Skip to main content
World Psychiatry logoLink to World Psychiatry
. 2002 Feb;1(1):16–20.

Understanding the impact of stigma on people with mental illness

PATRICK W CORRIGAN 1, AMY C WATSON 1
PMCID: PMC1489832  PMID: 16946807

Many people with serious mental illness are challenged doubly. On one hand, they struggle with the symptoms and disabilities that result from the disease. On the other, they are challenged by the stereotypes and prejudice that result from misconceptions about mental illness. As a result of both, people with mental illness are robbed of the opportunities that define a quality life: good jobs, safe housing, satisfactory health care, and affiliation with a diverse group of people. Although research has gone far to understand the impact of the disease, it has only recently begun to explain stigma in mental illness. Much work yet needs to be done to fully understand the breadth and scope of prejudice against people with mental illness. Fortunately, social psychologists and sociologists have been studying phenomena related to stigma in other minority groups for several decades. In this paper, we integrate research specific to mental illness stigma with the more general body of research on stereotypes and prejudice to provide a brief overview of issues in the area.

The impact of stigma is twofold, as outlined in Table 1. Public stigma is the reaction that the general population has to people with mental illness. Self-stigma is the prejudice which people with mental illness turn against themselves. Both public and self-stigma may be understood in terms of three components: stereotypes, prejudice, and discrimination. Social psychologists view stereotypes as especially efficient, social knowledge structures that are learned by most members of a social group (1-3). Stereotypes are considered "social" because they represent collectively agreed upon notions of groups of persons. They are "efficient" because people can quickly generate impressions and expectations of individuals who belong to a stereotyped group (4).

Table 1.

Comparing and contrasting the definitions of public stigma and self-stigma

Public stigma
Stereotype Negative belief about a group (e.g., dangerousness, incompetence, character weakness)
Prejudice Agreement with belief and/or negative emotional reaction (e.g., anger, fear)
Discrimination Behavior response to prejudice (e.g., avoidance, withhold employment and housing opportunities, withhold help)

Self-stigma
Stereotype Negative belief about the self (e.g., character weakness, incompetence)
Prejudice Agreement with belief, negative emotional reaction (e.g., low self-esteem, low self-efficacy)
Discrimination Behavior response to prejudice (e.g., fails to pursue work and housing opportunities)

The fact that most people have knowledge of a set of stereotypes does not imply that they agree with them (5). For example, many persons can recall stereotypes about different racial groups but do not agree that the stereotypes are valid. People who are prejudiced, on the other hand, endorse these negative stereotypes ("That's right; all persons with mental illness are violent!") and generate negative emotional reactions as a result ("They all scare me!") (1,3,6). In contrast to stereotypes, which are beliefs, prejudicial attitudes involve an evaluative (generally negative) component (7,8). Prejudice also yields emotional responses (e.g., anger or fear) to stigmatized groups.

Prejudice, which is fundamentally a cognitive and affective response, leads to discrimination, the behavioral reaction (9). Prejudice that yields anger can lead to hostile behavior (e.g., physically harming a minority group) (10). In terms of mental illness, angry prejudice may lead to withholding help or replacing health care with services provided by the criminal justice system (11). Fear leads to avoidance; e.g., employers do not want persons with mental illness nearby so they do not hire them (12). Alternatively, prejudice turned inward leads to self-discrimination. Research suggests self-stigma and fear of rejection by others lead many persons to not pursuing life opportunities for themselves (13,14). The remainder of this paper further develops examples of public and self-stigma. In the process, we summarize research on ways of changing the impact of public and self-stigma.

PUBLIC STIGMA

Stigmas about mental illness seem to be widely endorsed by the general public in the Western world. Studies suggest that the majority of citizens in the United States (13,15-17) and many Western European nations (18-21) have stigmatizing attitudes about mental illness. Furthermore, stigmatizing views about mental illness are not limited to uninformed members of the general public; even well-trained professionals from most mental health disciplines subscribe to stereotypes about mental illness (22-25).

Stigma seems to be less evident in Asian and African countries (26), though it is unclear whether this finding represents a cultural sphere that does not promote stigma or a dearth of research in these societies. The available research indicates that, while attitudes toward mental illness vary among non-Western cultures (26,27), the stigma of mental illness may be less severe than in Western cultures. Fabrega (26) suggests that the lack of differentiation between psychiatric and non-psychiatric illness in the three great non-Western medical traditions is an important factor. While the potential for stigmatization of psychiatric illness certainly exists in non-Western cultures, it seems to primarily attach to the more chronic forms of illness that fail to respond to traditional treatments. Notably, stigma seems almost nonexistent in Islamic societies (26-28). Cross-cultural examinations of the concepts, experiences, and responses to mental illness are clearly needed.

