Abstract
Women report more intense, more numerous, and more frequent bodily symptoms than men. This difference appears in samples of medical patients and in community samples, whether or not gynecologic and reproductive symptoms are excluded, and whether all bodily symptoms or only those which are medically unexplained are examined. More limited, but suggestive, literature on experimental pain, symptom reporting in childhood, and pain thresholds in animals are compatible with these findings in adults. A number of contributory factors have been implicated, supported by varying degrees of evidence. These include innate differences in somatic and visceral perception; differences in symptom labeling, description, and reporting; the socialization process, which leads to differences in the readiness to acknowledge and disclose discomfort; a sex differential in the incidence of abuse and violence; sex differences in the prevalence of anxiety and depressive disorders; and gender bias in research and in clinical practice. General internists need to keep these factors in mind in obtaining the clinical history, understanding the meaning and significance that symptoms hold for each patient, and providing symptom relief.
Keywords: somatic symptoms, gender differences
Women and men experience somatic symptoms, bodily distress, and physical health differently. In this paper, we review the empirical evidence regarding gender differences in symptoms, consider various explanations for these differences, and discuss their implications for clinical practice. Since all physical symptoms contain both somatic and psychological components, the distinction between “organic” and “functional” symptoms is theoretically unsound, arbitrary, and clinically impossible to establish. Accordingly, we will discuss somatic symptom reporting in general, including studies of symptoms which do and do not have a clearly demonstrable pathophysiologic correlate. The term sex is generally used to refer to biological characteristics differentiating males and females, while gender is used to refer to the socially allocated roles and expectations associated with sex, i.e., masculinity and femininity. In this review, however, we will use the terms interchangeably, as it is difficult to distinguish the biological and the social role aspects of somatic symptom reporting.
Methods
The medline and PsycInfo databases were searched for articles from 1966 through March 1999 that included any of the following MeSH terms: symptoms, somatoform disorders, pain, chronic illness, health behavior, and illness behavior combined with sex, sex differences, gender, gender role, and gender differences. The search was extended by using the bibliographies of selected, recent articles and systematic reviews. Articles were screened for relevance based on title, key words, and abstracts. Only English-language papers were included. The initial search identified approximately 450 articles. Cursory inspection of these led to the elimination of approximately 275 as insufficiently relevant or insufficiently scientific to warrant inclusion. The remaining 175 articles were then read and discussed by 2 of the 3 authors. If there was disagreement about whether to include the study in the review or how much emphasis to accord it, the third author read it, and we arrived at a consensus opinion. Although this literature was reviewed, analyzed, and synthesized, a formal meta-analysis was not conducted due to the broad scope of the subject and the wide variation in the types of articles reviewed. These included epidemiological and clinical studies, retrospective questionnaire surveys, laboratory experiments, review articles, and more speculative, conceptual articles. Articles were not strictly graded, but more weight was given to empirical research using rigorous instruments, larger and more broadly representative samples, standardized methods of symptom reporting, adjustment for confounding factors (such as sociodemographic characteristics and medical morbidity), more sophisticated data analytic methods, and an acknowledgment and discussion of the study limitations and generalizibility.
Prevalence Of Symptoms
Women generally report more bodily distress and more numerous, more intense, and more frequent somatic symptoms than men. These differences emerge regardless of the time period inquired about, the response format used, and whether symptoms are recorded prospectively or retrospectively.1 The differences are seen in community samples, in medical patients, and from adolescence through old age.1 These findings are outlined in Table 1. However, the interpretation of these findings is difficult because studies vary in the methods used to elicit and measure symptoms, in whether all bodily symptoms are included or only those which appear to be medically unexplained, and in whether confounding factors (such as social position or psychiatric disorder) are taken into account.
Table 1.
Summary of Findings in Different Areas | References |
---|---|
Nonpatient populations | |
Women report more numerous, more frequent, and more chronic symptoms than men. | 2–10 |
Young girls may also report more symptoms than young boys. | 13–15 |
Medical populations | |
Women report more numerous symptoms. | 16,17 |
This differential persists after adjusting for medical co-morbidity, psychiatric disorder, and gynecologic and reproductive symptoms. | 17–22 |
Some studies of specific disease conditions show no difference in symptom reporting by gender, or higher reporting in men. | 8,25,26 |
It is unclear if the particular constellation of symptoms resulting from specific medical disorders differs in men and women. | 27–29 |
Experimental and laboratory studies | |
Women have lower pain threshold and tolerance, but this differential may be small and is sensitive to experimental conditions. | 3,4,30–32 |
This may be more attributable to a difference in reporting style than in discriminative ability. | 31,33–35 |
Female animals show more pronounced responses to experimental pain. | 4,19,–42 |
Somatoform disorders | |
The prevalence of somatoform disorder is higher in women. | 45–53 |
Hypochondriasis, however, is equally prevalent in men and women. | 54–57 |
Nonpatient Populations
Large-scale surveys of community residents in the United States and elsewhere have found higher rates of somatic symptoms and lower levels of somatic well-being in women.2–10 Thus, in population-based surveys, women are found to report more severe pain, more frequent pain, pain in more sites, and pain of longer duration than men.2–5 This remains the case even after excluding gynecologic and reproductive symptoms. For example, Kroenke and Priece6 examined the lifetime prevalence of nonmenstrual complaints in a population sample of 13,538 community residents. Symptoms were elicited with a structured diagnostic interview, and classified by severity and probable cause. Twenty of the 22 most common symptoms were more frequent in women; in particular, constipation, nausea, vomiting, fainting, headache, fatigue, dizziness, palpitations, and insomnia. In an older study, Verbrugge and Ascione7 asked a probability sample of community-dwelling adults to record symptoms daily in health diaries for 6 weeks. Women averaged 17.9 symptomatic days compared to 13.2 for men; the pattern of symptoms, however, was similar for men and women.7 A small number of reports fail to find sex differences8 in specific subpopulations such as college students11 or particular racial or socioeconomic groups.12
Sex differences in symptom reporting appear in childhood, but this literature is inconsistent. Studies of pain sensitivity in 3- to 12-year-old children in general show girls to be more sensitive to pain.13 Girls respond to pain more often by crying and require longer to calm after having received routine innoculations14 and after minor playground accidents.13 Other studies, however, have not found such sex differences in pain, for example following venipuncture.15
Medical Patients
There is extensive literature on symptom reporting in ambulatory medical populations. Here too, women generally report more symptoms, poorer perceived physical health, and less somatic well-being. For example, when the medical records of 1,000 patients followed over 3 years in an internal medicine clinic were examined, it was found that women on average reported 1.47 more symptoms than men.16,17 Ten of 13 nongynecologic symptoms were significantly more common in women, on the order of 1.5 to 2 times more frequent.17 When symptoms due to demonstrable disease were omitted from the analysis and only medically unexplained complaints studied, the gender differences persisted.17 Hibbard and Pope18 minimized the confounding effect of medical morbidity by including only healthy individuals and found that women patients scored significantly higher than men on an index of somatic symptoms. Gijsbers et al.19 concluded that the higher prevalence of symptoms in women is reduced but not eliminated when gynecologic and obstetric conditions are excluded from the analyses.
