Abstract
OBJECTIVE
To perform a systematic review of interventions designed to improve health outcomes for persons with low literacy skills.
DATA SOURCES
We searched MEDLINE, Cumulative Index to Nursing and Allied Health (CINAHL), Educational Resources Information Center (ERIC), Public Affairs Information Service (PAIS), Industrial and Labor Relations Review (ILLR), PsycInfo, and Ageline from 1980 to 2003.
STUDY SELECTION
We included controlled and uncontrolled trials that measured literacy and examined the effect of interventions for people with low literacy on health outcomes, including health knowledge, health behaviors, use of health care resources, intermediate markers of disease status, and measures of morbidity or mortality. Two abstractors reviewed each study for inclusion. Disagreements were resolved by consensus among the research team.
DATA EXTRACTION
One reviewer abstracted data from each article into an evidence table; the second reviewer checked each entry. Disagreements about information in evidence tables were resolved by team consensus. Both data extractors independently completed an 11-item quality scale for each article; scores were averaged to give a final measure of article quality.
DATA SYNTHESIS
We identified 20 articles examining interventions designed to improve health among people with low literacy. The most common outcome studied was health knowledge; fewer studies examined health behaviors, intermediate markers, or measures of disease prevalence or severity. The effectiveness of interventions appeared mixed. Limitations in research quality and heterogeneity in outcome measures make drawing firm conclusions about effective strategies difficult. Only 5 articles examined the interaction between literacy level and the effect of the intervention; they also found mixed results.
CONCLUSIONS
Several interventions have been developed to improve health for people with low literacy. Limitations in study design, interventions tested, and outcomes assessed make drawing conclusions about effectiveness difficult. Further research is required to understand better the types of interventions that are most effective and efficient for overcoming literacy-related barriers to good health.
Keywords: literacy, health literacy, systematic review
Low literacy, defined as an inability to read, write, and use numbers effectively, is common and is associated with a wide range of adverse health outcomes.1–4 The association between low literacy and adverse health outcomes likely represents 2 related underlying processes. First, low literacy may have a direct, negative effect on health. We expect this effect to be particularly important for conditions that require substantial and complex self-care on the part of the patient, because of the barriers to accessing and using health information, particularly written information. Second, low literacy may be a marker for other conditions, such as poverty and lack of access to health care, that lead to poor health.
Over the past 10 years, researchers have developed and studied a variety of interventions to improve the health of patients with low literacy. Some have addressed direct literacy-related barriers primarily by testing interventions to make health education materials easier to understand. Others have focused on indirect barriers by providing more general supportive interventions. We are unaware of previous efforts to systematically identify and critically appraise studies of interventions that attempt to mitigate the effects of low literacy. In this systematic review, we identify, describe, and evaluate interventions to improve health outcomes for patients with low literacy and reduce disparities in health outcomes associated with low literacy.
METHODS
Our review of interventions to improve health in patients with low literacy is part of a larger review commissioned by the Agency for Healthcare Research and Quality (AHRQ) that examines two main questions: the relationship between literacy and adverse health outcomes and interventions to improve outcomes for patients with low literacy. The full report, including a more detailed description of the methods and full evidence tables, is available at http://www.ahrq.gov.2 The systematic review of the studies that examine the relationship between literacy and a range of health outcomes is reported separately in the Journal of General Internal Medicine.3
Literature Review Methods
Inclusion and Exclusion Criteria
We developed eligibility criteria in consultation with an advisory panel of experts in literacy-related research including physicians, health services researchers, nurses, and policy experts. To be included, studies had to 1) be conducted in a developed country (defined as United States, Canada, Western Europe, Japan, Australia, or New Zealand; we used this criterion to increase saliency for our U.S. target audience); 2) be published from 1980 to 2003; 3) be written in English (in order to select for research most applicable to U.S. populations and because of our lack of translation capabilities for non-English language articles); 4) use a controlled or uncontrolled experimental design; 5) study more than 10 subjects; 6) measure literacy directly among participants; and 7) measure the effect of an intervention on at least one health outcome. We defined eligible health outcomes to be:
Health knowledge, assessed by an objective scale; studies that measured only subjective knowledge or satisfaction were excluded. We also excluded studies that used reading ability as the outcome;
Health behaviors, such as smoking or dietary patterns;
Biochemical or biometric health outcomes with recognized relationships to illnesses or health conditions, such as blood pressure, dietary fat, or hemoglobin A1C;
Measures of disease incidence, prevalence, morbidity, and mortality, such as arthritis disease severity or the presence of depression;
Self-reported general health status;
Utilization of health services;
Cost of care; and
Interventions to reduce disparities in health outcomes on the basis of race, ethnicity, culture, or age.
Literature Search and Retrieval Process
The literature search procedures, including search terms used, are described elsewhere.2,3 In brief, we searched MEDLINE, Cumulative Index to Nursing and Allied Health (CINAHL), Educational Resources Information Center (ERIC), Public Affairs Information Service (PAIS), Industrial and Labor Relations Review (ILLR), PsycInfo, and Ageline from 1980 to 2003 to identify relevant articles. The starting date was chosen based on advice from the expert advisory panel that few or no studies meeting our eligibility criteria were published before 1980.
