Skip to main content
UKPMC Funders Author Manuscripts logoLink to UKPMC Funders Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2005 Dec 15.
Published in final edited form as: Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci. 2004 Jun;4(2):193–200. doi: 10.3758/cabn.4.2.193

When mirrors lie: ‘Visual capture’ of arm position impairs reaching performance

Nicholas P Holmes 1,, Gemma Crozier 1, Charles Spence 1
PMCID: PMC1314973  EMSID: UKMS5436  PMID: 15460925

Abstract

If we stand at a mirror’s edge, we can see one half of our body reflected in the mirror, as if it were the other half of our body, seen ‘through’ the mirror. We used this ‘mirror illusion’ to examine the effect of conflicts between visually and proprioceptively-specified arm position on subsequent reaching movements made with the unseen right arm. When participants viewed their static left arm in the mirror (i.e., as if it were their right arm), subsequent right-arm reaching movements were affected significantly more when there was conflict between the (apparent) visually- and proprioceptively-specified right-arm positions, than when there was no apparent conflict. This result demonstrates that ‘visual capture’ of arm position can occur when individual body parts are viewed in the mirror, and that this capture has a measurable effect on subsequent reaching movements made with an unseen arm. The result has implications for how the brain represents the body across different sensory modalities.

Introduction

Facing a mirror, we can observe ourselves as we usually see others, and as they usually see us – our reflection is left-right reversed (see Gregory, 1996, for extensive discussion of this phenomenon). Turn the mirror through ninety degrees, however, and now, standing at the mirror’s edge, with our head on one side and one arm stretched forward, we can see one side of our body reflected as if it were the other side. Thanks to the almost perfect symmetry of the human body (across the saggital midline), the visual appearance of one reflected side of our body can provide a strikingly realistic substitute for direct vision of the other side of our body. This ‘mirror illusion’ has recently been used to great effect in the rehabilitation of neuropsychological patients. For example, mirrors have been used with patients suffering phantom limb pain following arm amputation (e.g., Hunter, Katz, & Davis, 2003; Ramachandran & Rogers-Ramachandran, 1996, Ramachandran, Rogers-Ramachandran, & Cobb, 1996), or with sensorimotor deficits in their arms (Altschuler, Wisdom, Stone, Foster, Galasko, Llewellyn, & Ramachandran, 1999; Sathian, Greenspan, & Wolf, 2000).

In addition to the potential clinical uses of this form of ‘mirror therapy,’ the mirror also offers a powerful experimental tool with which to examine the integration of multiple sources of information concerning the position of our bodies in space, by inducing visual-proprioceptive conflicts (for various examples, see Holmes & Spence, 2004; Lajoie, Paillard, Teasdale, Bard, Fleury, Forget, & Lamarre, 1992; Maravita, Spence, & Driver, 2003). The positions of our hands are specified by a variety of sources of information, including proprioceptive, kinaesthetic, tactile, visual, vestibular, and sometimes even auditory cues (e.g., think of the sound heard when scratching the back of one’s neck!). In most situations, these different cues provide redundant information concerning the true location of our limbs in space. While looking in the mirror, however, visual information is brought into conflict with all the other positional cues. Despite this conflicting visual information, however, we are very good at locating our body parts in the mirror, can readily identify the mirror reflection as that of our own bodies rather than that of someone else behind the mirror, and can, with practice, locate and reach toward external objects while viewing them using only their mirror reflection to guide us (with certain types of conflict and tasks, however, our performance can be severely impaired – see, for instance, Burnett’s detailed studies of ‘The Aristotle Illusion’ published in 1904; see also Jackson & Zangwill, 1953). The ability to perform these apparently simple directed movements in the mirror can, however, be impaired selectively as the recently discovered neuropsychological syndromes of mirror agnosia and mirror ataxia suggest (Binkofski, Buccino, Dohle, Seitz, & Freund, 1999; Ramachandran, Altschuler, & Hillyer, 1997). Conversely, mirror-guided performance in complex visuomotor tasks can actually be enhanced in patients with selective loss of proprioceptive afferent inputs (Lajoie et al., 1992).

In the experiments reported here, we manipulated the position of the right arm held out-of-sight behind a mirror, while controlling visual cues concerning its apparent position by use of a mirror-reflection of the left arm. Participants made unseen reaching movements to a target with the right arm, while looking at the mirror reflection of their left arm, or else looking at an opaque screen over the mirror. Participants’ reaching movements were strongly influenced by the presence of the mirror, suggesting that visual information concerning the apparent position of the right arm can ‘capture’ proprioceptive positional information before (or perhaps during) reaching toward a target. Furthermore, subjective verbal estimates of the initial position of the right hand correlated significantly with the final end-point error of the reaching movements. These results suggest that reaching movements are guided by a combination of visual, proprioceptive, and kinaesthetic cues, and that in the mirror situation, visual cues may play a dominant role over the representation of the initial arm position.

