Abstract
This paper introduces the concept of neuroheuristics—a novel transdisciplinary paradigm designed to advance cognitive neurodynamics by integrating insights from molecular biology, computing, behavioral science, and clinical neuroscience. Contrasted with the traditional reductionist approach rooted in classical determinism, neuroheuristics emphasizes a flexible, problem-solving methodology for investigating brain function across multiple levels of complexity. The paper explores the epistemological interplay among genetic, epigenetic, and environmental factors in brain development and pathology. The neuroheuristic framework aims to elucidate complex cognitive phenomena-such as memory, decision-making, and creativity-by bridging bottom-up and top-down research strategies. By incorporating contemporary technologies and recognizing the brain’s dynamic, nonlinear properties, neuroheuristics proposes a transformative shift in cognitive neurodynamics, enabling a deeper understanding of human cognition, disease mechanisms, and artificial intelligence. Its applicability is demonstrated through ongoing interdisciplinary research spanning neurophysiological disorders, computational modeling, and data-driven analytical techniques.
Keywords: Neuroheuristics, Neural networks, Cognitive dynamics, Dynamical systems, Neuromodulation, Deterministic chaos
Introduction
Modern science is grounded in the assumption of an objective external reality, a perspective historically reinforced by the rational and mechanistic principles of causality and determinism. This philosophical stance, which originated with Descartes in the 17th century, has been fundamental to the development of classical empiricism. It has profoundly influenced progress in the physical and technical sciences, shaping the modern technological world we live in today. While these principles have led to major successes in the physical sciences, their limitations become evident in complex, adaptive systems-particularly in biology. In fluid dynamics, for example, understanding turbulent flow has necessitated the incorporation of chaos theory, which departs from classical deterministic frameworks (Frisch 1995; Pope 2000). Similarly, in biomedical research, the limits of classical approaches become evident, especially when studying aging and pathological processes. The apparent stability of living organisms is deceptive, as most intracellular molecules are constantly being renewed. This continuous cycle of anabolism and catabolism, the breakdown and rebuilding of cellular components, is a major consumer of energy in organisms. These transformations are not adequately explained by classical thermodynamics alone and are more accurately described through the framework of far-from-equilibrium thermodynamics, where energy dissipation facilitates transitions toward more stable organizational states (Prigogine and Stengers 1979).
Such reorganizations involve complex flows of information that resist quantification through traditional spatial metrics. When applied to brain function, this epistemological challenge becomes particularly pronounced. Cognition cannot be isolated from its neurobiological substrate, nor can it be fully explained without reference to mental representations, logic, and computational models. Biological systems are subject to both genetic and epigenetic variability, which can result in significant cellular disruptions during development. For instance, up to 20-50% of neurons undergo programmed cell death (apoptosis) after they have matured and established functional connections (Andersen 2004; Dekkers et al. 2013; Riva et al. 2019). Though counterintuitive from the standpoint of classical developmental biology, this pruning process is essential for enabling higher-order neural organization.
It remains problematic to assert that mental phenomena-particularly those shaped by individual experience-can be directly reduced to cellular or molecular levels of neural organization. Historical examples underscore this complexity. For instance, the brain of Anatole France, a Nobel Prize-winning writer (France 1921), was comparable in weight to that of Homo erectus (Java Man) (Keith 1927), and the Neanderthal brain was, in fact, larger than that of modern humans (Homo sapiens) (Falk 1991; Tattersall 2023). Such examples highlight that brain volume alone is insufficient to explain cognitive capacity. The functional architecture of the brain emerges from multiscale interactions that span molecular, cellular, individual, and social dimensions (Koch 2016; DeSilva et al. 2021).
The brain’s functional complexity—operating across these multiple organizational scales-poses a major challenge to any unified explanatory model of cognition. It is within this conceptual and methodological gap that the neuroheuristic paradigm is proposed: as a framework capable of addressing both the nonlinear, dynamic nature of brain function and the epistemological limitations of reductionist science.
From complexity to discovery: a new paradigm for neuroscience
In neuroscience, researchers have traditionally adopted a “bottom-up” approach, beginning with cellular or molecular processes and working upward toward the understanding of complex cognitive functions. Although widely employed in neuroscience, this reductionist strategy remains constrained by experimental limitations. Even the most basic neural architectures are too complex to be exhaustively characterized at the cellular level. Conversely, “top-down” methodologies—those that start with behavioral or cognitive phenomena and attempt to infer underlying mechanisms—can offer more practical heuristics for system-level modeling, yet often lack the mechanistic specificity needed to explain neural dynamics in detail. While each approach yields valuable insights, neither is sufficient in isolation. The convergence of “bottom-up” and “top-down” strategies holds transformative potential: their integration is not merely additive but generative, fostering the development of new conceptual frameworks and leading to unexpected discoveries. In the 21st century, this integrative approach must continuously incorporate emerging sciences and technologies. Two areas that are particularly crucial today are molecular biology and computer science.
The interdisciplinary convergence provided by their methodological and analytical tools drives the development of a truly transdisciplinary theoretical framework (Kötter and Balsiger 1999) in integrative neuroscience—a shift exemplified by the emergence of the neuroheuristic paradigm. The term neuroheuristics (alternatively, neuristic) derives from the Greek words νεῦρον [neuron], meaning ‘nerve’, and εὑρίσκειν [heuriskein], meaning ‘to find’ or ‘to discover’). It denotes a transdisciplinary epistemology that transcends the boundaries of specialized expertise, promoting iterative knowledge renewal as a fundamental feature of scientific inquiry. In this framework, outcomes are not easily classifiable as successes or failures, because progress is measured by the continual refinement and transformation of explanatory models. The set of procedures or frameworks that define how we come to know something, and how we determine that our knowledge is credible or meaningful (i.e., the epistemologic process) cannot be reduced to mere proficiency. The emphasis lies not in static expertise, but in adaptive methodologies capable of responding to the brain’s inherent dynamism. This stands in contrast to Henri Bergson’s (1859-1941) concept of psychophysical interactionism, Bergson (1896), wherein progression is driven by a “vital impulse” (élan vital)—a metaphysical force responsible for evolutionary transitions (Bergson 1907). Whereas Bergson attributes change to a life-force beyond physical properties, neuroheuristics conceptualizes transformation as an emergent property of complex interactions among existing system components.
One of the central aims in neuroheuristics is to explain how the brain integrates information across hierarchical levels of organization—from molecular interactions and neuronal circuits to cognitive processes and behavior. Recent scientific and technological developments, especially in genomics and computational neuroscience, offer unprecedented tools for probing such multilevel interactions and driving new insights and discoveries. For instance, the Human Genome Project (1990–2003) dramatically accelerated the understanding of genetic contributions to brain structure and function. Approximately 40% of human genes are expressed in the brain (Wang et al. 2018; Sjöstedt et al. 2020), and hundreds of heritable neurological disorders have been identified (Cooper and Jan 1999; Shukla et al. 2021). Although there are an estimated 20,000 to 25,000 genes that encode proteins, not all genes are constantly expressed in every cell. It is now widely recognized that gene expression is highly dynamic—modulated by cell type, developmental stage, and environmental stimuli. Furthermore, non-coding regions of the genome, once dismissed as “junk DNA”, play critical roles in gene regulation. The etiology of many neurological and psychiatric disorders involves complex interactions between polygenic risk factors and environmental exposures, presenting an analytical challenge that resists simplistic models of causality.
Historically, numerous scientific breakthroughs have emerged from the study of rare diseases and small populations. For example, Brown and Goldstein’s Nobel Prize-winning research on cholesterol metabolism focused on a rare genetic disorder characterized by extremely high cholesterol levels (Brown 1985; Goldstein 1985). Their findings ultimately illuminated mechanisms relevant to the broader population suffering from hypercholesterolemia. These cases illustrate the scientific value of studying atypical phenomena-an approach aligned with heuristic reasoning.
Unlike algorithmic methods, which guarantee correctness but often require impractically long computation, heuristic strategies prioritize adaptability and iterative progress. Though they do not ensure optimal solutions, heuristics enable the generation of satisfactory outcomes within well-defined constraints. In clinical medicine, for example, heuristic tools such as decision rules allow physicians to revise diagnoses in real time based on emerging patient data, fostering faster and more adaptive care (Guy et al. 2016). In neuroscience, Adaptive Resonance Theory (ART) models learning as a continuous process of hypothesis evaluation and revision, enabling neural networks to adapt flexibly to novel sensory inputs and changing environments (Grossberg 1987, 1999). Similarly, in neuroimaging, initial heuristic interpretations of brain activity often guide the design of follow-up experiments, leading to improved analytical tools and deeper insights into brain–behavior relationships (Poldrack et al. 2017; Masulli et al. 2020; Perrig et al. 2010; Michel et al. 2012; Loosen et al. 2024). Studies on hierarchical processing in perception and cognition have also shed light on how complex neural circuits contribute to unified representations and adaptive behavior (Grossberg and Mingolla 1985; Grossberg 1999).