We used the following key words for our search: literacy, numeracy, WRAT, Wide Range achievement, rapid estimate of adult, TOFHLA, test of functional health, reading ability, and reading skill. To identify additional relevant literature, we reviewed the National Library of Medicine Current Bibliography in Medicine-Health Literacy5 and the Annotated Bibliographies at the Harvard School of Public Health Department of Health Literacy Studies (http://www.hsph.harvard.edu/healthliteracy/literature.html).6 We also solicited articles from experts and peer reviewers.
Article Selection Process
To determine whether studies met our eligibility criteria, we first examined the abstracts of articles identified by our literature searches. One reviewer initially evaluated abstracts for inclusion or exclusion, using an eligibility checklist. If the first abstractor concluded the article should be included in the review, we retained it for the next phase of the analysis; articles excluded by the first reviewer received a second review and we retained them if the second reviewer did not agree about exclusion.
For full article review, one reviewer read each article and decided whether it met inclusion criteria. A second reviewer rereviewed all articles. The four senior Evidence-Based Practice Center (EPC) staff decided as a group whether to accept any once-excluded articles and resolved disagreements through discussion. Articles that did not meet our eligibility criteria were assigned a reason for exclusion. A full table of the excluded articles and reasons for exclusion is available at http://www.ahrq.gov.
Literature Synthesis
The first reviewer (MP, SS, or DAD) initially entered data from an article into the evidence table; the second reviewer (NB) checked and edited all table entries for accuracy and consistency. All disagreements concerning the information reported in the evidence tables were reconciled by the two abstractors, in consultation with other team members.
Rating the Quality of Individual Articles
Two independent reviewers rated the quality of each included article using a structured 11-item form based on the domains and elements recommended in a recent systematic review by West et al.7 The 11 items were distilled into 8 domains to create a quality score.2 We graded each study according to the adequacy of study population, comparability of subjects, validity and reliability of the literacy measurement, maintenance of comparable groups, appropriateness of the outcome measurement, appropriateness of statistical analysis, and adequacy of control of confounding. These elements were recommended because they represented a comprehensive but feasible means of identifying key factors that affect the possibility of bias.
Each of the 8 domains received a rating of “good,”“fair,” or “poor.” We then created a composite rating that gave each item equal weight. Specifically, we converted ratings for each domain into numeric values in which 0=poor, 1=fair, and 2=good. We totaled each evaluator's score for each article and then averaged the results to produce an article quality score. Articles with a mean score less than 1.0 were considered poor quality; those 1.0 to 1.49 fair quality; and those greater than or equal to 1.5 good quality.
We reconciled ratings in which one rater provided a score for the item and the second said the item was not applicable. Although our rating scale is based on the best available evidence for this type of assessment, it should be interpreted with caution, as it has not been validated.
Peer Review Process and Role of the Funding Source
The full report underwent extensive external peer review prior to journal submission. A list of peer reviewers is available at http://www.ahrq.gov. AHRQ also reviewed the full report and this manuscript, made editing suggestions, and gave approval for submission for publication.
RESULTS
Literature Search
Our initial literature searches identified 3,015 articles, of which 2,331 were excluded on review of the abstracts. Among the 684 articles that underwent full review, we excluded 611 and retained 73 articles. The main reasons for exclusion of studies at the full article review phase were: no original data (48% of articles), no health outcome (34% of articles), and no measure of literacy (10% of articles). Among the 73 articles retained, 44 examined associations between literacy and health outcomes and are reported in a separate publication.3 Of the 29 articles on interventions, we excluded 9 articles because they did not measure literacy in their study population8–15 or were conducted in a developing country.16
Thus, we retained 20 articles that examined interventions to improve health outcomes for patients with low literacy.17–36 See Table 1.
Table 1.