A few other studies have attempted to take into account the possible confounding effect of a sex differential in the prevalence of medical disease. Among consecutive patients presenting with the symptoms of acute myocardial infarction or unstable angina, women had lower rates of clinically significant coronary artery disease; 30.5% of women and 13.9% of men with unstable angina did not have clinically significant stenosis.20 Cunningham and Kelsey21 found that while women had more musculoskeletal complaints than men, musculoskeletal disorder was equally prevalent in the 2 groups on physical examination. In a study of coronary heart disease patients, women had higher rates of anginal chest pain, even after stratification on the number of stenosed vessels.22 Mechanic23,24 reported that although self-reports of hypertension and heart disease were higher in women than men, medical evaluation revealed similar rates of hypertension. Conversely, several studies suggest that the sex differential diminishes when studying symptoms which are due to demonstrable disease. Among patients with upper respiratory tract infections, men's symptoms were judged more disproportionate to physician expectations of symptom severity than women's.25 Symptoms in men and women were noted to be similar in colorectal cancer patients.26 In another study, men with osteoarthritis were more likely to report pain, independent of severity of disease.8
There has been relatively little investigation of the important question of whether men and women manifest the same diseases with different symptoms. Women sustaining an acute myocardial infarction have been found to report more nausea and dyspnea, and less diaphoresis than men.27 In acute appendicitis, men and women have been reported to present differing patterns of lower abdominal pain and rebound tenderness.28 Migraine headaches are more often accompanied by an aura in men than in women.29
Experimental and Laboratory Studies
The experimental induction of pain in the laboratory can also be used to study sex differences. In general, women appear to have a lower threshold and tolerance for experimental pain, and to report more discomfort than men.3,4,30–32 Although these findings are relatively consistent across studies, the sex differences are relatively small. In addition, the results may vary depending with the methods of pain induction and assessment,3 and may be influenced by confounding factors such as motivation, the experimental setting and procedure, and the investigator's conduct.3,4 Laboratory studies of pain are difficult to interpret but are valuable because when the results are subjected to a signal detection analysis, independent measures of discriminative acuity and of the tendency to describe or label all stimuli as painful (response bias) can be derived. Such signal detection analyses suggest that men and women differ more in their response bias than in their discriminative ability,31,33–35 i.e., they differ more in their general tendency to describe and label their sensations as painful than in their ability to distinguish accurately between slight differences in the intensity of painful stimuli.
Animal studies generally seem to disclose sex differences in behavioral and physiological responses to noxious stimuli.1,36 Female rodents, for example, exhibit a lower threshold for, and a more pronounced behavioral response to, acute and chronic experimentally induced pain.37–40 Female rats also show less analgesia with morphine than males,4,39,41,42 though these differences may vary depending upon the magnitude of the painful stimulus and the estrous cycle of the female rat.39,43,44
Somatoform Disorders
Medically unexplained symptoms are common in ambulatory medical patients, and are not necessarily psychopathological. Some patients, however, have medically unexplained symptoms that are so severe and intense, so disabling and disruptive, and so persistent and chronic that they are considered psychopathological and constitute a somatoform disorder. Such disorders are consistently more prevalent in women than in men,45–51 and the paradigmatic somatoform disorder, termed somatization disorder, occurs up to 10 times more frequently in women.47,52,53 Hypochondriasis is the major exception to this sex differential in somatoform disorders. Hypochondriasis, in which medically unexplained somatic symptoms are accompanied by the fear or belief that one has an undiagnosed disease, is equally prevalent in men and women.17,18,54–57 This suggests that women's elevated somatic distress is not accompanied by greater disease fears and health anxiety.
Factors Contributing To Sex Differences
Biological Differences
Neuroanatomical, neurophysiological, and neurobiological differences may give rise to differences in the perception, processing, and modulation of noxious somatic and visceral stimuli. Most of the work in this area has focused on pain perception. There may be sex differences in peripheral afferent pathways, in the central networks which integrate and process noxious sensation, and/or in the descending, efferent systems which modulate nociception.31 Little is known at present about sex differences in afferent pathways,31 but men and women may differ in the central processing of sensory information, in their pain regulatory systems58 and in their autonomic and physiological response to pain and other noxious stimuli.58 Thus, laboratory studies suggest that nociception may vary with the menstrual cycle. Women may be more sensitive to pain during the luteal phase,59,60 though there are also reports of heightened pain sensitivity premenstrually, at ovulation, and immediately following menses.31 Pain perception and inhibition appear to be influenced by (γ-aminobutyric acid (GABA) (and other neurotransmitter) activity, and GABA activity is hormonally dependent.4 In addition, the endogenous opioid systems are modulated by estrogen and other sex hormones.4,58
Symptom Appraisal and Assessment
Women and men may label and describe the same noxious sensation differently. Women may be more aware of and more attentive to weak or diffuse bodily stimuli which men do not perceive,61 and some studies suggest that women have greater bodily vigilance and awareness.62,63 This could result from the experiences of menstruation, menopause, pregnancy, and lactation which all serve to repeatedly call women's attention to their anatomy and physiology and to sensitize them to bodily changes.64 It is also possible that men and women appraise and evaluate bodily discomfort differently, and differ in how readily they conclude that ambiguous or mild sensations are indicative of disease.61 Thus, in one study, female college students were more likely to attribute a list of common somatic symptoms to serious disease than male students,62 though a survey of ambulatory medical patients did not confirm such a gender difference.65 Men and women may also differ in their recall of past medical experience, such that men forget past illness experiences more readily than women, and, in general, the differences in symptom reporting between men and women appear to be greater for past than for current symptoms.1 Thus, Pennebaker66 found that female college students recalled more somatic symptoms in the past month and past year than male students, but there were no differences when respondents were asked about current symptoms.66 This sort of recall bias is also suggested by a study of chronic pain patients who kept daily ratings of pain intensity and also provided a week-long summary rating of pain at the end of the week.67 Women summarized the previous week's pain experience as more intense than were the contemporaneous daily ratings made in real time, while men's retrospective recall and current ratings were less discrepant.67
Pennebaker, and Pennebaker and Roberts suggest that women use both external (e.g., situational information) and internal (somatic and visceral) cues in appraising bodily symptoms, while men rely more on internal cues.68,69 He points out that under controlled laboratory conditions, men seem to be more accurate perceivers of heart rate,70–72 stomach contractions,73 systolic blood pressure,74 blood glucose level,75 and finger temperature.66 However, in naturalistic studies conducted during daily life, no sex differences are evident in the accuracy of estimates of blood pressure, blood glucose, heart rhythm, and heart rate.69,74–76 Pennebaker, and Pennebaker and Roberts suggest that when they move from the laboratory into naturalistic settings, women are able to use external cues unavailable to them in the experimental setting (such as prior activity and exercise, food consumption, time of day, and social feedback), which improves the accuracy of their bodily perception.68,69