Methods

Participants

16 right-handed participants (8 male, 8 female, aged 20–34 years) with normal or corrected vision were recruited by advertisement. Participants gave their informed consent to participate prior to the experiments. All of the participants apart from one (one of the authors, C. S.) were naïve to the purposes of the study. The experiment was approved by the local research ethics committee.

Apparatus and materials

A mirror measuring 45 cm by 30 cm was positioned vertically in the middle of a large table, with the reflective surface facing to the participant’s left, and oriented parallel to participants’ saggital axis (see Figure 1A). An opaque flat platform measuring 45 cm x 45 cm and raised 20 cm above the table surface was positioned immediately to the right of the mirror. A small mark was positioned on the table 12 cm to the left side of the mirror and 30 cm in front of the participant. Similar marks were positioned 5, 12, 19, and 26 cm to the right of the mirror, out of the participant’s view. A fixed cardboard (10 cm x 10 cm, with a vertical arrow in the middle of the card) target indicator was positioned within the participant’s view on top and at the rear of the opaque platform, 12 cm right of the mirror. The target indicator pointed toward a position 12 cm to the right and at the back edge of the mirror (i.e., 45 cm along the table from the participant). In Experiment 2, a row of printed roman letters (A to Z, spaced 1 cm apart, and spanning a total of 25 cm) was positioned on top of the occluding platform 3 cm to the right of the mirror, and 30 cm from the participant’s body, running parallel to and directly above the right arm position marks. An opaque cloth was draped over the participants’ right arm and shoulder to minimise any visual cues pertaining to the position of the right arm.

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Experimental apparatus and results of Experiment 1. Panel A. The participant sat at a table, with a mirror positioned vertically in the centre, and an occluding platform to the participant’s right. The participant’s right hand was placed by the experimenter on one of the four hand positions marked on the table. The right arm and hand was never visible to the participant during the experiment, while the left arm and hand was visible directly in both conditions, and via the mirror reflection in the Mirror condition. When the right hand was positioned 12 cm to the right of the mirror, the mirror reflection of the left hand appeared to be in the same position as the true location of the right hand (as shown in the figure – solid and broken white lines show the approximate line of sight, via the mirror reflection, of the left hand). In all other right hand positions (5, 19, & 26 cm), the apparent visual appearance of the right hand (really the mirror-reflected left hand) was mismatched with respect to the true position of the right hand. After tapping the fingers of both hands synchronously, and experiencing the ‘mirror illusion’ (the feeling that one can see through the mirror to observe one’s right hand directly), participants reached with their right index finger to the position on the table where they judged the target position indicator line would meet the table (i.e., 12 cm to the right of the mirror, and 45 cm in front of the participant). Panel B. The bars show the mean (± standard error) of mean reaching errors across participants, separately for both conditions and the four arm positions. Open bars – Mirror condition; Filled bars – No-Mirror condition; x-axis – distance of right index finger from the mirror before reaching; y-axis – mean reaching error (measured in cm to the right of the target).

Design

In Experiment 1, there were two experimental conditions, namely the Mirror condition and the No-Mirror condition. The starting condition was counterbalanced across participants. In both conditions, the index finger of the participants’ right hand was positioned, 5 cm, 12 cm, 19 cm, or 26 cm to the right of the mirror. The index finger of the left hand was always positioned 12 cm to the left of the mirror. Five trials were performed for each of the four right hand positions and two experimental conditions (i.e., a total of 40 trials per participant). The order of arm positions within each condition was randomized on a trial-by-trial basis. In Experiment 2, an extra condition, namely, ‘Mirror-passive’ was included. Six counterbalanced block orders were constructed to which participants were assigned serially. Participants in Experiment 2 performed 10 trials per hand position and condition (i.e., 120 trials in total) to provide extra data points for a correlation analysis. Ten participants performed Experiment 1, and six new participants performed Experiment 2.

Procedure

Participants sat facing the table, and positioned themselves just to the left of the mirror. The index finger of the participant’s left hand was placed 12 cm to the left of the mirror and 30 cm in front of their body. The participant’s right arm was positioned under the platform, with their right index finger on one of the four marks. The participants were instructed to look at the reflection of their left hand in the mirror (specifically, at their left index finger). When both left and right hands were positioned on the 12 cm marks, the mirror reflection of the left arm thus appeared to be in an identical position to the true position of the right arm (as if the right arm were seen ‘through’ the mirror). In all other right hand positions, the visually-specified apparent position of the right hand, and the proprioceptively-specified true position of the right hand conflicted.

In a short practice session at the start of the experiment, the mirror was covered so that the left arm was visible only directly and not in the mirror. Participants practised reaching with their unseen right index finger to the target position on the table 45 cm in front of them. Feedback on reaching accuracy was given by the experimenter, and practice continued until the participant could reach consistently with their index finger landing within 2 cm of the target point. No further feedback was given after this practise session.