Author | Study Design | Literacy Measure | Type of Intervention | Intervention | Outcome Description | Quality |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Knowledge Outcomes | ||||||
Davis et al., 199617 | NRCT | REALM (mean=54) | Brochure | Polio vaccine information pamphlet written at 6th grade level, compared to standard pamphlet (10th grade level) | Low-literacy pamphlet better understood (based on comprehension score) than standard pamphlet. | Good |
Davis et al., 199818 | NRCT | REALM (mean=7th–8th grade level) | Brochure | Locally designed pamphlet written below 9th grade reading level, compared with an improved CDC pamphlet, also written at below 9th grade level | Intervention pamphlet better understood by patients with reading level below 9th grade level but not for patients with higher reading levels. | Good |
Davis et al., 199819 | NRCT | REALM (mean=52) | Brochure | Consent form written at 7th grade level, compared against standard consent form (16th grade level) | Special consent form written at 7th grade level (SMOG) did not improve patient comprehension (measured on a 10-item scale and scored as % correct) compared with standard form (16th grade level on SMOG) (Intervention: 58% [49%–67%] vs control: 56% [44%–67%]). | Good |
Eaton and Holloway 198020 | RCT | Adult Basic Learning Examination | Brochure | Warfarin educational materials written at 5th grade level, compared with standard materials (10th grade level), tested in non-warfarin-using VA patients | Patients receiving 5th grade materials had better comprehension (P <.0001). Literacy level explained 24% of variance in warfarin knowledge (P <.001). | Good |
Hayes 199824 | RCT | REALM (mean=59) | Person | Geragogy-based computer-generated discharge instructions (grade level 5), compared with standard printed instructions (grade 8) | Intervention participants had better medication knowledge after ED discharge. Mean difference was: 4.30 (0.51–8.09) on Knowledge of Medications scale (scale range 0–30). | Good |
Kim et al., 200121 | UCT | REALM (63% less than 9th grade level) | Computer | CD-ROM on prostate cancer administered to patients with newly diagnosed disease | Prostate cancer knowledge questionnaire, administered after CD-ROM. Correlation between PCKQ and REALM score: r =0.65, P =.0001. | Fair |
Meade et al., 199422 | RCT | WRAT (median=7th grade) | Video or audio | A simple written brochure or a videotape with similar content on colorectal cancer screening, compared with no intervention | Both the videotape and written brochure improved knowledge of colorectal cancer compared with no intervention (26% and 23% improvements from pre-intervention to post-intervention, compared with 3% for controls). There was no difference in knowledge between the written brochure and videotape, even among patients with low literacy. | Good |
Michielutte et al., 199223 | RCT | WRAT (median=46) | Brochure | Comparison of two different versions of a cervical cancer screening brochure: Version 1: illustrated, 8.4 grade level Version 2: bulleted, 7.7 grade level | Overall, no differences between version 1 and 2 on comprehension scores but when analyzed by reading level, illustrated materials were better comprehended by lower-literacy participants than bulleted materials (P =.007). Version 1: Low WRAT: 61%; High WRAT: 70% Version 2: Low WRAT: 35%; High WRAT: 72% | Good |
Murphy et al., 200026 | NRCT | REALM (mean=53) | Video or audio | 13-minute video on sleep apnea, compared against a brochure written at 12th grade level | No overall difference in 11-item knowledge scale. | Fair |
Pepe and Chodzko-Zajko 199733 | UCT | REALM (mean=63) | Video or audio | Cholesterol information videotape, compared with no intervention | Change in mean cholesterol knowledge score from baseline to T2 (2 weeks) and to T3 (6 weeks). ≥9th grade: baseline: 70% 2-week: 79% 6-week: 75% <9th grade: baseline: 57% 2-week: 63% 6-week 54% | Fair |
Raymond et al., 200234 | UCT | REALM (35%<9th grade) | Brochure | Package label for over-the-counter emergency contraception product | Women of lower literacy were significantly less likely to understand almost all objectives than more literate women (data not shown). However, 8 of the 11 objectives were each understood by more than 80% of women with low literacy, suggesting the material was understandable. | Fair |
Wydra 200127 | RCT | WRAT (60%–66% scored below Brochure09) | Computer | Interactive videodisc designed to improve self-care for cancer patients with fatigue | Intervention patients reported greater knowledge about self-care ability after the intervention (P <.0001); literacy level did not affect amount of self-care knowledge gained (P =.31). | Fair |
Health Behaviors | ||||||
Coleman et al., 200335 | NRCT | REALM (results not reported) | Brochure | Educational pamphlet on breast self-exam and clinical breast exam, written at 3rd grade level, with photographs, compared with similar pamphlet using illustrations | Women receiving the materials with photographs were more accurate in performing breast exam on silicone models and had increased knowledge. | Poor |
Howard-Pitney et al., 199728 | RCT | WRAT (66% 8th grade level or below) | Person | 6 nutrition classes specially designed for participants with low literacy (each class 90 minutes) compared with conventional nutrition curriculum | Intervention improved nutrition knowledge (net change in % correct: 7.7%, P =.01) and reduced calorie intake from saturated fat (−0.9% points, P =.02) in a predominantly low-literacy population. | Good |
Hussey 199429 | UCT/NRCT | Gates-MacGinitie (mean=3rd–4th grade) | Person | Verbal teaching about medication with or without color-coded medication schedule | Intervention improved knowledge (P <.001) and adherence (P =.007) for elderly patients (pre-to post-intervention) but the addition of a color-coded medication schedule did not improve knowledge or adherence overall. | Fair |
Murphy et al., 199625 | NRCT | REALM (mean=25) | Person | Educational program (8 sessions, 1 hour each), compared with no intervention | Intervention to improve dietary behavior for persons with low literacy had little effect on dietary outcomes, including ability to read labels and self-reported dietary behaviors. | Good |