Socialization and Social Roles
The socialization process, which begins in earliest childhood, may profoundly influence bodily experience and the willingness to disclose and communicate distress. Boys are taught to be less expressive about illness and discomfort, to be more stoical, and to use more denial.77,78 Men may therefore be more reticent because they have been impressed with the importance of a “stiff upper lip,” not crying or “acting like a baby,” and ignoring pain and not admitting weakness, impairment, or distress.64,79 Conversely, it may be more socially acceptable for women to openly acknowledge distress and pain.19 Thus, men scoring higher on an inventory of masculinity had higher thresholds for experimental pain and were more stoical than men who had less masculine ratings.80 In a study of male and female athletes and nonathletes undergoing a cold pressor test,81 the female nonathletes were significantly more sensitive to the pain than the other 3 groups, but the female athletes were not.81
Socialization also influences the readiness or reluctance with which one consults a physician and assumes the patient role. Women generally have a lower threshold for seeking medical attention,82,83 their per capita use of health services is significantly higher than men's, and they average significantly more physician visits per year.19,84–89 This difference may be attributable, at least in part, to the socialization process, in which men and women are taught to deal differently with dependency and the disclosure of distress. Women may be more accepting of the dependency and passivity entailed in becoming a patient and visiting a doctor. Because they are more interpersonally oriented, and more affiliative and relational, women may find it easier to seek interpersonal help.90 In addition, healthy, young women are encouraged to obtain annual gynecologic “check-ups” and to make routine, pregnancy-related visits. More frequent contact with doctors and more extensive medical care could in turn further sensitize women to bodily sensation and discomfort, heightening self-scrutiny and bodily vigilance which in turn could increase symptom reporting. More frequent medical contact does not entirely explain the higher prevalence of symptoms in women however, since, as noted earlier, population-based surveys of nonpatient populations find the same sex differential in symptom reporting.6,17,91
Abuse and Trauma
Physical and sexual abuse and domestic violence in childhood have been linked to the subsequent development of chronic pain (particularly pelvic pain) and a range of medically unexplained complaints in adulthood.85,86,92–97 A history of sexual abuse in childhood is associated with an increased incidence in adulthood of pelvic, gynecologic, urologic, gastrointestinal, and pain complaints.98–102 These findings emerge when surveying nonpatient populations, and when comparing medical patients with functional disorders to patients matched for comparable organic diseases. Although the literature contains widely divergent estimates, the prevalence of childhood sexual abuse appears to be between 12% to 17% in girls and between 5% and 8% in boys.103 Since sexual abuse is more common in girls, this may account for some of the increased prevalence of somatic symptoms in women.
Current physical abuse and interpersonal violence in adulthood is also vastly more common among women than men and is associated with increases in somatic symptoms and health care utilization.104–108 The prevalence of currently experienced interpersonal violence among women in primary care practice varies between 6% and 29%, depending upon the study.109–111 While comparable data for men are not available, women are 8 to 25 times more likely to be subject to interpersonal violence from an intimate partner than are men.109,112 Such current, interpersonal violence obviously results in more acute trauma and injury, as well as in more chronic symptoms such as headaches, chronic pain, menstrual symptoms, sleep disorders, gastrointestinal symptoms, and more somatization.104,105,109,113
Depressive and Anxiety Disorders, and Generalized Psychological Distress
Depressive and anxiety disorders are 2 to 3 times more prevalent in women.114–121 Although depressive and anxiety disorders may be diagnosed more readily in women than in men, the most careful and rigorous population-based, epidemiological surveys find a consistent sex differential which is thought to represent true prevalence rates.122 Since these disorders often have prominent somatic features123–126 and often go undetected and untreated by primary care physicians,127–129 they likely contribute to the higher prevalence of somatic complaints in women. In addition, anxiety and depressive disorders may assume a somatized guise more often in women than in men.130 However, the differential prevalence rates of these psychiatric disorders do not entirely explain the sex differences in somatic symptoms in primary care patients: When the presence of depressive and anxiety disorder is controlled for statistically, the sex differential in symptom reporting declines, but remains significant.17 However, in another study of primary care patients, the association between sex and somatic symptoms disappeared when a measure of psychiatric disorder was taken into account statistically.131 This underscores the complexity of these relationships and points out that these causes are not mutually exclusive but rather are more likely to be interactive.
Women have higher levels of negative affectivity than men,132,133 and this could also mediate the relationship between gender and somatic symptoms. Negative affectivity is a stable, enduring personality characteristic, the tendency to experience psychological distress in general and to report a broad array of psychiatric symptoms such as loneliness, anxiety, low self-esteem, and guilt. Since negative affectivity is highly correlated with somatic symptoms and bodily complaints,134–136 and since this personality trait is more prominent in women,132,133 it may account for some of the gender differences in somatic symptom reporting.
Gender Bias in Research and Clinical Practice
Some of the reported gender differences in symptoms may result from unacknowledged biases in epidemiological research and medical practice. Women may simply be more willing than men to reveal distress and health problems.138 Survey research may be confounded by an interaction effect between the sex of the respondent and that of the interviewer such that respondents may confide more readily in an interviewer of the same sex, and most interviewers may be female.138,139 In laboratory studies of pain, the sex of the experimenter may influence the subjects' pain response: Men may report less pain to a female than to a male experimenter,140 though this effect is not found consistently.2 Additionally, since positive findings are more likely to find their way into the literature than negative findings, gender differences are more likely to be emphasized and published than the absence of such differences.2
Gender bias in clinical practice may also contribute to reported differences in symptoms. To the extent that women patients more openly express emotional difficulties and psychosocial distress, they may be more readily viewed as emotionally disturbed, histrionic, or somatizing.141 Clinicians may then be quicker to conclude that diffuse or nonspecific symptoms have no medical explanation in women, more likely to ascribe such symptoms to psychosocial causes,142 and more ready to ascribe them to a somatoform disorder.143 This in turn could result in less vigorous attempts to ascertain a medical basis for the complaints, and less serious consideration of all possible medical etiologies. For example, some studies indicate that women with chest pain receive less aggressive medical assessment than men.144 Nurses may address pain differently in men and women, depending upon the nurses' opinions about gender differences in pain reporting.145
A summary of the factors contributing to gender differences in symptoms may be found in Table 2.
Table 2.
