Abstract
Peritraumatic responses, aside from dissociation, have been understudied in acute trauma populations. Participants were 172 female rape, 68 assault, and 80 robbery victims recruited through formal reporting agencies and assessed one month post-crime. Despite substantial overlap across crimes, rape victims reported more emotional responses reflecting fear, detachment, shame, and more non-active behavioral responses. Regression analysis examining the prediction of perceived threat by peritraumatic responses and crime variables indicated that increased duration of crimes, decreased calmness, increased fear, numbing, use of begging, pleading and crying, and attempts to reason with the perpetrator(s) were all significantly associated with increased appraisal of threat.
Keywords: peritraumatic responses, victim reactions, resistance, crime victims, sexual assault
PERITRAUMATIC RESPONSES AND THEIR RELATIONSHIP TO PERCEPTIONS OF THREAT IN FEMALE CRIME VICTIMS
In recent years there has been growing interest among trauma researchers in victims’ immediate reactions during a trauma, generally termed peritraumatic responses. The findings of several studies suggest that peritraumatic responses may influence victims’ posttraumatic distress (e.g., Bernat, Ronfeldt, Calhoun, & Arias, 1998; Marmar, Weiss, Schengler et al., 1994) and, in some cases, may contribute to victim-blaming (Kowalski, 1992; Shotland & Goodstein, 1992). However, few studies have examined the range of peritraumatic responses that victims experience, and even fewer have examined the peritraumatic responses of female crime victims. A more complete understanding of women’s peritraumatic responses is important for several reasons. First, there is evidence that women are more prone than men to develop posttraumatic stress symptoms after a traumatic experience (Kessler, Sonnega, Bromet, Hughes, & Nelson, 1995; Norris, 1992). In fact, the findings of several investigations of female rape victims indicate that emotional, dissociative (e.g., numbing), and behavioral peritraumatic responses are associated with subsequent posttraumatic stress symptoms (Bart & O'Brien, 1985; Gershuny, Cloitre, & Otto, 2003; Girelli, Resick, Marhoefer-Dvorak, & Hutter, 1986; Resick & Gerrol, 1988; Resick, Churchill, & Falsetti, 1990; Resnick, 1997). Victims’ peritraumatic perception of threat has been found to be an especially strong predictor of later distress (Bernat et al., 1998; Girelli et al., 1986). Gaining a better understanding of women’s peritraumatic responses could yield valuable information about the development of posttraumatic distress which, in turn, could lead to better prevention and treatment efforts.
In addition, peritraumatic responses of female victims may be viewed differently depending on the type of crime they experience. Compared to victims of most other crimes, rape victims are more likely to be blamed by others if their within-trauma behavior is not in keeping with cultural stereotypes about how “real rape victims” respond (Burt, 1991; Estrich, 1987; Rozee & Koss, 2001). That is, women who do not physically resist their attackers are more likely to be blamed for rape than are women who do physically resist (Branscombe & Weir, 1992; Estrich, 1987; Kowalski, 1992; Langley, Yost, O'Neal, & Taylor, 1991; Shotland & Goodstein, 1992; Wyer, Bodenhaus, & Gorman, 1985). Rape victims’ responses may be scrutinized and viewed as a means by which to determine the legitimacy of the rape (Whatley, 1996). For some, this scrutiny may be self-imposed; rape victims often feel that they are somehow to blame for what has happened to them (Calhoun & Townsley, 1991; Koss & Burkhart, 1989; Koss, Figueredo, & Prince, 2002; Resick & Schnicke, 1993). In addition, without normative data on victim responses, therapists may be less effective at challenging clients’ self-blame. Indeed, therapists themselves may hold stereotyped views about victim responses (Dye & Roth, 1990; White & Kurpius, 1999). Thus, an understanding of rape victims’ peritraumatic responses is relevant to both treatment concerns and post-crime social support.
Although few studies have focused specifically on peritraumatic responses, data from two different lines of research suggest that women have a broad range of responses during a crime. These lines of research include the literature on rape resistance and the literature on the influence of peritraumatic responses on later distress. Within the rape resistance literature, a few studies have reported the percentage of women who display various types of resistance (for a review, see Rozee & Koss, 2001), but resistance behaviors tend to be categorized using broad terms (e.g., verbal or physical resistance). Thus, little is known about the percentage of women who display specific behavioral responses. Much of what is known about other peritraumatic responses (e.g., emotional, cognitive, dissociative) has come out of research on peritraumatic responses as predictors of subsequent distress.
Among several peritraumatic responses that have been investigated, perceived threat has emerged as a particularly robust predictor of later distress. There is evidence that victims’ peritraumatic perception of threat of serious harm or death is an equally if not more powerful predictor of subsequent distress than are severity indices such as injury and the assailant's use of a weapon (e.g., Bernat et al., 1998; Girelli et al., 1986). Injury and perceived threat have both been found to predict distress and/or posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD; Acierno, Resnick, Kilpatrick, Saunders, & Best, 1999; Culbertson & Dehle, 2001; Davis, Taylor, & Lurigio, 1996; Epstein, Saunders, & Kilpatrick, 1997; Kilpatrick, Saunders, Amick-McMullen, Best, Veronen, & Resnick, 1989; Ullman & Filipas, 2001). In the study by Kilpatrick et al. (1989), perceived threat and actual injury were related, but the two variables independently predicted crime-related PTSD development in a hierarchical regression model. Girelli et al. (1986) found that peritraumatic distress and perceived threat were better predictors of subsequent posttraumatic stress symptoms than were assault variables such as perpetrator threats, weapons, and injuries. While peritraumatic perception of threat appears to be an important influence on subsequent distress, it is not yet clearly understood what factors influence women’s perception of threat.
