Skip to main content
The BMJ logoLink to The BMJ
. 2006 Jun 10;332(7554):1359. doi: 10.1136/bmj.38833.479560.80

Personality, lifestyle, and risk of cardiovascular disease and cancer: follow-up of population based cohort

Til Stürmer 1, Petra Hasselbach 2, Manfred Amelang 2
PMCID: PMC1476723  PMID: 16687457

Abstract

Objective To study the relation between measures of personality and risk of cardiovascular disease and cancer in a large cohort.

Design Follow-up of population based cohort.

Setting Heidelberg, Germany.

Participants 5114 women and men aged 40-65 in 1992-5.

Main outcome measures Psychological traits assessed by several standardised personality questionnaires in 1992-5, related to cause of death (to 2002-3) or reported incidence of cardiovascular diseases and cancer (validated by treating doctors). Relative risks (and 95% confidence intervals) for combined morbidity and mortality according to five important personality traits were estimated using multivariable Cox proportional hazards models.

Results During median follow-up of 8.5 years, 257 participants died and 72 were diagnosed with a heart attack, 62 with stroke, and 240 with cancer (morbidity and mortality combined). A high internal locus of control over disease was associated with a decreased risk of myocardial infarction (adjusted relative risk for an increase of 1 SD = 0.75; 95% confidence interval 0.58 to 0.96). An increase of 1 SD in time urgency was associated with a decreased risk of cancer (adjusted relative risk 0.83; 0.73 to 0.95). Other major personality traits—anger control, psychoticism, and symptoms of depression—were not consistently associated with myocardial infarction, stroke, or cancer.

Conclusion Internal locus of control over disease and time urgency seem to be associated with reduced risk for common chronic diseases, probably by affecting unmeasured health related behaviour. The other personality traits assessed had no major impact on cardiovascular disease and cancer.

Introduction

Medicine and psychology have long been separate disciplines. Despite the lack of interdisciplinary studies and cooperation, doctors are aware that psychological traits influence the incidence and the course of chronic diseases.

A recent large multinational case-control study linked permanent stress at work or home during the past year to increased incidence of myocardial infarction.1 The size of the effect of psychosocial factors was similar to that seen for abdominal obesity, diabetes, and hypertension.2 The proposed pathophysiological links between psychological stress and cardiovascular disease include clustering of “traditional” risk factors, endothelial dysfunction, myocardial ischaemia, plaque rupture, thrombosis, and malignant arrhythmias.3,4

Evidence for a link between psychological factors and cancer is weak. A recent review concluded that “there is not any psychological factor for which an influence on cancer development has been convincingly demonstrated in a series of studies.”5 A metaanalysis of stressful life events and risk of breast cancer did not support an overall association between such events and breast cancer, except for a modestly increased risk with the death of a spouse.6 In a large population based cohort study from Denmark, the incidence of cancer was 18% higher in mothers 7-18 years after the death of a child.7

We hypothesised that personality differences influence the incidence of and mortality from cardiovascular disease and cancer, independent of “traditional” risk factors. We empirically derived five personality scales that measure different independent personality traits8 and present results on all five scales without selection according to the strength of association with the disease outcomes.

Methods

Recruitment and follow-up of the cohort have been described.8 Briefly, we identified a representative sample of women and men aged 40-65 from the population registry of Heidelberg, Germany. Between 1992 and 1995, 5114 of these people completed an extensive baseline questionnaire on psychological traits, lifestyle factors, and comorbidity. All participants provided written informed consent to assess their health status 10 years later, including assessment of causes of death.

In 2002, we mailed a follow-up questionnaire to all participants and asked for information about chronic diseases that had been diagnosed since baseline. All participants who did not reply and could not be reached by telephone were followed up through population registries.

Personality variables

The personality scales used at baseline were selected for several reasons.8 Some scales were markers of broad aspects of personality (extraversion, neuroticism, psychoticism). Other scales had been used in research on health outcomes (scales to measure depression, time urgency, hostility, anger out, low sense of coherence, and irritability), some were hypothesised to be relevant (scales to measure optimism, anger in, anger control, social support, exaggerated social control, internal and external locus of control over disease, jealousy), and others were important with respect to response style (scales to measure social desirability).

Since many of these personality scales overlap and are highly correlated, we aggregated the information contained in these scales (by using factor analysis) to obtain five broad independent dimensions of personality.8 These dimensions are unique to the population from which they were derived. To make it possible to compare our results with reports in the literature, we used the personality scales that were most strongly correlated with these five broad dimensions, rather than the dimensions themselves. Both the derivation of the dimensions and the selection of scales relied solely on cross sectional baseline data (that is, were independent of the disease outcomes), and results are presented for all scales assessed.