The mirror was uncovered, and participants were asked to move the fingers of their left and right hands synchronously and in an identical manner, until they felt that the mirror reflection of their left hand seemed to be identical to the real right hand. This ‘latching-on’ to the mirror reflection was a purely subjective judgment, and in the practice session served only to illustrate to the participants the nature of the ‘mirror illusion’ they were expected to experience. In the experimental sessions, this ‘latching-on’ was the crucial determinant of the timing of the subsequent reaching movement, as described in more detail below. Participants were always aware of the presence or absence of the mirror and the identity of the hand (i.e., the left) seen reflected in it, though the onset and existence of the mirror illusion itself suggests that in some sense, participants were unsure of which hand they were viewing in the mirror. (This uncertainty is made particularly vivid when, after experiencing the illusion, one hand is moved while the other remains still.)

Each experimental trial began with the experimenter moving the index finger of the participant’s right hand under the screen into a position 5, 12, 19, or 26 cm to the right of the mirror. When the hand was in position, the participant was asked to begin tapping the fingers of their left and right hands synchronously and in an identical manner until they experienced the onset of the mirror illusion, which they reported verbally to the experimenter. In the No-Mirror condition, participants were instructed to move their fingers for approximately 6 to 8 seconds (the average illusion onset time as determined by earlier pilot research). Several seconds after the reported onset of the illusion (or after about 10 seconds in the No-Mirror condition), the experimenter gave the instruction to “reach,” at which point the participant gazed toward the target indicator, and reached forward with their right arm and attempted to place their right index finger on the target position directly below the indicator. The participants were instructed to make one quick, smooth movement with their right arm and to reach to the target as accurately as possible, leaving their index finger touching the table until the experimenter had recorded the reaching error (in centimetres to the left or right of the target with respect to the participant) with a ruler. The participant’s arm was then set to a new position, and the next trial began.

The No-Mirror condition differed from the Mirror condition in that an opaque covering was placed over the mirror, and participants were instructed to look at the covering as if they were looking through it toward their unseen right index finger (when placed in the 12 cm position). This same gaze direction was adopted for all right arm positions.

Experiment 2 was identical to Experiment 1 except for the following differences. Immediately prior to reaching in the Mirror and No-Mirror conditions, participants were instructed to look at a line of printed letters on top of the occluding platform and to report verbally the letter that they felt was directly above the tip of their right index finger. When they had reported a single letter, they reached toward the target as in Experiment 1. Participants were encouraged to be as accurate as possible in reporting the felt location of their right index finger, but also not to take more than one or two seconds to do so. Verbally-reported illusion onset times were not measured in Experiment 2. Participants were instructed to report the letter then reach toward the target when they began to feel the mirror illusion in the Mirror condition (rather than when instructed to do so as in Experiment 1), and after approximately 6 seconds of finger-tapping in the No-Mirror condition. Additionally, in the Mirror-Passive condition of Experiment 2, participants were asked simply to fixate on their mirror-reflected left index finger for approximately 2 seconds before reaching directly to the target as described above. This extra condition served to examine the effect of a brief visual exposure to the mirror situation in the absence of both movement of the left hand, and of a conscious effort to experience the ‘mirror illusion’.

Data Analysis

The measure of interest was the reaching error of the index finger of the right hand. The landing point of the right index finger was recorded in one dimension only, that is, the distance between the target and the landing point measured perpendicular to the mirror. Negative values indicate a landing point too close to the mirror, while positive values indicate a landing point too far from the mirror. In all cases, the target was positioned 12 cm to the right of the mirror, thus all measurements are taken from this origin. Additionally, for each trial in Experiment 1, the time taken to experience the illusion, according to the participant’s subjective report was recorded, and for each trial of the Mirror and No-Mirror conditions of Experiment 2, the letter reported by the participant was recorded.

Error data were analysed using a two-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) with the factors of Condition (Mirror vs. No-Mirror) and right hand Position (5, 12, 19, or 26 cm). Post-hoc tests were performed to examine individual levels of the main experimental factors. Illusion onset times were analysed with a 1-way ANOVA. The hand position judgment data from Experiment 2 was analysed on a trial-by-trial basis with the reaching error separately for each participant using Pearson’s product-moment correlation. The r-values and slope coefficients of the regression equations for these correlations were compared between conditions using Student’s 1-tailed paired t-tests. All measurement data given are the means of individual means, ± the standard errors of the means.

