Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2006 Aug 14.
Published in final edited form as: J Youth Adolesc. 2006 Apr;35(2):217–229. doi: 10.1007/s10964-005-9010-9

The Skinny on Body Dissatisfaction: A Longitudinal Study of Adolescent Girls and Boys

Sarah Kate Bearman 1, Erin Martinez 1, Eric Stice 1, Katherine Presnell 2
PMCID: PMC1540456  NIHMSID: NIHMS7756  PMID: 16912810

Abstract

The present study tested whether theoretically derived risk factors predicted increases in body dissatisfaction and whether gender moderated these relations with data from a longitudinal study of 428 adolescent girls and boys because few prospective studies have examined these aims, despite evidence that body dissatisfaction increases risk for various psychiatric disturbances. Body dissatisfaction showed significant increases for girls and significant decreases for boys during early adolescence. For both genders parental support deficits, negative affectivity, and self-reported dietary restraint, but not Ideal body internalization, body mass index, and eating pathology, showed significant relations to future increases in body dissatisfaction; peer support deficits showed a marginal relation to this outcome. Gender did not moderate these relations, despite adequate power to detect interactive effects.

Keywords: Body Dissatisfaction, Adolescence, Gender Differences

The Skinny on Body Dissatisfaction: A Longitudinal Study of Adolescent Girls and Boys

Considerable research has been devoted to understanding the consequences of body dissatisfaction, or displeasure with one's weight and shape (Thompson, Heinberg, Altabe & Tantleff-Dunn, 1999). Historically, much of this research has focused on females because of the dramatic rise in body dissatisfaction following puberty (Rosenblum & Lewis, 1999), and because of greater sociocultural emphasis on appearance and thinness for females. However, recent research has indicated that body dissatisfaction is a substantial concern among adolescent boys as well (Jones, 2004; Kostanski, Fisher, & Gullone, 2004; Presnell, Bearman, & Stice, 2004; Wood, Becker, & Thompson, 1996). For both genders, the desire to alter shape or weight is common (Ricciardelli & McCabe, 2001; Wood, Becker, & Thompson, 1996), and is associated with emotional distress (Johnson & Wardle, 2005), dramatic measures to alter appearance, such as cosmetic surgery or steroid use (Hoffman & Brownell, 1997; Thompson, Heinberg, Altabe, & Tantleff-Dunn, 1999), as well as psychiatric disturbances such as depression (McCreary & Sasse, 2000; Seigel, 2002; Stice & Bearman, 2001) and eating disorders (Keel, Fulkerson & Leon, 1997; Stice, Presnell, & Spangler, 2002). Although the deleterious impact of body dissatisfaction has been well established, particularly for girls, the factors that increase the risk for body image concerns are less well understood.

Recent research has identified factors that are associated with body dissatisfaction, but relatively few studies have examined these relations prospectively (see Jones, 2004; Presnell et al., 2004; and Stice & Whitenton, 2002 for exceptions), or examined the timing of the development of body dissatisfaction as adolescents progress through puberty. Moreover, little is known about whether the risk factors for body dissatisfaction differ by gender. Accordingly, the goals of the present study were to examine the developmental changes in body dissatisfaction and prospective influence of social, psychological, and biological factors on the development of body dissatisfaction for adolescent girls and boys, as well as test for gender differences in these risk factors. Such information is imperative in order to clarify etiologic models, inform preventive efforts, and help identify characteristics of sub-groups at high risk for body dissatisfaction and related problems.

Ideal Body Internalization

According to the gender intensification hypothesis (Hill & Lynch, 1983), as adolescents mature physically and emotionally they begin to identify more strongly with their same-gender stereotype. For girls, this stereotype emphasizes the importance of physical attractiveness as a key evaluative dimension for females (Stice, Hayward, Cameron, Killen, & Taylor, 2000; Wichstrom, 1999), and in Western culture, physical attractiveness in women is inextricably linked with thinness (Nichter & Nichter, 1991). In contrast to the thin-ideal espoused for females, research suggests that some boys subscribe to an ideal that emphasizes a mesomorphic build valuing muscularity over thinness (Jacobi & Cash, 1994; McCreary & Sasse, 2000; Moore, 1990; Smolak, Levine, & Thompson, 2001). Research confirms that adolescent boys are more likely than girls to engage in behaviors to increase weight and musculature (McCabe & Ricciardelli, 2001; 2004).

Theoretically, girls and boys who have internalized these ideals would be vulnerable to body dissatisfaction when this ideal is not actualized. For girls, the discrepancy between ideal and actual shape is amplified following puberty, because increases in adiposity moves girls further from this ideal (McCarthy, 1990). Simultaneously, girls increasingly identify with the female stereotype, and focus on appearance as its central evaluative dimension. This confluence of events during adolescence creates a period of significant vulnerability for girls. In support, thin-ideal internalization (i.e., the degree to which one “buys into” the importance of thinness) increases for girls during this time, and has been demonstrated to predict increases in body dissatisfaction for adolescent girls (Jones, 2004; Stice & Bearman, 2001; Stice & Whitenton, 2002). The relation between the internalization of the male ideal and body dissatisfaction in boys has received less examination, although research has demonstrated that drive for muscularity correlates with low self-esteem and efforts to increase body mass (McCreary & Sasse, 2000), and internalization of sociocultural attitudes toward appearance also correlates with weight control techniques for boys (Smolak, Levine, & Thompson, 2001). In one study that directly examined the relation between internalized appearance ideals and body dissatisfaction among boys, internalized appearance ideals were a robust predictor of changes in body dissatisfaction (Jones, 2004).