Morbidity and mortality

We validated all diagnoses of myocardial infarction, stroke, and cancer that had occurred since baseline by contacting the treating doctors (with the written informed consent of participants). Mortality follow-up was 99.6% (22 participants moved to an unknown address or left the country); we assessed the cause of death from death certificates for 98.8% of participants who had died. Two doctors coded the cause of death according to ICD-10 (international classification of diseases, 10th revision), and inconsistencies were resolved by discussion.

Covariates

We assessed the risk factors for chronic diseases that might be associated with personality variables at baseline. Besides age and sex, these were body mass index; smoking status; alcohol consumption (amount of beer, wine, and spirits drunk each week); exercise (hours each week); comorbidity (history of myocardial infarction, stroke, cancer, hypertension, hyperlipidaemia, and diabetes); family history of myocardial infarction, stroke, and cancer; and education (years of schooling).

Statistical analyses

For each participant, we calculated the time to death, diagnosis of disease, or end of follow-up, whichever came first. We used Cox proportional hazards models to assess the association between each personality scale (in thirds or as a continuous variable) and the incidence of myocardial infarction, stroke, or cancer. We excluded participants with the disease at baseline or in whom the incidence of disease could not be assessed because of missing information (247 for myocardial infarction, 274 for stroke, and 256 for cancer).

Results

Table 1 shows the baseline characteristics of the 4267 participants who replied to the follow-up questionnaire or died during follow-up (83.4% of the original cohort). Mean age was 53.4 years; 51.5% were women. Risk factors for chronic diseases, such as overweight or obesity, smoking, no regular physical exercise, hypertension, hyperlipidaemia, and a family history of myocardial infarction, stroke, or cancer were common.

Table 1.

Baseline characteristics of 4267 participants from the Heidelberg cohort. Values are number (percentage) unless otherwise indicated

Characteristic Value
Mean (SD) age 53.4 (7.1)
Age:
<50 years 1368 (32.1)
50 to <60 1945 (45.6)
954 (22.4)
Women 2197 (51.5)
Mean (SD) body mass index 25.3 (4.0)
Body mass index*:
<25 2183 (51.9)
25 to <30 1622 (38.6)
≥30 400 (9.5)
Smoking status:
Never 1821 (42.9)
Former 1524 (35.9)
Current 896 (21.1)
Alcohol consumption (g/day):
0 (never/rarely) 698 (16.6)
0.5 to <15 1814 (43.1)
15 to <30 940 (22.4)
≥30 753 (17.9)
Exercise (hours/week):
0 (none) 1153 (27.2)
<1 787 (18.6)
1-2 1387 (32.7)
>2 909 (21.5)
Comorbidity§:
Myocardial infarction 128 (3.2)
Stroke 64 (1.6)
Cancer 242 (5.9)
Hypertension 1256 (31.1)
Hyperlipidaemia 1824 (44.3)
Diabetes 245 (6.1)
Family history:
Myocardial infarction 1088 (25.9)
Stroke 966 (23.0)
Cancer 1685 (40.0)
Education (total years of schooling)**:
≥9 2035 (48.2)
10-11 920 (21.8)
12 345 (8.2)
≥13 923 (21.9)
*

Information missing for 62 participants.

Calculated from glasses of beer (0.5 litre, ∼1 pint), wine (0.25 litre, 1 glass), and spirits (0.02 litre, 1 shot) regularly drunk each week assuming an alcohol content (% volume) of 5 (beer), 11 wine, and 40 (shots), and a relative weight of alcohol of 0.8; information missing for 62 participants.

Information was missing for 31 participants.

§

Self reported at baseline; information was missing for 249 participants with myocardial infarction, 231 with stroke, 199 with cancer, 227 with hypertension, 148 with hyperlipidaemia, and 246 with diabetes.

Information missing for 61 participants with myocardial infarction, 60 with stroke, and 50 with cancer.

**

Information missing for 44 participants.

Table 2 shows the distribution of the five personality scales according to baseline characteristics (actual patient numbers are shown in table A on bmj.com). All scales were divided into low, medium, or high according to the observed distribution (with one third of participants in each category, except for psychoticism, which was split into 22%, 53%, and 26%). Older age was strongly associated with more symptoms of depression and higher anger control. Women were more likely than men to have more symptoms of depression, and men were more likely to have higher anger control, higher time urgency, and higher internal locus of control over disease (the patient's belief that the onset and process of an illness is the result of their behaviour). Higher frequency of exercise was strongly associated with fewer symptoms of depression and higher locus of control. Patients with any comorbidity—especially those with a history of myocardial infarction, stroke, diabetes, or cancer—had more symptoms of depression.

Table 2.