Results

Experiment 1

The mean reaching error data averaged across participants for both the Mirror and No-Mirror conditions, and for each hand position are presented in Figure 1B. The analysis of the reaching error revealed a significant main effect of Condition (F(1,9) = 28.46, p < .001). Comparison of the mean reaching error for the Mirror (1.74 ± 0.69 cm) versus the No-Mirror condition (0.3 ± 0.59 cm) revealed a significantly larger reaching error in the Mirror condition (mean ± s.e.m. difference between conditions = 1.44 ± 0.27, p < .001). Since the reaching error was always negative when the right hand was in the 5 cm position, the main effect of condition in this analysis is underestimated. If the reaching errors were instead converted to absolute errors (i.e., in terms of their distance from the target regardless of direction), then the mean and s.e.m. of the differences between the conditions was 1.67 ± 0.35 cm.

The main effect of hand Position was also significant (F(3,27) = 62.01, p < .001). Post-hoc Bonferroni tests between individual hand positions revealed statistically reliable differences between all comparisons (all mean differences > 1.6 cm; all p’s < .01). The significant trend in these data is toward larger reaching errors with increasing separation of the right hand from the target line. Furthermore, in all cases, the mean reaching error shows an underestimation of the distance to the target (positive values for 19 and 26 cm hand positions, negative values for the 5 cm hand position).

The crucial experimental comparison concerns the interaction between the mirror Condition and hand Position. If the visual appearance of the mirror reflection of the left hand affected the felt position of the right hand (i.e., if it ‘visually captured’ the felt position of the right hand), then pointing errors should have increased as the discrepancy between the visually- and the proprioceptively-specified hand position increased, and this increase should have been larger for the Mirror condition than for the No-Mirror condition. In short, the interaction between Condition and hand Position should have been significant. The ANOVA revealed this to be the case (F(3,27) = 44.5, p < .001). Post-hoc tests between the Mirror and No-Mirror conditions for individual hand positions revealed that the difference between mean reaching error when the right hand was placed 5 cm (t(9) = −2.94, p < .05), 19 cm (t(9) = 4.94, p < .01), and 26 cm (t(9) = 8.34, p < .001) from the mirror were all significant, while there was no significant difference between conditions when the right hand was placed 12 cm from the mirror.

The mean reaching error across participants was significantly correlated with hand position in both the Mirror condition (r = 1, p < .005), and the No-Mirror condition (r = 0.96, p < .005). However, in the Mirror condition, the slope of the best-fit line though the means was 0.42, compared to a slope of only 0.15 in the No-Mirror condition. Thus, for every centimetre that the right hand was moved away from the target in either direction, the reaching error increased in the same direction by an average of 0.42 cm in the Mirror condition, and by only 0.15 cm in the No-Mirror condition.

The analysis of the time taken to experience the illusion revealed no main effect of hand position, although the difference between the 26 cm and 19 cm positions was significant after correcting for multiple comparisons (26 cm = 8.1 ± 0.8 sec; 19 cm = 6.2 ± 0.6 sec; p < .05, 1-tailed t-test). For comparison, mean illusion times for 5 cm and 12 cm hand positions were 6.2 ± 1.0 sec and 6.1 ± 0.6 sec respectively.

Experiment 2

The results of Experiment 2 were very similar to those of Experiment 1. Analysis of the reaching error for the 6 additional participants revealed a significant main effect of Position (F(3,15) = 24.01, p < .001), and a significant interaction between Condition and Position (F(6,30) = 14.05, p < .001). There was no significant main effect of Condition. Further analysis of the Condition by Position interaction revealed significant differences between all Position pairs (planned pairwise comparisons) for the Mirror and Mirror-Passive conditions (all F’s > 6.3, all p’s < .05), and the Mirror and No-Mirror conditions but no significant differences between Mirror-passive and No-Mirror conditions (all F’s < 1, n.s.). The best-fit regression lines through the means of the reaching errors had slope coefficients of 0.30 (Mirror condition), 0.08 (No-Mirror condition), and 0.10 (Mirror-Passive condition).

The index finger position judgments made in the Mirror condition were significantly correlated with reaching error measurements on a trial-by-trial basis. Individual participants’ r-values for the Mirror condition ranged from 0.35 – 0.87 (all p’s < .025), while in the No-Mirror condition, correlation between these two measures was much lower (range, −0.11 – 0.53; 4 participants, p < .05, 2 participants, p > .05). Figure 2 shows the relationship between initial finger position judgment and final reaching error for two participants. Across all six participants, the coefficients of the best-fit regression slope for the Mirror (0.54 ± 0.17 cm−1) and the No-Mirror conditions (0.07 ± 0.03 cm−1) were significantly different from each other (t(5) = 4.05, p < .01), indicating that every centimetre difference in reported initial finger position had a greater influence on reaching error in the Mirror condition (i.e., by 0.54 cm) than in the No-Mirror condition (0.07 cm). The point for each participant at which the regression slopes for the Mirror and No-Mirror conditions crossed (i.e., the perceived initial hand position related to the same average reaching error in both Mirror and No-Mirror conditions) was 10.2 ± 2.2 cm to the right of the mirror (mean and s.e.m. of crossing points for 6 participants, n.s.d. from the actual point of visual-proprioceptive equality, at 12 cm to the right of the mirror).