The findings of several studies suggest that perception of threat is related to other peritraumatic responses as well as certain crime variables. Griffin, Resick, and Mechanic (1997) found that rape survivors who reported high levels of peritraumatic dissociation were more likely than low dissociators to report having high levels of perceived threat during a rape. In a large sample of college students, Bernat and colleagues (1998) examined gender, frequency of lifetime trauma exposure, peritraumatic emotions, peritraumatic dissociation, peritraumatic panic symptoms, and trauma severity (i.e., injury, witnessing harm to another, and perceived threat of death) as predictors of PTSD. PTSD was most strongly predicted by the victim’s number of lifetime traumatic experiences and perceived threat of death. The other indices of severity, specifically, injury and witnessing harm to another, did not predict PTSD. Moreover, peritraumatic emotional responses and dissociation made significant contributions in the prediction of PTSD above and beyond the contribution of vulnerability factors (i.e., being female, number of past traumas) and the severity of traumatic event. All of the peritraumatic responses investigated were interrelated. In addition, there is evidence that women’s peritraumatic perceptions of threat mediate the relationship between peritraumatic dissociation and PTSD. Specifically, Gershuny et al. (2003) found that the effect of peritraumatic dissociation on PTSD severity appeared to be indirect and explained by peritraumatic fears of death and lack of control. These findings suggest that perceived threat occurs in conjunction with other peritraumatic responses.
Victims’ perception of threat may also be influenced by the nature of the crime itself. Using data from one of the samples used in the present study, Resick and Gerrol (1988) found that, compared to female robbery victims, rape victims reported a greater perception of threat and more passive behavioral responses than did robbery victims. The authors hypothesized that some of the differences in responses between rape and robbery victims may have been due to differences in the perception of threat elicited by rape relative to that elicited by robbery.
To summarize, while individual studies have each focused on a limited number of peritraumatic responses (e.g., rape resistance alone, dissociation alone) the sum of findings from these studies suggests that women experience a variety of peritraumatic responses. A number of these responses predict subsequent distress, and perception of threat has consistently been found to play a significant role. Perception of threat appears to be related to other peritraumatic responses as well as characteristics of the crime itself. However, the nature of these relationships is not clear.
The purpose of the current study was to investigate the range of women’s peritraumatic responses to different crimes and the percentage of women who display particular responses. In an attempt to identify variables that may influence perceived threat, this study also investigated the relationship of peritraumatic responses and crime variables to women’s perception of threat of serious harm or death across three types of crimes: rape, robbery, and assault.
Method
Participants
Participants were included who had experienced a rape, robbery, or physical assault within two weeks of calling for an appointment. Rape was defined as any crime involving vaginal, oral, or anal penetration. First-degree physical assault and robbery were defined according to Missouri legal code. First-degree physical assault was therefore defined as a physical assault where the participant experienced an injury or felt that the perpetrator was trying to kill or injure them. Robbery was defined as a crime where the perpetrator forcibly stole the participant’s property. The only exclusion criteria for this study were illiteracy, apparent psychosis, or intoxication at the time of the assessment (for informed consent and validity purposes). Illiteracy was determined based on the participants’ ability to read and explain the consent form. Intoxication and psychosis were assessed based on interviewer clinical judgment.
This study consists of two samples from two community studies. Sample 1 consists of 51 female rape and 80 female robbery victims who were assessed within one month after the crime. Sample 2 consists of 68 female victims of first-degree physical assault and 121 female victims of rape assessed within one month after the crime.
Procedure
Participants were recruited through police departments, hospitals, and victim assistance agencies. They were informed of the project and were asked to call or return a postcard if they were interested in participating. Cards describing the study were given to crime victims by police officers responding at the scene or by victim service volunteers at the hospital emergency room or victim service agencies. In addition, the St. Louis City Police Department mailed postcards describing the study to victims who had filed a police report. Both mailed and hand-delivered cards described the research, provided the researchers' phone number, and included a return postcard the victims could mail to learn more about the study or schedule an appointment to participate.
For both samples, potential participants who returned postcards were contacted by phone or mail within two-weeks after the crime. If interested in participating, they were scheduled for interviews within four weeks post-crime. With regard to sampling, one variable, age, was controlled within the first sample. Pilot data indicated that robbery victims were somewhat older than rape victims; therefore, robbery victims over 35 were contacted to participate only when a rape victim over 35 participated in the project. For both samples, individuals who could not be scheduled within four weeks post-crime were not invited to participate or were dropped from the study.
Instruments
Participants were given an extensive battery of psychological inventories. The measure relevant to these analyses is the Trauma Interview.