These diverse research efforts emphasize the integration of information across multiple levels of organization-a core principle of the neuroheuristic framework. This approach provides a pathway for understanding both the fundamental biological substrates of brain function and the emergent properties underlying cognition and disease. A practical example is found in Alzheimer’s disease research, where genetic markers, molecular pathologies, and network-level disruptions are examined in tandem to explain both biochemical mechanisms and cognitive decline (Celesia et al. 1993; Iturria-Medina et al. 2016; Hampel et al. 2018; Jack et al. 2018). Similarly, studies on attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) integrate genetic vulnerabilities, neurotransmitter dynamics, and behavioral assessments to inform diagnosis and treatment across biological and clinical domains (Bonvicini et al. 2016; Mesrobian et al. 2018; Dotare et al. 2020; Cortese et al. 2022; Custodio et al. 2023). Neuromimetic computational models simulating these multilayered processes further provide experimental frameworks for exploring the complex causal pathways involved in neurological and psychiatric disorders (Villa 2008; Perrig et al. 2010).
The neuroheuristic approach in cognitive neurodynamics
Cognitive neurodynamics refers to the study of how cognitive processes—such as perception, memory, decision-making processes, and attention—are dynamically organized and processed in the brain. It focuses on the temporal and spatial patterns of neural activity that give rise to cognition. The brain integrates both genetic predispositions (nature) and environmental influences (nurture) in processing information. This classical dichotomy traces back to early philosophical debates, with thinkers such as John Locke advocating the tabula rasa (blank slate) hypothesis (Wood 1992), in which individuals are shaped entirely by experience. Conversely, Charles Darwin emphasized the role of innate biological traits through his theory of evolution (Boero 2015). Contemporary science recognizes that nature and nurture are fundamentally intertwined-genetic predispositions are modulated, enhanced, or attenuated by environmental factors. For instance, an individual with a genetic inclination for musical ability may never realize their potential without access to appropriate environmental stimuli, such as musical education.
Within this dynamic interplay, neuroheuristics aims to maintain epistemological balance by emphasizing the relationships that govern brain development and function. A key question arises: do underlying laws govern this dynamic equilibrium? One of the earliest biological laws—Mendel’s laws of inheritance—provided the foundation for modern genetics (Keynes and Cox 2008). However, these laws apply within limited contexts and are insufficient to explain the behavior of living systems, which operate far from equilibrium. Classical mechanistic approaches, for instance, often treat time as a symmetric variable, permitting reversibility. Yet living systems are fundamentally governed by the irreversible flow of time. This concept, central to the work of Ilya Prigogine, emphasizes that classical physics is appropriate primarily for stable systems, while biological organisms require alternative principles grounded in far-from-equilibrium dynamics (Prigogine 1977; Prigogine and Stengers 1979).
Neuroheuristics aligns with this perspective, suggesting that the organization of the nervous system is fundamentally geared toward temporal information processing, such as memory and the ability to project oneself into the future—a function that is operationally defined as prediction. The prefrontal cortex plays a critical role in mediating these temporal contingencies and has been pivotal in the evolutionary process of hominization (Fuster 2015). Research into the temporal dynamics of neural activity has demonstrated that cognitive functions such as decision-making and reward processing rely on the intricate timing of neural circuits (Richmond et al. 2003; Abeles 2004; Bouret and Richmond 2010; Abeles 2014). One notable observation is that key processes, such as the interplay between memory and prediction, occur largely during sleep (Gais and Born 2004; Klinzing et al. 2019). Studies have shown that sleep, particularly its deeper stages, consolidates memories and facilitates the brain’s ability to make predictions based on past experiences. Disruptions in sleep (Horne 1978; Irish et al. 2012) can lead to severe memory impairments and an inability to access “future memories”—the set of action plans based on previous learning that guide present decision-making. Sleep deprivation, therefore, can lead to behaviors such as apathy, lack of foresight, and poor decision-making (van der Helm et al. 2010; Walker 2009; Coles et al. 2015). Another intriguing phenomenon observed during sleep is the absence of chronological references, which allows memories to blend with imagined or impossible scenarios-similar to virtual reality (Beattie et al. 2015; Picard-Deland et al. 2021). This process points to a temporal dimension of brain activity that differs from classical mechanics and may explain the sudden emergence of insights, ideas, or intuitions, sometimes referred to as the “Gestalt switch” (Wright 1992; Wray 2021; Metzinger 2024). These moments involve breaking free from conventional time constraints and are central to understanding creativity and innovation (Fell 2012).
Biological laws, much like mathematical axioms, were initially derived from observations and, while mostly correct, were limited by their descriptive nature. In contrast, neuroheuristics moves beyond this limitation by incorporating a broader, more flexible understanding of brain dynamics (Taylor and Villa 2001). Information flows through neural circuits at varying speeds (Stein et al. 2005; Rama et al. 2018), causing temporal distortions that are not well-described by classical spatial or metric models (Villa 2000; Abeles 2004; Vardi et al. 2013; Cabessa and Villa 2018). This is analogous to different communication systems (e.g., internet, surface mail, bike riders, Alpine horns, or flag signaling) delivering the same message at various speeds, each with its own symbolic system and syntax. This metaphor raises a critical question: how does the brain simultaneously manage multiple information codes?
The brain’s capacity for parallel, multichannel information processing presents unique challenges to conventional models of cognition (Braitenberg 1977; Palm 1982; Abeles 1991; Amit 1992). Neuroheuristics addresses this complexity by emphasizing continuous epistemological renewal at each stage of inquiry. This is especially relevant for understanding volition and decision-making processes that involve the integration of distributed information streams (Zhu 2004; Gold and Shadlen 2007; Haggard 2008; Bouret et al. 2012). Unlike traditional cognitive models that often rest on static theoretical constructs, neuroheuristics anticipates that hypotheses will be revised or surpassed. While cognitive science has significantly advanced our understanding of brain function by distinguishing between declarative knowledge (“knowing what”) and procedural knowledge (“knowing how”) (Atkinson and Shiffrin 1968; Squire 2004; Ryle 2009), it has also tended to reduce cognition to symbolic or computational processes. While cognitive science often uses computational models to simulate aspects of thinking and behavior, these models are typically based on algorithmic logic, such as those employed in classical Turing machines. Neuroheuristics, by contrast, emphasizes a more flexible and biologically grounded approach. Rather than assuming the brain functions like a step-by-step computer program, neuroheuristics integrates concepts from systems theory, nonlinear dynamics, and probability (Tetko and Villa 1997; Villa 2008; Cabessa and Villa 2018). This allows researchers to better model how the brain produces unpredictable, adaptive, and emergent behaviors (Freeman 2000; Brooks 1991; Jordanous 2020; Davis et al. 2025).
Historically, such synergies have fueled major advances in neuroscience. In the 18th century, Albrecht von Haller conducted foundational experiments in animal physiology that investigated the brain’s responsiveness to physical stimuli (von Haller 1755). His work marked a turning point in neurophysiological thought, demonstrating the value of integrating principles from physics, chemistry, and natural philosophy. Later, the introduction of galvanic currents into the nervous system by Luigi Galvani (Galvani 1797) opened up a new frontier in neurophysiology and led to the birth of electrophysiology (Guarnieri 2014). Galvani’s findings, together with those of Luigi Rolando (Rolando 1809), marked a pivotal shift in neuroscience-from passive anatomical observation to systematic experimental investigation of brain function. This transition contributed to a paradigmatic transformation in scientific thinking, laying the conceptual groundwork for the development of modern theories of brain organization and function (Abbott 1999; De Palma and Pareti 2011; Agnati et al. 2007).