Body Mass

As girls advance through puberty, the increased adiposity moves them farther from the thin-ideal, thus contributing to decreased body image satisfaction. In support of this assertion, previous research has verified that increases in body mass also prospectively predict girls' body dissatisfaction (Barker & Galambos, 2003; Rosenblum & Lewis, 1999; Stice & Whitenton, 2003). In contrast, the changes brought about by puberty theoretically move boys closer to the larger, more muscular ideal. However, the evidence regarding the trajectory of male body dissatisfaction indicates that adolescent boys are often split between those who desire to lose versus gain weight (McCabe, Ricciardelli, & Banfield, 2001; Moore, 1990; Neumark-Sztainer, et al., 1999). Although body mass emerged as a significant predictor of body dissatisfaction for boys in one prospective study (Field et al., 2001), another found null effects (Barker & Galambos, 2003). These inconsistent findings may suggest a more complex relation between body mass and body dissatisfaction in boys. For example, one prospective investigation suggested that dissatisfaction in boys is associated only with being under or overweight, while average weight boys were the most satisfied with their physical appearance (Richards, Boxer, Petersen, & Albrecht, 1990). Further support comes from another study that found that body mass prospectively predicted body dissatisfaction in a sample of adolescent boys, but this relation showed a significant quadratic component, as opposed to the linear relation seen for girls (Presnell et al., 2004). Thus, being either underweight or overweight was associated with body dissatisfaction for males, whereas girls' dissatisfaction increased with increasing body mass.

Social Support

Social support has also been explored as a risk factor for the development of body dissatisfaction. Theoretically, gender intensification is triggered not only by girls' actual physical maturation and consequent weight gain, but also by the way in which peers and parents respond to these changes (Wichstrom, 1999). Deficiencies in both the quantity and quality of social support have been linked with a host of psychosocial concerns for adolescents, including low self-esteem and body dissatisfaction. Theoretically, adolescents who feel unconditionally accepted by their support network may be less likely to try to attain acceptance by conforming to the thin ideal. In contrast, those who experience rejection from peers and parents may attribute this lack of support in part to their physical appearance.

Empirical support for this relation has been inconsistent. Stice and Whitenton (2002) found that deficits in social support predicted body dissatisfaction for adolescent girls, and another prospective study found that a supportive maternal relationship was significantly associated with increased body satisfaction (Barker & Galambos, 2003). However, a third study with a smaller sample size failed to demonstrate the relation between social support and body dissatisfaction in girls (Byely, et al., 2000). Because appearance is not as central an evaluative dimension for boys, however, deficits in social support may not be as strongly linked to body dissatisfaction for boys. The one prospective study that investigated the effects of social support on both boys' and girls' body image failed to find a significant effect (Presnell et al., 2004), and another prospective study found that although parental support predicted girls' greater body satisfaction, it was not related to this outcome for boys (Barker & Galambos, 2003). Furthermore, a cross-sectional study found that parental feedback about weight was not correlated with body dissatisfaction for males (Schwartz, Phares, Tantleff-Dunn & Thompson, 1999). More research on this risk factor is warranted to determine the effects of interpersonal variables such as social support on boys' body dissatisfaction.

Dieting

For adolescent girls who believe that achieving the thin-ideal will result in psychosocial benefits and who have received messages that they deviate from this ideal, dieting may serve as a strategy for altering their physique (McCabe & Ricciardelli, 2001). Theoretically, as adipose tissue increases following puberty, adolescent girls may attempt to counter this change by restricting their caloric intake. Despite their attempts, however, research suggests that self-reported attempts to restrict caloric intake predict weight gain, rather than weight loss (Stice, Cameron, Killen, Hayward, & Taylor, 1999; Stice, Presnell, Shaw, & Rohde, 2005). Thus, dieting may result in the opposite of its intended effect—increasing girls' frustration and reducing their feelings of self-efficacy for producing weight change. In keeping with this theoretical assertion, girls' self-reported dieting attempts predicted increases in body dissatisfaction (Barker & Galambos, 2003).

As previously discussed, while body mass has a linear relation with body dissatisfaction for girls, it has a more complex relation for boys (Presnell et al., 2004). Research suggests that boys are split between those who wish to lose weight and those who wish to gain weight (McCabe, Ricciardelli, & Banfield, 2001; Moore, 1990; Neumark-Sztainer et al., 1999). Dieting may increase the risk for body dissatisfaction among boys who are trying to reduce body mass and experience the dietary failure and weight gain that has been associated with self-reported dieting in girls (Stice et al., 1999; Stice et al., 2005). However, some boys may be more likely to try to increase in size and musculature, as one cross-sectional study demonstrated (McCabe & Ricciardelli, 2001b). For these boys, changes in diet might reflect an attempt to increase lean muscle mass. However, attempts to manage weight did not predict body dissatisfaction in one prospective study of adolescent boys (Barker & Galambos, 2003). Thus, it will be important to disentangle this relation for boys.

