Abstract
Following pretraining with everyday objects, 1- to 4-year-old children received listener training with three pairs of arbitrary stimuli of differing shapes. For each pair, 9 children were trained to select one stimulus in response to the spoken word /zog/ and the other to the spoken word /vek/. Next, in the look-at-sample category match-to-sample test, none categorized the six stimuli correctly when asked to look at the sample before selecting from five comparisons. Seven of these children failed a subsequent test of corresponding speaker behavior (tact test); following tact training, 5 of them passed either a repeat of the look-at-sample category test (2 subjects) or an alternative category test (3 subjects) in which they were required to tact the sample before selecting comparisons. The remaining 2 failed both category tests. Of the 2 who passed the tact test, 1 passed the tact-sample category test; the other failed to complete category testing. Two children were next given a second stimulus set. One passed the look-at-sample category test and the tact test; the other failed both tests but passed the tact-sample category test after tact training. The results show that 1- to 4-year-old children may learn listener behavior without corresponding speaker behavior. The results also show that common listener behavior is not sufficient to establish arbitrary stimulus classes, and they are consistent with the proposition that naming may be necessary for categorization of such stimuli.
Full Text
The Full Text of this article is available as a PDF (467.5 KB).
Selected References
These references are in PubMed. This may not be the complete list of references from this article.
- Baldwin D. A. Infants' contribution to the achievement of joint reference. Child Dev. 1991 Oct;62(5):875–890. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Baldwin D. A., Markman E. M., Bill B., Desjardins R. N., Irwin J. M., Tidball G. Infants' reliance on a social criterion for establishing word-object relations. Child Dev. 1996 Dec;67(6):3135–3153. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bloom P. Intention, history, and artifact concepts. Cognition. 1996 Jul;60(1):1–29. doi: 10.1016/0010-0277(95)00699-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carr D., Wilkinson K. M., Blackman D., McIlvane W. J. Equivalence classes in individuals with minimal verbal repertoires. J Exp Anal Behav. 2000 Jul;74(1):101–114. doi: 10.1901/jeab.2000.74-101. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Catania A. C. Natural contingencies in the creation of naming as a higher order behavior class. J Exp Anal Behav. 1996 Jan;65(1):276–279. doi: 10.1901/jeab.1996.65-276. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dugdale N., Lowe C. F. Testing for symmetry in the conditional discriminations of language-trained chimpanzees. J Exp Anal Behav. 2000 Jan;73(1):5–22. doi: 10.1901/jeab.2000.73-5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Eikeseth S., Smith T. The development of functional and equivalence classes in high-functioning autistic children: the role of naming. J Exp Anal Behav. 1992 Jul;58(1):123–133. doi: 10.1901/jeab.1992.58-123. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fergus Lowe C., Horne Pauline J., Harris Fay D. A., Randle Valerie R. L. Naming and categorization in young children: vocal tact training. J Exp Anal Behav. 2002 Nov;78(3):527–549. doi: 10.1901/jeab.2002.78-527. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fields L., Reeve K., Rosen D., Varelas A., Adams B. Using the simultaneous protocol to study equivalence class formation: the facilitating effects of nodal number and size of previously established equivalence classes. J Exp Anal Behav. 1997 May;67(3):367–389. doi: 10.1901/jeab.1997.67-367. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fields Lanny, Matneja Priya, Varelas Antonios, Belanich James, Fitzer Adrienne, Shamoun Kim. The formation of linked perceptual classes. J Exp Anal Behav. 2002 Nov;78(3):271–290. doi: 10.1901/jeab.2002.78-271. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fields Lanny, Reeve Kenneth F., Matneja Priya, Varelas Antonios, Belanich James, Fitzer Adrienne, Shamoun Kim. The formation of a generalized categorization repertoire: effect of training with multiple domains, samples, and comparisons. J Exp Anal Behav. 2002 Nov;78(3):291–313. doi: 10.1901/jeab.2002.78-291. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gopnik A., Sobel D. M. Detecting blickets: how young children use information about novel causal powers in categorization and induction. Child Dev. 2000 Sep-Oct;71(5):1205–1222. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00224. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Green G. Differences in development of visual and auditory-visual equivalence relations. Am J Ment Retard. 1990 Nov;95(3):260–270. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Harris M., Yeeles C., Chasin J., Oakley Y. Symmetries and asymmetries in early lexical comprehension and production. J Child Lang. 1995 Feb;22(1):1–18. doi: 10.1017/s0305000900009600. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hayes S. C. Developing a theory of derived stimulus relations. J Exp Anal Behav. 1996 Jan;65(1):309–311. doi: 10.1901/jeab.1996.65-309. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hayes S. C. Nonhumans have not yet shown stimulus equivalence. J Exp Anal Behav. 1989 May;51(3):385–392. doi: 10.1901/jeab.1989.51-385. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hernstein R. J., Loveland D. H., Cable C. Natural concepts in pigeons. J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process. 1976 Oct;2(4):285–302. doi: 10.1037//0097-7403.2.4.285. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Horne P. J., Lowe C. F. On the origins of naming and other symbolic behavior. J Exp Anal Behav. 1996 Jan;65(1):185–241. doi: 10.1901/jeab.1996.65-185. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Horne P., Lowe C. Toward a theory of verbal behavior. J Exp Anal Behav. 1997 Sep;68(2):271–296. doi: 10.1901/jeab.1997.68-271. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Innis A., Lane S., Miller E., Critchfield T. Stimulus Equivalence: Effects Of A Default-response Option On Emergence Of Untrained Stimulus Relations. J Exp Anal Behav. 1998 Jul;70(1):87–102. doi: 10.1901/jeab.1998.70-87. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kastak C. R., Schusterman R. J., Kastak D. Equivalence classification by California sea lions using class-specific reinforcers. J Exp Anal Behav. 2001 Sep;76(2):131–158. doi: 10.1901/jeab.2001.76-131. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Keller M. F., Bucher B. D. Transfer between receptive and productive language in developmentally disabled children. J Appl Behav Anal. 1979 Summer;12(2):311–311. doi: 10.1901/jaba.1979.12-311. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kemler Nelson D. G., Frankenfield A., Morris C., Blair E. Young children's use of functional information to categorize artifacts: three factors that matter. Cognition. 2000 Nov 16;77(2):133–168. doi: 10.1016/s0010-0277(00)00097-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Laport Ginna G., Levine Bruce L., Stadtmauer Edward A., Schuster Stephen J., Luger Selina M., Grupp Stephan, Bunin Nancy, Strobl Frank J., Cotte Julio, Zheng Zhaohui. Adoptive transfer of costimulated T cells induces lymphocytosis in patients with relapsed/refractory non-Hodgkin lymphoma following CD34+-selected hematopoietic cell transplantation. Blood. 2003 May 22;102(6):2004–2013. doi: 10.1182/blood-2003-01-0095. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lee V. L. Prepositional phrases spoken and heard. J Exp Anal Behav. 1981 Mar;35(2):227–242. doi: 10.1901/jeab.1981.35-227. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lionello-DeNolf Karen M., Urcuioli Peter J. Stimulus control topographies and tests of symmetry in pigeons. J Exp Anal Behav. 2002 Nov;78(3):467–495. doi: 10.1901/jeab.2002.78-467. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lipkens R., Hayes S. C., Hayes L. J. Longitudinal study of the development of derived relations in an infant. J Exp Child Psychol. 1993 Oct;56(2):201–239. doi: 10.1006/jecp.1993.1032. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Liu J., Golinkoff R. M., Sak K. One cow does not an animal make: young children can extend novel words at the superordinate level. Child Dev. 2001 Nov-Dec;72(6):1674–1694. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00372. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lowe C. F., Horne P. J. Reflections on naming and other symbolic behavior. J Exp Anal Behav. 1996 Jan;65(1):315–353. doi: 10.1901/jeab.1996.65-315. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- O'Donnell Jennifer, Saunders Kathryn J. Equivalence relations in individuals with language limitations and mental retardation. J Exp Anal Behav. 2003 Jul;80(1):131–157. doi: 10.1901/jeab.2003.80-131. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- doi: 10.1901/jaba.1978.11-529. [DOI] [PMC free article] [Google Scholar]
- Pilgrim C., Chambers L., Galizio M. Reversal of baseline relations and stimulus equivalence: II. Children. J Exp Anal Behav. 1995 May;63(3):239–254. doi: 10.1901/jeab.1995.63-239. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pilgrim C., Galizio M. Reversal of baseline relations and stimulus equivalence: I. Adults. J Exp Anal Behav. 1995 May;63(3):225–238. doi: 10.1901/jeab.1995.63-225. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Randell T., Remington B. Equivalence relations between visual stimuli: the functional role of naming. J Exp Anal Behav. 1999 May;71(3):395–415. doi: 10.1901/jeab.1999.71-395. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Saunders R. R., Drake K. M., Spradlin J. E. Equivalence class establishment, expansion, and modification in preschool children. J Exp Anal Behav. 1999 Mar;71(2):195–214. doi: 10.1901/jeab.1999.71-195. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schafer G., Plunkett K. Rapid word learning by fifteen-month-olds under tightly controlled conditions. Child Dev. 1998 Apr;69(2):309–320. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sidman M. Equivalence relations and the reinforcement contingency. J Exp Anal Behav. 2000 Jul;74(1):127–146. doi: 10.1901/jeab.2000.74-127. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sidman M., Kirk B., Willson-Morris M. Six-member stimulus classes generated by conditional-discrimination procedures. J Exp Anal Behav. 1985 Jan;43(1):21–42. doi: 10.1901/jeab.1985.43-21. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sidman M., Tailby W. Conditional discrimination vs. matching to sample: an expansion of the testing paradigm. J Exp Anal Behav. 1982 Jan;37(1):5–22. doi: 10.1901/jeab.1982.37-5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sloutsky V. M., Lo Y. F., Fisher A. V. How much does a shared name make things similar? Linguistic labels, similarity, and the development of inductive inference. Child Dev. 2001 Nov-Dec;72(6):1695–1709. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00373. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smith L. B., Jones S. S., Landau B. Naming in young children: a dumb attentional mechanism? Cognition. 1996 Aug;60(2):143–171. doi: 10.1016/0010-0277(96)00709-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Spencer T. J., Chase P. N. Speed analyses of stimulus equivalence. J Exp Anal Behav. 1996 May;65(3):643–659. doi: 10.1901/jeab.1996.65-643. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sutton Jennifer E., Roberts William A. Failure to find evidence of stimulus generalization within pictorial categories in pigeons. J Exp Anal Behav. 2002 Nov;78(3):333–343. doi: 10.1901/jeab.2002.78-333. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Welder A. N., Graham S. A. The influence of shape similarity and shared labels on infants' inductive inferences about nonobvious object properties. Child Dev. 2001 Nov-Dec;72(6):1653–1673. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00371. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Werker J. F., Cohen L. B., Lloyd V. L., Casasola M., Stager C. L. Acquisition of word-object associations by 14-month-old infants. Dev Psychol. 1998 Nov;34(6):1289–1309. doi: 10.1037//0012-1649.34.6.1289. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]