Neuroheuristics in the age of computing: bridging historical insights with modern neuroscience
The heuristic paradigm introduced by early neuroscientists such as Albrecht von Haller and Luigi Rolando was not widely adopted by 19th-century researchers, which may partly explain why neuroscience lagged behind disciplines like mathematics, physics, and chemistry during that era. Yet, with hindsight, their approaches appear remarkably prescient. Just as electricity transformed neuroscience approximately two centuries ago, contemporary computing is reshaping the field today. The exponential growth of computational power enables neuroscientists to explore questions about information flow and brain dynamics that were once unreachable. This transformation exemplifies the neuroheuristic principle: discovery is not merely an accumulation of data but the continual refinement of models that link theory, computation, and empirical observation.
Today, computing has become indispensable to neuroscience, offering continually evolving tools that enable the study of increasingly complex systems. Computing power has progressed at such a rapid pace that hardware more than a few years old is often considered obsolete (Moore 2000; Brynjolfsson and McAfee 2014). This acceleration has enabled the resolution of problems once deemed intractable. For instance, cryptographic algorithms that would take millennia to solve through brute-force methods can now be tackled using heuristic strategies that deliver viable solutions within feasible timeframes (Rivest et al. 1978; Korf 1985; Kearns and Vazirani 1994; Papadimitriou 1994). This evolution challenges traditional notions of computational intractability and underscores the transformative role of heuristics in modern science.
Paradoxically, this rapid progress coexists with the need to preserve foundational structures, such as programming languages and architectural standards—a tension that is, in fact, an asset for neuroheuristics. While many computational models of neural networks emphasize dynamic properties, it is equally critical to account for biophysical constraints, such as the energetic demands of synaptic transmission and plasticity (Wang and Zhang 2007; Wang et al. 2015; Zhu et al. 2019). The neuroheuristic paradigm thrives on this duality, using computational advances not as ends in themselves but as flexible instruments for exploring the multilevel dynamics of brain function (Grossberg 2013).
Within this framework, it is useful to distinguish between two forms of rationality: instrumental and epistemic. Instrumental rationality concerns optimizing behavior to achieve specific goals, whereas epistemic rationality focuses on forming beliefs grounded in the best available evidence (Stanovich 2009). Both forms play pivotal roles in neuroscience and in real-world decision making. For instance, in legal contexts, judges must reach verdicts based not on absolute certainty, but on probabilistic reasoning supported by evidence-achieving a state of moral, if not empirical, certainty (Lietzau 1999; Guérin 2015). A similar principle applies in neuroscience, where hypotheses must be continuously tested and revised based on available data, acknowledging that complete certainty is rarely attainable.
Motivation is another central factor in understanding cognitive processing. Neural mechanisms underlying decision making frequently involve mutual inhibitory networks that resolve conflicts between competing action plans (Frith and Happé 1994; Berridge 2004). Dysfunction in these systems can result in maladaptive choices and impairments in personal and social behavior. Such deficits often correlate with learning difficulties and information-processing inefficiencies. These findings contributed to the formulation of the Somatic Marker Hypothesis, which posits that emotional processes play a critical role in guiding decision-making, especially under conditions of uncertainty (Bechara et al. 1994; Bechara and Damasio 2005). Cognitive and affective models of brain function help elucidate how decisions are shaped in contexts ranging from judicial reasoning to personal moral choice (Altimus 2017; Kraft and Giordano 2017).
Neuroheuristics employs computational models to explore the dynamic interactions between brain regions involved in learning, perception, and adaptation. Stephen Grossberg’s work on neural dynamics and Adaptive Resonance Theory (ART) provides foundational insight into how the brain stabilizes learning in the presence of complex, evolving stimuli (Grossberg 1987). His models illustrate how neural systems maintain internal coherence while remaining receptive to novel information-a process that aligns closely with the neuroheuristic approach to cognitive adaptability.
Advancing our understanding of cognitive function requires the formulation of provisional hypotheses-models that, while not universally valid, retain explanatory power within specific contexts (Villa 2008). For instance, studies on sensorimotor integration and decision-making processes, particularly in the auditory domain, benefit from a convergence of data sources: clinical observations in patients with executive function disorders, animal models, and computational simulations. This integrative methodology was instrumental in the founding of the Laboratory of Neuroheuristics at the University of Lausanne in 1995 (Villa 1985). From its inception, the laboratory aimed to overcome disciplinary compartmentalization by bringing together expertise from neurobiology, pharmacology, anatomy, physics, computer science, and electronics-laying the foundation for a genuinely transdisciplinary approach to brain research.
Exploring brain complexity: key research themes in neuroheuristics
Neuroheuristics encourages researchers to move beyond fixed objectives and provisional hypotheses, fostering an open-ended approach to scientific inquiry. A useful metaphor is that of a child exploring a garden—curiously approaching boundaries such as hedges, trellises, or barriers—not to reach a predefined destination, but to discover what lies beyond. This metaphor underscores the value of intellectual flexibility, suggesting that rigid adherence to predetermined goals may hinder rather than facilitate discovery (Taylor and Villa 2001; Farina and Villa 2023).
Although scientific tools have evolved dramatically since the Neolithic era, the fundamental relationship between discovery and the instruments that enable it has remained constant. In recent years, technological advancements-especially in computing-have opened new frontiers in neuroscience. Medical imaging, for example, has transformed the study of brain structure and function. Yet, the complexity of modern neuroscientific challenges demands far more than computational efficiency. Neuroheuristics seeks to redefine this relationship by establishing a paradigm that synthesizes advances in computing, neuroscience, and molecular biology. The brain operates across multiple levels of organization, each contributing uniquely to cognitive function. At the microscopic level, neurons communicate via electrical and chemical signaling, forming complex circuits that underlie macroscopic processes such as memory, attention, and decision making. Understanding how these layers interact is central to contemporary brain science. At the Laboratory of Neuroheuristics, one research direction focuses on developing neuromimetic models and artificial neural networks that integrate empirical findings across scales.
A wide range of single-neuron models has been developed, from phenomenological approaches that simulate action potential generation (e.g., the leaky integrate-and-fire model) to biophysically detailed models that describe ionic dynamics across membrane compartments (Hill and Villa 1997; Asai and Villa 2008, 2012). Because many such models resist analytical treatment, practical formulations often rely on describing membrane potential fluctuations driven by diffusive processes. Increasingly, artificial neurons are being modeled to replicate the computational and adaptive properties of their biological counterparts (Villa and Tetko 1997). These biologically inspired models-referred to as neuromimes-are especially relevant for investigating the temporal characteristics of neural coding (Villa 2000; Del Prete et al. 2004; Villa 2008). Within this framework, the spiking activity of neurons is analyzed through spike trains-sequences of action potentials recorded via electrophysiological techniques (Tetko and Villa 2001b; Iglesias et al. 2005; Villa et al. 2007; Iglesias and Villa 2010).
Neuronal networks function as complex systems governed by coupled, nonlinear dynamical interactions. In studying large-scale networks, researchers often treat firing rates as continuous variables and employ mean-field models-originally developed for spin-glass systems-to characterize collective behavior. Typically, synchronization between systems is defined as shared periodicity and, in some cases, phase alignment (Villa and Tetko 2010; Abe et al. 2024). In the brain, interregional communication is mediated by oscillatory activity that synchronizes neural processes. However, classical synchronization models are often insufficient to account for brain dynamics, particularly under conditions driven by non-periodic or stochastic signals (Cabessa and Villa 2014; Masulli and Villa 2015). Indeed, biological neural networks frequently exhibit forms of synchronization that include chaotic behavior (Malagarriga et al. 2017, 2019).
While the functional significance of chaos in neural systems remains a topic of ongoing investigation, growing evidence supports its critical role in higher cognitive functions such as memory consolidation, decision-making, and creative insight. Skarda and Freeman (1987) argued that the brain leverages chaos not as noise, but as an intrinsic mechanism for perceptual reorganization and adaptive flexibility. In their view, chaos enables the brain to escape rigid attractor states, facilitating transitions to new neural configurations that underlie novel interpretations and responses. Expanding on this, Tsuda (2001), theory of “chaotic itinerancy” describes how neural systems traverse a landscape of quasi-stable states, allowing for context-sensitive cognition and spontaneous shifts in mental state-phenomena closely linked to creativity and problem solving. Empirical findings further substantiate these theoretical perspectives: chaotic dynamics have been observed in the primate motor cortex during motor planning, suggesting that such complexity may support behavioral adaptability in dynamically changing environments (Villa et al. 1998). Additionally, the detection of deterministic chaos in EEG recordings has proven valuable for predicting seizure onset, underscoring its clinical relevance (Aksenova et al. 2007; Villa and Tetko 2010). Furthermore, computational models that incorporate chaotic features provide insights into how flexibility and variability emerge from seemingly stochastic neural activity (Asai et al. 2008; Malagarriga et al. 2017). Within the neuroheuristic framework, chaos is not merely a computational artifact but a functional principle that enables the brain’s dynamic negotiation between stability and novelty.
