Skip to main content
Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior logoLink to Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior
. 1997 May;67(3):275–301. doi: 10.1901/jeab.1997.67-275

A transformation of respondently conditioned stimulus function in accordance with arbitrarily applicable relations.

B Roche 1, D Barnes 1
PMCID: PMC1284607  PMID: 9163935

Abstract

Adult male subjects saw a sexual film clip paired with a nonsense syllable (C1). Similarly, an emotionally neutral film clip was paired with a second nonsense syllable (C3). Responses to the nonsense syllables were recorded as skin resistance responses. Subjects were also trained in a series of related conditional discriminations, using the C1 and C3 stimuli, from which the following equivalence relations were predicted; A1-B1-C1, A2-B2-C2, and A3-B3-C3. Some subjects were given matching-to-sample (equivalence) tests after the conditional discrimination training (Experiment 1), whereas others were not (Experiment 2). Subjects were tested for a transformation of eliciting functions by presenting the A1 and A3 stimuli, which were related through equivalence to C1 and C3, respectively. Five of the 6 subjects who showed significantly greater conditioned responses to C1 than to C3, also showed significantly greater skin resistance responses to A1 than to A3. Two additional subjects demonstrated a transformation of an eliciting stimulus function in accordance with five-member equivalence relations (Experiment 3), and another 5 subjects demonstrated similar effects in accordance with the relations of sameness and opposition (Experiment 4).

Full Text

The Full Text of this article is available as a PDF (421.9 KB).

Selected References

These references are in PubMed. This may not be the complete list of references from this article.

  1. AX A. F. GOALS AND METHODS OF PSYCHOPHYSIOLOGY. Psychophysiology. 1964 Jul;1:8–25. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.1964.tb02616.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Barnes D., Keenan M. A transfer of functions through derived arbitrary and nonarbitrary stimulus relations. J Exp Anal Behav. 1993 Jan;59(1):61–81. doi: 10.1901/jeab.1993.59-61. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Barnes D. Naming as a technical term: Sacrificing behavior analysis at the altar of popularity? J Exp Anal Behav. 1996 Jan;65(1):264–267. doi: 10.1901/jeab.1996.65-264. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Bourget D., Bradford J. Fire fetishism, diagnostic and clinical implications: a review of two cases. Can J Psychiatry. 1987 Aug;32(6):459–462. doi: 10.1177/070674378703200612. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Dougher M. J., Augustson E., Markham M. R., Greenway D. E., Wulfert E. The transfer of respondent eliciting and extinction functions through stimulus equivalence classes. J Exp Anal Behav. 1994 Nov;62(3):331–351. doi: 10.1901/jeab.1994.62-331. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Dymond S., Barnes D. A transfer of self-discrimination response functions through equivalence relations. J Exp Anal Behav. 1994 Sep;62(2):251–267. doi: 10.1901/jeab.1994.62-251. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Dymond S., Barnes D. A transformation of self-discrimination response functions in accordance with the arbitrarily applicable relations of sameness, more than, and less than. J Exp Anal Behav. 1995 Sep;64(2):163–184. doi: 10.1901/jeab.1995.64-163. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Fields L., Adams B. J., Verhave T., Newman S. The effects of nodality on the formation of equivalence classes. J Exp Anal Behav. 1990 May;53(3):345–358. doi: 10.1901/jeab.1990.53-345. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Gatch M. B., Osborne J. G. Transfer of contextual stimulus function via equivalence class development. J Exp Anal Behav. 1989 May;51(3):369–378. doi: 10.1901/jeab.1989.51-369. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Hayes S. C., Kohlenberg B. S., Hayes L. J. The transfer of specific and general consequential functions through simple and conditional equivalence relations. J Exp Anal Behav. 1991 Jul;56(1):119–137. doi: 10.1901/jeab.1991.56-119. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Kantorowitz D. A. An experimental investigation of preorgasmic reconditioning and postorgasmic deconditioning. J Appl Behav Anal. 1978 Spring;11(1):23–34. doi: 10.1901/jaba.1978.11-23. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Korff J., Geer J. H. The relationship between sexual arousal experience and genital response. Psychophysiology. 1983 Mar;20(2):121–127. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.1983.tb03276.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. LACEY J. I., SMITH R. L. Conditioning and generalization of unconscious anxiety. Science. 1954 Dec 24;120(3130):1045–1052. doi: 10.1126/science.120.3130.1045. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. LACEY J. I., SMITH R. L., GREEN A. Use of conditioned autonomic responses in the study of anxiety. Psychosom Med. 1955 May-Jun;17(3):208–217. doi: 10.1097/00006842-195505000-00005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Levis D. J., Smith J. E. Getting individual differences in autonomic reactivity to work for instead of against you: determining the dominant "psychological" stress channel on the basis of a "biological" stress test. Psychophysiology. 1987 May;24(3):346–352. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.1987.tb00307.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Lykken D. T., Venables P. H. Direct measurement of skin conductance: a proposal for standardization. Psychophysiology. 1971 Sep;8(5):656–672. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.1971.tb00501.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. MONTAGU J. D. HABITUATION OF THE PSYCHO-GALVANIC REFLEX DURING SERIAL TESTS. J Psychosom Res. 1963 Dec;7:199–214. doi: 10.1016/0022-3999(63)90004-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. McConaghy N. Subjective and penile plethysmograph responses following aversion-relief and apomorphine aversion therapy for homosexual impulses. Br J Psychiatry. 1969 Jun;115(523):723–730. doi: 10.1192/bjp.115.523.723. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Rizley R. C., Rescorla R. A. Associations in second-order conditioning and sensory preconditioning. J Comp Physiol Psychol. 1972 Oct;81(1):1–11. doi: 10.1037/h0033333. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Spiess W. F., Geer J. H., O'Donohue W. T. Premature ejaculation: investigation of factors in ejaculatory latency. J Abnorm Psychol. 1984 May;93(2):242–245. doi: 10.1037//0021-843x.93.2.242. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Steele D., Hayes S. C. Stimulus equivalence and arbitrarily applicable relational responding. J Exp Anal Behav. 1991 Nov;56(3):519–555. doi: 10.1901/jeab.1991.56-519. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Wincze J. P., Hoon P., Hoon E. F. Sexual arousal in women: a comparison of cognitive and physiological responses by continuous measurement. Arch Sex Behav. 1977 Mar;6(2):121–133. doi: 10.1007/BF01541704. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Wulfert E., Hayes S. C. Transfer of a conditional ordering response through conditional equivalence classes. J Exp Anal Behav. 1988 Sep;50(2):125–144. doi: 10.1901/jeab.1988.50-125. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. de Rose J. C., McIlvane W. J., Dube W. V., Galpin V. C., Stoddard L. T. Emergent simple discrimination established by indirect relation to differential consequences. J Exp Anal Behav. 1988 Jul;50(1):1–20. doi: 10.1901/jeab.1988.50-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior are provided here courtesy of Society for the Experimental Analysis of Behavior

RESOURCES