Skip to main content
Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior logoLink to Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior
. 1982 Nov;38(3):291–304. doi: 10.1901/jeab.1982.38-291

Topography of signal-centered behavior in the rat: Effects of deprivation state and reinforcer type

Graham C L Davey, Gary G Cleland
PMCID: PMC1347868  PMID: 16812301

Abstract

In a series of three experiments, groups of food-deprived and water-deprived rats were given pairings of a retractable lever (CS+) with response-independent deliveries of either solid or liquid reinforcers. In Experiment 1 food-deprived rats given a solid-pellet reinforcer differentially tended to sniff, paw, mouth, and bite the CS+ lever more often than a lever that was not paired with food (CS), whereas food-deprived rats given a liquid reinforcer tended to differentially sniff, paw, and lick the CS+ lever. 23½-hour water-deprived rats given liquid reinforcers showed very little CS+ contact. In Experiment 2 increasing the severity of water deprivation from 23½ to 47½ hours significantly increased CS+ contact. In Experiment 3, subjects that were simultaneously food and water deprived and given a water reinforcer failed to exhibit differential CS+ contact, but subjects that were simultaneously food and water deprived and given a food reinforcer did acquire differential CS+-contact behavior. These results suggest that (a) even under a single motivational state the nature of signal-centered behavior can be determined by type of reinforcer, (b) although water reinforcement produces less signal contact than food reinforcement, this can be facilitated with more severe water-deprivation levels, and (c) high CS-contact rates using food reinforcement are not simply a product of reductions in body weight with food deprivation.

Keywords: autoshaping, sign-tracking, response topography, reinforcer type, deprivation state, rats

Full text

PDF
291

Selected References

These references are in PubMed. This may not be the complete list of references from this article.

  1. BOLLES R., PETRINOVICH L. Bodyweight changes and behavioral attributes. J Comp Physiol Psychol. 1956 Apr;49(2):177–180. doi: 10.1037/h0042029. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Brown P. L., Jenkins H. M. Auto-shaping of the pigeon's key-peck. J Exp Anal Behav. 1968 Jan;11(1):1–8. doi: 10.1901/jeab.1968.11-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Davey G. C., Oakley D., Cleland G. G. Autoshaping in the rat: Effects of omission on the form of the response. J Exp Anal Behav. 1981 Jul;36(1):75–91. doi: 10.1901/jeab.1981.36-75. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Dickinson A., Mackintosh N. J. Classical conditioning in animals. Annu Rev Psychol. 1978;29:587–612. doi: 10.1146/annurev.ps.29.020178.003103. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Jenkins H. M., Moore B. R. The form of the auto-shaped response with food or water reinforcers. J Exp Anal Behav. 1973 Sep;20(2):163–181. doi: 10.1901/jeab.1973.20-163. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. PETRINOVICH L., BOLLES R. Deprivation states and behavioral attributes. J Comp Physiol Psychol. 1954 Dec;47(6):450–453. doi: 10.1037/h0054731. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Peterson G. B. Response selection properties of food and brain-stimulation reinforcers in rats. Physiol Behav. 1975 Jun;14(6):681–688. doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(75)90058-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Wilkie D. M., McDonald A. C. Autoshaping in the rat with electrical stimulation of the brain as the US. Physiol Behav. 1978 Sep;21(3):325–328. doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(78)90089-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Woodruff G., Williams D. R. The associative relation underlying autoshaping in the pigeon. J Exp Anal Behav. 1976 Jul;26(1):1–13. doi: 10.1901/jeab.1976.26-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior are provided here courtesy of Society for the Experimental Analysis of Behavior

RESOURCES