Temporal dynamics constitute another critical dimension of brain complexity. Neural processes are inherently time-dependent, shaped continuously by attention, perception, and memory (Villa et al. 1998, 1999b; Jaquerod et al. 2020). Neural plasticity-the capacity of the brain to reorganize itself-exemplifies this dynamic adaptability (Villa et al. 1999a; Shaposhnyk and Villa 2012; Lintas et al. 2021). One behavioral paradigm illustrating this involves learning to associate complex sounds with specific motor responses. Over time, such sensorimotor integration strengthens connections between auditory and motor systems (Carretta et al. 1999; Storozhuk et al. 2002; Farré-Castany et al. 2007; Jaquerod et al. 2024).
Cognitive flexibility—the ability to shift strategies or adapt to new contexts—relies heavily on neuromodulatory systems that regulate neurotransmitters such as dopamine (Villa and Lorenzana 1997; Storozhuk et al. 2004; Lintas et al. 2012), serotonin (Hajós et al. 1995, 2001), and acetylcholine (Villa et al. 1996, 2000). These neuromodulators play crucial roles in regulating mood, learning, attention, and decision making (Aston-Jones and Cohen 2005; Mesrobian et al. 2015; Lintas et al. 2017). Although the term neuroheuristics has yet to be standardized, it increasingly denotes approaches that incorporate neuroscientific principles—such as neuromodulation—into heuristic models of cognition (Doya 2002; Cools and Robbins 2004; de Berker et al. 2016; Shine 2019). The neurochemical systems underlying such models are enormously complex, involving multiple neuromodulators and diverse receptor subtypes that are often co-expressed within individual neurons. These systems act across molecular, cellular, and network scales and exhibit context-dependent effects, making them difficult to model in real time (Dayan 2012; Tritsch et al. 2012; Boureau and Dayan 2010; Friston et al. 2013; Clark et al. 2018; Marder 2012). While neuroheuristics does not claim to resolve these intricacies, it offers a conceptual and methodological framework that supports the modeling of such multiscale, nonlinear interactions. This is achieved by integrating empirical insights and theoretical tools from systems neuroscience, such as adaptive gain theory, probabilistic reinforcement learning, and network dynamics modeling (Doya 2002; Cools and Robbins 2004; Villa 2008; Rutledge et al. 2014; de Berker et al. 2016; Grossberg 2013; Cabessa and Villa 2018; Shine 2019). Neuroheuristic methodologies thus facilitate the development of testable hypotheses concerning neuromodulatory function in both clinical and normative brain contexts, without overstating predictive precision.
The detection of recurrent neural patterns through advanced mathematical and statistical techniques is essential for addressing neurological and psychiatric conditions (Tetko and Villa 2001a; Aksenova et al. 2003; Asai et al. 2005; Guy et al. 2016; Masulli et al. 2020). By integrating computational modeling with experimental data, neuroheuristics offers a robust framework for investigating the interaction between high-order neural circuits and clinical pathologies, including Alzheimer’s disease (Celesia et al. 1993), Parkinson’s disease (Chibirova et al. 2005; Lintas et al. 2012), sleep disorders (Abe et al. 2021; Perrig et al. 2024), addiction (Synytsky et al. 2002; Spiga et al. 2011), epilepsy (Aksenova et al. 2007; Villa and Tetko 2010), and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) (Mesrobian et al. 2018; Dotare et al. 2020). In therapeutic domains, neuroheuristics supports the development of personalized, non-pharmacological interventions. For instance, targeted working memory training has shown promise for ADHD (Jaquerod et al. 2020; Dotare et al. 2021; Lintas et al. 2021), while cognitive behavioral therapy has proven effective for insomnia (Perrig et al. 2009).
Conclusion
Despite substantial scientific progress, fundamental questions about the brain remain unresolved. How do large-scale neural networks integrate information across multiple sensory modalities to generate a unified experience? What are the precise neural correlates of consciousness? How can we improve our understanding and treatment of neurodegenerative and psychiatric disorders? These few questions underscore the extraordinary complexity of the brain and the persistent challenges it poses to researchers across disciplines. Unraveling the brain’s dynamic, multiscale, and temporally structured functions requires integrative strategies that bridge biology, computation, and cognition. Recent advancements in neuroscience, bolstered by technological innovation, are enabling researchers to make meaningful strides in deciphering the mechanisms underlying cognition, emotion, and self-awareness. In this context, neuroheuristics emerges as a promising transdisciplinary paradigm-one that promotes iterative hypothesis refinement, methodological flexibility, and the integration of diverse knowledge systems. By embracing the epistemological pluralism embodied in the neuroheuristic approach, contemporary neuroscience is positioned to move beyond static models and disciplinary silos. The convergence of emerging computational tools, molecular insights, and system-level models promises to accelerate our understanding of brain function and dysfunction. As this paradigm continues to evolve, it brings us closer to comprehending the core processes that underlie human thought, emotion, and behavior. thought and behavior.
Author contributions
A.E.P.V. wrote and reviewed the main manuscript text.
Funding
Open access funding provided by University of Lausanne
Data availability
No datasets were generated or analysed during the current study.
Declarations
Conflict of interest
The authors declare no Conflict of interest.
Footnotes
Publisher's Note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
References
- Abbott LF (1999) Lapicque’s introduction of the integrate-and-fire model neuron (1907). Brain Res Bull 50(5–6):303–4. 10.1016/s0361-9230(99)00161-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Abe T, Asai Y, Dotare M, Hayano T, Perrig SH, Jaquerod ME, Lintas A, Villa AEP (2021) Causal interactions among cortical regions during sleep based on fNIRS recordings. In: Lintas A, Enrico P, Pan X, Wang R, Villa AEP (eds) Advances in cognitive neurodynamics (VII). Springer, Singapore, pp 273–274 [Google Scholar]
- Abe T, Asai Y, Lintas A, Villa AEP (2024) Detection of quadratic phase coupling by cross-bicoherence and spectral granger causality in bifrequencies interactions. Sci Rep 14(1):8521. 10.1038/s41598-024-59004-8 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Abeles M (1991) Corticonics: neural circuits of the cerebral cortex, 1st edn. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, p 14 [Google Scholar]
- Abeles M (2004) Neuroscience. Time is precious. Science 304(5670):523–4. 10.1126/science.1097725 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Abeles M (2014) Revealing instances of coordination among multiple cortical areas. Biol Cybern 108(5):665–75. 10.1007/s00422-013-0574-2 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Agnati LF, Genedani S, Leo G, Rivera A, Guidolin D, Fuxe K (2007) One century of progress in neuroscience founded on golgi and cajal’s outstanding experimental and theoretical contributions. Brain Res Rev 55(1):167–189. 10.1016/j.brainresrev.2007.03.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Aksenova TI, Chibirova OK, Dryga OA, Tetko IV, Benabid AL, Villa AEP (2003) An unsupervised automatic method for sorting neuronal spike waveforms in awake and freely moving animals. Methods 30(2):178–187. 10.1016/S1046-2023(03)00079-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Aksenova TI, Volkovych VV, Villa AEP (2007) Detection of spectral instability in EEG recordings during the preictal period. J Neural Eng 4(3):173–178. 10.1088/1741-2560/4/3/001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Altimus CM (2017) Neuroscience has the power to change the criminal justice system. eNeuro. 10.1523/ENEURO.0362-16.2016 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Amit DJ (1992) Modeling brain function: the world of attractor neural networks, 2nd edn. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, p 524 [Google Scholar]