Trauma Interview (Resick, 1986; Resick, Jordan, Girelli, Hutter, & Marhoefer-Dvorak, 1988). This structured interview yielded descriptive information on the current trauma including: circumstances of the crime, assailant information, restraint or violence during the crime, victim responses during the crime, and actual injuries sustained during the crime. Two questions assessed the victim's perceptions of risk of death or injury: “During the incident, did you think about being killed or seriously injured?” (responses ranged from 0 = “not at all” to 4 = “thought about it all the time”) and “During the incident how certain were you that you were going to be killed?” (responses ranged from 0 = “completely certain that I would not be killed” to 4 = “completely certain that I would be killed”). The scores on these two items were summed to generate a perceived threat score for each participant such that the range of possible scores was 0–8. Twenty-one questions assessed victim responses during the crime and included emotional and behavioral responses. Items were coded using a Likert-type scale ranging from 0–4 (i.e., 0 = “None of the time”, 4 = “All of the time”). One item, pertaining to whether the victim had used a weapon during the assault, was endorsed by less than 10% of the sample and was thus dropped from any further analysis. The total scale assessing within-trauma responses had a Cronbach’s alpha of .74.
Results
Participants
Demographic information for both samples is presented in Table 1. For sample 1, there were no significant age, race, or educational differences between the rape and robbery groups. However, there were more single rape than robbery victims, χ2 (1, n = 131) = 17.75, p<.001. Furthermore, there were no significant differences between rape and robbery victims in the relationship of the assailant to the victim or the use of a weapon. As can be seen in Table 1, most of the perpetrators were strangers to the victims. The majority of the crimes involved the use or display of a weapon and involved only one perpetrator. More robberies than rapes involved more than one perpetrator, t (128) = −3.77, p<.001 and there was more use of physical restraint during the crime for rape victims, χ2 (1, n = 130) = 35.69, p<.001.
Table 1.
Sample 1 |
Sample 2 |
|||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Rape | Robbery | Rape | Assault | |||||
n | % | n | % | n | % | n | % | |
Age | ||||||||
17–30 | 38 | 75 | 51 | 64 | 74 | 61* | 27 | 40* |
31–45 | 10 | 20 | 25 | 31 | 41 | 34 | 34 | 50 |
46-Over 65 | 3 | 6 | 4 | 5 | 6 | 5 | 7 | 10 |
Marital Status | ||||||||
Married/Cohabitating | 1 | 2 | 26 | 33 | 9 | 8 | 15 | 23 |
Currently Single | 50 | 98** | 54 | 68** | 110 | 92* | 51 | 77* |
Race | ||||||||
African-American | 23 | 45 | 33 | 41 | 36 | 30 | 15 | 22 |
Caucasian | 28 | 55 | 47 | 59 | 85 | 70 | 53 | 78 |
Assailant | ||||||||
Known | 13 | 26 | 11 | 14 | 54 | 45 | 37 | 57 |
Stranger | 37 | 74 | 69 | 86 | 67 | 55 | 28 | 43 |
Displayed a weapon | 33 | 75 | 48 | 61 | 61 | 51*** | 55 | 81*** |
Use of Restraint | 40 | 80*** | 21 | 26*** | 96 | 81*** | 20 | 30*** |
Years Education - Mean (sd) | 12.8 (2.8) | 12.8 (4.1) | 12.5 (2.1) | 12.4 (2.6) | ||||
Injury - Mean (sd) | 26.3 (9.8) | 22.8 (9.7) | 17.4 (1.6)** | 18.5 (2.6)** | ||||
Number Assailants - Mean (sd) | 1.1 (.4)*** | 1.5 (.7)*** | 1.4 (.9) | 1.5 (.9) |
Note:
p<.05.
p<.01.
p<.001
For sample 2, there were no group differences on race or education. However, more rape than assault victims were ages 17 to 30, χ2 (2, n = 189) = 8.40, p<.05, and there were more single rape victims, χ2 (1, n = 185) = 8.65, p<.01. There were no significant differences between the rape and assault groups in the relationship of the assailant to the victim or the number of perpetrators. Slightly over half of the perpetrators were known to the victims, and the majority of the crimes involved only one perpetrator. More of the physical assaults involved the use or display of a weapon, χ2 (1, n = 188) = 16.59, p<.001, and involved more injuries than in the rape group, t(169) = 3.69, p<.001. There was significantly more use of physical restraint within the rape group χ2 (1, n = 185) = 48.48, p<.001.
Rape victims in sample 1 and sample 2 were compared on demographic and assault characteristics. There were no significant differences between the two samples on age, marital status, race, education, or in the use of restraint during the assault; however, there were significant differences across the samples in some crime characteristics. Specifically, sample 1 had a greater proportion of stranger assaults than sample 2, χ2(1, n = 171) = 5.15, p<.05, had a greater proportion of assaults where a weapon was present, χ2(1, n = 164) = 7.69, p<.01, and had more severe injuries than in sample 2, t(158) =8.98, p<.001. Individuals in sample 2 were more likely to have more than one assailant, t(169) = −2.40, p<.05. Given that there were no significant demographic differences between the two samples and that the differences in crime characteristics were seen as creating a more representative sexual assault sample, the two samples were combined into a single group for further analyses.
