Skip to main content

Some NLM-NCBI services and products are experiencing heavy traffic, which may affect performance and availability. We apologize for the inconvenience and appreciate your patience. For assistance, please contact our Help Desk at info@ncbi.nlm.nih.gov.

The BMJ logoLink to The BMJ
. 2002 Dec 14;325(7377):1391–1392. doi: 10.1136/bmj.325.7377.1391

Does declaration of competing interests affect readers' perceptions? A randomised trial

Samena Chaudhry a, Sara Schroter a, Richard Smith a, Julie Morris b
PMCID: PMC138516  PMID: 12480854

Conflict of interest has been defined as a set of conditions in which professional judgment concerning a primary interest (such as patient welfare or the validity of research) can be influenced by a secondary interest (such as financial gain).1 Despite increasing evidence that conflict of interest influences authors' conclusions,24 we found no research into the effect on readers' perceptions of research. We studied whether a declaration of financial competing interest influences readers' perceptions of the interest, importance, relevance, validity, and believability of a study.

Participants, methods, and results

We randomly selected 300 BMJ readers from the BMA's membership database, which contains individual reader characteristics. All readers were sent a short report indicating that the impact of pain from herpes zoster on patients' daily functioning may be substantial.5 Readers in group 1 were sent a version of this paper with different named authors to the original and with a declaration that they were employees of a fictitious company and potentially held stock options in the company. Readers in group 2 were sent a version of the same paper with the same named authors as in group 1 but with a statement that these authors were from an ambulatory care centre and had no competing interest. Readers were asked to rate the study in terms of interest, importance, relevance, validity, and believability on 5 point Likert scales (1=extremely uninteresting to 5=extremely interesting). We estimated that 86 readers were needed in each group to achieve a power of 90% to detect a difference in scores between the groups of 0.5 units on the 5 point scale. We used a simple two sample t test with the conventional 5% significance level and assumed a common standard deviation of 1.0.

Three questionnaires were returned by family members as the addressee was considered unfit to participate. Six were sent to the wrong address. We excluded one more as the recipient was an institution. In total, 170/290 (59%) questionnaires were returned (86 in group 1, 84 in group 2). Non-responders were significantly younger (mean age 40.7 years (SD 13.9, range 19-93 years)) than responders (44.7 (SD 15.5, 20-82) years in group 1 and 44.8 (16.9, 19-86) years in group 2) (t=−2.3, df=276.2; P=0.022). There was no significant difference in the proportion of men between non-responders (65%) and responders (58% in group 1; 64% in group 2) (χ2=0.44, df=1; P=0.51).

Independent t tests showed that readers in group 1 thought the study was significantly less interesting, important, relevant, valid, and believable than readers in group 2 (table). Within each group, scores for each item were poorly correlated to age (r⩽0.22), and there were no significant differences in scores by sex.

Comment

Declaration of competing interests may have a significant effect on readers' perceptions of the scientific credibility of published medical research. BMJ readers reported that data showing the impact of pain from herpes were less interesting, important, relevant, valid and believable when the authors were employees of a fictitious pharmaceutical company compared with an ambulatory care centre.

Our study has several weaknesses. The response rate was low and responders were older than non-responders. However, within each group there was no relation between age or sex and scores. Readers knew they were being studied and thus responses might not reflect the way they usually evaluate a manuscript. We used only one manuscript with one competing interest statement. It is possible that the unfamiliar name of the fictitious company might have influenced readers' responses. Future research should explore different types of competing interest statements with different manuscripts and samples of readers.

Table.

Score* distributions for readers' perceptions of papers with and without declaration of competing interests. Figures are percentage of readers

Question
Group 1: with competing interests (n=86)†
Group 2: without competing interests (n=84)‡
P value¶
Positive§ rating
Negative§ rating
Mean (SD)
Positive§ rating
Negative§ rating
Mean (SD)
Interest 15.1 45.4 2.59 (0.87) 33.3 31.0 2.99 (0.91) 0.004
Importance 16.3 45.3 2.59 (0.90) 31.0 35.8 2.94 (0.96) 0.016
Relevance 26.7 45.4 2.70 (1.1) 41.6 31.0 3.15 (1.1) 0.006
Validity 20.9 52.3 2.53 (0.99) 40.5 32.1 3.04 (1.0) 0.001
Believability 31.4 43.0 2.73 (1.1) 51.2 19.1 3.33 (0.92) <0.001 
*

Low scores indicate low interest, importance, relevance, validity, and believability. 

Competing interests: the authors are employees of Tohen Research Laboratories, Tohen and Co, Inc, Connecticut, and potentially own stock and/or hold stock options in the company. 

Competing interests: none declared. 

§

Rating of 4 or 5 was positive; rating of 1 or 2 was negative. 

Independent t test. 

Acknowledgments

We thank the authors of the original manuscript for allowing us to use it and the readers for taking part in our study.

Footnotes

Editorial by Smith

Funding: No additional funding.

Competing interests: RS is the editor of the BMJ and is responsible for devising its policy on competing interests. He is paid a fixed salary and will not be affected financially by the success or failure of the policy on competing interests. This study was peer reviewed in the normal way, except that RS played no part in the process.

References

  • 1.Thompson DF. Understanding financial conflicts of interest. N Engl J Med. 1993;329:573–576. doi: 10.1056/NEJM199308193290812. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Lee KP, Schotland M, Bacchetti P, Bero LA. Association of journal quality indicators with methodological quality of clinical research articles. JAMA. 2002;287:2805–2808. doi: 10.1001/jama.287.21.2805. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Barnes DE, Bero LA. Why review articles on the health effects of passive smoking reach different conclusions. JAMA. 1998;279:1566–1570. doi: 10.1001/jama.279.19.1566. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Lenzer J. Alteplase for stroke: money and optimistic claims buttress the “brain attack” campaign. BMJ. 2002;324:723–726. doi: 10.1136/bmj.324.7339.723. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5. Coplan PM. Clinical relevance of zoster associated pain (ZAP) [electronic response to: Helgason S et al. Prevalence of postherpetic neuralgia after a first episode of herpes zoster: prospective study with long term follow up]. BMJ 2000 bmj.com/cgi/eletters/321/7264/794#13902 (accessed 14 May 2002).

Articles from BMJ : British Medical Journal are provided here courtesy of BMJ Publishing Group

RESOURCES