- Andersen JK (2004) Oxidative stress in neurodegeneration: cause or consequence? Nat Med 10(Suppl):S18-25. 10.1038/nrn1434 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Asai Y, Villa AEP (2008) Reconstruction of underlying nonlinear deterministic dynamics embedded in noisy spike trains. J Biol Phys 34:325–340. 10.1007/s10867-008-9093-0 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Asai Y, Villa AEP (2012) Integration and transmission of distributed deterministic neural activity in feed-forward networks. Brain Res 1434:17–33. 10.1016/j.brainres.2011.10.012 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Asai Y, Aksenova TI, Villa AEP (2005) Unsupervised recognition of neuronal discharge waveforms for on-line real-time operation. Lect Notes Comput Sci 3704:29–38. 10.1007/11565123_3 [Google Scholar]
- Asai Y, Guha A, Villa AEP (2008) Deterministic neural dynamics transmitted through neural networks. Neural Netw 21(6):799–809. 10.1016/j.neunet.2008.06.014 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Aston-Jones G, Cohen JD (2005) An integrative theory of locus coeruleus-norepinephrine function: adaptive gain and optimal performance. Annu Rev Neurosci 28(1):403–50. 10.1146/annurev.neuro.28.061604.135709 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Atkinson RC, Shiffrin RM (1968) Human memory: a proposed system and its control processes. In: Spence KW, Spence JT (eds) Psychology of learning and motivation. Academic Press, New York, pp 89–195 [Google Scholar]
- Beattie L, Kyle SD, Espie CA, Biello SM (2015) Social interactions, emotion and sleep: a systematic review and research agenda. Sleep Med Rev 24:83–100. 10.1016/j.smrv.2014.12.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bechara A, Damasio AR (2005) The somatic marker hypothesis: a neural theory of economic decision. Games Econ Behav 52(2):336–372. 10.1016/j.geb.2004.06.010 [Google Scholar]
- Bechara A, Damasio AR, Damasio H, Anderson SW (1994) Insensitivity to future consequences following damage to human prefrontal cortex. Cognition 50(1–3):7–15. 10.1016/0010-0277(94)90018-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bergson H (1896) Matière et mémoire (Matter and Memory). Félix Alcan, Paris, France [Google Scholar]
- Bergson H (1907) L’Évolution créatrice (Creative Evolution). Félix Alcan, Paris, France [Google Scholar]
- Berridge KC (2004) Motivation concepts in behavioral neuroscience. Physiol Behav 81(2):179–209. 10.1016/j.physbeh.2004.02.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Boero F (2015) From Darwin’s origin of species toward a theory of natural history. F1000Prime Rep 7:49. 10.12703/P7-49 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bonvicini C, Faraone SV, Scassellati C (2016) Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder in adults: A systematic review and meta-analysis of genetic, pharmacogenetic and biochemical studies. Mol Psychiatry 21(7):872–884. 10.1038/mp.2016.74 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Boureau YL, Dayan P (2010) Opponency revisited: competition and cooperation between dopamine and serotonin. Neuropsychopharmacology 36(1):74–97. 10.1038/npp.2010.151 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bouret S, Richmond BJ (2010) Ventromedial and orbital prefrontal neurons differentially encode internally and externally driven motivational values in monkeys. J Neurosci 30(25):8591–601. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0049-10.2010 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bouret S, Ravel S, Richmond BJ (2012) Complementary neural correlates of motivation in dopaminergic and noradrenergic neurons of monkeys. Front Behav Neurosci 6:40. 10.3389/fnbeh.2012.00040 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Braitenberg V (1977) On the texture of brains: an introduction to neuroanatomy for the cybernetically minded. Heidelberg Science Library, Springer, New York, p 127 [Google Scholar]
- Brooks RA (1991) Intelligence without representation. Artif Intell 47(1–3):139–159. 10.1016/0004-3702(91)90053-m [Google Scholar]
- Brown MS (1985) The nobel prize in physiology or medicine 1985
- Brynjolfsson E, McAfee A (2014) The second machine age: work, progress, and prosperity in a time of brilliant technologies. W.W. Norton & Company, New York [Google Scholar]
- Cabessa J, Villa AEP (2014) An attractor-based complexity measurement for Boolean recurrent neural networks. PLoS ONE 9(4):e94204. 10.1371/journal.pone.0094204 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cabessa J, Villa AEP (2018) Attractor dynamics of a Boolean model of a brain circuit controlled by multiple parameters. Chaos 28(10):106318. 10.1063/1.5042312 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carretta D, Hervé-Minvielle A, Bajo VM, Villa AEP, Rouiller EM (1999) c-Fos expression in the auditory pathways related to the significance of acoustic signals in rats performing a sensory-motor task. Brain Res 841(1–2):170–183. 10.1016/s0006-8993(99)01840-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Celesia GG, Villa AEP, Brigell M, Rubboli G, Bolcioni G, Fiori MG (1993) An electrophysiological study of visual processing in Alzheimer’s disease. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 87(3):97–104. 10.1016/0013-4694(93)90116-d [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chibirova OK, Aksenova TI, Benabid AL, Chabardes S, Larouche S, Rouat J, Villa AE (2005) Unsupervised spike sorting of extracellular electrophysiological recording in subthalamic nucleus of parkinsonian patients. Biosystems 79(1–3):159–171. 10.1016/j.biosystems.2004.09.028 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Clark JE, Watson S, Friston KJ (2018) What is mood? A computational perspective. Psychol Med 48(14):2277–2284. 10.1017/s0033291718000430 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Coles ME, Schubert JR, Nota JA (2015) Sleep, circadian rhythms, and anxious traits. Curr Psychiatry Rep 17(9):73. 10.1007/s11920-015-0613-x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cools R, Robbins TW (2004) Chemistry of the adaptive mind. Philos Trans R Soc London Ser A Math Phys Eng Sci 362(1825):2871–2888. 10.1098/rsta.2004.1468 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cooper EC, Jan LY (1999) Ion channel genes and human neurological disease: recent progress, prospects, and challenges. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 96(9):4759–66. 10.1073/pnas.96.9.4759 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cortese S, Sabé M, Chen C, Perroud N, Solmi M (2022) Half a century of research on attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a scientometric study. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 140:104769. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2022.104769 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Custodio RJP, Hengstler JG, Cheong JH, Kim HJ, Wascher E, Getzmann S (2023) Adult ADHD: it is old and new at the same time - what is it? Rev Neurosci 35(2):225–241. 10.1515/revneuro-2023-0071 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Davis J, Kirk IJ, Kozma R (2025) Two methodologies for brain signal analysis derived from Freeman Neurodynamics. Front Syst Neurosci 19:1570231. 10.3389/fnsys.2025.1570231 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dayan P (2012) Twenty-five lessons from computational neuromodulation. Neuron 76(1):240–256. 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.09.027 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- de Berker AO, Rutledge RB, Mathys C, Marshall L, Cross GF, Dolan RJ, Bestmann S (2016) Computations of uncertainty mediate acute stress responses in humans. Nat Commun 7(1):10996. 10.1038/ncomms10996 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- De Palma A, Pareti G (2011) Bernstein’s long path to membrane theory: radical change and conservation in nineteenth-century German electrophysiology. J Hist Neurosci 20(4):306–337. 10.1080/0964704x.2010.532024 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dekkers MPJ, Nikoletopoulou V, Barde YA (2013) Cell biology in neuroscience: death of developing neurons: new insights and implications for connectivity. J Cell Biol 203(3):385–93. 10.1083/jcb.201306136 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Del Prete V, Martignon L, Villa AEP (2004) Detection of syntonies between multiple spike trains using a coarse-grain binarization of spike count distributions. Network 15(1):13–28. 10.1088/0954-898X/15/002 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- DeSilva JM, Traniello JFA, Claxton AG, Fannin LD (2021) When and why did human brains decrease in size? A new change-point analysis and insights from brain evolution in ants. Front Ecol Evol 9:742639. 10.3389/fevo.2021.742639 [Google Scholar]