Biochemical or Biometric Markers | ||||||
Hartman et al., 199730 | RCT | ABLE | Person | Novel nutritional education program, compared with standard materials | Intervention improved self-reported eating patterns modestly, but had little effect on intake of total calories, total fat, saturated fat, or dietary cholesterol or on blood cholesterol levels. | Fair |
Kumanyika et al., 199931 | RCT | Specially designed scale (48% below 8th grade level) | Person | Special cardiovascular nutrition program using special food cards and a nutrition guide, plus audiotapes and 4 monthly classes, compared with the food cards and nutrition guide alone | No difference in change in total cholesterol (Int:−0.41 mmol/l vs control:−0.43 mmol/l) or systolic blood pressure (Int:−7.4 mmHg vs control:−10.6 mm Hg at 12 months). | Good |
Measures of Disease Incidence, Prevalence, or Severity | ||||||
Poresky and Daniels 200136 | RCT | Comprehensive Adult Student Assessment Scale | Person | Enhanced Head Start program using case manager, with emphasis on employment and literacy skill building, compared with traditional Head Start | Intervention participants were more likely to resolve depression as measured on CES-D than control patients (25 percentage point reduction compared with 2 percentage point reduction; P <.05). | Fair |
Use of (Preventive) Health Care Services | ||||||
Davis et al., 199832 | NRCT | REALM (mean=4th–6th grade level) | Video or Audio | 12-minute video-based coaching tool (along with a verbal recommendation and brochure), compared with a verbal recommendation alone or verbal recommendation plus brochure | Mammography at 6 months Verbal alone: 21% Verbal+brochure 18% Video+verbal+brochure 29% (bivariate P =.05, multivariate P =.03). Mammography at 24 months 1. Verbal alone 37% 2. Verbal+brochure 34% 3. Video+verbal+brochure 40% (P.05). | Fair |
ECT, randomized controlled trial in medicine; REALM, Rapid Evaluation of Adult Literacy; NRCT, nonrandomized controlled trial; UCT, uncontrolled trial; CES-D, Center for Epidemiologic Studies-Depression; ED, emergency department.
Study Characteristics
The 20 studies were of 3 types: randomized controlled trials (n =9), nonrandomized controlled trials (in which subjects were assigned to intervention or control groups by the day or the week or some other nonrandom fashion; n =8), and uncontrolled, single-group trials (n =3). The number of participants enrolled ranged from 28 to 1,744; most studies had between 100 and 500 participants. All but 2 studies were conducted in the United States. Most interventions and outcome assessments were administered in single sessions. Interventions to improve dietary behavior26,28,30,31,33 and 1 other study36 delivered multisession interventions and/or followed participants longitudinally to assess changes in outcomes.
All 20 studies used previously validated instruments to measure the literacy of each trial participant. The most commonly used instrument was the Rapid Estimate of Adult Literacy in Medicine (REALM; n =11), followed by the Wide Range Achievement Test (WRAT)4; 5 studies used other instruments. No intervention study used the Test of Functional Health Literacy in Adults (TOFHLA), which has been commonly employed in studies of the relationship between literacy and health.3,37,38
Only 5 controlled trials stratified their results by literacy level. Each of these studies measured knowledge as its only outcome.18,22,23,26,27 In addition to literacy, most studies reported participants' mean age, ethnicity, and mean education levels. Information on participants' income level and health insurance status was available for a minority of studies. These data are available in the full evidence report.2
Included studies tested a wide range of interventions for improving health outcomes in patients with low literacy. Most interventions attempted to make health information more accessible to patients with limited literacy. Primary types of interventions were easy-to-read printed materials (n =4) videotapes or audiotapes (n =4); CD-ROM, computer program, or interactive videodisc (n =3); and in-person instruction, individually or in groups (n =9). Interventions were sometimes compared against standard information delivery or materials known to be more difficult to read; other studies compared their interventions against usual care or no intervention. Some studies compared different formats or styles of information with similar readability levels.
Outcomes Studied
Included studies measured the following types of outcomes: health knowledge, health behaviors (e.g., smoking rates, dietary patterns, self-care), biochemical or other intermediate markers (e.g., cholesterol levels, weight, blood pressure), and use of health services (e.g., mammography rates). Knowledge outcomes were used most frequently. Only 1 study directly measured health outcomes that participants could feel and report on directly, in this case depression. Other than dietary behavior, few outcomes were examined in more than 1 or 2 studies, making outcome-specific comparisons and conclusions difficult.
Knowledge and Comprehension
Twelve studies examined health knowledge or comprehension of health materials as their main outcome of interest.17–24,26,27,33,34 The effects of interventions on health knowledge were mixed: some found increased knowledge, and others found no effect. Two studies examined the effect of literacy on postintervention knowledge but did not compare or report the overall effect of the intervention itself.21,34 Five controlled trials stratified the effect of the intervention by literacy status.18,22,23,26,27 They reached mixed conclusions about the relationship between literacy level and intervention effectiveness. In a controlled trial among patients at a sleep apnea clinic, Murphy et al. used a written 11-item questionnaire to compare the effect of a videotape educational tool against the effect of a brochure written at 12th grade level (similar to the grade level of the video script).26 No net effect on knowledge was observed for patients with high or low literacy.