Figure 2.

Figure 2

Scatterplots of the reaching error (y-axis) against the reported initial finger position (x-axis), for two representative participants in Experiment 2 (selected for clarity of display). Each data point represents one trial. Perfect performance would be evidenced by a horizontal line of data points along the x-axis (i.e., with a reaching error of zero). Conversely, complete visual capture at all initial right hand positions would be evidenced by a vertical line of data points running along the y-axis (the participant would always feel his or her hand to be 12 cm right of the mirror). Triangles – participant 1; Circles –participant 2; Open symbols and broken regression lines – Mirror conditions; Filled symbols and solid regression lines – No-Mirror conditions.

Discussion

When participants reached toward a target with their unseen right hand, the presence of a mirror-reflection of their left hand significantly influenced the accuracy of their reaching (at least when tested immediately after experiencing the mirror illusion). The influence of the mirror-reflection was strongest when the discrepancy between the apparent initial position of the right hand (visually-specified) and the real initial position of the right hand (proprioceptively-specified) was largest. The reaching error increased as a function of the absolute separation of hand and target position in both the Mirror and the No-Mirror conditions, but this increase was much greater in the Mirror condition (slope = 0.42 in Experiment 1, 0.30 in Experiment 2) than in the No-Mirror condition (slope = 0.15 in E1, 0.08 in E2). Thus, the present experiments demonstrate that the ‘mirror illusion,’ where the reflected left hand both appears to occupy the space of the right hand, and moreover actually feels as if it is the right hand, can have a measurable effect on subsequent unseen reaching movements made with the right hand. Furthermore, as shown by Experiment 2, the perceived initial position of the unseen finger was highly correlated with the final reaching error, suggesting that the effect of the felt position of the right hand is crucial in producing the reaching errors we observed. This conclusion is made even stronger by the results of the Mirror-Passive condition in Experiment 2; When participants made reaching movements after just 2 seconds of passive visual exposure to the reflected left hand (compared with an average of approximately 6 seconds active experience in the Mirror condition), there was little modulation of reaching performance as compared to the No-Mirror condition. The slight difference in the regression slope between Mirror-Passive (0.10) and No-Mirror conditions (0.08) may be suggestive of a residual effect of visual information alone, which should be addressed in future studies. It would be interesting to study the effect of visual exposure time on any modulation of reaching error in both active and passive mirror conditions – can the same level of reaching error be produced with active and passive conditions, and is there a minimum exposure time for any effects on reaching error to occur?

The source of the reaching errors

What might account for the effect of the mirror reflection on participants’ reaching performance? The simplest explanation, that the participants were unaware that they were not viewing their right hand, can be discounted. All of the participants were aware of the mirror’s presence at all times, and at no point could they see their right hand or arm. Indeed, as soon as the participant made a reaching movement with the right hand, the lack of any visual feedback regarding this movement would have been registered. A second explanation might be that participants were simply biased by the experimental situation toward making errors in a certain direction. This can presumably be discounted, as the experimenter encouraged participants always to reach as accurately as possible to the target, the indicator for which was always in view. An explanation based on a constant reaching error (which differed between conditions) can also be discounted, since the direction of the reaching error reversed when the right hand was brought closer to the mirror (at the 5 cm position), and the correlation of hand position with mean reaching error was perfect (r-squared = 1 in E1, 0.998 in E2). Finally, an explanation in terms of proprioceptive drift throughout the pointing trials (which were performed under ‘open loop’ conditions without visual feedback or information about reaching accuracy) can be discounted, as the trial presentation order was randomised, so any gradual proprioceptive drift would affect all right hand positions equally. We believe the most appropriate explanation to involve the effect of conflicting visual information concerning the (apparent) position of the right hand on subsequent reaching movements made with that hand. This effect is a form of ‘visual capture’ of initial hand position in the mirror. One question that remains to be determined is to what extent the effects observed here are due simply to the displaced visual image of the hand, or to the feeling that the hand is one’s own. Initial results from our second experiment suggest that it is largely the felt position of the hand that determines the direction and a large proportion of the magnitude of the reaching error, rather than simply the visual image of the hand.

That the effect of the mirror illusion was not complete (reaching error was an average of 30–42% of the difference between initial left and right hand positions in the two experiments) might be explained in several ways. First, insufficient time may have elapsed for the full illusion to take hold. Longer experience of the sensory conflict might lead to greater recalibration of reaching performance along with an enhanced feeling of the mirror illusion, although it is an open question as to whether visual capture occurs prior to any visual-proprioceptive recalibration (which leads to the reaching errors), or whether recalibration is a necessary precondition for visual capture to occur. Second, the visual modality was only one source of limb-position information. All the other cues combined (proprioceptive, somatosensory, and auditory cues, plus any ‘higher,’ cognitive influences – for example remembering in which direction one’s hand was moved at the beginning of a given trial, and deliberately compensating for this directional shift when reaching) might also have limited the effect of the conflicting visual information concerning hand position. Finally, once the reaching movement began, dynamic positional or kinaesthetic information was also available to the participant, and mid-reach trajectory corrections may have occurred (although see Rossetti, Desmurget, & Prablanc, 1995). Examining the trajectory of the right hand during the reaching movement would allow the effect of this dynamic position information to be assessed in more detail.