Summary of Findings in Different Areas | References |
---|---|
Biological differences | |
There may be differences in nociception. | 31,58 |
There may be differences in autonomic and physiological responses to pain. | 4,58 |
Symptom appraisal and assessment | |
Women may have greater somatic awareness generally. | 61–64 |
Women have greater recall of prior symptoms. | 1,66,67 |
Women use more external cues in assessing somatic sensations. | 68,69 |
Socialization and social roles | |
Men are socialized to be more stoical. | 64,77,78,79 |
Women are encouraged to acknowledge distress. | 19,80,81 |
Men resist assuming sick role more than women. | 19,82–90 |
Abuse and trauma | |
Childhood abuse is associated with greater symptom reporting in adulthood. | 85,86,92–102 |
The incidence of abuse is higher in girls. | 103 |
Current abuse is associated with greater symptom reporting. | 104–109, 113 |
The incidence of current abuse higher in women. | 110–113 |
Depressive and anxiety disorders, and generalized psychological distress | |
Depressive and anxiety disorders more prevalent in women. | 114–121 |
Self-reported, generalized psychological distress is greater in women. | 132, 133 |
Gender bias | |
There is evidence of gender bias in research. | 137–140 |
There is evidence of gender bias in clinical practice. | 141–144 |
Implications For Medical Practice
Women appear to experience more numerous, more frequent, and more intense bodily symptoms than men. This seems to be the case whether one examines all symptoms, excludes gynecologic and reproductive symptoms, or restricts the inquiry to medically unexplained symptoms. The finding emerges when studying community residents, medical populations, and laboratory subjects, and a similar differential may exist in children and in animals. These generalizations obscure many exceptions, complexities, and discrepancies in the phenomena being studied and the limitations and inadequacies of our studies. The gender differential is likely to be affected by ethnicity, race, upbringing, personality, medical history, and a myriad of factors that have barely been examined. However, an overview of this large and complex literature suggests at least 3 factors which contribute to differences between men and women. First, women have a higher prevalence of several common psychiatric disorders (particularly anxiety and depression) which themselves have prominent somatic features. Second, women have higher rates of current and past abuse and trauma, which in turn are associated with medical help-seeking and somatic symptom reporting. Third, women and men seem to differ in their thresholds for judging and considering a given sensation to be noxious, unpleasant, and bothersome—i.e., for labeling and describing the sensation as a symptom. This reporting bias may well result from powerful socialization forces which begin in childhood. The necessarily tentative nature of these conclusions underscores the need for further study. In particular, future efforts should explore the nature of the sex differential rather than its magnitude; it is particularly crucial to examine the ways in which common diseases manifest themselves differently in men and women. If the symptom presentations of men and women with the same disease differ, this has important implications for the history taking and the diagnostic process.
How are clinicians to interpret these differences? The findings surveyed in this paper may be statistically significant, but what is their clinical significance? Population studies conceal vast interindividual variability, and the aggregated findings from large data sets can not be applied to any given patient. Although populations of men and women may differ on average, there is enormous overlap, and many individual men and women do not differ, or may even differ in a direction opposite to that of their gender as a whole. The clinician must therefore be careful in applying these generalizations to individual patients. Although these findings should never provide a rationale for suspecting the authenticity or credibility of a given patient's symptoms, or for taking their potential medical significance more lightly, they do have some clinical implications.
First, one should not conclude that women are overreporters who dramatize and exaggerate trivial sensations and benign dysfunction; it can be equally concluded that men are insensitive perceivers and poor historians who ignore, suppress, or are unaware of much bodily experience. Indeed, as we have seen, some findings suggest that men forget more previous symptoms and illnesses than women do. Men's and women's styles of symptoms reporting are simply different.
Second, some patients may find it more difficult to relate their symptoms to clinicians of the opposite sex, and at times, the clinician may need to ask whether this poses a problem in telling their stories. Some men, for example, may need explicit encouragement to acknowledge the extent or severity of their distress to a female physician.
Third, this review serves to remind us that the symptoms physicians elicit from patients are sensitive to, and influenced by, many factors. The symptoms reported to a given physician on a given occasion may depend on the patient's reporting style, readiness or reluctance to disclose distress, history of trauma, and the circumstantial and social input the patient used in evaluating and appraising his/her symptoms, as well as on how the questions are asked and whether the symptoms asked about are current or past. Symptoms may be thought of as a final common pathway, the end result of a number of disparate forces. These include not only organ pathology, but also learned illness behaviors, life stress, social and interpersonal communication patterns, and psychological distress. When somatic symptoms are disproportionate to demonstrable pathology, the physician should inquire about prior and current abuse and trauma, and should search for evidence of somatized anxiety and depressive disorder. It must be made clear to all patients that such factors are part of comprehensive medical care, are a legitimate subject for physician-patient dialog, and, when present, deserve an ambitious therapeutic response.
Fourth, the findings reported here point out the gender bias which can occur in clinical practice and clinical research. This review should serve to increase physicians' awareness of possible sources of gender bias as much as to increase their awareness of gender differences.
Finally, it is important to remember that symptoms have 2 distinct sorts of clinical significance. They are indicators of the onset or progression of a disease process and, as such, serve as guides in the physician's diagnostic search. However, symptoms are also significant in themselves because they are the substance of the patient's experience. As sources of distress and suffering, they deserve palliation in their own right, and they form the foundation of the therapeutic alliance, regardless of whatever diagnostic information they may contain. In this sense, women's tendency to higher levels of somatic distress means they may require more strenuous efforts at symptomatic treatment. (That this may be a particular problem is suggested by a study disclosing that while both female and male patients are undermedicated for pain, the gap is greater for women.146 It is the physician's task to ameliorate and assuage the distress of all patients; to the degree that women tend to have more such distress, the physician's responsibility is thereby increased.
This discussion also serves as a reminder that all symptoms are “real,” and underscores the paramount clinical imperative to elicit, attend to, and understand the personal meaning and significance of each individual patient's symptoms.
Acknowledgments
Supported by research grant MH40487 from the National Institute of Mental Health. The authors would like to acknowledge the assistance of Beverly Woo, MD and Leonard M. Rubin, MD in reviewing the manuscript.