- Dotare M, Bader M, Mesrobian SK, Asai Y, Villa AEP, Lintas A (2020) Attention networks in ADHD adults after working memory training with a dual n-back task. Brain Sci 10(10):715. 10.3390/brainsci10100715 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dotare M, Asai Y, Mesrobian SK, Bader M, Villa AEP, Lintas A (2021) Training parameters with dual n-back task affect the outcome of the attentional network task in ADHD patients. In: Lintas A, Enrico P, Pan X, Wang R, Villa AEP (eds) Advances in cognitive neurodynamics (VII). Springer, Singapore, pp 281–282 [Google Scholar]
- Doya K (2002) Metalearning and neuromodulation. Neural Netw 15(4–6):495–506. 10.1016/s0893-6080(02)00044-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Falk D (1991) 3.5 Million years of hominid brain evolution. Semin Neurosci 3(5):409–416. 10.1016/1044-5765(91)90031-I [Google Scholar]
- Farina A, Villa AEP (2023) On the semantics of ecoacoustic codes. Biosystems 232:105002. 10.1016/j.biosystems.2023.105002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Farré-Castany MA, Schwaller B, Gregory P, Barski J, Mariethoz C, Eriksson JL, Tetko IV, Wolfer D, Celio MR, Schmutz I, Albrecht U, Villa AEP (2007) Differences in locomotor behavior revealed in mice deficient for the calcium-binding proteins parvalbumin, calbindin D-28k or both. Behav Brain Res 178(2):250–261. 10.1016/j.bbr.2007.01.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fell E (2012) Duration, temporality, self. Prospects for the future of Bergsonism. Peter Lang, Lausanne, p 237 [Google Scholar]
- France A (1921) The Nobel prize in literature 1921
- Freeman WJ (2000) Neurodynamics: an exploration in mesoscopic brain dynamics. Perspectives in neural computing, 1st edn. Springer, London [Google Scholar]
- Frisch U (1995) Turbulence: the legacy of A. N. Kolmogorov. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, p 296 [Google Scholar]
- Friston K, Schwartenbeck P, FitzGerald T, Moutoussis M, Behrens T, Dolan RJ (2013) The anatomy of choice: active inference and agency. Front Hum Neurosci 7:e00598. 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00598 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Frith U, Happé F (1994) Autism: beyond ’theory of mind". Cognition 50(1–3):115–132 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fuster JM (2015) The prefrontal cortex, 5th edn. Academic Press, p 460 [Google Scholar]
- Gais S, Born J (2004) Declarative memory consolidation: mechanisms acting during human sleep. Learn Mem 11(6):679–85. 10.1101/lm.80504 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Galvani L (1797) Memorie sulla elettricità animale. Clemente Maria Sassi, Bologna
- Gold JI, Shadlen MN (2007) The neural basis of decision making. Annu Rev Neurosci 30:535–74. 10.1146/annurev.neuro.29.051605.113038 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goldstein JL (1985) The Nobel prize in physiology or medicine 1985
- Grossberg S (1987) Competitive learning: from interactive activation to adaptive resonance. Cogn Sci 11(1):23–63. 10.1111/j.1551-6708.1987.tb00862.x [Google Scholar]
- Grossberg S (1999) The link between brain learning, attention, and consciousness. Conscious Cogn 8(1):1–44. 10.1006/ccog.1998.0372 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grossberg S (2013) Adaptive resonance theory: how a brain learns to consciously attend, learn, and recognize a changing world. Neural Netw 37:1–47. 10.1016/j.neunet.2012.09.017 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grossberg S, Mingolla E (1985) Neural dynamics of perceptual grouping: textures, boundaries, and emergent segmentations. Percept Psychophys 38(2):141–71. 10.3758/bf03198851 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Guarnieri M (2014) Electricity in the age of enlightenment [Historical]. IEEE Ind Electron Mag 8(3):60–63. 10.1109/MIE.2014.2335431 [Google Scholar]
- Guérin MC (2015) Intime conviction et doute. Au-delà d’une antinomie apparente... Ce que révèle leur étude comparée. Annales Médico-psychologiques 173(7): 610–614
- Guy TV, Kárný M, Lintas A, Villa AEP (2016) Theoretical models of decision-making in the ultimatum game: fairness vs. reason. In: Wang R, Pan X (eds) Advances in cognitive neurodynamics (V), Chapter 26. Springer, Singapore, pp 185–191 [Google Scholar]
- Haggard P (2008) Human volition: towards a neuroscience of will. Nat Rev Neurosci 9(12):934–46. 10.1038/nrn2497 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hajós M, Gartside SE, Villa AEP, Sharp T (1995) Evidence for a repetitive (burst) firing pattern in a sub-population of 5-hydroxytryptamine neurons in the dorsal and median raphe nuclei of the rat. Neuroscience 69(1):189–197 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hajós M, Hoffmann WE, Tetko IV, Hyland B, Sharp T, Villa AEP (2001) Different tonic regulation of neuronal activity in the rat dorsal raphe and medial prefrontal cortex via 5-HT(1A) receptors. Neurosci Lett 304(3):129–132. 10.1016/s0304-3940(01)01751-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hampel H, Toschi N, Babiloni C, Baldacci F, Black KL, Bokde AL, Bun RS, Cacciola F, Cavedo E, Chiesa PA, Colliot O, Coman CM, Dubois B, Duggento A, Durrleman S, Ferretti MT, George N, Genthon R, Habert MO, Herholz K, Koronyo Y, Koronyo-Hamaoui M, Lamari F, Langevin T, Lehéricy S, Lorenceau J, Neri C, Nisticò R, Nyasse-Messene F, Ritchie C, Rossi S, Santarnecchi E, Sporns O, Verdooner SR, Vergallo A, Villain N, Younesi E, Garaci F, Lista S (2018) Revolution of Alzheimer precision neurology. Passageway of systems biology and neurophysiology. J Alzheimers Dis 64(1):47–105. 10.3233/jad-179932 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hill SL, Villa AEP (1997) Dynamic transitions in global network activity influenced by the balance of excitation and inhibtion. Network 8:165–184. 10.1088/0954-898X_8_2_004 [Google Scholar]
- Horne JA (1978) A review of the biological effects of total sleep deprivation in man. Biol Psychol 7(1–2):55–102. 10.1016/0301-0511(78)90042-x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Iglesias J, Villa AEP (2010) Recurrent spatiotemporal firing patterns in large spiking neural networks with ontogenetic and epigenetic processes. J Physiol Paris 104:137–146. 10.1016/j.jphysparis.2009.11.016 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Iglesias J, Eriksson J, Grize F, Tomassini M, Villa AE (2005) Dynamics of pruning in simulated large-scale spiking neural networks. Biosystems 79(1):11–20. 10.1016/j.biosystems.2004.09.016 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Irish M, Piguet O, Hodges JR (2012) Self-projection and the default network in frontotemporal dementia. Nat Rev Neurol 8(3):152–161. 10.1038/nrneurol.2012.11 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Iturria-Medina Y, Sotero RC, Toussaint PJ, Mateos-Pérez JM, Evans AC (2016) The Alzheimer’s disease neuroimaging initiative. Early role of vascular dysregulation on late-onset Alzheimer’s disease based on multifactorial data-driven analysis. Nat Commun 7(1):11934. 10.1038/ncomms11934 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jack CR, Bennett DA, Blennow K, Carrillo MC, Dunn B, Haeberlein SB, Holtzman DM, Jagust W, Jessen F, Karlawish J, Liu E, Molinuevo JL, Montine T, Phelps C, Rankin KP, Rowe CC, Scheltens P, Siemers E, Snyder HM, Sperling R, Elliott C, Masliah E, Ryan L, Silverberg N (2018) NIA-AA research framework: toward a biological definition of Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimers Dementia 14(4):535–562. 10.1016/j.jalz.2018.02.018 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jaquerod ME, Mesrobian SK, Villa AEP, Bader M, Lintas A (2020) Early attentional modulation by working memory training in young adult ADHD patients during a risky decision-making task. Brain Sci 10(1):38. 10.3390/brainsci10010038 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jaquerod ME, Knight RS, Lintas A, Villa AEP (2024) A dual role for the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) in auditory deviance detection. Brain Sci 14(10):994. 10.3390/brainsci14100994 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jordanous A (2020) Intelligence without representation: a historical perspective. Systems 8(3):31. 10.3390/systems8030031 [Google Scholar]
- Kearns M, Vazirani UV (1994) An introduction to computational learning theory. The MIT Press, Cambridge, p 222 [Google Scholar]
- Keith A (1927) The brain of Anatole France. Br Med J 2(3491):1048–1049 [Google Scholar]
- Keynes M, Cox TM (2008) William Bateson, the rediscoverer of Mendel. J R Soc Med 101(3):104. 10.1258/jrsm.2008.081011 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Klinzing JG, Niethard N, Born J (2019) Mechanisms of systems memory consolidation during sleep. Nat Neurosci 22(10):1598–1610. 10.1038/s41593-019-0467-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Koch C (2016) Does brain size matter? Sc Am Mind 27(1):22–25 [Google Scholar]