Visual capture by different experimental means

In a study similar to the experiments reported here, Rossetti and colleagues (1995) asked participants to reach with their right hand toward one of four visible ‘virtual’ targets in a darkened room from one initial starting position. Prior to the reaching movement, participants received a brief visual cue (the illumination of a yellow LED attached to their index fingertip) concerning the position of their reaching hand, but throughout the reaching movement, only the target was visible, and no positional feedback was given concerning hand position. In one condition, the initial hand position was displaced 8.5° to the right by lenses mounted in the mirror used to display the virtual targets, while in the control condition there was no visual displacement of initial hand position. In both conditions, vision of the targets themselves remained undistorted throughout the experiment. Rossetti et al. showed that in the displaced hand condition, reaching movements were biased by a constant amount (approximately 13 mm, or 32% of the total visual displacement) toward the visually-specified initial hand position for all four targets. Since no visual feedback of the hand was given during the reaching movements, this result suggests that adaptation to conflicts between visually and proprioceptively specified hand position can be as large as 30% of the disparity based simply on initial hand position alone. Our result of a 30–42% bias toward vision (or, more specifically, a 22–27% visually-induced proprioceptive bias in the Mirror condition, added to the 8–15% residual reaching bias in the No-Mirror and Mirror-Passive conditions), along with the lack of visual feedback concerning the reaching movement itself therefore concords well with Rossetti et al.’s findings.

In a study of the effects of adaptation to prism-induced shifts of the visual field on the ability to match the position of left and right index fingers, Mon-Williams, Wann, Jenkinson, and Rushton (1997) found that matching errors were lower for an active matching task than for a more passive matching task. When participants moved their visible right hand to a target, and then moved their unseen left hand to match the position of the right hand under a table, the matching error was 63 % of the visual matching error (which was fully biased by the prismatic shift). When the right hand was placed on the target by the experimenter (passive condition), the matching error was now biased 90 % toward the visual displacement. The finding of an increased ability to localise body parts when moved actively in Mon-Williams and colleagues’ study contrasts with the decreased reaching accuracy we found when participants moved their fingers actively prior to reaching. This dissociation of the effects of active and passive movements on visual capture and sensorimotor adaptation to prismatic displacement suggests these two processes may depend on quite different neural systems (see Holmes & Spence, 2004, for a discussion of a variety of methodological approaches to these issues; and Welch, Widawski, Harrington, & Warren, 1979).

Our findings raise important theoretical questions concerning the relationship between visual capture induced by different means. Hay, Pick, and Ikeda (1963) demonstrated visual capture of felt arm position when participants wore displacing prisms over their eyes, and there are more recent examples of this phenomenon (e.g., Mon-Williams et al., 1997). Another form of visual capture is that induced by artificial body parts. This phenomenon was first studied by Tastevin in 1937, who reported that observers tended to attribute proprioceptive sensations to a visible body part (an artificial finger), when their own corresponding body part was not visible. This visual ‘capturing’ of proprioception by artificial body parts has been demonstrated more recently by Botvinick and Cohen (1998), who replicated Tastevin’s findings based solely on subjective report, and demonstrated a significant empirical effect of visual capture on reaching performance. Pavani, Spence, and Driver (2000) have studied this phenomenon using a visuotactile interference paradigm. They examined the effect of visual distractors on tactile discrimination reaction times when the distractors were presented near a rubber arm that was either in an anatomically realistic alignment with the participant’s own body or in an unrealistic position. When the rubber arm was in alignment with the participant’s body, the effect of the visual distractors was significantly stronger than when the arms were not aligned, suggesting that an artificial, but plausibly aligned, arm can visually capture tactile stimuli. These recent methodological approaches could be integrated into the current mirror experiments – what is the effect of seeing a rubber arm in the mirror in place of one’s own arm? If there is some visual capture with a rubber arm, then what is the effect of a misaligned, alien, or asymmetric rubber arm on this phenomenon (see Austen, Soto-Faraco, Eich, Enns, & Kingstone, 2003; Austen, Soto-Faraco, Pinel, & Kingstone, 2001; Pavani et al., 2000; and see Niebauer, Aselage, & Schutte, 2002; and Peled, Ritsmer, Hirschmann, Geva, & Modai, 2000, for participant-specific influences on the rubber-hand illusion)?