Michielutte et al. performed a randomized trial to examine the effect of an illustrated brochure on cervical cancer compared with a brochure using bulleted text only. Readability levels were similar (8.4 grade level vs 7.7 grade level).23 Overall, there was no difference in knowledge on postintervention questionnaires. Patients with literacy scores below the median (46) on WRAT understood the illustrated materials better than the bulleted text version (61% vs 35%; P =.007). For patients with higher literacy, no difference was detected (70% vs 72%).
Wydra performed a randomized trial to examine the effect of an interactive videodisc to improve knowledge of self-care for cancer fatigue symptoms compared with no intervention among cancer patients.27 Patients who received the intervention had greater self-care knowledge, but this effect was not related to the literacy level of the patient as measured on WRAT (P =.31).
Davis et al. performed a controlled trial comparing a locally developed polio vaccine information pamphlet designed for patients with low literacy against a Centers for Disease Control and Prevention pamphlet that had also been designed for easy readability (both brochures were written at the sixth grade level).18 Patients with low literacy (third grade reading level or less) did not differ in their comprehension of the 2 pamphlets; among persons with higher literacy levels on REALM, the locally developed pamphlet was associated with better comprehension.
Meade et al. examined the effectiveness of educational materials on colorectal cancer screening that were intended to be appropriate for persons with low literacy in a randomized trial of 1,100 patients at the Milwaukee County Hospital primary care clinic.22 Participants were assigned to 1 of 2 interventions (a videotape or an easy-to-read brochure written at the fifth to sixth grade level) or to a usual care control group. Patients receiving either intervention had greater improvements in knowledge scores after the educational materials compared with the control group (26% for the video, 23% for the brochure, 3% for controls). Both low-and high-literacy groups (stratified at the seventh grade level based on WRAT) who received either intervention showed improved knowledge between the pre-and posttests compared with the control group. However, differences in improvement between the 2 literacy groups were small and not statistically significant.
Health Behaviors
Several studies examined the effect of interventions on health behaviors as their main outcomes.25,28,29,35 The behaviors studied included ability to perform breast examination, smoking, dietary patterns, or medication adherence. Outcomes were mixed.
Coleman et al. found that women given educational materials about breast examinations written at the third grade level and accompanied by photographs had greater knowledge and were more accurate in performing exams on silicone breast models than patients given materials at the same grade level with illustrations.35 Hussey found that seniors who were given verbal teaching concerning medication compliance improved adherence; adding a color-coded medication schedule did not provide additional benefit, however.29 Studies addressing dietary behaviors produced small or no changes in dietary behavior.26,28,30
Biochemical or Biometric Markers
Two studies used changes in biochemical or biometric markers to test the effect of their interventions and found small or no improvements.30,31
Kumanyika et al. conducted a randomized trial to examine the effect of a special cardiovascular nutrition program for African Americans on lipid levels and blood pressure that included 4 monthly classes plus audiotapes.31 They enrolled 330 participants recruited from grocery stores in Washington, DC. They found no difference between groups in the change in cholesterol levels from preprogram to postprogram. Net difference in blood pressure was 3.2 mm Hg among women and 1.7 mm Hg among men, but neither result was statistically significant. Hartman et al. also found no significant difference in cholesterol levels with a nutritional intervention targeted to persons with low literacy.30
Measures of Disease Incidence, Prevalence, or Severity
One study examined the effect of an intervention that included direct literacy skill building and measured the outcome of persistent depression. Poresky and Daniels conducted a randomized trial and found that a comprehensive family services center, compared with standard Head Start, could improve parental reading skill and reduce the prevalence of parental depression.36
Global Health Status
We identified no studies of interventions that used a self-reported global health measure such as the SF-36 as the health outcome of interest.
Use of Preventive Care Services
One study examined an intervention to affect the use of preventive care services. In a nonrandomized controlled trial, Davis et al. found that an intervention consisting of a 12-minute video, coaching tool, verbal recommendation, and brochure improved mammography utilization by 11% at 6 months, but not at 24 months, when compared with a verbal recommendation and brochure alone.32
Costs of Health Care
We did not identify any studies examining the effect of an intervention on costs, charges, or reimbursements.
Disparities in Health Outcomes or Health Care Service Use
We found no studies examining whether interventions for patients with low literacy affected health disparities based on race, ethnicity, culture, or age.
Study Quality
We rated 10 articles as having good quality, 9 as fair quality, and 1 as poor quality (see Table 1). Common and important limitations in design included 1) use of uncontrolled before-and-after designs; 2) failure to measure literacy or analyze results by literacy level; 3) failure to account for multiple comparisons in the analysis; 4) inability to isolate the impact of overcoming literacy barriers compared with other co-interventions, such as increased contact time; 5) failure to assign persons randomly, with concealed allocation, to the intervention and control arms; 6) incomplete statistical reporting; and 7) in some studies, an inadequate sample size to exclude clinically important effects.