- Korf RE (1985) Depth-first iterative-deepening. Artif Intell 27(1):97–109. 10.1016/0004-3702(85)90084-0 [Google Scholar]
- Kötter R, Balsiger PW (1999) Interdisciplinarity and transdisciplinarity: a constant challenge to the sciences. Issues Integrat Stud 17:87–120 [Google Scholar]
- Kraft CJ, Giordano J (2017) Integrating brain science and law: neuroscientific evidence and legal perspectives on protecting individual liberties. Front Neurosci 11:621. 10.3389/fnins.2017.00621 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lietzau WK (1999) Checks and balances and elements of proof: structural pillars for the international criminal court. Cornell Int Law J 32(3):477–488 [Google Scholar]
- Lintas A, Silkis IG, Albéri L, Villa AEP (2012) Dopamine deficiency increases synchronized activity in the rat subthalamic nucleus. Brain Res 1434:142–151. 10.1016/j.brainres.2011.09.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lintas A, Vysyaraju SC, Jaquerod M, Villa AEP (2017) Event related potentials reveal fairness in willingness-to-share. Lect Notes Comput Sci 10613:191–198. 10.1007/978-3-319-68600-4_23 [Google Scholar]
- Lintas A, Sánchez-Campusano R, Villa AEP, Gruart A, Delgado-García JM (2021) Operant conditioning deficits and modified local field potential activities in parvalbumin-deficient mice. Sci Rep 11(1):2979. 10.1038/s41598-021-82519-3 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lintas A, Mesrobian SK, Bader M, Villa AEP (2021) ERPs in controls and ADHD patients during dual n-back task. In: A. Lintas, P. Enrico, X. Pan, R. Wang, and A.E.P. Villa EDs Advances in Cognitive Neurodynamics (VII) Chapter 20, Springer, Singapore, pp 189–203
- Loosen AM, Kato A, Gu X (2024) Revisiting the role of computational neuroimaging in the era of integrative neuroscience. Neuropsychopharmacology 50(1):103–113. 10.1038/s41386-024-01946-8 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Malagarriga D, Villa AEP, Garcia-Ojalvo J, Pons AJ (2017) Consistency of heterogeneous synchronization patterns in complex weighted networks. Chaos 27(3):031102. 10.1063/1.4977972 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Malagarriga D, Pons AJ, Villa AEP (2019) Complex temporal patterns processing by a neural mass model of a cortical column. Cogn Neurodyn 13(4):379–392. 10.1007/s11571-019-09531-2 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Marder E (2012) Neuromodulation of neuronal circuits: back to the future. Neuron 76(1):1–11. 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.09.010 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Masulli P, Villa A (2015) Algebro-topological invariants in network theory. Int J Complex Syst Sci 5(1):13–17 [Google Scholar]
- Masulli P, Masulli F, Rovetta S, Lintas A, Villa AEP (2020) Fuzzy clustering for exploratory analysis of EEG event-related potentials. IEEE Trans Fuzzy Syst 28(1):28–38. 10.1109/TFUZZ.2019.2910499 [Google Scholar]
- Mesrobian SK, Bader M, Götte L, Villa AEP, Lintas A (2015) Imperfect decision making and risk taking are affected by personality. In: Guy, T.V., M. Kárný, and D.H. Wolpert eds. Decision making: uncertainty, imperfection, deliberation and scalability, Volume 538 of Studies in Computation Intelligence, Chapter 6, Springer, Cham, pp 145–184. 10.1007/978-3-319-15144-1_6
- Mesrobian SK, Villa AEP, Bader M, Götte L, Lintas A (2018) Event-related potentials during a gambling task in young adults with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Front Hum Neurosci 12:79. 10.3389/fnhum.2018.00079 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Metzinger T (2024) The elephant and the blind: the experience of pure consciousness: philosophy, science, and 500+ experiential reports. MIT Press, Boston, p 616 [Google Scholar]
- Michel V, Gramfort A, Varoquaux G, Eger E, Keribin C, Thirion B (2012) A supervised clustering approach for fMRI-based inference of brain states. Pattern Recogn 45(6):2041–2049. 10.1016/j.patcog.2011.04.006 [Google Scholar]
- Moore GE (2000) Cramming more components onto integrated circuits. In: Hill MD, Jouppi NP, Sohi GS (eds) Readings in computer architecture. Morgan Kaufmann Publishers Inc., San Francisco, pp 56–59 [Google Scholar]
- Palm G (1982) Neural assemblies: an alternative approach to artificial intelligence, Volume 7 of Studies of Brain Function. Springer-Verlag, Berlin, p 244
- Papadimitriou CH (1994) Computational complexity class notes computational complexity. Addison-Wesley Publishing, Reading [Google Scholar]
- Perrig S, Dutoit P, Espa-Cervena K, Shaposhnyk V, Pelletier L, Berger F, Villa AEP (2009) Changes in quadratic phase coupling of EEG signals during wake and sleep in two chronic insomnia patients, before and after cognitive behavioral therapy. Front Artif Intell Appl 204:217–228. 10.3233/978-1-60750-072-8-217 [Google Scholar]
- Perrig S, Iglesias J, Shaposhnyk V, Chibirova O, Dutoit P, Cabessa J, Espa-Cervena K, Pelletier L, Berger F, Villa AEP (2010) Functional interactions in hierarchically organized neural networks studied with spatiotemporal firing patterns and phase-coupling frequencies. Chin J Physiol 53(6):382–395. 10.4077/CJP.2010.AMM039 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Perrig S, Lintas A, Jaquerod M, Villa A (2024) Sleep stages dependency of distinct distribution of oscillatory activity across frontal and central cortical areas studied by frequency-domain functional near-infrared spectroscopy. Sleep Med 115:29. 10.1016/j.sleep.2023.11.117 [Google Scholar]
- Picard-Deland C, Nielsen T, Carr M (2021) Dreaming of the sleep lab. PLoS ONE 16(10):e0257738. 10.1371/journal.pone.0257738 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Poldrack RA, Baker CI, Durnez J, Gorgolewski KJ, Matthews PM, Munafò MR, Nichols TE, Poline JB, Vul E, Yarkoni T (2017) Scanning the horizon: towards transparent and reproducible neuroimaging research. Nat Rev Neurosci 18(2):115–126. 10.1038/nrn.2016.167 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pope SB (2000) Turbulent flows turbulent flows. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, p 771 [Google Scholar]
- Prigogine IR (1977) The Nobel prize in chemistry 1977
- Prigogine I, Stengers I (1979) La Nouvelle Alliance: : Métamorphose de la science. Gallimard, Paris, France, p 302
- Rama S, Zbili M, Debanne D (2018) Signal propagation along the axon. Curr Opin Neurobiol 51:37–44. 10.1016/j.conb.2018.02.017 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Richmond BJ, Liu Z, Shidara M (2003) Neuroscience. Predicting future rewards. Science 301(5630):179–80. 10.1126/science.1087383 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Riva M, Genescu I, Habermacher C, Orduz D, Ledonne F, Rijli FM, López-Bendito G, Coppola E, Garel S, Angulo MC, Pierani A (2019) Activity-dependent death of transient Cajal-Retzius neurons is required for functional cortical wiring. Elife 8:e50503. 10.7554/eLife.50503 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rivest RL, Shamir A, Adleman L (1978) A method for obtaining digital signatures and public-key cryptosystems. Commun ACM 21(2):110–126. 10.1145/359340.359342 [Google Scholar]
- Rolando L (1809) Saggio sopra la vera struttura del cervello dell’uomo e degli animali e sopra le funzioni del sistema nervoso. Stampeia da S.S.R.M. Privilegiata, Sassari [Google Scholar]
- Rutledge RB, Skandali N, Dayan P, Dolan RJ (2014) A computational and neural model of momentary subjective well-being. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 111(33):12252–12257. 10.1073/pnas.1407535111 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ryle G (2009) The concept of mind (60th, anniversary. Routledge, London, p 314 [Google Scholar]
- Shaposhnyk V, Villa AEP (2012) Reciprocal projections in hierarchically organized evolvable neural circuits affect EEG-like signals. Brain Res 1434:266–276. 10.1016/j.brainres.2011.08.018 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shine JM (2019) Neuromodulatory influences on integration and segregation in the brain. Trends Cogn Sci 23(7):572–583. 10.1016/j.tics.2019.03.007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shukla A, Kaur P, Narayanan DL, do Rosario MC, Kadavigere R, Girisha KM (2021) Genetic disorders with central nervous system white matter abnormalities: an update. Clin Genet 99(1):119–132. 10.1111/cge.13863 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sjöstedt E, Zhong W, Fagerberg L, Karlsson M, Mitsios N, Adori C, Oksvold P, Edfors F, Limiszewska A, Hikmet F, Huang J, Du Y, Lin L, Dong Z, Yang L, Liu X, Jiang H, Xu X, Wang J, Yang H, Bolund L, Mardinoglu A, Zhang C, von Feilitzen K, Lindskog C, Pontén F, Luo Y, Hökfelt T, Uhlén M, Mulder J (2020) An atlas of the protein-coding genes in the human, pig, and mouse brain. Science 367(6482):eaay5947. 10.1126/science.aay5947 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Skarda CA, Freeman WJ (1987) How brains make chaos in order to make sense of the world. Behav Brain Sci 10(2):161–195 [Google Scholar]