Negative Affect

Affective disturbances have also been implicated in the development of body dissatisfaction (Taylor & Cooper, 1992). Theoretically, depressed affect induces a preference for, and selective attention to, negative information about oneself and the world (Beck, 1976). This information processing bias may result in increased attention to displeasing body characteristics and foster negative comparisons of one's body with others' bodies. This hypothesis has received mixed empirical support, however. Acute negative affect inductions have resulted in acute body dissatisfaction (Baker, Williamson, & Sylve, 1995; Taylor & Cooper, 1992), suggesting that there is at least a short-term relation between negative mood and body distress. However, prospective studies have found that neither negative affect (Presnell et al., 2004) nor depressive symptoms (Stice & Whitenton, 2002) predicted body dissatisfaction for adolescent girls. There is some evidence that this relation may be stronger for boys than girls. One study found that negative affect predicted body dissatisfaction in boys, but not girls (Presnell et al., 2004). In addition, negative affect was associated with body change strategies in a sample of adolescent boys (Ricciardelli & McCabe, 2003). Further examination of this variable is needed to determine the nature of the relationship between affective disturbances and body dissatisfaction, and whether this relation differs by gender.

Eating Pathology

It has been suggested that eating disordered behaviors, such as recurrent binge eating, fasting, purging, or excessive exercise may promote feelings of body dissatisfaction, especially since these behaviors are associated with feelings of guilt, frustration, and loss of control over one's eating. Although the incidence of eating pathology among boys remains relatively low, rates of eating disordered behavior among boys appears to be increasing (Ricciardelli & McCabe, 2004); therefore, identifying the consequences of such behaviors for boys and girls alike is of growing importance.

Whereas prior studies provide some preliminary support for several of the hypothesized risk factors for body dissatisfaction, and potential gender differences among those risk factors, this literature has certain limitations. As has been discussed, pubertal weight gain is thought to increase girls' risk of body dissatisfaction, whereas for boys increases in weight may increase satisfaction with one's weight and shape, assuming that this increase represents lean muscle mass. Indeed, a recent study by McCabe and Ricciardelli (2004) noted that early-maturing girls and girls who physically matured at the same time as their peers reported higher levels of body dissatisfaction than girls whose pubertal development was delayed relative to peers. In contrast, boys who physically matured earlier than their same-sex peers had the highest levels of body satisfaction. Thus, one might assume that age for girls would be associated with increasing levels of body dissatisfaction, whereas for boys, the reverse would be true. Indeed, prior research as demonstrated that boys show a decline in body dissatisfaction over time (Jones, 2004; Rosenblum & Lewis, 1999). For girls, some research has indicated that older girls evidence significantly greater levels of body dissatisfaction than their younger counterparts (Jones, 2004). However, this has not been explicitly tested using a co-ed sample of adolescents . Furthermore, much of this previous research has examined risk factors for body dissatisfaction separately in samples of boys and girls (e.g. Jones, 2004; McCabe & Ricciardelli, 2004). This study improves upon prior research by testing whether gender moderates the relation of each risk factor to later development of body dissatisfaction. An explicit test of the gender-by-risk factor interaction is required in order to demonstrate that the predictor variables are associated with different levels of risk for girls versus boys (Baron & Kenny, 1986). In view of these gaps in the literature, the primary aims of this study were to examine developmental changes in body dissatisfaction and to test for gender differences in the above set of putative risk factors for body dissatisfaction in a coed sample of adolescents using longitudinal data.

Method

Participants

Participants were 247 adolescent girls and 181 adolescent boys from four public and four private middle schools in a large metropolitan area of the Southwestern United States. Adolescents ranged in age from 12 to 16 (M = 13.57). The sample was composed of 2% Asian/Pacific Islanders, 4% African Americans, 64% Caucasians, 18% Hispanics, 1% Native Americans, and 5% who specified “other” or mixed racial heritage, which was representative of the ethnic composition of the schools from which we sampled (2% Asian/Pacific Islanders; 8% African Americans, 65% Caucasians, 21% Hispanics; 4% “other or mixed”). Highest educational attainment for parents ranged from grade school graduate (2%) to graduate degree (19%) with a mode of college graduate (42%).

Procedures

The study was presented to parents and participants as an investigation of adolescent mental and physical health behaviors. Parents of all eighth grade girls and boys from the participating schools were sent a description of the study along with an informed consent letter, and active parental consent and adolescent assent was obtained from all participants. This resulted in an average participation rate of 53% of eligible students across schools. This participation rate was similar to that observed in other school-recruited samples that used active consent procedures and involved structured interviews (e.g., 61% for Lewinsohn et al., 1994).

Participants completed a self-report questionnaire, participated in a structured psychiatric interview, and had their weight and height measured by research assistants at baseline (T1) and at 1- and 2-year follow-up (T2 & T3, respectively). Assessments took place during elective courses during regular school hours, immediately after school on the school campus, or in the participants' homes. Interviews were conducted by clinical assessors with a bachelors, masters, or doctoral degree in psychology. Clinical assessors attended 24 hours of training, and were required to show a minimum (kappa) agreement with expert raters of .80 before starting data collection. Participants received a $15 gift certificate to a local book and music store as compensation for participating in the study. This project received human subject's approval from the University of Texas Institutional Review Boards, as well as from the Austin Independent School District.