The present experiment presents a new kind of visual capture induced by a mirror (see also Nielsen, 1963; and Ro, Wallace, Hagedorn, Farnè, & Pienkos, 2004, for related findings). This method has several benefits over the prismatic displacement or artificial body part techniques used in many previous studies. First, during prismatic displacement, the whole visual field is usually shifted by an equal amount in one direction, which does not allow a body-part specific manipulation of visual and proprioceptive information. Visual capture due to prism adaptation could therefore affect all subsequent pointing movements, not just those concerned with individual body parts, although the method of Rossetti et al. (1995) overcomes this problem to some extent. Moreover, several different neural mechanisms may be implicated in prism-adaptation, such as those involved in the control of eye-movements and hand-eye coordination, as well as those concerned with representing the position of individual body parts, or representing allocentric visual space (see Harris, 1965, for a review of various sensorimotor consequences of displaced vision). Second, the use of a mirror to present visual information regarding limb position allows for the possibility of studying active movements made out-of sight, while a passive limb is still always visible. The use of mirrors, prisms, and artificial body parts to investigate the different sensorimotor mechanisms contributing to the visual capture of somatosensory and proprioceptive information may best be used in conjunction on similar tasks (see Holmes & Spence, 2004; Maravita et al., 2003). It might be, for instance, that these three experimental techniques affect independent sensorimotor systems, and are thus not directly comparable. Alternatively, they may all influence the same sensorimotor system or systems and the findings of one task may be replicated exactly using a different methodology. The truth probably lies somewhere between these extremes, and the different manipulations presumably make different demands on different, but closely interacting sensorimotor systems.

Neural substrates of rubber-arm and mirror illusions?

What might be the neural correlates of the behavioral effects we have found here? Several interesting properties of single cells in the posterior parietal cortex of macaque monkeys may be of relevance to these behavioral effects. For example, Graziano, Cooke, and Taylor (2000) recorded from single cells in posterior parietal cortex (Brodmann’s area 5) of macaque monkeys, which seemed to represent specific arm positions. Single neurons in this cortical area were shown to fire more strongly, for example, when the monkey’s left arm was positioned on the left side, than when it was positioned on the right side. Some of these cells would also increase their response when a taxidermied left monkey arm was presented on the left side, but not to a stuffed right arm presented on the left side – that is, these cells were sensitive to the apparent ‘mirror reversal’ of the arm. It is tempting to speculate that cells in the human posterior parietal cortex may also be responsible for mediating the behavioral effects of the mirror illusion reported here. When presented with a visual image of the (apparent) right arm, in a position on the right side of space, such cells would increase their firing rates accordingly, signalling the apparent position of the right arm. Other cells, which are not sensitive to the discrepant visual information (approximately half of all cells in the study by Graziano et al., 2000), would signal the proprioceptively-specified position of the right arm. If the two sources of information were congruent subsequent reaching accuracy would be unaffected. If there was an inconsistency, however, reaching movements might be biased according to the strength of the two signals, and according to other signals arising before or during the subsequent reaching movement.

Summary and conclusions

The results of the present study illustrate the dominance of visual information over other sources of positional information when we view our body in a mirror. One consequence of this visual dominance is that participants subjectively reported that their mirror-reflected left hand, when actively moved in mirror symmetry and in synchrony with their unseen right hand, felt like it was their right hand. Another consequence, closely correlated with the subjective impression of hand position, is that reaching movements to an unseen target were significantly affected by the ‘mirror illusion.’ It seems that, even though we may ‘know’ that our hand is in one position, we cannot help but be influenced by the apparent sight of it in another position. This plasticity of the brain’s multisensory representation of the body in space may explain the remarkable capacity of humans and some other primates both to guide their own movements and even to recognise themselves in the mirror.

Footnotes

N.P.H. is supported by a Wellcome Prize Studentship (number 065696/Z/01/A) from The Wellcome Trust. Correspondence regarding this article should be addressed to Nicholas Holmes, Room B121, Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Oxford, Oxford, UK. OX1 3UD. E-mail: nicholas.holmes@psy.ox.ac.uk.