References
- 1.van Wijk CM, Kolk AM. Sex differences in physical symptoms: the contribution of symptom perception theory. Soc Sci Med. 1997;45:231–46. doi: 10.1016/s0277-9536(96)00340-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Unruh AM. Gender variations in clinical pain experience. Pain. 1996;65:123–67. doi: 10.1016/0304-3959(95)00214-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Riley JL, Robinson ME, Wise EA, Myers CD, Fillingim RB. Sex differences in the perception of noxious experimental stimuli: a meta-analysis. Pain. 1998;74:181–7. doi: 10.1016/s0304-3959(97)00199-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Berkley KJ. Sex differences in pain. Behav Brain Sci. 1997;20:371–80. doi: 10.1017/s0140525x97221485. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Neitzert CS, Davis C, Kennedy SH. Personality factors related to the prevalence of somatic symptoms and medical complaints in a healthy student population. Br J Med Psychol. 1997;70:93–101. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8341.1997.tb01889.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Kroenke K, Price RK. Symptoms in the community. Arch Intern Med. 1993;153:2474–80. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Verbrugge LM, Ascione FJ. Exploring the iceberg: common symptoms and how people care for them. Med Care. 1987;25:539–69. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Davis MA. Sex differences in reporting osteoarthritic symptoms: a sociomedical approach. J Health Soc Behav. 1981;22:298–310. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Green RL. The MMPI: An Interpretive Manual. New York: Grune & Stratton; 1980. [Google Scholar]
- 10.Piccinelli M, Simon G. Gender and cross-cultural differences in somatic symptoms associated with emotional distress. Psychol Med. 1997;27:433–44. doi: 10.1017/s0033291796004539. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Pennebaker JW, Burnam MA, Schaeffer MA, Harper DC. Lack of control as a determinant of perceived physical symptoms. J Personal Soc Psychol. 1998;35:167–74. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.35.3.167. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.McDonald RL, Gynther MD. MMPI differences associated with sex, race and class in two adolescent samples. J Consult Psychol. 1963;27:112–6. [Google Scholar]
- 13.Fearon I, McGrath PJ, Achat H. ‘Booboos’: the study of everyday pain among young children. Pain. 1996;68:55–62. doi: 10.1016/S0304-3959(96)03200-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Schecter NL, Bernstein BA, Beck A, Hart L. Individual differences in children's response to pain: role of temperament and parental characteristics. Pediatrics. 1991;83:171–7. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Fowler-Kelly S, Lander J. Assessment of sex differences in children's and adolescents' self-reported pain from venipuncture. J Ped Psychol. 1991;16:783–93. doi: 10.1093/jpepsy/16.6.783. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Kroenke K, Mangelsdorff AD. Common symptoms in ambulatory care: incidence, evaluation, therapy, and outcome. Am J Med. 1989;86:262–6. doi: 10.1016/0002-9343(89)90293-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Kroenke K, Spitzer RL. Gender differences in the reporting of physical and somatoform symptoms. Psychosom Med. 1998;60:150–5. doi: 10.1097/00006842-199803000-00006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Hibbard JH, Pope CR. Gender roles, illness orientation and the use of medical services. Soc Sci Med. 1983;17:129–37. doi: 10.1016/0277-9536(83)90246-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Gijsbers van Wijk CM, van Vliet KP, Kolk AM, Everaerd WT. Symptom sensitivity and sex differences in physical morbidity: a review of health surveys in the United States and the Netherlands. Women Health. 1991;17:91–124. doi: 10.1300/J013v17n01_06. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Hochman JS, Tamis JE, Thompson TD, et al. Sex, clinical presentation, and outcome in patients with acute coronary syndromes. New Engl J Med. 1999;341:226–32. doi: 10.1056/NEJM199907223410402. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Cunningham LS, Kelsey JL. Epidemiology of musculoskeletal impairments and associated disability. Am J Public Health. 1984;74:574–9. doi: 10.2105/ajph.74.6.574. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Davis KB, Chaitman B, Ryan T, Bittner V, Kennedy JW. Comparison of 15-year survival for men and women after initial medical or surgical treatment for coronary artery disease: a CASS registry study. Coronary Artery Surgery Study. J Am Coll Cardiol. 1995;25:1000–9. doi: 10.1016/0735-1097(94)00518-u. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Mechanic D. Medical Sociology. 2nd ed. New York: The Free Press (Macmillan); 1978. [Google Scholar]
- 24.Mechanic D. Sex, illness, illness behavior and the use of health services. J Hum Stress. 1976;2:29–40. [Google Scholar]
- 25.Macintyre S. Gender differences in the perceptions of common cold symptoms. Soc Sci Med. 1993;36:15–20. doi: 10.1016/0277-9536(93)90301-J. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Marshall JR, Funch DP. Gender and illness behavior among colorectal cancer patients. Women Health. 1986;11:67–82. doi: 10.1300/j013v11n03_06. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Meischke H, Larsen MP, Eisenberg MS. Gender differences in reported symptoms for acute myocardial infarction: impact of prehospital delay time interval. Am J Emerg Med. 1998;16:363–6. doi: 10.1016/s0735-6757(98)90128-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Eskelinen M, Ikonen J, Lipponen P. Sex-specific diagnostic scores for acute appendicitis. Scand J Gastroenterol. 1994;29:59–66. doi: 10.3109/00365529409090438. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Rasmussen BK, Jensen R, Schroll M, Olesen J. Interrelations between migraine and tension-type headache in the general population. Arch Neurol. 1992;49:914–8. doi: 10.1001/archneur.1992.00530330036012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Wolfe F, Ross K, Anderson J, Russell IJ. Aspects of fibromyalgia in the general population: sex, pain threshold, and fibromyalgia symptoms. J Rheumatol. 1995;22:151–6. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Fillingim RB, Maixner W. Gender differences in the response to noxious stimuli. Pain Forum. 1995;4:209–21. [Google Scholar]
- 32.Feine JS, Bushnell MC, Miron D, Duncan GH. Sex differences in the perception of oxious heat stimuli. Pain. 1991;44:255–62. doi: 10.1016/0304-3959(91)90094-E. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Clark WC. Pain measurement by signal detection theory. In: Adelman G, editor. Encyclopedia of Neuroscience. Boston: Birkhauser; 1987. pp. 911–3. In: [Google Scholar]
- 34.Clark WC, Mehl L. A secondary decision theory analysis of the effect age and sex on d', various response criteria, and 50% pain threshold. J Abnorm Psychol. 1971;78:202–12. doi: 10.1037/h0031800. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Clark WC, Goodman JS. Effects of suggestion on d' and cx for pain detection and pain tolerance. J Abnorm Psychol. 1974;83:364–72. doi: 10.1037/h0036790. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Aloisi AM. Sex differences in pain-induced effects on the septo-hippocampal system. Brain Res Rev. 1997;25:397–406. doi: 10.1016/s0165-0173(97)00030-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Aloisi AM, Albonetti ME, Carli G. Sex differences in the behavioural response to persistent pain in rats. Neurosci Let. 1994;179:79–82. doi: 10.1016/0304-3940(94)90939-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Beatty WW, Beatty PA. Hormonal determinations of sex differences in avoidance behavior and reactivity to electric shock in the rat. J Comp Physiol Psychol. 1970;73:446–55. doi: 10.1037/h0030216. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Bodnar RJ, Romero MT, Kramer E. Organismic variables and pain inhibition: roles of gender and aging. Brain Res Bull. 1988;21:947–53. doi: 10.1016/0361-9230(88)90032-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Steenbergen HL, Farabollini F, Heinsbrook RPW, Van de Poll NE. Sex-dependent effects of aversive stimulation on holeboard and elevated plus-maze behavior. Behav Brain Res. 1991;43:159–63. doi: 10.1016/s0166-4328(05)80066-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Baamonde AI, Hidalgo A, Andres-Trelles F. Sex related differences in the effects of morphine and stress on visceral pain. Neuropharmacology. 1989;28:967–70. doi: 10.1016/0028-3908(89)90197-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Kepler KL, Standifer KM, Paul D, Kest B, Pasternak GW, Bodnar RJ. Gender effects and central opioid analgesia. Pain. 1991;45:87–94. doi: 10.1016/0304-3959(91)90168-W. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Forman LJ, Tingle V, Estilow S, Cater J. The response to analgesia testing is influenced by gonadal steroids in the rat. Life Sci. 1989;45:447–54. doi: 10.1016/0024-3205(89)90631-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Martinez-Gomez M, Cruz Y, Salas M, Hudson R, Pacheco P. Assessing pain threshold in the rat: changes with estrus and time of day. Physiol Behav. 1994;55:651–7. doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(94)90040-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Escobar JI, Rubio-Stipec M, Canino G, Karno M. Somatic Symptom Index (SSI): a new abridged somatization construct. Prevalence and epidemiological correlates in two large community samples. J Nerv Ment Dis. 1989;177:140–6. doi: 10.1097/00005053-198903000-00003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Escobar JI, Burnham A, Karno M, Forsythe A, Golding JM. Somatization in the community. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1987;44:713–8. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1987.01800200039006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Swartz M, Landerman R, George L, et al. Somatization disorder. In: Robins LN, Regier DA, et al., editors. Psychiatric Disorders in America. New York: Free Press; 1991. pp. 220–57. In: [Google Scholar]
- 48.Smith GR. Somatization disorder and undifferentiated somatoform disorder. In: Gabbard GO, editor. Treatments of Psychiatric Disorders. 2nd ed. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press; 1995. pp. 1718–33. In: [Google Scholar]
- 49.Portegijs PJM, van der Horst FG, Proot IM, Kraan HF, Gunther NCHF, Knottnerus JA. Somatization in frequent attenders of general practice. Soc Psychiat Psychiatr Epidemiol. 1996;31:29–37. doi: 10.1007/BF00789119. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Kroenke K, Spitzer RL, deGruy FV, et al. Multisomatoform disorder. An alternative to undiffferentiated somatoform disorder for the somatizing patient in primary care. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1997;54:352–8. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1997.01830160080011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Toner BB. Gender differences in somatoform disorders. In: Seeman MV, editor. Gender and Psychopathology. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press; 1995. pp. 287–310. In: [Google Scholar]
- 52.Swartz M, Blazer D, George L, Landerman R. Somatization disorder in a community population. Am J Psychiatry. 1986;143:1403–8. doi: 10.1176/ajp.143.11.1403. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Smith GR, Monson RA, Livingston RL. Somatization disorder in men. Gen Hosp Psychiatry. 1985;7:4–8. doi: 10.1016/0163-8343(85)90003-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Hernandez J, Kellner R. Hypochondriacal concerns and attitudes toward illness in males and females. Int J Psychiatry Med. 1992;22:251–63. doi: 10.2190/W9KG-6HU9-5QJX-NW76. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Kirmayer LJ, Robbins JM. Three forms of somatization in primary care: prevalence, co-occurrence and sociodemographic characteristics. J Nerv Ment Dis. 1991;179:647–55. doi: 10.1097/00005053-199111000-00001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Barsky AJ, Wyshak G, Klerman GL. Hypochondriasis: an evaluation of the DSM-III criteria in medical outpatients. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1986;43:493–500. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1986.01800050099013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Barsky AJ, Wyshak G, Klerman GL, Latham KS. The prevalence of hypochondriasis in medical outpatients. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol. 1990;25:89–94. doi: 10.1007/BF00794988. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Derbyshire SWG. Sources of variation in assessing male and female responses to pain. New Ideas Psychol. 1997;15:83–95. [Google Scholar]
- 59.Fillingim RB, Maixner W, Girdler SS, et al. Ischemic but not thermal pain sensitivity varies across the menstrual cycle. Psychosom Med. 1997;59:512–20. doi: 10.1097/00006842-199709000-00008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Fleeger P, Straneva P, Fillingim RB, Maixner W, Girdler SS. Menstrual cycle, blood pressure and ischemic pain sensitivity in women: a preliminary investigation. Int J Psychophysiol. 1997;27:161–6. doi: 10.1016/s0167-8760(97)00058-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Verbrugge LM. Sex differences in complaints and diagnoses. J Behav Med. 1980;3:327–55. doi: 10.1007/BF00845289. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Lieban RW. Gender and symptom sensitivity: report on a Philippine study. Am J Orthopsychiatry. 1985;55:446–50. doi: 10.1111/j.1939-0025.1985.tb03462.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Warner CD. Somatic awareness and coronary artery disease in women with chest pain. Heart Lung. 1995;24:436–43. doi: 10.1016/s0147-9563(95)80021-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Verbruggev LM. Sex differentials in health. Public Health Rep. 1982;97:417–37. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Barsky AJ, Coeytaux RR, Sarnie MK, Cleary PD. Hypochondriacal patients'beliefs about good health. Am J Psychiatry. 1993;150:1085–9. doi: 10.1176/ajp.150.7.1085. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Pennebaker JW. The Psychology of Physical Symptoms. New York: Springer-Verlag; 1982. [Google Scholar]
- 67.Jamison RN, Sbrocco T, Parris WCV. The influence of physical and psychosocial factors on accuracy of memory for pain in chronic pain patients. Pain. 1989;37:289–94. doi: 10.1016/0304-3959(89)90193-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Pennebaker JW. Beyond laboratory-based cardiac perception: ecological interoception. In: Vaitl D, Schandry R, editors. From the Heart to the Brain: The Psychophysiology of Circulation-Brain Interaction. Frankfurt, Germany: Peter Lang Publishers; 1995. pp. 389–406. In: [Google Scholar]
- 69.Pennebaker JW, Roberts TA. Toward a his and hers theory of emotion: gender differences in visceral perception. J Soc Clin Psychol. 1992;11:199–212. [Google Scholar]
- 70.Jones GE, O'Leary RT, Pipkin BL. Comparison of the Brener-Jones and Whitehead procedures for assessing cardiac awareness. Psychophysiology. 1984;21:143–8. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.1984.tb00196.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71.Katkin ES, Blascovich J, Goldband S. Empirical assessment of visceral self-perception: individual and sex differences in the acquisition of heartbeat discrimination. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1981;40:1095–101. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.40.6.1095. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72.Pennebaker JW, Hoover CW. Visceral perception versus visceral detection: a comparison of methods and assumptions. Biofeedback Self-Regulation. 1984;9:339–52. doi: 10.1007/BF00998977. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73.Whitehead WE, Drescher VM. Perception of gastric contractions and self-control of gastric motility. Psychophysiology. 1980;17:552–8. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.1980.tb02296.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74.Pennebaker JW, Watson D. Blood pressure estimation and beliefs among normotensives and hypertensives. Health Psychol. 1988;7:309–28. doi: 10.1037//0278-6133.7.4.309. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 75.Cox DJ, Clarke WL, Gonder-Frederick LA, et al. Accuracy of perceiving blood glucose in IDDM. Diabetes Care. 1985;8:529–35. doi: 10.2337/diacare.8.6.529. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 76.Barsky AJ, Cleary PD, Barnett MC, Christiansen CL, Ruskin JN. The accuracy of symptom reporting in patients complaining of palpitations. Am J Med. 1994;97:214–21. doi: 10.1016/0002-9343(94)90003-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 77.Chesler P. Women and Madness. New York: Avon; 1972. [Google Scholar]