Several themes describe misconceptions about mental illness and corresponding stigmatizing attitudes. Media analyses of film and print have identified three: people with mental illness are homicidal maniacs who need to be feared; they have childlike perceptions of the world that should be marveled; or they are responsible for their illness because they have weak character (29-32). Results of two independent factor analyses of the survey responses of more than 2000 English and American citizens parallel these findings (19,33):

  1. fear and exclusion: persons with severe mental illness should be feared and, therefore, be kept out of most communities;

  2. authoritarianism: persons with severe mental illness are irresponsible, so life decisions should be made by others;

  3. benevolence: persons with severe mental illness are childlike and need to be cared for.

Although stigmatizing attitudes are not limited to mental illness, the public seems to disapprove persons with psychiatric disabilities significantly more than persons with related conditions such as physical illness (34-36). Severe mental illness has been likened to drug addiction, prostitution, and criminality (37,38). Unlike physical disabilities, persons with mental illness are perceived by the public to be in control of their disabilities and responsible for causing them (34,36). Furthermore, research respondents are less likely to pity persons with mental illness, instead reacting to psychiatric disability with anger and believing that help is not deserved (35,36,39).

The behavioral impact (or discrimination) that results from public stigma may take four forms: withholding help, avoidance, coercive treatment, and segregated institutions. Previous studies have shown that the public will withhold help to some minority groups because of corresponding stigma (36,40). A more extreme form of this behavior is social avoidance, where the public strives to not interact with people with mental illness altogether. The 1996 General Social Survey (GSS), in which the Mac Arthur Mental Health Module was administered to a probability sample of 1444 adults in the United States, found that more than a half of respondents are unwilling to: spend an evening socializing, work next to, or have a family member marry a person with mental illness (41). Social avoidance is not just self-report; it is also a reality. Research has shown that stigma has a deleterious impact on obtaining good jobs (13,42-44) and leasing safe housing (45-47).

Discrimination can also appear in public opinion about how to treat people with mental illness. For example, though recent studies have been unable to demonstrate the effectiveness of mandatory treatment (48,49), more than 40% of the 1996 GSS sample agreed that people with schizophrenia should be forced into treatment (50). Additionally, the public endorses segregation in institutions as the best service for people with serious psychiatric disorders (19,51).

STRATEGIES FOR CHANGING PUBLIC STIGMA

Change strategies for public stigma have been grouped into three approaches: protest, education, and contact (12). Groups protest inaccurate and hostile representations of mental illness as a way to challenge the stigmas they represent. These efforts send two messages. To the media: STOP reporting inaccurate representations of mental illness. To the public: STOP believing negative views about mental illness. Wahl (32) believes citizens are encountering far fewer sanctioned examples of stigma and stereotypes because of protest efforts. Anecdotal evidence suggests that protest campaigns have been effective in getting stigmatizing images of mental illness withdrawn. There is, however, little empirical research on the psychological impact of protest campaigns on stigma and discrimination, suggesting an important direction for future research.

Protest is a reactive strategy; it attempts to diminish negative attitudes about mental illness, but fails to promote more positive attitudes that are supported by facts. Education provides information so that the public can make more informed decisions about mental illness. This approach to changing stigma has been most thoroughly examined by investigators. Research, for example, has suggested that persons who evince a better understanding of mental illness are less likely to endorse stigma and discrimination (17,19,52). Hence, the strategic provision of information about mental illness seems to lessen negative stereotypes. Several studies have shown that participation in education programs on mental illness led to improved attitudes about persons with these problems (22,53-56). Education programs are effective for a wide variety of participants, including college undergraduates, graduate students, adolescents, community residents, and persons with mental illness.

Stigma is further diminished when members of the general public meet persons with mental illness who are able to hold down jobs or live as good neighbors in the community. Research has shown an inverse relationship between having contact with a person with mental illness and endorsing psychiatric stigma (54,57). Hence, opportunities for the public to meet persons with severe mental illness may discount stigma. Interpersonal contact is further enhanced when the general public is able to regularly interact with people with mental illness as peers.