Distribution of observed personality scales in 4267 participants from the Heidelberg cohort according to baseline characteristics. Values are percentages (table A on bmj.com gives the actual numbers)*

Variable
Symptoms of depression
Anger control
Time urgency
Internal locus of control over disease
Psychoticism
Low Medium High Low Medium High Low Medium High Low Medium High Low Medium High
Age:
<50 40 32 28 33 36 31 37 32 31 31 34 35 25 51 24
50 to <60 27 36 38 30 37 33 34 31 35 32 33 35 22 53 26
≥60 24 32 44 22 38 40 34 30 36 31 34 35 15 54 31
Sex:
Female 23 34 43 35 38 27 40 31 29 36 32 32 20 55 25
Male 38 34 28 23 36 41 30 31 39 27 35 39 22 50 28
Body mass index:
<25 33 34 33 31 38 31 38 32 30 33 33 34 20 54 26
25 to <30 29 34 37 27 35 37 31 32 38 30 34 36 22 51 27
≥30 22 33 45 29 38 33 31 29 40 30 33 37 20 51 30
Smoking status:
Never 28 34 38 29 37 34 36 32 32 32 34 34 20 53 28
Former 33 35 32 29 37 34 35 30 36 30 35 35 22 53 25
Current 31 31 38 29 37 34 33 33 33 32 31 37 22 51 27
Alcohol consumption:
0 (never or rarely) 27 29 44 30 38 33 39 29 32 33 32 35 21 47 32
0.5 to <15 28 35 37 32 37 31 36 32 32 32 34 34 21 55 24
15 to <30 35 36 29 27 37 36 33 32 35 28 35 37 22 54 25
≥30 36 33 31 25 35 39 31 30 39 32 33 36 21 52 27
Exercise:
0 (none) 21 33 46 32 36 32 34 31 36 36 31 34 18 51 31
<1 29 32 39 28 38 34 35 29 36 31 34 34 20 53 26
1 to 2 30 35 34 30 37 33 35 33 32 32 35 34 22 54 24
>2 43 34 23 27 36 37 37 31 32 26 34 40 24 52 24
Comorbidity:
Myocardial infarction 19 28 53 33 32 35 29 20 52 31 34 34 19 48 34
Stroke 19 22 59 30 36 34 23 31 45 25 30 45 9 39 52
Cancer 19 33 48 30 35 34 39 34 27 33 37 30 25 46 29
Hypertension 23 35 42 29 36 35 34 29 37 30 35 34 19 54 27
Hyperlipidaemia 24 34 42 29 37 34 32 30 38 29 34 37 21 53 27
Diabetes 21 31 48 29 33 37 29 31 40 25 31 44 20 47 34
Family history:
Myocardial infarction 29 33 38 28 37 35 35 33 32 30 34 35 23 52 25
Stroke 28 33 39 31 36 33 34 31 35 33 32 35 23 52 25
Cancer 29 32 39 31 38 32 35 32 34 34 33 33 20 54 26
Education (years):
≥9 23 34 44 30 39 32 33 30 37 30 32 38 21 53 27
10-11 31 35 34 32 35 33 38 30 31 30 36 35 23 53 24
12 40 34 26 25 35 40 36 32 32 29 33 37 20 57 24
≥13 43 34 23 28 36 36 35 34 31 36 35 29 20 51 30
*

Percentage of participants classified as low, medium, or high in each personality scale according to the observed distribution (one third each for all traits except psychoticism, where the distribution of values was 22%, 53%, and 26%); 10 values missing for anger control, nine for psychoticism, and nine for symptoms of depression; see table 1 for information on the missing data for the covariates.

Calculated from glasses of beer (0.5 litre, ∼1 pint), wine (0.25 litre, 1 glass), and spirits (0.02 litre, 1 shot) regularly drunk each week assuming an alcohol content (% volume) of 5 (beer), 11 wine, and 40 (shots), and a relative weight of alcohol of 0.8.

Self reported at baseline.

During a median follow-up of 8.5 years, 257 participants died, 72 participants had a heart attack, 62 participants had a stroke, and 240 participants were diagnosed with cancer (morbidity and mortality combined).

Tables 3, 4 and 5 show the five personality scales (representing the five broad personality dimensions) at baseline as predictors of incident myocardial infarction, stroke, and cancer during follow-up. For each personality scale, we performed two separate analyses; one compared the low and the high category to the medium category and one used the z transformed scores from the original personality scale as a continuous variable, so that the relative risk corresponds to an increase of 1 SD (standard deviation) in the personality scale. The first set of analyses assesses non-monotonic trends and the second assumes a monotonic log-linear trend.

Table 3.

Personality variables and incident myocardial infarction (morbidity and mortality) in the Heidelberg cohort (4267 participants)*

Personality scale
Person years (n=32 875)
No of events (n=72)
Incidence/100 000 person years (219 overall)
Relative risk (95% CI)
Unadjusted Adjusted for age and sex Fully adjusted
Symptoms of depression
Low 10 288 21 204 0.81 (0.46 to 1.42) 0.80 (0.45 to 1.41) 0.89 (0.48 to 1.64)
Medium 11 146 28 251 1.00 1.00 1.00
High 11 366 23 202 0.80 (0.46 to 1.39) 0.87 (0.50 to 1.51) 0.80 (0.44 to 1.43)
1 SD increase 1.08 (0.86 to 1.35) 1.17 (0.93 to 1.49) 1.09 (0.84 to 1.40)
Anger control
Low 9 504 16 168 0.71 (0.39 to 1.31) 0.88 (0.48 to 1.63) 0.99 (0.53 to 1.86)
Medium 12 198 29 238 1.00 1.00 1.00
High 11 103 27 243 1.02 (0.60 to 1.72) 0.81 (0.48 to 1.37) 0.80 (0.45 to 1.40)
1 SD increase 1.17 (0.92 to 1.48) 0.97 (0.77 to 1.23) 0.91 (0.71 to 1.17)
Time urgency
Low 11 570 26 228 1.30 (0.72 to 2.38) 1.43 (0.79 to 2.62) 1.85 (0.94 to 3.62)
Medium 10 424 18 173 1.00 1.00 1.00
High 10 881 28 257 1.49 (0.82 to 2.69) 1.30 ()0.72 to 2.36 1.68 (0.86 to 3.25)
1 SD increase 1.09 (0.87 to 1.38) 0.99 (0.78 to 1.26) 0.99 (0.77 to 1.26)
Internal locus of control over disease
Low 10 218 30 294 1.47 (0.85 to 2.55) 1.65 (0.95 to 2.86) 1.84 (1.01 to 3.32)
Medium 11 007 22 200 1.00 1.00 1.00
High 11 650 20 172 0.86 (0.47 to 1.57) 0.81 (0.44 to 1.48) 0.77 (0.41 to 1.46)
1 SD increase 0.87 (0.69 to 1.09) 0.80 (0.63 to 1.01) 0.75 (0.58 to 0.96)
Psychoticism
Low 6 980 18 258 1.45 (0.81 to 2.59) 1.55 (0.87 to 2.79) 1.59 (0.88 to 2.87)
Medium 17 414 31 178 1.00 1.00 1.00
High 8 411 23 273 1.53 (0.89 to 2.63) 1.48 ()0.86 to 2.54 1.28 (0.71 to 2.30)
1 SD increase 1.22 (1.06 to 1.41) 1.23 (1.05 to 1.44) 1.21 (1.01 to 1.45)
*

128 participants had myocardial infarction at baseline; 247 participants lacked information on incident myocardial infarction during follow-up; 72 incident cases of myocardial infarction (including 31 deaths) occurred.

Incidence rate ratios and their 95% confidence intervals from Cox proportional hazards model controlling for all variables in table 1: age (continuous), sex, body mass index (3 categories), smoking (never, former, current), alcohol consumption (4 categories), exercise (4 categories), comorbidity (history of stroke, cancer, hypertension, hyperlipidaemia, and diabetes), family history of myocardial infarction, and education (4 categories). Fully adjusted models based on 3700 participants for anger control, 3705 for time urgency, 3705 for internal locus of control over disease, 3698 for psychoticism, and 3697 for symptoms of depression, and 66 events owing to missing information on covariates.

Table 4.

Personality variables and incident stroke (morbidity and mortality) in the Heidelberg cohort (4267 participants)*

Personality scale
Person years (n=33 247)
No of events (n=62)
Incidence/100 000 person years (186 overall)
Relative risk (95% CI)
Unadjusted Adjusted for age and sex Fully adjusted
Symptoms of depression
Low 10 329 12 116 0.73 (0.35 to 1.51) 0.79 (0.38 to 1.65) 0.91 (0.43 to 1.94)
Medium 11 322 18 159 1.00 1.00 1.00
High 11 522 32 278 1.74 (0.98 to 3.10) 1.68 (0.94 to 3.02) 1.53 (0.83 to 2.80)
1 SD increase 1.26 (1.02 to 1.55) 1.26 (1.01 to 1.58) 1.13 (0.88 to 1.46)
Anger control
Low 9 665 14 145 0.78 (0.40 to 1.52) 0.91 (0.47 to 1.76) 0.87 (0.44 to 1.75)
Medium 12 298 23 187 1.00 1.00 1.00
High 11 213 25 223 1.19 (0.68 to 2.10) 1.04 (0.59 to 1.84) 1.15 (0.64 to 2.07)
1 SD increase 1.14 (0.88 to 1.46) 1.01 (0.78 to 1.30) 1.04 (0.80 to 1.36)
Time urgency
Low 11 718 16 137 0.84 (0.42 to 1.66) 0.84 (0.42 to 1.67) 0.76 (0.38 to 1.54)
Medium 10 404 17 163 1.00 1.00 1.00
High 11 125 29 261 1.60 (0.88 to 2.91) 1.45 (0.80 to 2.65) 1.28 (0.69 to 2.36)
1 SD increase 1.24 (0.97 to 1.59) 1.19 (0.93 to 1.53) 1.15 (0.89 to 1.48)
Internal locus of control over disease
Low 10 328 17 165 0.77 (0.41 to 1.43) 0.79 (0.42 to 1.47) 0.79 (0.42 to 1.49)
Medium 11 126 24 216 1.00 1.00 1.00
High 11 794 21 178 0.82 (0.46 to 1.48) 0.79 (0.44 to 1.42) 0.65 (0.35 to 1.21)
1 SD increase 1.14 (0.89 to 1.47) 1.11 (0.86 to 1.43) 1.01 (0.78 to 1.30)
Psychoticism
Low 7 111 14 197 1.01 (0.54 to 1.89) 1.18 (0.63 to 2.21) 1.20 (0.64 to 2.27)
Medium 17 543 34 194 1.00 1.00 1.00
High 8 525 14 164 0.85 (0.46 to 1.58) 0.83 (0.45 to 1.55) 0.70 (0.36 to 1.36)
1 SD increase 0.94 (0.72 to 1.25) 0.89 (0.67 to 1.20) 0.81 (0.58 to 1.12)
*

64 participants had stroke at baseline; 274 participants lacked information on incident stroke during follow-up; 62 incident cases of stroke (no deaths) occurred.

Incidence rate ratios and their 95% confidence intervals from Cox proportional hazards model controlling for all variables in table 1: age (continuous), sex, body mass index (3 categories), smoking status (never, former, current), alcohol consumption (4 categories), exercise (4 categories), comorbidity (history of stroke, cancer, hypertension, hyperlipidaemia, and diabetes), family history of stroke, and education (4 categories). Fully adjusted models based on 3741 participants for anger control, 3746 for time urgency, 3746 for internal locus of control over disease, 3739 for psychoticism, and 3738 for symptoms of depression, and 59 events owing to missing information on covariates.

Table 5.

Personality variables and incident cancer (morbidity and mortality) in the Heidelberg cohort (4267 participants)*

Personality scale
Person years (n=31 257)
No of events (n=240)
Incidence/100 000 person years (768 overall)
Relative risk (95% CI)
Unadjusted Adjusted for age and sex Fully adjusted
Symptoms of depression
Low 9 878 79 800 1.26 (0.91 to 1.75) 1.37 (0.99 to 1.91) 1.35 (0.96 to 1.88)
Medium 10 493 66 629 1.00 1.00 1.00
High 10 810 95 879 1.39 (1.02 to 1.91) 1.31 (0.95 to 1.79) 1.18 (0.85 to 1.63)
1 SD increase 1.09 (0.97 to 1.23) 1.06 (0.93 to 1.20) 1.00 (0.87 to 1.15)
Anger control
Low 9 106 56 615 0.80 (0.57 to 1.12) 0.86 (0.62 to 1.20) 0.87 (0.62 to 1.23)
Medium 11 615 90 775 1.00 1.00 1.00
High 10 479 93 887 1.14 (0.85 to 1.53) 1.09 (0.81 to 1.46) 1.14 (0.84 to 1.54)
1 SD increase 1.10 (0.97 to 1.25) 1.04 (0.91 to 1.19) 1.06 (0.93 to 1.22)
Time urgency
Low 10 856 93 857 1.04 (0.77 to 1.40) 1.06 (0.78 to 1.42) 1.08 (0.80 to 1.47)
Medium 9 746 81 831 1.00 1.00 1.00
High 10 654 66 619 0.74 (0.54 to 1.03) 0.71 (0.51 to 0.98) 0.68 (0.49 to 0.95)
1 SD increase 0.87 (0.76 to 0.99) 0.85 (0.74 to 0.96) 0.83 (0.73 to 0.95)
Internal locus of control over disease
Low 9 716 85 875 1.25 (0.92 to 1.71) 1.25 (0.92 to 1.72) 1.20 (0.87 to 1.66)
Medium 10 391 73 703 1.00 1.00 1.00
High 11 149 82 735 1.04 (0.76 to 1.43) 1.01 (0.74 to 1.38) 1.00 (0.72 to 1.38)
1 SD increase 0.99 (0.87 to 1.13) 0.98 (0.86 to 1.11) 0.98 (0.86 to 1.12)
Psychoticism
Low 6 577 55 836 1.17 (0.85 to 1.62) 1.29 (0.93 to 1.77) 1.34 (0.97 to 1.85)
Medium 16 614 118 710 1.00 1.00 1.00
High 7 996 67 838 1.18 (0.87 to 1.59) 1.16 (0.86 to 1.57) 1.16 (0.85 to 1.58)
1 SD increase 1.06 (0.95 to 1.19) 1.04 (0.92 to 1.16) 1.01 (0.89 to 1.14)
*