- 78.Mechanic D. Social psychologic factors affecting the presentation of bodily complaints. New Engl J Med. 1972;285:1132–9. doi: 10.1056/NEJM197205252862105. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 79.Lipsitt DR. The painful woman: complaints, symptoms, and illness. In: Notman MT, Nadelson CC, editors. Women in Context: The Woman Patient. New York: Plenum; 1982. pp. 147–52. In: [Google Scholar]
- 80.Otto MW, Dougher MJ. Sex differences and personality factors in responsivity to pain. Percept Mot Skills. 1985;61:383–90. doi: 10.2466/pms.1985.61.2.383. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 81.Hall EG, Davies S. Gender differences in perceived intensity and affect of pain between athletes and nonathletes. Percept Mot Skills. 1991;73:779–86. doi: 10.2466/pms.1991.73.3.779. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 82.Verbrugge LM. Gender and health: an update on hypotheses and evidence. J Health Soc Behav. 1985;26:156–82. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 83.Lewis CE, Lewis MA, Lorimer A. The use of school nurturing services by children in an “adult-free” system. Pediatrics. 1977;60:499–507. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 84.Mustard CA, Kaufert P, Kozyrskyj A, Mayer T. Sex differences in the use of health care services. New Engl J Med. 1998;338:1678–83. doi: 10.1056/NEJM199806043382307. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 85.McCauley J, Kern DE, Kolodner K, et al. Clinical characteristics of women with a history of childhood abuse. JAMA. 1997;277:1362–8. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 86.Craig TKJ, Boardman AP, Mills K, Daley-Jones O, Drake H. The South London Somatization Study. I: Longitudinal course and the influence of early life experiences. Br J Psychiatry. 1993;163:579–88. doi: 10.1192/bjp.163.5.579. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 87.Waldron I. Sex differences in illness incidence, prognosis, and mortality: issues and evidence. Soc Sci Med. 1983;17:1107–23. doi: 10.1016/0277-9536(83)90004-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 88.Gove WR. Gender differences in mental and physical illness: the effects of fixed roles and nurturant roles. Soc Sci Med. 1984;19:77–91. doi: 10.1016/0277-9536(84)90273-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 89.Hoeper EW, Nycz GR, Regier DA, Goldberg ID, Jacobson A, Harkin J. Diagnosis of mental disorder in adults and increased use of health services in four outpatient settings. Am J Psychiatry. 1980;137:207–10. doi: 10.1176/ajp.137.2.207. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 90.Moore SM. A comparison of women's and men's symptoms during home recovery after coronary artery bypass surgery. Heart Lung. 1995;24:495–501. doi: 10.1016/s0147-9563(95)80027-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 91.Hammond EC. Some preliminary findings on physical complaints from a prospective study of 1,064,004 men and women. Am J Public Health. 1964;54:11–23. doi: 10.2105/ajph.54.1.11. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 92.Briere J, Runtz M. Symptomatology associated with childhood sexual victimization in a nonclinical adult sample. Child Abuse Negl. 1988;12:51–9. doi: 10.1016/0145-2134(88)90007-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 93.Walker EA, Katon W, Hansom J, Harrop-Griffiths J, Holm L, Jones L. Medical and psychiatric symptoms in women with childhood sexual abuse. Psychosom Med. 1992;54:658–64. doi: 10.1097/00006842-199211000-00005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 94.Drossman DA, Leserman J, Nachman G, Zhiming Le M, Gluck H, Tooney TC. Sexual and physical abuse in women with functional or organic gastrointestinal disorders. Ann Intern Med. 1990;113:828–33. doi: 10.7326/0003-4819-113-11-828. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 95.Morrison J. Childhood sexual histories of women with somatization disorder. Am J Psychiatry. 1989;146:239–41. doi: 10.1176/ajp.146.2.239. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 96.Lowenstein RJ. Somatoform disorders in victims of incest and child abuse. In: Kluft R, editor. Incest-Related Syndromes of Adult Psychopathology. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press; 1990. pp. 75–111. In: [Google Scholar]
- 97.Cunningham J, Pearce T, Pearce P. Childhood sexual abuse and medical complaints in adult women. J Interpers Viol. 1988;3:131–44. [Google Scholar]
- 98.Laws A. Does a history of sexual abuse in childhood play a role in women's medical problems? A review. J Wom Health. 1993;2:165–72. [Google Scholar]
- 99.Talley NJ, Fett SL, Zinsmeister NR, Milton LJ. Gastrointestinal tract symptoms and self-reported abuse: a population-based study. Gastroenterology. 1994;107:1040–9. doi: 10.1016/0016-5085(94)90228-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 100.Lechner ME, Vogel ME, Garcia-Shelton LM, Leichner JL, Steibel KR. Self-reported medical problems of adult female survivors of childhood sexual abuse. J Fam Pract. 1993;36:633–8. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 101.Walker E, Katon W, Harrop-Griffiths J, Holm L, Russo J, Hickok LR. Relationship of chronic pelvic pain to psychiatric diagnoses and childhood sexual abuse. Am J Psychiatry. 1988;145:75–9. doi: 10.1176/ajp.145.1.75. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 102.Leserman J, Drossman DA. Sexual and physical abuse history and medical practice. Gen Hosp Psychiatry. 1995;17:71–4. doi: 10.1016/0163-8343(94)00069-p. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 103.Gorey KM, Leslie DR. The prevalence of child sexual abuse: integrative review adjustment for potential response and measurement biases. Child Abuse Negl. 1997;21:391–8. doi: 10.1016/s0145-2134(96)00180-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 104.McCauley J, Kern DE, Kolodner K, et al. The “battering syndrome”: prevalence and clinical characteristics of domestic violence in primary care internal medicine practices. Ann Intern Med. 1995;123:774–81. doi: 10.7326/0003-4819-123-10-199511150-00001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 105.Jaffe P, Wolfe DA, Wilson S, Zak L. Emotional and physical health problems of battered women. Can J Psychiatry. 1986;31:625–9. doi: 10.1177/070674378603100705. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 106.Haber JD, Roos C. Effects of spouse abuse in the development and maintenance of chronic pain in women. Adv Pain Res. 1985;9:889–95. [Google Scholar]
- 107.Pribor EF, Yutzy SH, Dean T, Wetzel RD. Briquet's syndrome, dissociation, and abuse. Am J Psychiatry. 1993;150:1507–11. doi: 10.1176/ajp.150.10.1507. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 108.Reiter RC, Shakerin LR, Gambone DO, Milburn AK. Correlation between sexual abuse and somatization in women with somatic and nonsomatic chronic pelvic pain. Am J Obstet Gynecol. 1991;165:104–9. doi: 10.1016/0002-9378(91)90235-j. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 109.Naumann P, Langford D, Torres S, Campbell J, Glass N. Women battering in primary care practice. Fam Pract. 1999;16:343–52. doi: 10.1093/fampra/16.4.343. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 110.McCauley J, Kern DE, Kolodner K, Derogatis LR, Bass EB. Relation of low-severity violence to women's health. J Gen Intern Med. 1998;13:687–91. doi: 10.1046/j.1525-1497.1998.00205.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 111.Elliott BA, Johnson MM. Domestic violence in a primary care setting. Patterns and prevalence. Arch Fam Med. 1995;4:113–9. doi: 10.1001/archfami.4.2.113. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 112.Haywood YC, Haile-Mariam T. Violence against women. Emerg Med Clin North Am. 1999;17:603–15. doi: 10.1016/s0733-8627(05)70085-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 113.Koss MP, Heslet L. Somatic consequences of violence against women. Arch Fam Med. 1992;1:53–9. doi: 10.1001/archfami.1.1.53. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 114.Linzer M, Spitzer R, Kroenke K, et al. Gender, quality of life, and mental disorders in primary care: results from the PRIME-MD 1000 study. Am J Med. 1996;101:526–33. doi: 10.1016/s0002-9343(96)00275-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 115.Katon W. Panic Disorder in the Medical Setting. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press; 1991. [Google Scholar]