Responses by Type of Crime
In order to investigate sexual trauma as a characteristic that distinguishes rape from other types of crime (i.e., assault, robbery), the percentage of women reporting each response was calculated according to the type of crime. Peritraumatic responses were initially compared according to whether victims were rape or robbery/assault (R/A) victims. Responses were collapsed across the 5 point Likert scales and were counted as having occurred if women reported experiencing them “A little of the time” to “All of the time”. Chi-square analyses were conducted to determine if there were significant differences in the percentage of women reporting each peritraumatic response. The results of these analyses are presented in Table 2. As can be seen in the table, rape victims were significantly more likely than R/A victims to report that they felt betrayed; afraid; detached, as if in a dream; humiliated; and numb during the crime. They were also more likely to report that they begged, pleaded, or cried; tried to reason with the assailant; tried to struggle free; kept quiet and motionless; and did exactly as told. Rape victims were significantly less likely than R/A victims to report that they felt calm. There were no group differences on feeling angry, anxious, and confused, or on kicking, hitting, or punching; screaming or yelling for help; biting or scratching; or passing out. In addition, high percentages of rape and R/A survivors reported that they perceived the threat of serious harm or injury during the crime, but there was no significant difference between the groups.
Table 2.
Percent Reporting | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|
Response | df | χ2 | ||
Rape | R/A | |||
Emotional | ||||
Afraid | 97.2 | 90.6 | 1,330 | 6.6** |
Angry | 86.2 | 88.0 | 1,331 | .24 |
Anxious | 89.5 | 82.0 | 1,331 | 3.9 |
Betrayed | 86.2 | 75.0 | 1,329 | 6.6** |
Calm | 26.0 | 41.3 | 1,331 | 8.8** |
Confused | 81.9 | 74.7 | 1,332 | 2.5 |
Detached | 73.9 | 61.3 | 1,330 | 5.9* |
Guilty | 52.9 | 24.4 | 1, 331 | 29.9*** |
Humiliated | 91.1 | 68.7 | 1,330 | 26.7*** |
Numb | 75.3 | 64.0 | 1,332 | 5.0* |
Behavioral | ||||
Begged, pleaded, cried | 87.9 | 36.3 | 1,328 | 94.8*** |
Bit/scratched | 22.7 | 19.2 | 1,327 | .59 |
Cursed/threatened | 32.4 | 30.1 | 1, 325 | .19 |
Did exactly as told | 79.9 | 45.3 | 1,316 | 40.8*** |
Kicked, hit, punched | 37.6 | 28.6 | 1,328 | 2.9 |
Passed out | 9.9 | 10.5 | 1,347 | .02 |
Tried to reason | 81.3 | 39.2 | 1,325 | 60.1*** |
Kept quiet/motionless | 74.0 | 41.5 | 1,328 | 35.6*** |
Screamed/yelled for help | 49.7 | 50.0 | 1,329 | .002 |
Tried to struggle free | 74.2 | 45.8 | 1,326 | 27.3*** |
Perceived threat of serious injury or death | 89.9 | 87.7 | 1,316 | .38 |
Note:
p <.05.
p <.01.
p < .001.
Perceived Threat and Within-Crime Variables
One of the aims of this study was to examine the relationship of peritraumatic responses to perceptions of threat within a heterogeneous population of female crime victims. For this purpose, the rape and R/A groups were collapsed for the regression analysis. A hierarchical multiple regression analysis was performed to examine the relationship between perceived threat of harm or injury and the following within-crime characteristics: rape, crime variables, peritraumatic emotional responses, and peritraumatic behavioral responses. The first block consisted of whether the crime was a rape, to examine differences in perceived threat by type of crime (i.e., rape versus robbery or assault). Five crime variables were entered in the second block including: acquaintance status, duration of crime, level of injury, restraint during the crime, and display of a weapon. In the third block, peritraumatic emotions were entered, and in the fourth block, peritraumatic behaviors were entered. The rationale for entering peritraumatic emotions before behaviors is based on current theory that fear precedes other responses including behaviors (Foa, Riggs, & Gershuny, 1995). The results of this analysis are presented in Table 3.
Table 3.
Step and variable | B | SEB | β | R2 | ΔR2 |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Step 1 | .08*** | ||||
Sexual Assault | .44 | .87 | .04 | ||
Step 2 | .17*** | .09*** | |||
Assailant | −1.08 | .73 | −.10 | ||
Display of Weapon | 1.11 | .71 | .10 | ||
Injury | −.07 | .04 | −.10 | ||
Duration | .59 | .25 | .16* | ||
Restraint | −1.08 | .79 | −.10 | ||
Step 3 | .29*** | .12*** | |||
Afraid | .82 | .30 | .19** | ||
Angry | .09 | .23 | .03 | ||
Anxious | .05 | .22 | .02 | ||
Betrayed | −.06 | .23 | −.02 | ||
Calm | −.53 | .26 | −.12* | ||
Confused | −.18 | .22 | −.05 | ||
Detached | .13 | .20 | .04 | ||
Guilty | −.13 | .23 | −.04 | ||
Humiliated | −.007 | .22 | −.002 | ||
Numb | .46 | .22 | .14* | ||
Step 4 | .34*** | .06* | |||
Begged, pleaded, cried | .57 | .27 | .18* | ||
Bit/scratched | −.07 | .36 | −.02 | ||
Cursed/threatened | −.03 | .25 | −.008 | ||
Did exactly as told | .12 | .25 | .04 | ||
Kicked, hit, punched | .37 | .34 | .10 | ||
Passed out | .68 | .53 | .07 | ||
Tried to reason | .44 | .23 | .14* | ||
Kept quiet/motionless | −.01 | .25 | −.004 | ||
Screamed/yelled for help | .05 | .26 | .01 | ||
Tried to struggle free | −.21 | .28 | −.06 |
Note.
p <.05.
p <.01.
p < .001.
In the analysis, sexual assault predicted 8% of the variance in perceived threat and crime characteristics predicted an additional 9% of the variance. Peritraumatic emotions contributed an additional 12% of the variance and peritraumatic behaviors contributed an additional 6% of the variance in perceived threat. The final regression equation predicted 34% of the variance in perceived threat. As shown in Table 3, the variables with significant standardized beta scores in the final equation were: duration of assault, fear, calm (in reverse direction), numbing, trying to reason with the perpetrator(s), and begging, pleading, or crying.
Discussion
The results of this study suggest that female crime victims display a range of emotional, cognitive, and behavioral peritraumatic responses. The majority of women in both the rape and R/A groups reported peritraumatic emotional responses that reflect distress and negative arousal (e.g., confusion, anxiety, anger) as well as the perception of imminent harm or death. Approximately one-third of women reported behaviors traditionally viewed as comprising active physical resistance (i.e., biting, cursing, kicking). Despite some commonalties in response patterns, women’s peritraumatic responses tended to vary with the type of crime experienced. Compared to robbery and assault victims, a higher percentage of rape victims reported behavioral responses that were relatively non-active and characterized by strategies that seem to reflect attempts at negotiation (e.g., begged, reasoned). Rape victims displayed a concomitant set of emotional responses that reflect fear, emotional detachment, and shame (e.g., afraid, betrayal, detached, numb, humiliation, guilty). In addition, the results of a regression analysis indicated that perceived threat of imminent harm is related to only one crime variable (i.e., crime duration), three peritraumatic emotions (i.e., feeling afraid, numb, and less calm), and two peritraumatic behaviors (i.e., begging, pleading, or crying and trying to reason with the perpetrator) across all three types of crimes. Interestingly, none of the other crime variables examined (e.g., injury, weapon) were associated with perceived threat nor was the type of crime, once peritraumatic responses were entered into the equation.
In this sample, high percentages of victims reported peritraumatic distress and concerns that they would be harmed or killed and low percentages reported having engaged in active physical resistance. All of the crimes investigated in this study are violent by definition, and these findings suggest that women share some fundamental responses to such crimes. However, rape and robbery/assault victims differed in terms of how often they reported experiencing several peritraumatic responses. Although emotions reflecting general distress were common to both groups, rape victims were more apt to describe emotions that reflect fear and detachment and that seem to connote a sense of personal responsibility or shame felt by the victim. The differences in peritraumatic responses between rape and non-rape victims suggests that crimes that involve rape may elicit different responses than other crimes
The finding that only one-third of rape victims physically fought with the assailant is different from other reports including Koss’ (1988) finding that 70% of date rape victims attempted to fight back physically. However, this difference may be accounted for by sampling. Koss’ study was restricted to perpetrations by known assailants and the sample was exclusively college students. In the current study, the majority of assailants were strangers and the sample was more heterogenous. Rape victims in the current study were also highly likely to use non-active resistance methods, including begging, crying, and pleading and trying to reason with the assailant. These results also highlight the discrepancy between actual behavior during a rape and findings that resistance during a sexual assault may have a positive effect on the outcome. That is, previous research has found that many women report that they believe an attempt to fight off a rapist would not work; rather, it would make things worse (Gordon & Riger, 1989; Furby, Fischhoff, & Morgan, 1989; Norris, Nurius, & Dimeff, 1996). This is in spite of the fact that the research literature documents a consistent finding that resistance may prevent rape and does not pose an additional risk of injury (see reviews by Rozee & Koss, 2001, and Ullman, 1997). This discrepancy highlights a need for better education and rape prevention strategies.
The different response patterns of rape and non-rape victims have implications in terms of how they are judged by others. As previously noted, rape victims who do not physically resist their attackers are more likely to be blamed than are women who do physically resist (Branscombe & Weir, 1992; Estrich, 1987; Krulewitz & Nash, 1979; Langley et al., 1991; Shotland & Goodstein, 1992; Wyer et al., 1985). Used as a means by which to measure the violence and severity of the crime, victim resistance may be conceptualized as a means by which observers determine whether or what occurred was a rape. Such judgments may influence jurors’ verdicts and judges’ decisions regarding punishment (Langley et al., 1991), as well as police decisions regarding whether or not a case should be investigated or deemed “unfound”.
Clearly, the findings of this study are not congruent with cultural myths about how “real rape victims” respond. A common rape myth is that “any healthy women can resist a rapist if she really wants to” (Burt, 1991, p. 31). The corollary is that if she got raped, she must not have resisted enough, and therefore must have wanted it or consented (Burt, 1991). Judging from the high percentages of rape victims who thought that they would be harmed or killed, it is clear that these women were not consenting. Rather, they were afraid and concerned about being seriously harmed or killed. In short, the findings of this study indicate that little or no active resistance on the part of a rape victim should not be interpreted as consent.
Previous findings indicate that perceived threat is related to other peritraumatic responses (Gershuny et al., 2003; Griffin et al., 1997) and crime variables (Kilpatrick et al., 1989). The findings of this study provide additional insight into variables that are related to perceived threat. Perceived threat was predicted by three peritraumatic emotions: feeling afraid, numb, and less calm. These results support previous findings that perceived threat is related to peritraumatic emotions (Bernat et al., 1998; Griffin et al., 1997). As noted, Gershuny and colleagues (2003) found that perceived threat mediated the relationship between dissociation and PTSD. The authors hypothesized that trauma-related fears of death might be conceptualized as an element of panic, which leads to dissociation in the absence of an opportunity for physical escape. The findings in the current study are consistent with Gershuny's findings. Although there was no measure of temporal sequence in this study, it may be that the perception of threat functioned as a cue for feeling afraid and numb.
Regarding the relationship between duration and victim perception of imminent harm, it may be that the longer the crime, the more time a victim has to consider the possibility that she will be seriously injured or killed. Although the number of assailant threats and violent acts were not measured in this study, it may be that longer crimes were characterized by more threats or physical violence on the part of the assailant. It is interesting to note that none of the other crime variables included in the regression (e.g., injury, weapon, restraint) were associated with victims’ perception of imminent harm. Several of these severity indices have been examined in past research as predictors of post-crime distress, reflecting the assumption that the level of violence (e.g., penetration, injury) in a crime can adequately capture the victim’s emotional or cognitive reaction to it. The findings of this study suggest that traditional measures of actual violence and other crime variables (e.g., acquaintance status) may not be related to the victim’s perception of imminent harm. It is worth noting that when only crime variables had been entered into the regression, rape was a significant predictor of perceived threat. Once peritraumatic emotions were entered, rape was no longer significant. Thus, it appears that peritraumatic emotions were better than sexual assault in explaining the variance in perceived threat.
Two of the peritraumatic behavioral responses predicted perceived threat, begged, pleaded, and cried and tried to reason with him. Some researchers (e.g., Bart & O'Brien, 1985) have theorized that women's perceptions that they will be killed versus raped may elicit different behavioral responses during a rape. Specifically, women who are concerned about being killed or seriously injured are hypothesized to be more likely to respond passively (e.g., remain quiet and motionless, do as told). Women whose primary concern is not about being killed are hypothesized to be more likely to respond in a more active fashion (e.g., kicking, screaming). These findings suggest that women may be more likely to attempt to negotiate rather than to either respond passively or to physically resist, when women perceive increased threat of harm or death.
The findings of this study represent an important step toward understanding the pattern of peritraumatic responses experienced by female victims of both sexual and non-sexual violent crimes. This study and future efforts to identify normative peritraumatic responses should inform the work of legal, medical, and mental health professions who work with female crime victims. For example, normative data on rape victim responses might be used revise stereotyped notions that victim resistance is a means by which to determine the legitimacy of a rape. In addition, such data also may help therapists educate clients and challenge their self-blame more effectively.
This study is characterized by certain conditions that may limit the generalizability of the conclusions. Specifically, the sample consisted of crime victims who reported their experiences to police or other authorities. Thus, this sample overrepresented severe crimes, those that involved assaults by strangers, and the presence of a weapon. Given the severity of the crimes it is not surprising that nearly 90% of the total sample endorsed worrying to some extent that they might be killed or injured during the event. In addition, the rape sample included only completed rapes which prevents us from examining situations where women may have avoided an assault or the “near misses” (Testa, VanZile-Tamsen, Livingston, & Koss, 2004). All of these factors relating to increased severity of the crimes may have decreased the degree to which women engaged in active resistance strategies.
Furthermore, this study did not take into consideration previous trauma history which may have influenced peritraumatic responses to the current crime. Prior trauma history has been associated with increased risk of future victimization (Arata, 2000; Nishith, Mechanic, & Resick, 2000). However, the aim of this study was to examine how peritraumatic responses are related to perceived threat in a recent crime. Future research should attend to the relationship between prior trauma history and peritraumatic responses to examine whether this may help to explain whether prior trauma history peritraumatic responses are a mediator between prior trauma history and the associated increased risk of future victimization.
Despite these limitations, this study represents a important departure from previous research in this area in that it examines a broad range of specific peritraumatic responses. This study also used responses collected within one month of the crime thereby reducing the risk of errors associated with retrospective reporting. Few other studies have looked at these variables within such a short period of time after the crime. Future research on peritraumatic responses should include victims of completed and non-completed crimes, as well as victims who either did not report their crimes or who do not acknowledge that they were victimized (e.g., the unacknowledged rape victims described by Koss, 1985). Research on the temporal relationship of perception of imminent harm and other peritraumatic responses would also add to our current understanding of how and why female crime victims respond as they do. Moreover, the relationship between peritraumatic responses and the development of subsequent PTSD warrants examination. Although a number of studies have documented a relationship between peritraumatic dissociation and PTSD (e.g., Bernat et al., 1998; Gershuny et al., 2003; Marmar et al., 1994), less is known about how other peritraumatic responses affect the development of PTSD. Finally, in light of the greater scrutiny experienced by rape victims, research focusing on patterns among rape victims’ peritraumatic responses would be of particular value in efforts to educate women, helping-professionals, and the legal community about how “real rape victims” respond.
Acknowledgments
This work was supported by grants from the National Institute of Mental Health (NIH-RO1-MH6992 and NIMH-RO1-MH37296) and the National Institute of Justice (NIJ-85-IJ-CX-0042) awarded to Patricia A. Resick, Ph.D. The authors thank Katie Berezniak, Dana Cason, Kate Chard, Sarah Coller, Terese Evans, Steven Girelli, Felicia Glenn, Ronald Gerrol, Michael Griffin, Elizabeth Hekemian-Williams, Linda Housman, Katherine Kotsis Hutter, Leslie Kimball, Susan Marhoefer-Dvorak, Mindy Mechanic, Michelle Myers, Bruce Nelson, Lisa Parker, Gail Pickett, Monica Schnicke, and Terri Weaver for their assistance on the project. The authors also thank the St. Louis Police Department and victim’s advocacy agencies in St. Louis City and County for their assistance in participant recruitment.
References
- Acierno R, Resnick H, Kilpatrick DG, Saunders B, Best CL. Risk factors for rape, physical assault, and posttraumatic stress disorder in women: Examination of differential multivariate relationships. Journal of Anxiety Disorders. 1999;13:541–563. doi: 10.1016/s0887-6185(99)00030-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Arata CM. From child victim to adult victim: A model for predicting sexual revictimization. Child Maltreatment. 2000;5(1):28–38. doi: 10.1177/1077559500005001004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bart, P.B., & O'Brien, P.H. (1985). Stopping rape: Effective survival strategies New York: Pergamon.
- Bernat JA, Ronfeldt HM, Calhoun KS, Arias I. Prevalence of traumatic events and peritraumatic predictors of posttraumatic stress symptoms in a nonclinical sample of college students. Journal of Traumatic Stress. 1998;11:645–664. doi: 10.1023/A:1024485130934. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Branscombe NR, Weir JA. Resistance as stereotype inconsistency: Consequences for judgments of rape victims. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology. 1992;11:80–102. [Google Scholar]
- Burt, M.R (1991). Rape myths and acquaintance rape. In A. Parrot & L. Bechhofer (Eds.) Acquaintance rape: The hidden crime (pp. 26–40). New York: Wiley & Sons.
- Calhoun, K.S., & Townsley, R.M. (1991). Attributions of responsibility for acquaintance rape. In A. Parrot & L. Bechhofer (Eds.), Acquaintance rape: The hidden crime (pp. 57–69). New York: John Wiley.
- Culbertson KA, Dehle C. Impact of sexual assault as a function of perpetrator type. Journal of Interpersonal Violence. 2001;16:992–1007. [Google Scholar]
- Davis RC, Taylor B, Lurigio AJ. Adjusting to criminal victimization: The correlates of postcrime distress. Violence and Victims. 1996;11:21–38. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dye E, Roth S. Psychotherapists’ knowledge about and attitudes toward sexual assault victim clients. Psychology of Women Quarterly. 1990;14:191–212. [Google Scholar]
- Epstein JN, Saunders BE, Kilpatrick DG. Predicting PTSD in women with a history of childhood rape. Journal of Traumatic Stress. 1997;10:573–599. doi: 10.1023/a:1024841718677. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Estrich, S. (1987) Real rape: How the legal system victimizes women who say no. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.
- Foa EB, Riggs DS, Gershuny B. Arousal, numbing, and intrusion: Symptom structure of PTSD following assault. American Journal of Psychiatry. 1995;152:116–120. doi: 10.1176/ajp.152.1.116. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Furby L, Fischhoff B, Morgan M. Judged effectiveness of common rape prevention and self-defense strategies. Journal of Interpersonal Violence. 1989;4:44–64. [Google Scholar]
- Gershuny BS, Cloitre M, Otto MW. Peritraumatic dissociation and PTSD severity: Do event-related fears about death and control mediate their relation? Behavior Research and Therapy. 2003;41:157–166. doi: 10.1016/s0005-7967(01)00134-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Girelli SA, Resick PA, Marhoefer-Dvorak S, Hutter CK. Subjective distress and violence during rape: Their long-term effects on long-term fear. Victims and Violence. 1986;1:35–45. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gordon, M.T., & Riger, S. (1989). The Female Fear New York: Free Press.
- Griffin MG, Resick PA, Mechanic MB. Objective assessment of peritraumatic dissociation: Psychophysiological indicators. American Journal of Psychiatry. 1997;154:1081–1088. doi: 10.1176/ajp.154.8.1081. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kessler RC, Sonnega A, Bromet E, Hughes M, Nelson CB. Posttraumatic stress disorder the National Comorbidity Survey. Archives of General Psychiatry. 1995;52:1048–1060. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1995.03950240066012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kilpatrick DG, Saunders BE, Amick-McMullen A, Best CL, Veronen LG, Resnick HS. Victims and crime factors associated with the development of crime-related posttraumatic stress disorder. Behavior Therapy. 1989;20:199–214. [Google Scholar]
- Koss MP. The hidden rape victim: Personality, attitudinal, and situational characteristics. Psychology of Women Quarterly. 1985;9:193–212. [Google Scholar]
- Koss, M.P. (1988). Hidden rape: Sexual aggression and victimization in a national sample of students in higher education. In A.W. Burgess (Ed.), Rape and sexual assault (Vol. 2, pp. 3–25). New York: Garland.
- Koss MP, Burkhart BR. A conceptual analysis of rape victimization: Long-term effects and implications. Psychology of Women Quarterly. 1989;13:27–40. [Google Scholar]
- Koss MP, Figueredo AJ, Prince RJ. Cognitive mediation of rape's mental, physical and social health impact: Tests of four models in cross-sectional data. Journal of Consulting & Clinical Psychology. 2002;70(4):926–941. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kowalski RM. Nonverbal behaviors and perceptions of sexual intentions: Effects of sexual connotativeness, verbal responses, and rape outcome. Basic and Applied Social Psychology. 1992;13:427–445. [Google Scholar]
- Krulewitz JE, Nash JE. Effects of rape victim resistance, assault outcome, and sex of observer on attributions about rape. Journal of Personality. 1979;47:557–574. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.1979.tb00209.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Langley T, Yost EA, O'Neal EC, Taylor SL. Models of rape judgment; Attributions concerning event, perpetrator, and victim. Journal of Offender Rehabilitation. 1991;17:43–54. [Google Scholar]
- Marmar CR, Weiss DS, Schengler WE, Fairbank JA, Jordan BK, Kulka RA, Hough RL. Peritraumatic dissociation and posttraumatic stress in male Vietnam theater veterans. American Journal of Psychiatry. 1994;151:902–907. doi: 10.1176/ajp.151.6.902. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nishith P, Mechanic MB, Resick PA. Prior interpersonal trauma: The contribution to current PTSD symptoms in female rape victims. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2000;109(1):20–25. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Norris FH. Epidemiology of trauma: Frequency and impact of different potentially traumatic events on different groups. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 1992;60:409–418. doi: 10.1037//0022-006x.60.3.409. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Norris J, Nurius P, Dimeff L. Through her eyes: Factors affecting women's perception of resistance to sexual aggression threat. Psychology of Women Quarterly. 1996;20:123–145. doi: 10.1111/j.1471-6402.1996.tb00668.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Resick PA. Post-Traumatic Stress Disorder in a Vietnam nurse: Behavioral analysis of a case study. Women & Therapy. 1986;5:55–65. [Google Scholar]
- Resick, P.A., Churchill, M., & Falsetti, S.A. (1990, October). Assessment of cognitions in trauma victims: A pilot study Paper presented at the 6th annual meeting of the Society for Traumatic Stress Studies. New Orleans, LA.
- Resick PA, Jordan CG, Girelli SA, Hutter CK, Marhoefer-Dvorak A comparative outcome study of behavioral group therapy for sexual assault victims. Behavior Therapy. 1988;19:385–401. [Google Scholar]
- Resick, P. A., & Gerrol, R. (1988, November). The effect of within-assault cognitive appraisals, behavior, and emotions on subsequent distress in female victims of crime. In R. L. Collins (Chair), Victimization of women. Symposium conducted at the 22nd annual convention of the Association for Advancement of Behavior Therapy, New York, NY.
- Resick, P.A., & Schnicke, M.K. (1993). Cognitive processing therapy for rape victims Newbury Park, CA: Sage Publications.
- Resnick HS. Acute panic reactions among rape victims: Implications for prevention of postrape psychopathology. National Center for PTSD Quarterly. 1997;7:41–45. [Google Scholar]
- Rozee PD, Koss MP. Rape: A century of resistance. Psychology of Women Quarterly. 2001;25:295–311. [Google Scholar]
- Shotland RL, Goodstein L. Sexual precedence reduces the perceived legitimacy of sexual refusal: An examination of attributions concerning date rape and consensual sex. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. 1992;18:756–764. [Google Scholar]
- Testa M, VanZile-Tamsen C, Livingston JA, Koss MP. Assessing women's experiences of sexual aggression using the sexual experiences survey: Evidence for validity and implications for research. Psychology of Women Quarterly. 2004;28(3):256–265. [Google Scholar]
- Ullman SE. Review and critique of empirical studies of rape avoidance. Criminal Justice and Behavior. 1997;24:177–204. [Google Scholar]
- Ullman SE, Filipas HH. Predictors of PTSD symptom severity and social reactions in sexual assault victims. Journal of Traumatic Stress. 2001;14:369–389. doi: 10.1023/A:1011125220522. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Whatley MA. Victim characteristics influencing attributions of responsibility to rape victims: A meta-analysis. Aggression and Violent Behavior. 1996;1:81–91. [Google Scholar]
- White BH, Kurpius SE. Attitudes toward rape victims. Journal of Interpersonal Violence. 1999;14:989–995. [Google Scholar]
- Wyer RS, Bodenhausen GV, Gorman TF. Cognitive mediators of reactions to rape. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1985;48:324–338. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]