Full text
PDF![294](https://cdn.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/blobs/e02b/1357195/5228e940d020/jphysiol01183-0141.png)
![295](https://cdn.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/blobs/e02b/1357195/bed7a830e687/jphysiol01183-0142.png)
![296](https://cdn.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/blobs/e02b/1357195/f534ab11efa3/jphysiol01183-0143.png)
![297](https://cdn.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/blobs/e02b/1357195/7e985c573bd9/jphysiol01183-0144.png)
![298](https://cdn.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/blobs/e02b/1357195/89b8ab9a4ac7/jphysiol01183-0145.png)
![299](https://cdn.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/blobs/e02b/1357195/b326c6de6c09/jphysiol01183-0146.png)
![300](https://cdn.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/blobs/e02b/1357195/7ddf958dcb95/jphysiol01183-0147.png)
![301](https://cdn.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/blobs/e02b/1357195/b44db586ceae/jphysiol01183-0148.png)
![302](https://cdn.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/blobs/e02b/1357195/72e44428298a/jphysiol01183-0149.png)
![303](https://cdn.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/blobs/e02b/1357195/8452f9457525/jphysiol01183-0150.png)
![304](https://cdn.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/blobs/e02b/1357195/7705771f65b1/jphysiol01183-0151.png)
![305](https://cdn.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/blobs/e02b/1357195/950c2b9295bb/jphysiol01183-0152.png)
![306](https://cdn.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/blobs/e02b/1357195/4eb7566ff2eb/jphysiol01183-0153.png)
![307](https://cdn.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/blobs/e02b/1357195/d363cc602751/jphysiol01183-0154.png)
![308](https://cdn.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/blobs/e02b/1357195/f75ce9e72d0f/jphysiol01183-0155.png)
![309](https://cdn.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/blobs/e02b/1357195/d1aca5a10b5a/jphysiol01183-0156.png)
![310](https://cdn.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/blobs/e02b/1357195/385910dbb776/jphysiol01183-0157.png)
Selected References
These references are in PubMed. This may not be the complete list of references from this article.
- BARLOW H. B. Increment thresholds at low intensities considered as signal/noise discriminations. J Physiol. 1957 May 23;136(3):469–488. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1957.sp005774. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- BARLOW H. B. Temporal and spatial summation in human vision at different background intensities. J Physiol. 1958 Apr 30;141(2):337–350. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1958.sp005978. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- BATTERSBY W. S., WAGMAN I. H. Neural limitations of visual excitability. I. The time course of monocular light adaptation. J Opt Soc Am. 1959 Aug;49(8):752–759. doi: 10.1364/josa.49.000752. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- CORNSWEET T. N. The staircrase-method in psychophysics. Am J Psychol. 1962 Sep;75:485–491. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- HARTLINE H. K., RATLIFF F. Inhibitory interaction of receptor units in the eye of Limulus. J Gen Physiol. 1957 Jan 20;40(3):357–376. doi: 10.1085/jgp.40.3.357. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- HEINEMANN E. G. The relation of apparent brightness to the theshold for differences in luminance. J Exp Psychol. 1961 May;61:389–399. doi: 10.1037/h0047624. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- HERRICK R. M. Foveal luminance discrimination as a function of the duration of the decrement or increment in luminance. J Comp Physiol Psychol. 1956 Oct;49(5):437–443. doi: 10.1037/h0046317. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- RUSHTON W. A. Kinetics of cone pigments measured objectively on the living human fovea. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 1959 Nov 12;74(2):291–304. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1958.tb39552.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]