242 participants had cancer at baseline; 256 participants lacked information on incident cancer during follow-up; 240 incident cases of myocardial infarction (including 83 deaths) occurred.

Incidence rate ratios and their 95% confidence intervals from Cox proportional hazards model controlling for all variables in table 1: age (continuous), sex, body mass index (3 categories), smoking status (never, former, current), alcohol consumption (4 categories), exercise (4 categories), comorbidity (history of stroke, cancer, hypertension, hyperlipidaemia, and diabetes), family history of cancer, and education (4 categories). Fully adjusted models based on 3587 participants for anger control, 3591 for time urgency, 3591 for internal locus of control over disease, 3584 for psychoticism, and 3583 for symptoms of depression, and 230 events owing to missing information on covariates.

Anger control, time urgency, and symptoms of depression were not associated with the incidence of myocardial infarction (table 3). Participants with a low internal locus of control over disease had an adjusted relative risk of 1.84 (95% confidence interval 1.01 to 3.32) compared with those with a medium internal locus of control, and participants with a high internal locus of control over disease had a relative risk of 0.77 (0.41 to 1.46). An increase of 1 SD in internal locus of control over disease was associated with a relative risk of 0.75 (0.58 to 0.96). The relative risks of myocardial infarction were 1.59 (0.88 to 2.87) and 1.28 (0.71 to 2.30) for participants with low and high psychoticism compared with those with a medium degree of psychoticism. Although this indicates a U-shaped association, an increase of 1 SD was associated with a relative risk of 1.21 (1.01 to 1.45).

None of the personality scales was strongly associated with the incidence of stroke (table 4). Higher values of symptoms of depression, anger control, and time urgency showed a monotonic association with increased risk of incident stroke. In the fully adjusted models, these trends were less pronounced. Higher degrees of psychoticism were associated with a decreased risk of stroke, and this trend was more pronounced after we controlled for confounding.

Table 5 shows that anger control, internal locus of control over disease, psychoticism, and symptoms of depression were not associated with cancer. Time urgency was inversely associated with the risk of cancer; participants with high time urgency had a relative risk for cancer of 0.68 (0.49 to 0.95) compared with those with medium time urgency, and an increase in time urgency of 1 SD was associated with an adjusted relative risk for cancer of 0.83 (0.73 to 0.95).

Discussion

In a large, population based cohort study with a median follow-up of 8.5 years (during which 257 participants died, 72 had a heart attack, 62 had a stroke, and 240 developed cancer) some personality traits were risk factors for morbidity and mortality, independent of lifestyle factors. A low internal locus of control over disease and high and low psychoticism were risk factors for myocardial infarction. A high time urgency was associated with reduced risk of developing cancer. Overall, however, the personality traits had no major impact on the incidence of and mortality from cardiovascular disease and cancer.

Comparison with other studies

Research on personality variables as risk factors for cardiovascular disease has focused on stress, social support, low sense of coherence, and hardiness.9 Findings from the Western Collaborative Group study and the Framingham heart study seemed to establish type A behaviour (the triad of competitive ambition, time urgency, and hostility) as a risk factor for cardiovascular disease.10,11 But at least one large study (MRFIT; multiple risk factor intervention trial) found no association, and a later analysis of the Western Collaborative Group study found that the association between type A behaviour and all cause mortality is more complex.12,13 Because type A behaviour has now been deconstructed, we analysed anger control (inversely associated with hostility) and time urgency separately.9 Neither personality trait was associated with cardiovascular disease, so we cannot confirm the finding of an increased risk of non-fatal myocardial infarction with increasing time urgency seen in a recent case-control study.14

A high internal locus of control over disease has consistently been linked with reduced risk of cardiovascular disease.9 Our results on myocardial infarction agree with those on control over life circumstances (not disease) seen in a large case-control study.1 Residual confounding by healthy behaviours apart from those that we (and others) could control for is a likely explanation for at least part of the reduced risk.