References

  1. Altschuler EL, Wisdom SB, Stone L, Foster C, Galasko D, Llewellyn DM, Ramachandran VS. Rehabilitation of hemiparesis after stroke with a mirror. Lancet. 1999;353:2035–2036. doi: 10.1016/s0140-6736(99)00920-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Austen EL, Soto-Faraco S, Eich E, Enns JT, Kingstone AF. Limits to body schema remapping in the fake hand illusion. Abstracts of the Psychonomic Society. 2003;8:112. [Google Scholar]
  3. Austen EL, Soto-Faraco S, Pinel JPJ, Kingstone AF. Virtual body effect: Factors influencing visual-tactile integration. Abstracts of the Psychonomic Society. 2001;6:2. [Google Scholar]
  4. Binkofski F, Buccino G, Dohle C, Seitz RJ, Freund H. Mirror agnosia and mirror ataxia constitute different parietal lobe disorders. Annals of Neurology. 1999;46:51–61. doi: 10.1002/1531-8249(199907)46:1<51::aid-ana9>3.0.co;2-q. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Botvinick M, Cohen J. Rubber hands ‘feel’ touch that eyes see. Nature. 1998;391:756. doi: 10.1038/35784. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Burnett CT. Studies in the influence of abnormal position upon the motor impulse. Psychological Review. 1904;11:370–394. [Google Scholar]
  7. Graziano MSA, Cooke DF, Taylor CSR. Coding the location of the arm by sight. Science. 2000;290:1782–1786. doi: 10.1126/science.290.5497.1782. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Gregory, R. L. (1996). Mirrors in mind New York: Freeman.
  9. Harris CS. Perceptual adaptation to inverted, reversed and displaced vision. Psychological Review. 1965;72:419–444. doi: 10.1037/h0022616. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Hay JC, Pick HL, Jr, Ikeda K. Visual capture produced by prism spectacles. Psychonomic Science. 1963;2:215–216. [Google Scholar]
  11. Holmes, N. P., & Spence, C. (2004). Body schema and body image: Toward a taxonomy of multisensory bodily manipulations Manuscript submitted for publication.
  12. Hunter JP, Katz J, Davis KD. The effect of tactile and visual sensory inputs on phantom limb awareness. Brain. 2003;126:579–589. doi: 10.1093/brain/awg054. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Jackson CV, Zangwill OL. Experimental finger dyspraxia. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. 1953;4:1–10. [Google Scholar]
  14. Lajoie Y, Paillard J, Teasdale N, Bard C, Fleury M, Forget R, Lamarre Y. Mirror drawing in a deafferented patient and normal subjects: Visuoproprioceptive conflict. Neurology. 1992;42:1104–1106. doi: 10.1212/wnl.42.5.1104. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Maravita A, Spence C, Driver J. Multisensory integration and the body schema: Close to hand and within reach. Current Biology. 2003;13:R531–R539. doi: 10.1016/s0960-9822(03)00449-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Mon-Williams M, Wann JP, Jenkinson M, Rushton K. Synaesthesia in the normal limb. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, B: Biological Sciences. 1997;264:1007–1010. doi: 10.1098/rspb.1997.0139. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Niebauer CL, Aselage J, Schutte C. Hemispheric interaction and consciousness: Degree of handedness predicts the intensity of a sensory illusion. Laterality. 2002;7:85–96. doi: 10.1080/13576500143000159. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Nielsen TI. Volition: A new experimental approach. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology. 1963;4:225–230. [Google Scholar]
  19. Pavani F, Spence C, Driver J. Visual capture of touch: Out-of-the-body experiences with rubber gloves. Psychological Science. 2000;11:353–359. doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.00270. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Peled A, Ritsner M, Hirschmann S, Geva AB, Modai I. Touch feel illusion in schizophrenic patients. Biological Psychiatry. 2000;48:1105–1108. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(00)00947-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Ramachandran VS, Altschuler EL, Hillyer S. Mirror agnosia. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, B: Biological Sciences. 1997;264:645–647. doi: 10.1098/rspb.1997.0091. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Ramachandran VS, Rogers-Ramachandran D. Synaesthesia in phantom limbs induced with mirrors. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, B: Biological Sciences. 1996;263:377–386. doi: 10.1098/rspb.1996.0058. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Ramachandran VS, Rogers-Ramachandran D, Cobb S. Touching the phantom limb. Nature. 1995;377:489–490. doi: 10.1038/377489a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Ro T, Wallace R, Hagedorn JC, Farnè A, Pienkos E. Visual enhancing of tactile perception in posterior parietal cortex. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 2004;16:24–30. doi: 10.1162/089892904322755520. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Rossetti Y, Desmurget M, Prablanc C. Vectorial coding of movement: Vision, proprioception, or both? Journal of Neurophysiology. 1995;74:457–463. doi: 10.1152/jn.1995.74.1.457. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Sathian K, Greenspan AI, Wolf SL. Doing it with mirrors: A case study of a novel approach to neurorehabilitation. Neurorehabilitation and Neural Repair. 2000;14:73–76. doi: 10.1177/154596830001400109. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Tastevin J. En partent de l’experience d’Aristotle: Les déplacements artificiels des parties du corps ne sont pas suivis par le sentiment de ces parties ni pas les sensations qu’on peut y produire [Starting from Aristotle’s experiment: The artificial displacements of parts of the body are not followed by feeling in these parts or by the sensations which can be produced there]. . L’Encephale. 1937;32:57–84. 140–58. [Google Scholar]
  28. Welch RB, Widawski MH, Harrington J, Warren DH. An examination of the relationship between visual capture and prism adaptation. Perception & Psychophysics. 1979;25:126–132. doi: 10.3758/bf03198798. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES