Skip to main content
Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior logoLink to Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior
. 1978 May;29(3):363–374. doi: 10.1901/jeab.1978.29-363

Clustering in the output of behavior1

Michael D Zeiler, E R Davis
PMCID: PMC1332836  PMID: 16812063

Abstract

Pigeons exposed to fixed-interval schedules of 3, 5, 15, 40, and 120 minutes all maintained considerable variability in daily response rates for as many as 104 sessions. However, variations did not occur at random. Instead, rate in a session appeared dependent on those occurring previously. The series displayed a wave-like form arising because a group of high rates was followed by a group of low rates and vice versa. These sequential relations produced a curve having irregular periodicity, sometimes superimposed on a declining or rising linear trend. Whether grouping of response rates stemmed from experimental or extra-experimental sources was not determined. If the phenomenon was either totally or partially produced by the schedule itself, it suggests that response rate is determined by a combination of positive and negative feedback. Control by factors extrinisic to the experiment itself implies that response rate may be influenced by some rhythmic physiological process.

Keywords: fixed-interval schedule, first-order deviation, sequential dependency, steady-state performance, regulation, rhythms, pigeons

Full text

PDF
363

Selected References

These references are in PubMed. This may not be the complete list of references from this article.

  1. Blough D. S. Interresponse time as a function of continuous variables: a new method and some data. J Exp Anal Behav. 1963 Apr;6(2):237–246. doi: 10.1901/jeab.1963.6-237. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Blough P. M., Blough D. S. The distribution of interresponse times in the pigeon during variable-interval reinforcement. J Exp Anal Behav. 1968 Jan;11(1):23–27. doi: 10.1901/jeab.1968.11-23. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Cumming W. W., Schoenfeld W. N. Behavior Stability Under Extended Exposure to a Time-Correlated Reinforcement Contingency. J Exp Anal Behav. 1960 Jan;3(1):71–82. doi: 10.1901/jeab.1960.3-71. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. GOLLUB L. R. THE RELATIONS AMONG MEASURES OF PERFORMANCE ON FIXED-INTERVAL SCHEDULES. J Exp Anal Behav. 1964 Sep;7:337–343. doi: 10.1901/jeab.1964.7-337. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. HEARST E. The behavioral effects of some temporally defined schedules of reinforcement. J Exp Anal Behav. 1958 Jan;1:45–55. doi: 10.1901/jeab.1958.1-45. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. HERRNSTEIN R. J., MORSE W. H. A conjunctive schedule of reinforcement. J Exp Anal Behav. 1958 Jan;1:15–24. doi: 10.1901/jeab.1958.1-15. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Weiss B., Laties V. G., Siegel L., Goldstein D. A computer analysis of serial interactions in spaced responding. J Exp Anal Behav. 1966 Nov;9(6):619–626. doi: 10.1901/jeab.1966.9-619. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior are provided here courtesy of Society for the Experimental Analysis of Behavior

RESOURCES