Our finding of increased risk for myocardial infarction with increasing psychoticism needs to be interpreted with caution because of the inconsistency of the results. The analysis based on dividing participants into thirds does not indicate a monotonic association, so that the analysis assuming a monotonic (log linear) trend is probably driven by influential observations at the high end of the highly skewed scale. High values of psychoticism have been associated with less health conscious behaviour in at least one study, but not with myocardial infarction.15

Our finding of no association between personality traits and stroke is especially interesting in the light of emerging research on the individual components of composite end points and the greater contribution of stroke than myocardial infarction to overall risk of cardiovascular disease in women.16

A recent review of the association between psychological factors and cancer concluded that life events (other than loss of a spouse or child), negative emotional states, fighting spirit, stoic acceptance or fatalism, active coping, personality factors, and locus of control have little influence on whether people develop cancer or not.5 We found that a high time urgency was associated with a lower risk of cancer, but the strength of the association was moderate, and risk was not increased in the category of low time urgency. High time urgency has been associated with a decreased risk for all cause mortality in women after myocardial infarction but not with a decreased risk for any type of cancer.17 High time urgency might be associated with delayed diagnosis and thus lower incidence of cancer. Secondary analyses restricted to 83 deaths caused by cancer showed similar results, but with an adjusted hazards ratio of 0.58 (0.32 to 1.05) for high versus medium time urgency and of 0.80 (0.64 to 1.00) for an increase of 1 SD. By looking at cancer overall, we may have missed associations with individual types of cancer. A meta-analysis found a modest increase in risk of breast cancer after the death of a spouse but did not support an overall association between stressful life events and risk of breast cancer.6 A previous review reached similar conclusions.18

Limitations

We lack information on important risk factors for cardiovascular disease and cancer, including blood lipids, markers of subclinical systemic inflammation, diet, and use of drugs (for example, low dose aspirin, statins) during follow-up. Whereas an association between personality variables and biological markers is unlikely, personality variables could be associated with long term use of preventive drugs, screening practices, and reporting of symptoms.19 It might be better not to control for such covariates that are consequences rather than confounders.

Despite the population based recruitment, participants probably do not represent the whole range of personalities in the population. We might have missed associations between extreme patterns of these traits and disease.

Implications

Psychological traits such as the personality variables considered in our analysis are probably stable over time and are likely to have a genetic component.20,21 The more these traits are predetermined, the more they should be seen as part of a risk prediction rather than be used for “victim blaming”. Life events seem to influence the stability of these traits, and some events (such as becoming unemployed) might be avoidable at the level of the individual or society.20,22 We assessed personality variables at baseline with an extensive list of validated scales under standardised conditions, and we used factor analysis to find five personality scales as indicators for five independent personality dimensions without information on outcomes.8 An analysis that adjusted for multiple comparisons found no statistically significant association. We did not adjust the results for multiple comparisons because it is unlikely that all associations between personality traits and disease are random, because we chose all scales without knowledge of the outcomes, and because we present all the associations that we evaluated.23

Mortality and morbidity follow-up were almost complete and the incidence of the chronic diseases was validated, so the estimates of relative risks should be valid, even if we missed some events.

We adjusted our analyses for various lifestyle factors at baseline (smoking, alcohol consumption, body mass index, physical exercise, and education). Since the personality variables and these lifestyle factors were assessed at the same time, we cannot tell how personality variables influenced these lifestyle factors. Psychological traits probably influence the risk of disease only after long latency periods, but they might have more immediate effects through variables such as lifestyle factors (smoking, alcohol consumption, and weight).24 We therefore adjusted for age and sex only as well as for these two factors plus lifestyle factors. Repeated measures and more sophisticated models are needed to determine the causal effect of personality differences on the risk of disease.

What is already known on this topic

Evidence of an effect of personality on disease is best for stress and cardiovascular disease, but little evidence exists for cancer

What this study adds

Overall, the personality traits assessed had no major impact on incidence of and mortality from cardiovascular disease and cancer

Higher internal locus of control over disease (a patient's belief that the onset and progress of disease is a result of their behaviour) may be associated with a reduced risk of myocardial infarction but not of stroke or cancer

Higher time urgency may be associated with a reduced risk of cancer but not of cardiovascular disease (myocardial infarction and stroke)

Supplementary Material

[extra: Additional table]

Inline graphicAn additional table is on bmj.com

Contributors: MA and TS designed the study and obtained funding; PH collected the data; TS and PH analysed the data; TS drafted the paper; TS, PH, and MA helped to write the paper. MA is guarantor.

Funding: German Research Foundation (research grant AM 37/19-1).

Competing interests: None declared.

Ethical approval: Ethics committee-I of the medical faculty of Heidelberg, Ruprecht-Karls University of Heidelberg, Germany.

