Skip to main content
Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior logoLink to Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior
. 1997 Jan;67(1):109–129. doi: 10.1901/jeab.1997.67-109

Key-peck probability and topography in a concurrent variable-interval variable-interval schedule with food and water reinforcers.

B O Ploog 1, H P Zeigler 1
PMCID: PMC1284584  PMID: 9037783

Abstract

The relation between variables that modulate the probability and the topography of key pecks was examined using a concurrent variable-interval variable-interval schedule with food and water reinforcers. Measures of response probability (response rates, time allocation) and topography (peck duration, gape amplitude) were obtained in 5 water- and food-deprived pigeons. Key color signaled reinforcer type. During baseline, response rates and time allocations were greater to the food key than to the water key, and food-key pecks had larger gapes and shorter durations. Relative probability measures (for the food key) were increased by prewatering and decreased by prefeeding. Deprivation effects upon topography measures were apparent only when food- and water-key pecks were analyzed separately. Food-key gape amplitudes increased with prewatering and decreased with prefeeding. The clearest effect occurred with prewatering. There were no consistent effects upon water-key gapes. The key color-reinforcer relation was reversed for 3 pigeons to determine how response topography was modulated during the transition from food- to water-key pecks. Reacquisition was faster for the probability than for the topography measures. Analysis of gape-amplitude distributions during reversal indicated that response-form modulation proceeded through the generation of intermediate gape sizes.

Full Text

The Full Text of this article is available as a PDF (299.9 KB).

Selected References

These references are in PubMed. This may not be the complete list of references from this article.

  1. Allan R. W., Zeigler H. P. Autoshaping the pigeon's gape response: acquisition and topography as a function of reinforcer type and magnitude. J Exp Anal Behav. 1994 Sep;62(2):201–223. doi: 10.1901/jeab.1994.62-201. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Bout R., Zeigler H. P. Drinking behavior and jaw muscle (EMG) activity in the pigeon (Columba livia). J Comp Physiol A. 1994 Apr;174(4):443–450. doi: 10.1007/BF00191710. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Bout R., Zeigler H. P. Jaw muscle (EMG) activity and amplitude scaling of jaw movements during eating in pigeon (Columba livia) J Comp Physiol A. 1994 Apr;174(4):433–442. doi: 10.1007/BF00191709. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Brown P. L., Jenkins H. M. Auto-shaping of the pigeon's key-peck. J Exp Anal Behav. 1968 Jan;11(1):1–8. doi: 10.1901/jeab.1968.11-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Davey G. C., Cleland G. G. Topography of signal-centered behavior in the rat: Effects of deprivation state and reinforcer type. J Exp Anal Behav. 1982 Nov;38(3):291–304. doi: 10.1901/jeab.1982.38-291. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Deich J. D., Houben D., Allan R. W., Zeigler H. P. "On-line" monitoring of jaw movements in the pigeon. Physiol Behav. 1985 Aug;35(2):307–311. doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(85)90354-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. FLESHLER M., HOFFMAN H. S. A progression for generating variable-interval schedules. J Exp Anal Behav. 1962 Oct;5:529–530. doi: 10.1901/jeab.1962.5-529. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Harris-Warrick R. M., Marder E. Modulation of neural networks for behavior. Annu Rev Neurosci. 1991;14:39–57. doi: 10.1146/annurev.ne.14.030191.000351. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Herrnstein R. J. On the law of effect. J Exp Anal Behav. 1970 Mar;13(2):243–266. doi: 10.1901/jeab.1970.13-243. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Jenkins H. M., Moore B. R. The form of the auto-shaped response with food or water reinforcers. J Exp Anal Behav. 1973 Sep;20(2):163–181. doi: 10.1901/jeab.1973.20-163. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Lucas G. A., Vodraska A., Wasserman E. A. A direct fluid delivery system for the pigeon. J Exp Anal Behav. 1979 Mar;31(2):285–288. doi: 10.1901/jeab.1979.31-285. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Nevin J. A., Tota M. E., Torquato R. D., Shull R. L. Alternative reinforcement increases resistance to change: Pavlovian or operant contingencies? J Exp Anal Behav. 1990 May;53(3):359–379. doi: 10.1901/jeab.1990.53-359. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Ploog B. O., Zeigler H. P. Effects of food-pellet size on rate, latency, and topography of autoshaped key pecks and gapes in pigeons. J Exp Anal Behav. 1996 Jan;65(1):21–35. doi: 10.1901/jeab.1996.65-21. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Preston R. A., Fantino E. Conditioned reinforcement value and choice. J Exp Anal Behav. 1991 Mar;55(2):155–175. doi: 10.1901/jeab.1991.55-155. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Shurtleff D., Warren-Boulton F. R., Silberberg A. Income and choice between different goods. J Exp Anal Behav. 1987 Sep;48(2):263–275. doi: 10.1901/jeab.1987.48-263. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Stanhope K. J. The representation of the reinforcer and the force of the pigeon's keypeck in first- and second-order conditioning. Q J Exp Psychol B. 1992 Feb;44(2):137–158. doi: 10.1080/02724999208250607. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Willis R. D., Van Hartesveldt C., Loken K. K., Hall D. C. Motivation in concurrent variable-interval schedules with food and water reinforcers. J Exp Anal Behav. 1974 Sep;22(2):323–331. doi: 10.1901/jeab.1974.22-323. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Woodruff G., Williams D. R. The associative relation underlying autoshaping in the pigeon. J Exp Anal Behav. 1976 Jul;26(1):1–13. doi: 10.1901/jeab.1976.26-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior are provided here courtesy of Society for the Experimental Analysis of Behavior

RESOURCES