due to greater access to calorie-dense options.

• Comment 4: Clarification of 'Income' Variable

We agree with the reviewer. The term “income” was clarified in the Methods section as referring to students’ available personal funds, including family support, stipends, part-time jobs, and scholarships. It serves as a proxy for financial flexibility rather than independence.

• Comment 5: Economic Status and BMI Interpretation

Thank you for this thoughtful comment. We agree that the original phrasing could be refined to better reflect the complexity of factors influencing dietary choices. In response, we have revised this section of the discussion to emphasize that dietary behavior is shaped by a multidimensional interplay of economic status, physiological characteristics (e.g., BMI), psychological dispositions, and social-environmental influences such as food marketing, peer behavior, and cultural norms. We also revised the second sentence to reflect that effective interventions should not only address affordability and access but also tackle social motivations and environmental cues that shape food choices among students. These revisions now provide a more comprehensive and context-sensitive interpretation of the findings.

• Comment 6: Social Context of Fast-Food Consumption

We fully agree. While our study did not explicitly assess whether students consumed fast food alone or in social groups, we agree that the social context of consumption is a critical factor in understanding food choices among young adults. In response, we have revised the Discussion section to acknowledge the likely social function of fast-food consumption, such as peer bonding, group outings, or convenience in communal settings. We also note that social motivations and norms may significantly influence dietary behavior, potentially limiting the effectiveness of health messaging alone. The revised text emphasizes the need for interventions that consider social dynamics, and we recommend that future research explore whether students eat alone or with peers, and how group behaviors shape consumption decisions.

Additional comments:

• Comment: Methods and Results - Clarity and Consistency

Thank you for the feedback. The Methods and Results sections have been revised for clarity and conciseness. To address the discrepancy, we clarified that 408 students participated and ensured consistency across the abstract and body of the paper. The phrasing "around 400" in the methodology reflects the initial planning stage; the final figure of 408 is used consistently in the Results and Abstract.

• Comment: Language and Typographical Errors

We appreciate this note. The entire manuscript has undergone a thorough language and grammar review. Typos and stylistic inconsistencies have been corrected to improve clarity and ensure professional quality.

• Comment: Sampling and Representativeness

We acknowledge this important point. In response, we have clarified in the Methods section that the University of Jordan was selected due to its size, diversity of academic programs, and geographic centrality. These features make it a suitable and meaningful cross-section for studying university students in Jordan. While the sample may not fully represent all students nationwide, its demographic diversity provides strong grounds for initial exploration. We also addressed the gender imbalance in the sample, noting that the high proportion of female participants (approximately 75%) aligns with national enrollment patterns in Jordanian universities, particularly within the humanities and social sciences. This clarification supports the interpretability and relevance of the findings within the context of the local higher education landscape.

• Comment: The Introduction section needs revision.

The Introduction has been thoroughly revised to include a clear research aim, theoretical assumptions, justification for the population focus, explicit objectives, and a statement of the study’s originality and contribution to the field.

We thank the editor and reviewers for their valuable comments, which significantly improved the clarity and rigor of this paper.

Attachment

Submitted filename: Response to Reviewers.docx

pone.0326347.s005.docx (18.1KB, docx)

Decision Letter 1

António Raposo

Analysing Fast Food Consumption Behaviour Patterns: The Case of Jordan

PONE-D-24-31310R1

Dear Dr. Abu Asab,

We’re pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been judged scientifically suitable for publication and will be formally accepted for publication once it meets all outstanding technical requirements.

Within one week, you’ll receive an e-mail detailing the required amendments. When these have been addressed, you’ll receive a formal acceptance letter and your manuscript will be scheduled for publication.

An invoice will be generated when your article is formally accepted. Please note, if your institution has a publishing partnership with PLOS and your article meets the relevant criteria, all or part of your publication costs will be covered. Please make sure your user information is up-to-date by logging into Editorial Manager at Editorial Manager®  and clicking the ‘Update My Information' link at the top of the page. If you have any questions relating to publication charges, please contact our Author Billing department directly at authorbilling@plos.org.

If your institution or institutions have a press office, please notify them about your upcoming paper to help maximize its impact. If they’ll be preparing press materials, please inform our press team as soon as possible -- no later than 48 hours after receiving the formal acceptance. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information, please contact onepress@plos.org.

Kind regards,

António Raposo

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Additional Editor Comments (optional):

Reviewers' comments:

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Author

Reviewer #1: All comments have been addressed

**********

2. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions??>

Reviewer #1: Yes

**********

3. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously? -->?>

Reviewer #1: Yes

**********

4. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available??>

The PLOS Data policy

Reviewer #1: Yes

**********

5. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English??>

Reviewer #1: Yes

**********

Reviewer #1: After the revision, the article appears clearer and more understandable. It is stronger and more consistent overall. All sections are well explained, and it offers new insights into students' food habits, providing a focused perspective through the case of Jourdan.

**********

what does this mean? ). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy

Reviewer #1: No

**********

Acceptance letter

António Raposo

PONE-D-24-31310R1

PLOS ONE

Dear Dr. Abu Asab,

I'm pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been deemed suitable for publication in PLOS ONE. Congratulations! Your manuscript is now being handed over to our production team.

At this stage, our production department will prepare your paper for publication. This includes ensuring the following:

* All references, tables, and figures are properly cited

* All relevant supporting information is included in the manuscript submission,

* There are no issues that prevent the paper from being properly typeset

You will receive further instructions from the production team, including instructions on how to review your proof when it is ready. Please keep in mind that we are working through a large volume of accepted articles, so please give us a few days to review your paper and let you know the next and final steps.

Lastly, if your institution or institutions have a press office, please let them know about your upcoming paper now to help maximize its impact. If they'll be preparing press materials, please inform our press team within the next 48 hours. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information, please contact onepress@plos.org.

If we can help with anything else, please email us at customercare@plos.org.

Thank you for submitting your work to PLOS ONE and supporting open access.

Kind regards,

PLOS ONE Editorial Office Staff

on behalf of

Dr. António Raposo

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Associated Data

    This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

    Supplementary Materials

    S1 Dataset. Minimal data set containing coded variables and values used in all statistical analyses, including those used to generate figures and regression models.

    (XLSX)

    pone.0326347.s001.xlsx (136.4KB, xlsx)
    S1 Data. De-identified Arabic-language questionnaire responses collected during the study.

    Provided for reference and contextual transparency.

    (XLSX)

    pone.0326347.s002.xlsx (98.2KB, xlsx)
    Attachment

    Submitted filename: Review_Fast Food Jordan.pdf

    pone.0326347.s003.pdf (51.3KB, pdf)
    Attachment

    Submitted filename: Response to Reviewers.docx

    pone.0326347.s005.docx (18.1KB, docx)

    Data Availability Statement

    All relevant data are within the paper and its Supporting Information files.


    Articles from PLOS One are provided here courtesy of PLOS

    RESOURCES