SELF-STIGMA

One might think that people with psychiatric disability, living in a society that widely endorses stigmatizing ideas, will internalize these ideas and believe that they are less valued because of their psychiatric disorder. Self-esteem suffers, as does confidence in one's future (7,58,59). Given this research, models of self-stigma need to account for the deleterious effects of prejudice on an individual's conception of him or herself. However, research also suggests that, instead of being diminished by the stigma, many persons become righteously angry because of the prejudice that they have experienced (60-62). This kind of reaction empowers people to change their roles in the mental health system, becoming more active participants in their treatment plan and often pushing for improvements in the quality of services (63).

Low self-esteem versus righteous anger describes a fundamental paradox in self-stigma (64). Models that explain the experience of self-stigma need to account for some persons whose sense of self is harmed by social stigma versus others who are energized by, and forcefully react to, the injustice. And there is yet a third group that needs to be considered in describing the impact of stigma on the self. The sense of self for many persons with mental illness is neither hurt, nor energized, by social stigma, instead showing a seeming indifference to it altogether.

We propose a situational model that explains this paradox, arguing that an individual with mental illness may experience diminished self-esteem/self-efficacy, righteous anger, or relative indifference depending on the parameters of the situation (64). Important factors that affect a situational response to stigma include collective representations that are primed in that situation, the person's perception of the legitimacy of stigma in the situation, and the person's identification with the larger group of individuals with mental illness. This model has eventual implications for ways in which persons with mental illness might cope with self-stigma as well as identification of policies that promote environments in which stigma festers.

CONCLUSIONS

Researchers are beginning to apply what social psychologists have learned about prejudice and stereotypes in general to the stigma related to mental illness. We have made progress in understanding the dimensions of mental illness stigma, and the processes by which public stereotypes are translated into discriminatory behavior. At the same time, we are beginning to develop models of self-stigma, which is a more complex phenomenon than originally assumed. The models developed thus far need to be tested on various sub-populations, including different ethnic groups and power-holders (legislators, judges, police officers, health care providers, employers, landlords). We are also learning about stigma change strategies. Contact in particular seems to be effective for changing individual attitudes. Researchers need to examine whether changes resulting from anti-stigma interventions are maintained over time.

All of the research discussed in this paper examines stigma at the individual psychological level. For the most part, these studies have ignored the fact that stigma is inherent in the social structures that make up society. Stigma is evident in the way laws, social services, and the justice system are structured as well as ways in which resources are allocated. Research that focuses on the social structures that maintain stigma and strategies for changing them is sorely needed.