- 116.Barrett JE, Barrett JA, Oxman TE, Gerber PD. The prevalence of psychiatric disorders in a primary care practice. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1988;45:1100–6. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1988.01800360048007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 117.Murphy JM. Trends in depression and anxiety: men and women. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 1986;73:113–27. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.1986.tb10576.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 118.Weissman MM, Klerman GL. Sex differences and the epidemiology of depression. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1977;34:98–111. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1977.01770130100011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 119.Regier DA, Boyd JH, Burke Jd, Jr, et al. One-month prevalence of mental disorders in the United States. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1988;45:977–86. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1988.01800350011002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 120.Eaton WW, Dryman A, Weissman MM. Panic and Phobia. In: Robins LN, Regier DA, editors. Psychiatric Disorders in America. New York, NY: The Free Press; 1991. pp. 155–79. In: [Google Scholar]
- 121.Weissman MM, Bruce ML, Leaf PJ, Florio LP, Holzer C. Affective Disorders. In: Robins LN, Regier DA, editors. Psychiatric Disorders in America. New York, NY: The Free Press; 1991. pp. 53–80. In: [Google Scholar]
- 122.Robins LN, Regier DA. Psychiatric Disorders in America. New York, NY: The Free Press; 1991. [Google Scholar]
- 123.Kroenke K, Spitzer RL, Williams JBW, et al. Physical symptoms in primary care: predictors of psychiatric disorders and functional impairment. Arch Fam Med. 1994;3:774–9. doi: 10.1001/archfami.3.9.774. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 124.Katon W, Kleinman A, Rosen G. Depression and somatization: a review, part I. Am J Med. 1982;72:127–35. doi: 10.1016/0002-9343(82)90599-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 125.Katon W, Lin E, Von Korff M, Russo J, Lipscomb P, Bush T. Somatization: a spectrum of severity. Am J Psychiatry. 1991;148:34–40. doi: 10.1176/ajp.148.7.A34. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 126.Nielsen AC, Williams TA. Depression in ambulatory medical patients: prevalence by self-report questionnaire and recognition by nonpsychiatric physicians. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1980;37:999–1004. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1980.01780220037003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 127.Kirmayer LJ, Robbins JM, Dworkind M, Yaffe MJ. Somatization and the recognition of depression and anxiety in primary care. Am J Psychiatry. 1993;150:734–41. doi: 10.1176/ajp.150.5.734. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 128.Oxman TE, Harrigan J, Kues J. Diagnostic patterns of family physicians for somatoform, depressive, and anxiety disorders. J Fam Pract. 1983;17:439–46. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 129.Marks JN, Goldberg DP, Hillier VF. Determinants of the ability of general practitioners to detect psychiatric illness. Psychol Med. 1979;9:337–53. doi: 10.1017/s0033291700030853. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 130.Silverstein B, Blumenthal E. Depression mixed with anxiety, somatization and disordered eating: relationships with gender-role-related limitations experienced by females. Sex Roles. 1997;36:709–24. [Google Scholar]
- 131.Piccinelli M, Simon G. Gender and cross-cultural differences in somatic symptoms associated with emotional distress. An international study in primary care. Psychol Med. 1997;27:433–44. doi: 10.1017/s0033291796004539. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 132.Lynn R, Martin T. Gender differences in extroversion, neuroticism, and psychoticism in 37 nations. J Soc Psychol. 1997;137:369–73. doi: 10.1080/00224549709595447. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 133.Jorm AF. Sex differences in neuroticism: a quantitative synthesis of published research. Aust N Z J Psychiatry. 1987;21:501–6. doi: 10.3109/00048678709158917. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 134.Kirmayer LJ, Robbins JM, Paris J. Somatoform disorders: personality and the social matrix of distress. J Abnorm Psychol. 1994;103:125–36. doi: 10.1037//0021-843x.103.1.125. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 135.Pennebaker JW, Watson D. The psychology of somatic symptoms. In: Kirmayer LJ, Robbins JM, editors. Current Concepts of Somatization: Research and Clinical Perspectives. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association Press; 1991. pp. 24–35. In: [Google Scholar]
- 136.Larsen RJ. Neuroticism and selective encoding and recall of symptoms: evidence from a combined concurrent-retrospective study. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1992;62:480–8. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.62.3.480. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 137.Verbrugge LM, Wingard DL. Sex differentials in health and mortality. Women Health. 1987;12:103–45. doi: 10.1300/J013v12n02_07. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 138.Nathanson CA. Sex, illness and medical care: a review of data, theory and method. Soc Sci Med. 1977;11:13–26. doi: 10.1016/0037-7856(77)90141-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 139.Tousignant M, Brosseau R, Tremblay L. Sex biases in mental health scales: do women tend to report less serious symptoms and confide more in men? Psychol Med. 1987;17:203–15. doi: 10.1017/s003329170001309x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 140.Levine FM, DeSimone LL. The effects of experimenter gender on pain report in male and female subjects. Pain. 1991;44:69–72. doi: 10.1016/0304-3959(91)90149-R. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 141.Kirmayer LJ, Taillefer S. Somatoform disorders. In: Hersen M, Turner S, editors. Adult Psychopathology. 3rd ed. New York: John Wiley; 1997. pp. 333–82. In: [Google Scholar]
- 142.Kirmayer LJ. Mind and body as metaphors: hidden values in biomedicine. In: Lock M, Gordon D, editors. Biomedicine Examined. Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Kluwer; 1988. pp. 57–9. In: [Google Scholar]
- 143.Slavney PR, Teitelbaum ML, Chase GA. Referral for medically unexplained somatic complaints: the role of histrionic traits. Psychosomatics. 1985;26:103–9. doi: 10.1016/S0033-3182(85)72886-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 144.Wenger NK, Speroff L, Packard B. Cardiovascular health and disease in women. New Engl J Med. 1993;329:247–56. doi: 10.1056/NEJM199307223290406. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 145.McCaffery M, Ferrell BR. Does the gender gap affect our pain control decisions? Nursing. 1992;22:48–51. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 146.Cleeland C, Gonin R, Hatfield AK, et al. Pain and its treatment in outpatients with cancer. New Engl J Med. 1994;330:592–6. doi: 10.1056/NEJM199403033300902. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]