DISCUSSION
Studies of interventions to improve the health of persons with low literacy have increased in number over the past 10 years but remain relatively uncommon. Most completed studies have examined the effect of interventions on health knowledge or behaviors; fewer studies have examined interventions designed to mitigate the effects of low literacy on intermediate markers, measures of disease incidence or prevalence, or use of health services. No research to date has examined how interventions affect the general health status of persons with low literacy or whether interventions can affect health care costs or health disparities based on race, ethnicity, culture, or age. Because too few studies examined each type of intervention (brochure, videotape, computerized tool, or oral presentation), we are also unable to comment about which types of interventions might be most effective.
Completed studies to date have found mixed results: some have shown positive effects on health, others have found no effect. The diverse range of outcomes examined limited our ability to draw conclusions about effectiveness. Differences in study quality, as measured by our rating scale, did not appear to explain differences in effectiveness. Although several studies showed improved overall outcomes, most had not been designed to measure whether the intervention helped the participants with low literacy less or more than (or equally to) patients with higher literacy. We identified only 5 studies that did measure whether an intervention had different effects in persons with low versus high literacy. These studies all used controlled designs, measured literacy in all participants, and stratified their results according to literacy level, but to date such studies have examined only knowledge outcomes. Their findings were also mixed with respect to differences in their ability to improve health knowledge based on the user's literacy level: some found that interventions worked similarly in low-and high-literacy patients, others found that low-literacy patients benefited more than high-literacy patients, and still others found the opposite.
Although our review is to our knowledge the first to systematically identify and evaluate interventions for low-literacy patients, it should be interpreted in the context of several limitations. First, as with all systematic reviews, its findings depend on the quality of the information in the published literature. Limitations of the available intervention studies include frequent use of nonrandomized designs and nonconcealment of allocation; infrequent reporting of how health outcomes were assessed, including whether assessors were blinded to literacy and intervention status and whether the questions were administered in ways that would allow accurate responses by participants with limited literacy; poor description of interventions; and use of multimodal interventions that make it difficult to know which portions produced positive effects. Moreover, published research has focused on knowledge rather than more meaningful health outcomes, and we encountered so many different outcomes that quantitative synthesis (meta-analysis) of results was not possible.
Our review process also had some limitations. We did not include studies that did not measure literacy directly. As such, we may have failed to identify interventions that could be beneficial for patients with low literacy. Our quality grading system, although based on previous research, has not been independently validated, relies solely on information reported in published articles, and should be interpreted cautiously. In addition, we did not, for time and resource constraint reasons, conduct dual, independent, blinded review of all articles for inclusion or for abstraction of information into evidence tables. We believe, however, our modified process allows sufficient rigor to minimize potential selection bias.
Because currently available intervention studies leave many important questions unanswered, additional research is key to advancing this field. In particular, we need more intervention studies that examine whether the association between low literacy and adverse health outcomes is mainly direct (meaning that outcomes could be improved by interventions designed to overcome limitations in reading and quantitative reasoning) or indirect (such that outcomes might be better addressed by focusing on other underlying causes of health disparities such as poverty, lack of access to care, or racism).
Future studies should carefully specify research questions and comparisons to allow stronger conclusions to be drawn about the interventions' true value and to help clarify whether interventions work directly by affecting the ability of patients to understand health information or indirectly by overcoming other barriers.
Intervention studies should stratify results by literacy level. Without such analysis, readers cannot determine whether the intervention is more or less effective in the low-literacy group and whether it helped to ameliorate the differences in outcome according to literacy status. We have highlighted in this report the studies that perform this type of analysis. All used health knowledge as the main outcome, but their conclusions about the interaction between the effectiveness of the intervention and patient literacy levels were mixed.
Intervention studies have generally focused mostly on short-term knowledge outcomes or health behaviors. Future studies should link short-term knowledge changes to important, longer-term health outcomes that matter more directly to patients and their families. Interventions aimed at changing health behaviors such as dietary intake for patients with low literacy also face the challenge that changing such behavior has been difficult for patients, regardless of literacy level. Further, many interventions had multiple components. Analyses that examine the individual effects of the key intervention components could significantly advance the field and help us determine “how much” intervention is enough to improve health.
Another limitation of the literature is that few studies presented details about the nature of the intervention materials beyond reading level or about how patient outcome assessments were performed. Research suggests that people with low literacy skills can learn appropriate self-care tasks if the tasks are organized and presented in a fashion that facilitates incorporation into everyday life, but doing so effectively requires more than just developing easy-to-read written materials. More studies that examine and describe the tasks required for effective self-care and measure the ability of patients to learn those tasks may lead to more effective interventions. For example, rather than asking diabetes knowledge questions, researchers could analyze patient logs of insulin use based on glucose levels.
Finally, we need to conduct more studies that examine the effect on health outcomes of teaching persons with low literacy skills to read better. To do so will require collaboration between health researchers and experts in adult education and literacy training.