References

  • 1.Rosengren A, Hawken S, Ounpuu S, Sliwa K, Zubaid M, Almahmeed WA, et al. Association of psychosocial risk factors with risk of acute myocardial infarction in 11 119 cases and 13 648 controls from 52 countries (the INTERHEART study): case-control study. Lancet 2004;364: 953-62. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Yusuf S, Hawken S, Ounpuu S, Dans T, Avezum A, Lanas F, et al. Effect of potentially modifiable risk factors associated with myocardial infarction in 52 countries (the INTERHEART study): case-control study. Lancet 2004;364: 937-52. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Merz CNB, Dwyer J, Nordstrom CK, Walton KG, Salerno JW, Schneider RH. Psychological stress and cardiovascular disease: pathophysiological links. Behav Med 2002;27: 141-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Black PH, Garbutt LD. Stress, inflammation and cardiovascular disease. J Psychosom Res 2002;52: 1-12. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Garssen B. Psychological factors and cancer development: evidence after 30 years of research. Clin Psychol Rev 2004;24: 315-38. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Duijts SF, Zeegers MP, Borne BV. The association between stressful life events and breast cancer risk: a meta-analysis. Int J Cancer 2003;107: 1023-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Johansen C, Hansen D, Olsen J. Cancer incidence in parents who lost a child. Cancer 2002;95: 2237-42. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Amelang M, Hasselbach P, Stürmer T. Personality, cardiovascular disease, and cancer. Z Gesundheitspsychol 2004;12: 102-15. [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Sutton S, Baum A, Johnston M, eds. The Sage handbook of health psychology. London: Sage, 2004.
  • 10.Ragland DR, Brand RJ. Type A behaviour and mortality from coronary heart disease. N Engl J Med 1988;318: 65-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Haynes SG, Feinleib M, Kannel WB. The relationship of psychosocial factors to coronary heart disease in the Framingham study. III. Eight-year incidence of coronary heart disease. Am J Epidemiol 1980;111: 37-58. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Shekelle RB, Hulley SB, Neaton JD, Billings JH, Borhani NO, Gerace TA, et al. The MRFIT behaviour pattern study. II. Type A behaviour and incidence of coronary heart disease. Am J Epidemiol 1985;122: 559-70. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Carmelli D, Swan GE. The relationship of type A behaviour and its components to all-cause mortality in an elderly subgroup of men from the western collaborative group study. J Psychosom Res 1996;40: 475-83. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Cole SR, Kawachi I, Liu S, Gaziano JM, Manson JE, Buring JE, Hennekens CH. Time urgency and risk of non-fatal myocardial infarction. Int J Epidemiol 2001;30: 363-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Brayne C, Do KA, Green L, Green AC. Is health protective behaviour in adolescents related to personality? A study of sun protective behaviour and the Eysenck personality questionnaire (junior version) in Queensland. Pers Individ Dif 1998;25: 889-95. [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Glynn RJ, Rosner B. Methods to evaluate risks for composite end points and their individual components. J Clin Epidemiol 2004;57: 113-22. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Powell LH, Shaker LA, Jones BA, Vaccarino LV, Thoresen CE, Pattillio JR. Psychosocial predictors of mortality in 83 women with premature acute myocardial infarction. Psychosom Med 1993;55: 426-33. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Butow PN, Hiller JE, Price MA, Thackway SV, Kricker A, Tennant CC. Epidemiological evidence for a relationship between life events, coping style, and personality factors in the development of breast cancer. J Psychosom Res 2000;49: 169-81. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Macleod J, Smith GD, Heslop P, Metcalfe C, Carroll D, Hart C. Psychological stress and cardiovascular disease: empirical demonstration of bias in a prospective observational study of Scottish men. BMJ 2002;324: 1247-51. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Vaidya JG, Gray EK, Haig J, Watson D. On the temporal stability of personality: evidence for differential stability and the role of life experiences. J Pers Soc Psychol 2002;83: 1469-84. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Borkenau P, Riemann R, Angleitner A, Spinath FM. Genetic and environmental influences on observed personality: evidence from the German observational study of adult twins. J Pers Soc Psychol 2001;80: 655-68. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Vahtera J, Kivimaki M, Pentti J, Linna A, Virtanen M, Virtanen P, et al. Organisational downsizing, sickness absence, and mortality: 10-town prospective cohort study. BMJ 2004;328: 555. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Rothman KJ. No adjustments are needed for multiple comparisons. Epidemiology 1990;1: 43-6. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Weitkunat R, Wildner M. Exploratory causal modeling in epidemiology: are all factors created equal? J Clin Epidemiol 2002;55: 436-44. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

[extra: Additional table]

Articles from BMJ : British Medical Journal are provided here courtesy of BMJ Publishing Group

RESOURCES