References

  • 1.Hilton J. von Hippel W. Stereotypes. Annu Rev Psychol. 1996;47:237–271. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.47.1.237. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Judd C. Park B. Definition and assessment of accuracy in stereotypes. Psychol Rev. 1996;100:109–128. doi: 10.1037/0033-295x.100.1.109. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Krueger J. Personal beliefs and cultural stereotypes about racial characteristics. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1996;71:536–548. [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Hamilton DL. Sherman JW. Stereotypes. In: Wyer RS Jr, Srull TK, editors. Handbook of social cognition. 2nd ed. Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum; 1994. pp. 1–68. [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Jussim L. Nelson TE. Manis M, et al. Prejudice, stereotypes, and labeling effects: sources of bias in person perception. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1995;68:228–246. [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Devine PG. Stereotypes and prejudice: their automatic and controlled components. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1989;56:5–18. [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Allport GW. The nature of prejudice. New York: Doubleday Anchor Books; 19541979. [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Eagly AH. Chaiken S. The social psychology of attitudes. Fort Worth: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich; 1993. [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Crocker J. Major B. Steele C. Social stigma. In: Gilbert D, editor; Fiske ST, editor; Lindzey G, editor. The handbook of social psychology. 4th ed. Vol. 2. New York: McGraw- Hill; 1998. pp. 504–553. [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Weiner B. Judgments of responsibility: a foundation for a theory of social conduct. New York: Guilford Press; 1995. [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Corrigan PW. Mental health stigma as social attribution: implications for research methods and attitude change. Clin Psychol Sci Pract. 2000;7:48–67. [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Corrigan PW. Penn DL. Lessons from social psychology on discrediting psychiatric stigma. Am Psychol. 1999;54:765–776. doi: 10.1037//0003-066x.54.9.765. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Link BG. Understanding labeling effects in the area of mental disorders: an assessment of the effects of expectations of rejection. Am Sociol Rev. 1987;52:96–112. [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Link BG. Struening EL. Rahav M, et al. On stigma and its consequences: evidence from a longitudinal study of men with dual diagnoses of mental illness and substance abuse. J Health Soc Behav. 1997;38:177–190. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Phelan J. Link B. Stueve A, et al. Public conceptions of mental illness in 1950 and 1996: what is mental illness and is it to be feared? J Health Soc Behav. 2000;41:188–207. [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Rabkin JG. Public attitudes toward mental illness: a review of the literature. Psychol Bull. 1974;10:9–33. doi: 10.1093/schbul/1.10.9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Roman PM., Jr Floyd HH., Jr Social acceptance of psychiatric illness and psychiatric treatment. Soc Psychiatry. 1981;16:16–21. [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Bhugra D. Attitudes toward mental illness: a review of the literature. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 1989;80:1–12. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.1989.tb01293.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Brockington I. Hall P. Levings J, et al. The community's tolerance of the mentally ill. Br J Psychiatry. 1993;162:93–99. doi: 10.1192/bjp.162.1.93. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Hamre P. Dahl A. Malt U. Public attitudes to the quality of psychiatric treatment, psychiatric patients, and prevalence of mental disorders. Norwegian J Psychiatry. 1994;4:275–281. [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Madianos MG. Madianou DG. Vlachonikolis J, et al. Attitudes toward mental illness in the Athens area. Implications for community mental health intervention . Acta Psychiatr Scand. 1987;75:158–165. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.1987.tb02768.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Keane M. Contemporary beliefs about mental illness among medical students: implications for education and practice. Acad Psychiatry. 1990;14:172–177. doi: 10.1007/BF03341291. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Lyons M. Ziviani J. Stereotypes, stigma, and mental illness: learning from fieldwork experiences. Am J Occup Ther. 1995;49:1002–1008. doi: 10.5014/ajot.49.10.1002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Mirabi M. Weinman ML. Magnetti SM, et al. Professional attitudes toward the chronic mentally ill. Hosp Commun Psychiatry. 1985;36:404–405. doi: 10.1176/ps.36.4.404. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Scott DJ. Phillip AE. Attitudes of psychiatric nurses and patients. Br J Med Psychol. 1985;58:169–173. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8341.1985.tb02630.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Fabrega H. Psychiatric stigma in non- Western societies. Compr Psychiatry. 1991;32:534–551. doi: 10.1016/0010-440x(91)90033-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Ng CH. The stigma of mental illness in Asian cultures. Aust N Zeal J Psychiatry. 1996;31:382–390. doi: 10.3109/00048679709073848. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Dols MW. Insanity and its treatment in Islamic society. Med History. 1987;31:1–14. doi: 10.1017/s0025727300046287. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Gabbard GO. Gabbard K. Cinematic stereotypes contributing to the stigmatization of psychiatrists. In: Fink PJ, editor; Tasman A, editor. Stigma and mental illness. Washington: American Psychiatric Press; 1992. pp. 113–126. [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Hyler SE. Gabbard GO. Schneider I. Homicidal maniacs and narcissistic parasites. Stigmatization of mentally ill persons in the movies. Hosp Commun Psychiatry. 1991;42:1044–1048. doi: 10.1176/ps.42.10.1044. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Mayer A. Barry DD. Working with the media to destigmatize mental illness. Hosp Commun Psychiatry. 1992;43:77–78. doi: 10.1176/ps.43.1.77. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Wahl OF. Media madness: public images of mental illness. New Brunswick: Rutgers University Press; 1995. [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Taylor SM. Dear MJ. Scaling community attitudes toward the mentally ill. Schizophr Bull. 1980;7:225–240. doi: 10.1093/schbul/7.2.225. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Corrigan PW. River LP. Lundin RK, et al. Stigmatizing attributions about mental illness. J Commun Psychol. 2000;28:91–103. [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Socall DW. Holtgraves T. Attitudes toward the mentally ill: the effects of label and beliefs. Sociol Quart. 1992;33:435–445. [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Weiner B. Perry RP. Magnusson J. An attributional analysis of reactions to stigmas. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1988;55:738–748. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.55.5.738. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Albrecht G. Walker V. Levy J. Social distance from the stigmatized: a test of two theories. Soc Sci Med. 1982;16:1319–1327. doi: 10.1016/0277-9536(82)90027-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Skinner LJ. Berry KK. Griffith SE, et al. Generalizability and specificity of the stigma associated with the mental illness label: a reconsideration twenty-five years later. J Commun Psychol. 1995;23:3–17. [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Corrigan PW. Rowan D. Green A, et al. Challenging two mental illness stigmas: personal responsibility and dangerousness. Submitted for publication. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Piliavin IM. Rodin J. Piliavin JA. Good Samaritanism: an underground phenomenon? J Pers Soc Psychol. 1969;13:289–299. doi: 10.1037/h0028433. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Martin JK. Pescosolido BA. Tuch SA. Of fear and loathing: the role of 'disturbing behavior', labels, and causal attributions in shaping public attitudes toward people with mental illness. J Health Soc Behav. 2000;41:208–223. [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Bordieri J. Drehmer D. Hiring decisions for disabled workers: looking at the cause. J Appl Soc Psychol. 1986;16:197–208. [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Link BG. Mental patient status, work, and income: an examination of the effects of a psychiatric label. Am Sociol Rev. 1982;47:202–215. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Wahl OF. Mental health consumers' experience of stigma. Schizophr Bull. 1999;25:467–478. doi: 10.1093/oxfordjournals.schbul.a033394. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Page S. Psychiatric stigma: two studies of behavior when the chips are down. Can J Commun Ment Health. 1983;2:13–19. [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Page S. Effects of the mental illness label in 1993: acceptance and rejection in the community. J Health Soc Policy. 1995;7:61–68. doi: 10.1300/j045v07n02_05. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Segal S. Baumohl J. Moyles E. Neighborhood types and community reaction to the mentally ill: a paradox of intensity. J Health Soc Behav. 1980;21:345–359. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Steadman HJ. Gounis K. Dennis D, et al. Assessing the New York City involuntary outpatient commitment pilot program. Psychiatr Serv. 2001;52:330–336. doi: 10.1176/appi.ps.52.3.330. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Swartz MS. Swanson JW. Hiday VA, et al. A randomized controlled trial of outpatient commitment in North Carolina. Psychiatr Serv. 2001;52:325–329. doi: 10.1176/appi.ps.52.3.325. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Pescosolido B. Monahan J. Link B, et al. The public's view of the competence, dangerousness, and need for legal coercion of persons with mental health problems. Am J Public Health. 1999;89:1339–1345. doi: 10.2105/ajph.89.9.1339. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Farina A. Fisher J. Fischer E. Societal factors in the problems faced by deinstitutionalized psychiatric patients. In: Fink P, editor; Tasman A, editor. Stigma and mental illness. Washington: American Psychiatric Press; 1992. pp. 167–184. [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Link BG. Cullen FT. Contact with the mentally ill and perceptions of how dangerous they are. J Health Soc Behav. 1986;27:289–302. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Corrigan PW. River LP. Lundin RK, et al. Three strategies for changing attributions about severe mental illness. Schizophr Bull. 2001;27:187–195. doi: 10.1093/oxfordjournals.schbul.a006865. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Holmes E. Corrigan P. Williams P, et al. Changing attitudes about schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull. 1999;25:447–456. doi: 10.1093/oxfordjournals.schbul.a033392. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Morrison JK. Cocozza JJ. Vanderwyst D. An attempt to change the negative, stigmatizing image of mental patients through brief reeducation. Psychol Rep. 1980;47:334. doi: 10.2466/pr0.1980.47.1.334. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Penn D. Guynan K. Daily T, et al. Dispelling the stigma of schizophrenia: what sort of information is best? Schizophr Bull. 1994;20:567–578. doi: 10.1093/schbul/20.3.567. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Corrigan PW. Edwards A. Green A, et al. Prejudice, social distance, and familiarity with mental illness. Schizophr Bull. 2001;27:219–225. doi: 10.1093/oxfordjournals.schbul.a006868. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Holmes PE. River LP. Individual strategies for coping with the stigma of severe mental illness. Cogn Behav Pract. 1998;5:231–239. [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Jones EE. Farina A. Hastorf AH, et al. Social stigma: the psychology of marked relationships. New York: Freeman; 1984. [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Chamberlin J. Citizenship rights and psychiatric disability. Psychiatr Rehabil J. 1998;21:405–408. [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Crocker J. Hastorf AH. Major B, et al. Social stigma and self-esteem: the self-protective properties of stigma. Psychol Rev. 1989;96:608–630. [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Deegan PE. Spirit breaking: when the helping professions hurt. Human Psychol. 1990;18:301–313. [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Corrigan PW. Empowerment and serious mental illness: treatment partnerships and community opportunities. Psychiatr Q. 1984 doi: 10.1023/a:1016040805432. in press. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Corrigan PW. Watson AC. The paradox of self-stigma and mental illness. Clin Psychol Sci Pract. 1984 in press. [Google Scholar]

Articles from World Psychiatry are provided here courtesy of The World Psychiatric Association

RESOURCES