This review has important implications for practitioners as well. Health care providers, including physicians, nurses, and other health care personnel, should be alert to the widespread problem of low literacy, and should consider how to convey important health care information in ways that do not require advanced reading skills. They should have access to tools that have been shown to be effective, including videotapes, computer programs, and group education curricula. Practitioners can use the “teach-back” method to check to see that patients understand health information, a technique that has been associated with better outcomes in an observational study of diabetes.39 Structural changes in the way care is delivered, such as the use of disease management programs, may also have important benefits for patients with low literacy.40–42
Acknowledgments
Supported by a contract to the Agency for Healthcare Research and Quality (290-02-0016), Rockville, MD.
REFERENCES
- 1.Kirsch I, Jungeblut A, Jenkins L, Kolstat A. Adult literacy in America: a first look at the findings of the national adult literacy survey. 2002:1–201. [Google Scholar]
- 2.Berkman ND, Dewalt DA, Pignone MP, et al. (RTI International-University of North Carolina Evidence-based Practice Center): Literacy and health outcomes: evidence report/technology assessment number. Available at: http://www.ahrq.gov/clinic/litinv.htm/xlink. Accessed February 13, 2004.
- 3.DeWalt DA, Berkman ND, Sheridan SL, Lohr KN, Pignone M. Literacy and healthoutcomes: a systematic review of the literature. J Gen Intern Med. 2004;19:1228–39. doi: 10.1111/j.1525-1497.2004.40153.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Nielsen-Bohlman L, Panzer AM, Kindig DA. Health literacy: a prescription to end confusion. Available at: http://www.nap.edu/books/0309091179/html/. Accessed February 11, 2004. [PubMed]
- 5.Selden CR, Zorn M, Ratzan S, Parker RM. Health literacy [bibliography online]. Current Bibliographies in Medicine; no. 2000-1. Vol. 2003. National Library of Medicine. Available at: http://www.nlm.nih.gov/pubs/cbm/hliteracy.html. Accessed March 2, 2004.
- 6.Rudd RE, Colton T, Schacht R. (National Center for the Study of Adult Learning and Literacy). An overview of the medical and public health literature addressing literacy issues: an annotated bibliography. Available at: http://www.hsph.harvard.edu/healthliteracy/litreview.pdf. Accessed March 3, 2004.
- 7.West SL, King V, Carey TS. (Agency for Healthcare Research and Quality). Systems to rate the strength of scientific evidence. Available at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/books/bv.fcgi?rid=hstat1.chapter.70996. Accessed February 15, 2004.
- 8.Lillington L, Royce J, Novak D, Ruvalcaba M, Chlebowski R. Evaluation of a smoking cessation program for pregnant minority women. Cancer Pract. 1995;3:157–63. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Mulrow C, Bailey S, Sonksen PH, Slavin B. Evaluation of an audiovisual diabetes education program: negative results of a randomized trial of patients with non-insulin-dependent diabetes mellitus. J Gen Intern Med. 1987;2:215–9. doi: 10.1007/BF02596442. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Fitzgibbon ML, Stolley MR, Avellone ME, Sugerman S, Chavez N. Involving parents in cancer risk reduction: a program for Hispanic American families. Health Psychol. 1996;15:413–22. doi: 10.1037//0278-6133.15.6.413. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Bill-Harvey D, Rippey R, Abeles M, et al. Outcome of an osteoarthritis education program for low-literacy patients taught by indigenous instructors. Patient Educ Couns. 1989;13:133–42. doi: 10.1016/0738-3991(89)90056-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Fouad MN, Kiefe CI, Bartolucci AA, Burst NM, Ulene V, Harvey MR. A hypertension control program tailored to unskilled and minority workers. Ethn Dis. 1997;7:191–9. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Jacobson TA, Thomas DM, Morton FJ, Offutt G, Shevlin J, Ray S. Use of a low-literacy patient education tool to enhance pneumococcal vaccination rates. A randomized controlled trial. JAMA. 1999;282:646–50. doi: 10.1001/jama.282.7.646. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Gans K, Lovell H, Fortunet R. Low-literacy audio intervention for lowering fat intake. J Nutr Educ. 1998;30:410B. [Google Scholar]
- 15.Powell EC, Tanz RR, Uyeda A, Gaffney MB, Sheehan KM. Injury prevention education using pictorial information. Pediatrics. 2000;105:e16. doi: 10.1542/peds.105.1.e16. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Hugo J, Skibbe A. Facing visual illiteracy in South African health education: a pilot study. J Audiov Media Med. 1991;14:47–50. doi: 10.3109/17453059109055340. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Davis TC, Bocchini JA, Jr, Fredrickson D, et al. Parent comprehension of polio vaccine information pamphlets. Pediatrics. 1996;97:804–10. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Davis TC, Fredrickson DD, Arnold C, Murphy PW, Herbst M, Bocchini JA. A polio immunization pamphlet with increased appeal and simplified language does not improve comprehension to an acceptable level. Patient Educ Couns. 1998;33:25–37. doi: 10.1016/s0738-3991(97)00053-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Davis TC, Holcombe RF, Berkel HJ, Pramanik S, Divers SG. Informed consent for clinical trials: a comparative study of standard versus simplified forms. J Natl Cancer Inst. 1998;90:668–74. doi: 10.1093/jnci/90.9.668. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Eaton ML, Holloway RL. Patient comprehension of written drug information. Am J Hosp Pharm. 1980;37:240–3. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Kim SP, Knight SJ, Tomori C, et al. Health literacy and shared decision making for prostate cancer patients with low socioeconomic status. Cancer Invest. 2001;19:684–91. doi: 10.1081/cnv-100106143. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Meade CD, McKinney WP, Barnas GP. Educating patients with limited literacy skills: the effectiveness of printed and videotaped materials about colon cancer. Am J Public Health. 1994;84:119–21. doi: 10.2105/ajph.84.1.119. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Michielutte R, Bahnson J, Dignan MB, Schroeder EM. The use of illustrations and narrative text style to improve readability of a health education brochure. J Cancer Educ. 1992;7:251–60. doi: 10.1080/08858199209528176. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Hayes KS. Randomized trial of geragogy-based medication instruction in the emergency department. Nurs Res. 1998;47:211–8. doi: 10.1097/00006199-199807000-00006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Murphy PW, Davis TC, Mayeaux EJ, Sentell T, Arnold C, Rebouche C. Teaching nutrition education in adult learning centers: linking literacy, health care, and the community. J Community Health Nurs. 1996;13:149–58. doi: 10.1207/s15327655jchn1303_2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Murphy PW, Chesson AL, Walker L, Arnold CL, Chesson LM. Comparing the effectiveness of video and written material for improving knowledge among sleep disorders clinic patients with limited literacy skills. South Med J. 2000;93:297–304. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Wydra EW. The effectiveness of a self-care management interactive multimedia module. Oncol Nurs Forum. 2001;28:1399–407. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Howard-Pitney B, Winkleby MA, Albright CL, Bruce B, Fortmann SP. The Stanford Nutrition Action Program: a dietary fat intervention for low-literacy adults. Am J Public Health. 1997;87:1971–6. doi: 10.2105/ajph.87.12.1971. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Hussey LC. Minimizing effects of low literacy on medication knowledge and compliance among the elderly. Clin Nurs Res. 1994;3:132–45. doi: 10.1177/105477389400300206. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Hartman TJ, McCarthy PR, Park RJ, Schuster E, Kushi LH. Results of a community-based low-literacy nutrition education program. J Community Health. 1997;22:325–41. doi: 10.1023/a:1025123519974. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Kumanyika SK, Adams-Campbell L, Van Horn B, et al. Outcomes of a cardiovascular nutrition counseling program in African-Americans with elevated blood pressure or cholesterol level. J Am Diet Assoc. 1999;99:1380–91. doi: 10.1016/S0002-8223(99)00336-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Davis TC, Berkel HJ, Arnold CL, Nandy I, Jackson RH, Murphy PW. Intervention to increase mammography utilization in a public hospital. J Gen Intern Med. 1998;13:230–3. doi: 10.1046/j.1525-1497.1998.00072.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Pepe MV, Chodzko-Zajko WJ. Impact of older adults' reading ability on the comprehension and recall of cholesterol information. J Health Educ. 1997;28:21–7. [Google Scholar]
- 34.Raymond EG, Dalebout SM, Camp SI. Comprehension of a prototype over-the-counter label for an emergency contraceptive pill product. Obstet Gynecol. 2002;100:342–9. doi: 10.1016/s0029-7844(02)02086-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Coleman EA, Coon S, Mohrmann C, et al. Developing and testing lay literature about breast cancer screening for African American women. Clin J Oncol Nurs. 2003;7:66–71. doi: 10.1188/03.CJON.66-71. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Poresky RH, Daniels AM. Two-year comparison of income, education, and depression among parents participating in regular Head Start or supplementary Family Service Center Services. Psychol Rep. 2001;88:787–96. doi: 10.2466/pr0.2001.88.3.787. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Parker RM, Baker DW, Williams MV, Nurss JR. The test of functional health literacy in adults: a new instrument for measuring patients' literacy skills. J Gen Intern Med. 1995;10:537–41. doi: 10.1007/BF02640361. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Wilkinson GS. Wide-range Achievement Test: Administration Manual. Wilmington, DE: Wide Range, Inc.; 1993. [Google Scholar]
- 39.Schillinger D, Piette J, Grumbach K, et al. Closing the loop: physician communication with diabetic patients who have low health literacy. Arch Intern Med. 2003;163:83–90. doi: 10.1001/archinte.163.1.83. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Rothman R, Malone R, Bryant B, Horlen C, Pignone M. Pharmacist-led, primary care-based disease management improves hemoglobin A1c in high-risk patients with diabetes. Am J Med Qual. 2003;18:51–8. doi: 10.1177/106286060301800202. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Schillinger D. Improving the quality of chronic disease management for populations with low functional health literacy: a call to action. Dis Manag. 2003;4:103–9. [Google Scholar]
- 42.DeWalt DA, Pignone MP, Malone R. Development and pilot testing of a disease management program for low literacy patients with heart failure. Patient Educ Couns. 2004;55:78–86. doi: 10.1016/j.pec.2003.06.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]