Abstract
Four college students were exposed to a Sidman avoidance procedure to determine if an avoidance contingency involving 20% carbon dioxide-enriched air (CO2) would produce and maintain responding. In Phase 1, two conditions (contingent and noncontingent) were conducted each day. These conditions were distinguished by the presence or absence of a blue or green box on a computer screen. In the contingent condition, CO2 presentation were delivered every 3 s unless a subject pulled a plunger. Each plunger pull postponed CO2 presentations for 10 s. In the noncontingent condition, CO2 presentations occurred on the average of every 5 min independent of responding. Following stable responding in Phase 1, condition-correlated stimuli were reversed. In both conditions, plunger response rate was high during the contingent condition and low or zero during the noncontingent condition. Furthermore, subjects avoided most CO2 presentations. However, CO2 presentations did not increase verbal reports of fear. Overall, the results from the present study suggest that CO2 can be used effectively in basic studies of aversive control and in laboratory analogues of response patterns commonly referred to as anxiety.
Full Text
The Full Text of this article is available as a PDF (141.8 KB).
Selected References
These references are in PubMed. This may not be the complete list of references from this article.
- ADER R., TATUM R. Free-operant avoidance conditioning in human subjects. J Exp Anal Behav. 1961 Jul;4:275–276. doi: 10.1901/jeab.1961.4-275. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- ADER R., TATUM R. Free-operant avoidance conditioning in individual and paired human subjects. J Exp Anal Behav. 1963 Jul;6:357–359. doi: 10.1901/jeab.1963.6-357. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Eke M., McNally R. J. Anxiety sensitivity, suffocation fear, trait anxiety, and breath-holding duration as predictors of response to carbon dioxide challenge. Behav Res Ther. 1996 Aug;34(8):603–607. doi: 10.1016/0005-7967(96)00044-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hineline P. N., Rachlin H. Escape and avoidance of shock by pigeons pecking a key. J Exp Anal Behav. 1969 Jul;12(4):533–538. doi: 10.1901/jeab.1969.12-533. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- LINDSLEY O. R. Operant conditioning methods applied to research in chronic schizophrenia. Psychiatr Res Rep Am Psychiatr Assoc. 1956 Jun;5:118-39; discussion, 140-53. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rapee R. M., Brown T. A., Antony M. M., Barlow D. H. Response to hyperventilation and inhalation of 5.5% carbon dioxide-enriched air across the DSM-III-R anxiety disorders. J Abnorm Psychol. 1992 Aug;101(3):538–552. doi: 10.1037//0021-843x.101.3.538. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- SIDMAN M. Classical avoidance without a warning stimulus. J Exp Anal Behav. 1962 Jan;5:97–104. doi: 10.1901/jeab.1962.5-97. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sanderson W. C., Rapee R. M., Barlow D. H. The influence of an illusion of control on panic attacks induced via inhalation of 5.5% carbon dioxide-enriched air. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1989 Feb;46(2):157–162. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1989.01810020059010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Weiner H. Conditioning history and the control of human avoidance and escape responding. J Exp Anal Behav. 1969 Nov;12(6):1039–1043. doi: 10.1901/jeab.1969.12-1039. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- de Silva P., Rachman S. Does escape behaviour strengthen agoraphobic avoidance? A preliminary study. Behav Res Ther. 1984;22(1):87–91. doi: 10.1016/0005-7967(84)90038-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]