- Spiga S, Lintas A, Diana M (2011) Altered mesolimbic dopamine system in THC dependence. Curr Neuropharmacol 9(1):200–4. 10.2174/157015911795017083 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Squire LR (2004) Memory systems of the brain: a brief history and current perspective. Neurobiol Learn Mem 82(3):171–7. 10.1016/j.nlm.2004.06.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stanovich KE (2009) What intelligence tests miss: the psychology of rational thought. Yale University Press, New Haven [Google Scholar]
- Stein RB, Gossen ER, Jones KE (2005) Neuronal variability: noise or part of the signal? Nat Rev Neurosci 6(5):389–97. 10.1038/nrn1668 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Storozhuk VM, Khorevin VI, Rozumna NM, Villa AEP, Tetko IV (2002) Dopamine modulation of activity of cat sensorimotor cortex neurons during conditioned reflexes. Neurosci Lett 330(2):171–174. 10.1016/S0304-3940(02)00775-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Storozhuk VM, Khorevin VI, Rozumna NM, Villa AEP, Tetko IV (2004) Dopamine modulation of glutamate metabotropic receptors in conditioned reaction of sensory motor cortex neurons of the cat. Neurosci Lett 356(2):127–130. 10.1016/j.neulet.2003.11.039 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Synytsky V, Gruzdev G, Trofimchouk G, Voronovskaya V, Tetko I, Villa A (2002) Neurophysiological and psychophysiological effects of sulpiride in the norm and in patients with opiate addiction. Neurophysiology 34(4):313–320. 10.1023/A:1021291815652 [Google Scholar]
- Tattersall I (2023) Endocranial volumes and human evolution. F1000Res 12:565. 10.12688/f1000research.131636.1 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Taylor JG, Villa AEP (2001) The ’conscious I’: a neuroheuristic approach to the mind, In: Baltimore, D., R. Dulbecco, F. Jacob, R. Levi Montalcini eds. Frontiers of Life, Volume III. Academic Press, pp 349–368
- Tetko IV, Villa AEP (1997) Fast combinatorial methods to estimate the probability of complex temporal patterns of spikes. Biol Cybern 76(5):397–408 [Google Scholar]
- Tetko IV, Villa AEP (2001a) A pattern grouping algorithm for analysis of spatiotemporal patterns in neuronal spike trains. 1. Detection of repeated patterns. J Neurosci Methods 105(1):1–14. 10.1016/s0165-0270(00)00336-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tetko IV, Villa AEP (2001b) A pattern grouping algorithm for analysis of spatiotemporal patterns in neuronal spike trains. 2. Application to simultaneous single unit recordings. J Neurosci Methods 105(1):15–24. 10.1016/s0165-0270(00)00337-x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tritsch NX, Ding JB, Sabatini BL (2012) Dopaminergic neurons inhibit striatal output through non-canonical release of GABA. Nature 490(7419):262–266. 10.1038/nature11466 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tsuda I (2001) Toward an interpretation of dynamic neural activity in terms of chaotic dynamical systems. Behav Brain Sci 24(5):793–810 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- van der Helm E, Gujar N, Walker MP (2010) Sleep deprivation impairs the accurate recognition of human emotions. Sleep 33(3):335–42 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vardi R, Timor R, Marom S, Abeles M, Kanter I (2013) Synchronization by elastic neuronal latencies. Phys Rev E: Stat, Nonlin, Soft Matter Phys 87(1):012724. 10.1103/PhysRevE.87.012724 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Villa AEP (1985) The laboratory of neuroheuristics
- Villa AEP (2000) Empirical Evidence about Temporal Structure in Multi-unit Recordings, In: R Miller eds. Time and the brain, Volume 3 of Conceptual advances in brain research, Chapter 1, 1–61. Harwood Academic Publishers, London (CRC Press) 10.4324/9780203304570_chapter_1
- Villa AEP (2008) Neural coding in the neuroheuristic perspective, In: M Barbieri ed. The codes of life: the rules of macroevolution., Volume Biosemiotics 1 of Biosemiotics, Chapter 16. Springer, Berlin, pp 357–377
- Villa AEP, Lorenzana VMB (1997) Ketamine modulation of the temporal pattern of discharges and spike train interactions in the rat substantia Nigra pars reticulata. Brain Res Bull 43(6):525–535 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Villa AEP, Tetko IV (1997) Efficient partition of learning data sets for neural network training. Neural Netw 10(8):1361–1374 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Villa AEP, Tetko IV (2010) Cross-frequency coupling in mesiotemporal EEG recordings of epileptic patients. J Physiol Paris 104:197–202. 10.1016/j.jphysparis.2009.11.024 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Villa AEP, Bajo Lorenzana VM, Vantini G (1996) Nerve growth factor modulates information processing in the auditory thalamus. Brain Res Bull 39(3):139–147. 10.1016/0361-9230(95)02085-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Villa AEP, Tetko IV, Celletti A, Riehle A (1998) Chaotic dynamics in the primate motor cortex depend on motor preparation in a reaction-time task. Cah Psychol Cogn 17:763–780 [Google Scholar]
- Villa AEP, Eriksson J, Eriksson C, Haeberli C, Hyland B, Najem A (1999a) Stimulus congruence affects perceptual processes in a novel Go/Nogo conflict paradigm in rats. Behav Proc 48:69–88. 10.1016/S0376-6357(99)00070-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Villa AEP, Tetko IV, Hyland B, Najem A (1999b) Spatiotemporal activity patterns of rat cortical neurons predict responses in a conditioned task. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 96(3):1106–1111. 10.1073/pnas.96.3.1106 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Villa AEP, Tetko IV, Dutoit P, Vantini G (2000) Non-linear cortico-cortical interactions modulated by cholinergic afferences from the rat basal forebrain. Biosystems 58(1–3):219–228. 10.1016/s0303-2647(00)00126-x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Villa AEP, Asai Y, Segundo JP (2007) Influence of the temporal distribution of electric pulses on transcallosal single unit responses. Biosystems 89:143–153. 10.1016/j.biosystems.2006.04.013 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- von Haller A (1755) A dissertation on the sensible and irritable parts of animals. J. Nourse at the Lamb opposite Katherine-street in the Strand, London, p 79
- Walker MP (2009) The role of sleep in cognition and emotion. Ann N Y Acad Sci 1156:168–97. 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2009.04416.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wang R, Zhang Z (2007) Energy coding in biological neural networks. Cogn Neurodyn 1(3):203–12. 10.1007/s11571-007-9015-z [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wang R, Tsuda I, Zhang Z (2015) A new work mechanism on neuronal activity. Int J Neural Syst 25(3):1450037. 10.1142/S0129065714500373 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wang D, Liu S, Warrell J, Won H, Shi X, Navarro FCP, Clarke D, Gu M, Emani P, Yang YT, Xu M, Gandal MJ, Lou S, Zhang J, Park JJ, Yan C, Rhie SK, Manakongtreecheep K, Zhou H, Nathan A, Peters M, Mattei E, Fitzgerald D, Brunetti T, Moore J, Jiang Y, Girdhar K, Hoffman GE, Kalayci S, Gümüş ZH, Crawford GE, PsychENCODE Consortium, Roussos P, Akbarian S, Jaffe AE, White KP, Weng Z, Sestan N, Geschwind DH, Knowles JA, Gerstein MB (2018) Comprehensive functional genomic resource and integrative model for the human brain. Science 362(6420):1aat8464. 10.1126/science.aat8464 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- Wood N (1992) Tabula rasa, social environmentalism, and the “English Paradigm’’. J Hist Ideas 53(4):647–668. 10.2307/2709942 [Google Scholar]
- Wray KB (2021) Interpreting Kuhn: critical essays. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge
- Wright E (1992) Discussion: Gestalt Switching: Hanson, Aronson, and Harré. Philos Sci 59(3):480–486. 10.1086/289685 [Google Scholar]
- Zhu J (2004) Locating volition. Conscious Cogn 13(2):302–22. 10.1016/j.concog.2003.09.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zhu F, Wang R, Pan X, Zhu Z (2019) Energy expenditure computation of a single bursting neuron. Cogn Neurodyn 13(1):75–87. 10.1007/s11571-018-9503-3 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Data Availability Statement
No datasets were generated or analysed during the current study.