Measures

Ideal body internalization. The Thinness and Restricting Expectancy Inventory (TREI; Hohlstein, Smith, & Atlas, 1998) assessed ideal-body internalization for the girls. Participants indicated their level of agreement with statements concerning expected social and psychological benefits from achieving thinness using a 5-point response format ranging from 1 = strongly disagree to 5 = strongly agree. The TREI has adequate internal consistency (α = .98), test-retest reliability (r = .80), and convergent validity (Hohlstein et al., 1998). Because it has been demonstrated that the ideal body type for boys differs from that of girls (Smolak, Levine & Thompson, 2001) items were modified to reflect the expected benefits from achieving leanness and muscularity as well as thinness for males. This scale had a α= .80 for the combined sample at T1 (α= 85 at T1 for boys and a α= .80 at T1 for girls).

Body mass. The body mass index (BMI = Kg/M2) was used to reflect adiposity (Pietrobelli et al., 1998). Height was measured to the nearest millimeter using stadiometers and weight was measured with digital scales. Two measures of height and weight were obtained and averaged. The BMI shows convergent validity (r .80 - .90) with direct measures of total body fat such as dual energy x-ray absorptiometry (Pietrobelli et al., 1998).

Social Support. Perceived social support was measured with items adapted from the Network of Relationships Inventory (Furman & Buhrmester, 1985) assessing companionship, guidance, intimacy, affection, admiration, and reliable alliance from parents and peers. Items are averaged for analyses to form separate scales of parental support and peer support. The internal consistency (M α=.89), test-retest reliability (M 1-month r =.69), and convergent and criterion validity of this measure have been documented (Furman & Buhrmester, 1985; Furman, 1996). At T1, parental support had a α= .87 and peer support had a α = .89.

Dieting. The Dutch Restrained Eating Scale (DRES; van Strein, Frijters, Van Staveren, Defares, & Deurenberg, 1986) was used to assess dieting. Participants indicate the frequency of dieting behaviors using 5-point scales (1 = never to 5 = always) and items are averaged for analyses. This scale has shown internal consistency (α = .95), test-retest reliability (r = .82), convergent validity (with self-reported caloric intake), and predictive validity for future increases in bulimic symptoms (Stice, 2001; van Strein, Frijters, Bergers, & Defares, 1986). This scale had an α = .92 at T1.

Negative affectivity. Twelve items from Buss and Plomin's (1984) Negative Affect Scale were used to assess a propensity toward becoming emotionally distressed. Items were averaged for analyses. Research has found this scale to possess acceptable internal consistency (α= .82), test-retest reliability (r = .80), and predictive validity for onset of depression (Buss & Plomin, 1984; Hayward, Killen, Kraemer, & Taylor, 1998). This scale had a α=.79 at T1.

Eating pathology. The diagnostic items from the Eating Disorder Examination (EDE; Fairburn & Cooper, 1993), a structured interview, assessed symptoms of eating pathology including binge-eating and compensatory behaviors such as fasting, purging, and exercise over the past 12 months. The 17 diagnostic items for bulimia were averaged to create an overall symptom composite. The symptom composite from the streamlined version of this interview used showed internal consistency (x = .96), 1-month test-retest reliability (r = .95), convergent validity with alternative measures of eating pathology, and sensitivity to detecting intervention effects in previous studies (Stice, Fisher, & Martinez, 2004).

Body dissatisfaction. Body dissatisfaction was assessed with an adapted form of the Satisfaction and Dissatisfaction with Body Parts Scale (Berscheid, Walster & Bohrnstedt, 1973), which asks participants to indicate their level of satisfaction with 9 body parts using a 5 point scale ranging from 1 (extremely satisfied) to 5 (extremely dissatisfied). Items are averaged for analyses. This scale has shown internal consistency (α= .94), test-retest reliability (r = .90), and predictive validity for future increases in bulimic symptoms (Stice, 2001). Because some of the items on this scale appeared to assess satisfaction with body parts more relevant for females than males, four of the items were modified on the questionnaires completed by males. This scale had a α = .92 at T1 (α = .93 for girls and α = .89 for boys).

Results

Analytic Overview

Preliminary analyses tested for differences between girls and boys on all study variables and demographic factors. Attrition analyses tested whether participants who dropped from the study differed significantly from those who did not. Next, hierarchical linear models (HLM; Bryk, Raudenbush, Cheong, & Congdon, 2000) tested the univariate prospective relations between all T1 risk factors and age on increases in body dissatisfaction over time. T1 values for the outcome (initial body dissatisfaction) were used to estimate the parameters of change over time for each individual, thereby ensuring a truly prospective test (Raudenbush & Byrk, 2002). For all models, baseline levels of the risk factors were entered as Level 2 time-invariant covariates, and the following equation was generated:

Level1:Y=B0i+B1i(TIMEi)+RiLevel2:B0i=G00+U0iB1i=G10+G11(RISKFACTOR)+U1i

To assess whether gender moderated the relation between T1 risk factors and future growth in body dissatisfaction, HLM models were generated to assess the effect of Level 2 time-invariant covariates of gender (0 or 1), T1 risk factor, and the interaction of gender and the T1 risk factor on the slope of the Level 1 unconditional model of body dissatisfaction over time:

Level1:Y=B0i+B1i(TIMEi)+RiLevel2:B0i=G00+U0iB1i=G10+G11(SEX)+G12(RISKFACTOR)+G11(SEXRISKFACTOR)+U1i

For significant interactions, follow up analyses of the simple effects of the T1 risk factor on growth in body dissatisfaction were conducted separately for boys and girls. Finally, risk factors that showed a significant prospective relation with growth in body dissatisfaction were entered simultaneously into a multivariate model in order to determine the unique contribution of each significant predictor to growth in body dissatisfaction.

Preliminary Analyses

Of the original 428 participants, 15 did not provide data at T2 (3%), and 19 did not provide data at T3 (4%), although only 10 participants did not provide data at both T2 and T3 (2%). Attrition analyses indicated that participants who dropped out of the study did not differ from those who provided complete data on any of the variables considered in this study at T1. Because HLM uses full-information maximum likelihood estimation for missing data, the effective N for analyses was 428. Independent t-tests indicated that girls reported higher levels of body dissatisfaction, peer social support, negative affectivity and dietary restraint at T1; no other gender differences were significant. Means and standard deviations for all baseline variables, and the correlations among them, are provided in Table 1. Means and standard deviation for all baseline measures by gender are reported in Table 2.

Table 1.

Correlations among the T1 Putative Risk Factors and T1, T2, & T3 Body Dissatisfaction, along with Means and Standard Deviation for Boys and Girls

2. 3. 4. 5. 6. 7. 8. 9. 10. M SD
1. Ideal Body Intern. −.06 .12 .01 −.01 .16 .17 .11 .07 .10 3.22 0.75
2. Body Mass Index .09 −.06 −.04 .42 .29 .30 .31 .19 21.66 4.69
3. Negative Affect −.19 −.02 .25 .26 .32 .28 .26 2.68 0.61
4. Support-Parent .10 −.08 −.25 −.32 −.22 −.28 3.96 0.84
5. Support-Peer −.08 −.04 −.10 −.14 −.17 4.16 0.77
6. Dietary Restraint .38 .38 .31 .32 2.02 0.87
7. Eating Pathology .42 .28 .25 0.32 0.50
8. T1 Body Dis. .61 .54 2.66 0.88
9. T2 Body Dis. .66 2.68 0.84
10. T3 Body Dis. 2.74 0.83

Note. Absolute correlations greater than .09 are significant at p <.05

Table 2.

Means and Standard Deviation for each T1 variable by Gender

Variable Girls
Boys
M SD M SD
Ideal body internalization 3.22 0.67 3.22 0.84
Body mass 21.56 4.75 21.79 4.65
Negative affect 2.78a 0.60 2.57b 0.59
Social support—Parents 3.92 0.90 4.00 0.74
Social support—Peers 4.35a 0.72 3.90b 0.75
Dietary restraint 2.13a 0.90 1.87b 0.80
Eating Pathology 0.35 0.53 0.27 0.44
T1 body dissatisfaction 2.74 a 0.95 2.55b 0.77
T2 body dissatisfaction 2.82 a 0.90 2.48 b 0.77
T3 body dissatisfaction 2.91 a 0.84 2.49 b 0.75

Note. Means in the same row with different subscripts are significantly different at p < .05.

Descriptive Analyses

The mean body dissatisfaction score for girls was 2.7 at T1, 2.8 at T2, and 2.9 at T3, which reflects a neutral level of satisfaction. The mean body dissatisfaction score for boys was 2.6 at T1, and 2.5 at T2 and T3, corresponding with the midpoint between neutral and moderately satisfied. If body dissatisfaction is defined as a score that corresponds to an average response of moderately dissatisfied or extremely dissatisfied, the rates of body dissatisfaction were 37% (T1), 35% (T2), and 44% (T3) for girls and 23% (T1), 19% (T2), and 16% (T3) for boys. This is consistent with the relatively higher rates of body dissatisfaction among girls than boys found in other studies (e.g. Keel, Fulkerson, & Leon, 1997) and similar to the rates of body dissatisfaction found in other studies (Fox, Page, Peters, Armstrong, & Kirby, 1994; Stice & Whitenton, 2002). It is noteworthy that girls who reported being extremely satisfied or moderately satisfied with their bodies had a significantly lower mean body mass (M = 19.78) than boys who reported being similarly satisfied (M = 21.98, t = −3.09, p < .005). Girls who reported being extremely or moderately dissatisfied with their weight (M = 24.36) did not differ statistically from males who reported similar dissatisfaction (M = 21.72).1

To probe the relation between age and increases in body dissatisfaction, the univariate relation between age and body dissatisfaction was first investigated in an individual model to test whether T1 age predicted growth in body dissatisfaction over time. The main effect of age was not significant. To test whether this relation was moderated by gender, gender was entered at Level 2, and results indicated that the cross-level interaction of age and gender was significant, with age predicting increases in body dissatisfaction for girls but not for boys (β =−0.12, t = -5.62, p < .001). Next, independent t-tests compared the means of girls' and boys' reported levels of body dissatisfaction across all measurement periods at ages 13, 14, 15 and 16 to determine the age at which gender differences in body dissatisfaction emerged. At age 13, boys and girls reported similar levels of body dissatisfaction (M girls = 2.71; M boys = 2.58) and there were no significant differences. By age 14, however, girls reported significantly higher levels of body dissatisfaction (M = 2.74) compared to boys (M = 2.56, t = 2.03, p < .05), and girls' level of body dissatisfaction continued to increase by age while body dissatisfaction decreased for boys, as evidenced by levels of body dissatisfaction reported at ages 15 (M girls = 2.90; M boys = 2.49, t = 4.75, p < .001) and at age 16 (M girls = 2.95; M boys = 2.35,t = 5.62, p< .001).

Risk Factors for Increases in Body Dissatisfaction

As hypothesized, initial elevations in dietary restraint ((β = 0.05, t = 2.39, p < .05), negative affect (β = 0.07, t = 2.20, p < .05), and deficits in parental social support (β = -0.05, t = -2.02, p < .05), predicted growth in body dissatisfaction in the combined sample of boys and girls over the study period. Deficits in peer social support approached statistical significance as a predictor of growth in body dissatisfaction (β = 0.04, t = 1.89, p = .06). However, ideal body internalization, body-mass index, and eating pathology, did not show significant prospective relations. These models are reported in Table 3.

Table 3.

Univariate Relations of Each Risk Factor to Adolescent Boys' and Girls' Increases in Body Dissatisfaction

Fixed Effect Random Effect
Effect Parameter Coefficient se t p Parameter Variance X2 d.f. p
Ideal Body β0  2.65 0.042 63.22 <.001 U0 0.540 1557.3 420 < .001
Internalization β1  0.01 0.024  0.61 ns U1 0.041 562.68 419 <.001


Body Mass β0  2.65 0.042 63.22 <.001 U0 0.540 1557.2 420 <.001
Index β1  0.00 0.004  0.47 ns U1 0.042 565.37 419 <.001


Negative β0  2.65 0.042 63.22 <.001 U0 0.541 1555.6 420 <.001
Affectivity β1  0.07 0.030  2.20 <.05 U1 0.044 574.00 419 <.001


Parental β0  2.65 0.042 63.22 <.001 U0 0.541 1555.6 420 < .001
Social Support β1 −0.05 0.022 −2.20 <.05 U1 0.044 573.91 419 <.001


Peer Social β0  2.65 0.042 63.58 <.001 U0 0.541 1565.7 420 < .001
Support β1 −0.03 0.019 −1.89 <.06 U1 0.040 564.31 419 <.001


Dietary β0  2.65 0.042 63.22 <.001 U0 0.541 1556.7 420 < .001
Restraint β1  0.05 0.020  2.39 <.05 U1 0.046 579.10 419 <.001


Eating β0  2.65 0.042 63.22 <.001 U0 0.541 1556.4 420 < .001
Pathology β1  0.01 0.032  0.20 ns U1 0.041 563.06 419 <.001

To test whether gender moderated any of the univariate effects of the risk factors, interaction terms were computed for gender and each of the T1 independent variables and added to the models described above. No significant interactions with gender were observed among the putative risk factors.

Risk factors showing significant univariate relations were then included in a multivariate regression model. This model tested whether negative affect, dietary restraint, and deficits in peer and parental social support predicted T3 body dissatisfaction. Main effects of peer social support became nonsignificant in the multivariate model and negative affect was reduced to the trend level, but dietary restraint and parental support deficits showed significant unique relations to subsequent increases in body dissatisfaction in the multivariate model. These results are listed in Table 4.

Table 4.

Multivariate Model of Risk Factors for Increases in Body Dissatisfaction for Adolescent Girls and Boys

Fixed Effect
Random Effect
Effect Parameter Coefficient se t p Parameter Variance X2 d.f. p
Body Dissatisfaction B0 2.65 0.042 63.22 <.001 U0 0.540 1555.1 420 <.001
Negative. Affect G1 0.05 0.030 1.71 =.08  U1 0.053 603.70 416 <.001
Support-Parental G2 −0.04  0.022 −2.02  <.05 
Support-Peer G3 −0.03  0.022 −1.45  ns 
Dietary Restraint G4 0.04 0.021 2.16 <.05

Discussion

This study was designed to explore the developmental course of body dissatisfaction in a sample of adolescent boys and girls and test whether ideal body internalization, body mass index, negative affect, deficits in social support, self-reported dietary restraint, and eating pathology predicted increases in body dissatisfaction. Gender differences in the relations of the putative risk factors to body dissatisfaction were also examined.

Developmental Course of Body Dissatisfaction

Age at T1 did not predict increases in body dissatisfaction for the combined sample of girls and boys; however, this relation was moderated by gender. Follow up analyses demonstrated that for girls, increases in age were associated with increases in body dissatisfaction. Although there were no significant differences between levels of body dissatisfaction for boys and girls in the sample at age 13, by age 14 girls were significantly more dissatisfied. This effect remained at ages 15 and 16. These findings mirror those described by Jones (2004), who also found that high-school girls endorsed higher levels of body dissatisfaction than middle school girls. Consistent with previous studies (Gardner, Friedman, & Jackson, 1999; Hargreaves & Tiggemann, 2002; Jones, 2004; Presnell et al., 2004), rates of body dissatisfaction increased overall for the girls over the 2-year course of the study, while it decreased for boys, and girls were generally more dissatisfied with their bodies than boys in the current sample at each timepoint. This is reflective of a developmental model of body dissatisfaction, wherein girls become increasingly more body dissatisfied during adolescence as they move away from the thin-ideal, and boys become more satisfied as they move toward the mesomorphic ideal (Smolak, Levine, & Thompson, 2001). Furthermore, girls who reported satisfaction with their body size and shape were significantly thinner than boys who expressed similar levels of satisfaction. This suggests that while average weight boys are relatively content with their physique, girls' body satisfaction is correspondent with a physique that more closely approximates the current thin-ideal for females.

Risk Factors for Increases in Body Dissatisfaction

Despite this, ideal body internalization did not emerge as a prospective predictor of body dissatisfaction in this study. This is inconsistent with some previous research that has supported this relation for girls (Jones, 2004; Stice & Whitenton, 2002) and for boys (Jones, 2004). However, another prospective study that examined thin-ideal internalization in a coed sample also did not find support for the predictive relation to body dissatisfaction (Presnell et al., 2004). Ideal body internalization has been thought to lead to body dissatisfaction because of the feelings of frustration engendered by failing to attain an ideal one holds in high esteem. Because there was no interaction of gender and ideal body internalization, these null findings cannot be attributed to gender differences in the way adolescents conceptualize the ideal body size and shape. As an aside, ideal body internalization also did not interact with obesity to predict body dissatisfaction for the current sample, suggesting that this variable does not predict increases in body dissatisfaction even among those adolescents who deviated substantially from the culturally sanctioned ideal.

Curiously, in the current study BMI also did not predict body dissatisfaction for adolescent boys and girls. This is in contrast to previous research (Barker & Galambos, 2003; Presnell et al., 2004; Rosenblum & Lewis, 1999), which has demonstrated that BMI is a consistent predictor of girls' dissatisfaction and, in one study (Field et al., 2001) for boys as well. Theoretically, increases in body weight cause one's body to diverge from the ideal, thus leading to dissatisfaction (McCarthy, 1990). Because gender did not moderate the relation between body mass and body dissatisfaction, this study does not offer evidence that body weight operates differently for girls and boys in terms of predicting body dissatisfaction, as suggested by theorists who maintain that boys strive for increases in body mass (Jacobi & Cash, 1994; McCreary & Sasse, 2000; Moore, 1990; Smolak, Levine, & Thompson, 2001). It should be noted, however, that our measure of body dissatisfaction does not distinguish between displeasure with being too large versus being too small.

It is also worth noting that whereas girls in this study showed a linear association between BMI and body dissatisfaction at all measurement points, boys deviated from a linear association throughout the duration of the study. More puzzling, boys who endorsed the highest levels of dissatisfaction with their bodies were nearly identical in weight to boys who endorsed the greatest satisfaction. This suggests that one's perception of weight may be of greater psychological relevance than one's physical dimensions. It may also be the case that BMI is too broad a measurement of the physical changes that adolescent girls and boys undergo during this important developmental phase, as it does not distinguish between weight gained via lean muscle versus fatty tissue. Thus, boys who weigh more due to increased muscle mass may be very satisfied with their physique, whereas boys whose weight increases as a result of adipose tissue may be displeased with this increase.

Initial elevations in negative affect predicted increased body dissatisfaction for the combined sample. Theoretically, the processing biases associated with affective disturbances could result in a preference for information about one's physique that confirms a negative sense of oneself (Beck, 1976). Although consistent with a previous study that found that negative affect was a prospective predictor of boys' body dissatisfaction (Presnell et al., 2004) this relation was not moderated by gender in this sample, and thus is difficult to reconcile with previous prospective studies that found that affective disturbances did not predict body dissatisfaction for girls (Presnell et al., 2004; Stice & Whitenton, 2002). To our knowledge, this is the first study to demonstrate this effect for a sample of both boys and girls.

Deficits in social support from parents—and to a lesser extent from peers—predicted body dissatisfaction for both boys and girls. Presumably, deficits in social support might escalate vulnerability to body dissatisfaction as individuals strive to gain social acceptance through conformity with idealized body images, whereas supportive relationships with friends and family might offer protection from feelings of body dissatisfaction. This relation for girls has received support in prior research (Stice & Whitenton, 2002), although Jones (2004) found that peer acceptance about appearance predicted girls' body dissatisfaction, but did not predict in a separate sample of boys. In the current sample, parental support deficits were a more robust predictor of body dissatisfaction than were peer social support, and remained a significant predictor of body dissatisfaction in the multivariate analyses while peer support deficits dropped out of the model. Although it is widely believed that peer influence becomes increasingly salient throughout adolescence, this study supplies some evidence that family relationships retain their relevance and remain a powerful predictor of adolescent body dissatisfaction.

As hypothesized, increases in self-reported dietary restraint predicted increases in body dissatisfaction. In theory, attempts to manage one's weight via dietary restraint could cultivate body dissatisfaction for both girls and boys because the frustration associated with dietary failure may increase displeasure with one's shape and weight. Furthermore, self-reported dieting has been found to predict weight gain (Klesges, Isbell, & Klesges, 1992), which may also amplify feelings of body dissatisfaction. Consistent with this evidence, it has recently been suggested that identifying oneself as a dieter is a marker for a propensity to overeat, and therefore individuals who express the need to employ dietary restraint do so because they have a tendency to overeat when not actively attempting to restrict their caloric intake (Lowe & Levine, 2005; Presnell & Stice, 2003). Although this relation has been supported for girls (Barker & Galambos, 2003, Byely et al., 1990), attempts to manage weight did not predict body dissatisfaction for boys in the one prospective study we located that examined this variable (Barker & Galambos, 2003). Gender did not moderate this relation, indicating that self-reported dietary restraint does not exert differential risk for boys versus girls.

Eating pathology did not predict increases in body dissatisfaction in this study, and this relation was not moderated by gender. In theory, loss of control over one's eating, coupled with frustrating attempts to compensate for binge episodes by fasting, purging, or excessive exercise might increase dissatisfaction with one's physique due to the guilt and shame associated with such behaviors. As well, because such behaviors are associated with weight gain rather than their intended effect of weight loss, they may also increase the discrepancy between an individual's ideal and actual weight. However, in the current study this theoretical relation did not receive support.

None of the risk factors that predicted body dissatisfaction for the coed sample were moderated by gender—despite adequate power (>.88) to detect small effects in this sample (Cohen, 1988). This is an important point, because the bulk of research in this area focuses primarily on girls, and researchers have theorized that the processes by which males and females become body dissatisfied are dissimilar (Keel, Klump, Leon & Fulkerson, 1998; Tiggemann & Pennington, 1990). Although girls in this study evidenced higher overall levels of body dissatisfaction than boys, the rates of body dissatisfaction among boys were not insubstantial, and the risk factors were equivalent. This suggests that interventions targeting youth at risk for developing body dissatisfaction—as well as the psychiatric outcomes that are associated with body dissatisfaction (e.g. depression, eating disorders)—should focus on both boys and girls. Results do suggest, however, that while girls uniformly wish to be thinner, boys are divided into those who desire to gain weight and those who desire to lose weight. Thus, interventions for boys may need to target both ends of the spectrum. The results of this study further indicate that an intervention aimed at individuals who express the need to diet may be especially helpful in decreasing levels of body satisfaction. Additionally, interventions that focus on increasing positive affect and peer and parental support would be particularly useful. Finally, this study indicated that the pivotal time to intervene with adolescents in order to prevent body dissatisfaction may differ for boys and girls. Boys appear to be most body dissatisfied in early adolescence, whereas girls become increasingly displeased with their physique as they progress into middle and late adolescence.

Study Limitations

It is important to consider the limitations of this study when interpreting the findings. First, the low participation rate limits the generalizability of this sample. Second, because both the measure of body dissatisfaction and thin-ideal internalization were modified in order to include items relevant for both genders, it is possible that this may have introduced some measurement artifact. However, our results are commensurate with previous studies that have examined these constructs, increasing our confidence that they were adequately assessed. Thirdly, this study did not differentiate between dissatisfaction with body parts that are perceived to be too small versus too large, a distinction that may have important theoretical consequences—especially for boys. Finally, whereas longitudinal data provide information regarding temporal precedence, third-variable explanations cannot be ruled out with a non-experimental design. Therefore, it is possible that some shared causal variable increases both the risk factors and body dissatisfaction.

Conclusions

In this study, girls showed higher rates of body dissatisfaction than boys, and these rates increased over the course of the study while the reverse was true for boys. Nonetheless, our data suggest that a considerable number of adolescents experience body dissatisfaction, and that the variables that increase risk are analogous regardless of gender. Because body dissatisfaction is a ubiquitous concern for adolescents and associated with significant emotional distress as well as psychological impairment, efforts to clarify its etiology and interrupt this trajectory are of vital importance.

Footnotes

1

Because previous research indicates that body mass may deviate from a linear relation for boys (Muth & Cash, 1997; Presnell, Bearman, & Stice, 2004), we tested for higher-order effects. An orthogonal polynomial analysis of the Gender X BMI effect indicated a significant Gender X Linear interaction (r = .685,p = <.001) Follow-up tests of the simple effects separately for boys versus girls suggested that while the relation of BMI and body dissatisfaction had a significant linear component for girls, but not for boys. On the other hand, a quadratic (u-shaped) model was supported at T1 for boys (r = .280, p = <.05), but not for girls. Although the quadratic model was no longer supported for boys at T2 and T3, BMI and body dissatisfaction continued to deviate from a linear association for boys in this sample.

This study was supported by a career award (MH01708) and an individual research service award (MH12834) from the National Institutes of Mental Health. Thanks go to project research assistants, Melissa Fisher, Katy Whitenton, and Natalie McKee, as well as to the many undergraduate volunteers who assisted with this project. Our gratitude also goes to the Austin Independent School District and the participants who made this study possible.

References

  1. Baker JD, Williamson DA, Sylve C. Body image disturbance, memory bias, and body dysphoria: Effects of negative mood induction. Behavior Therapy. 1995;26:747–759. [Google Scholar]
  2. Barker ET, Galambos NL. Body dissatisfaction of adolescent girls and boys: Risk and resource factors. Journal of Early Adolescence. 2003;23:141–165. [Google Scholar]
  3. Baron RM, Kenny DA. The moderator-mediator distinction in social psychological research: Conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1986;51:1173–1182. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.51.6.1173. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Beck AT. Cognitive therapy and the emotional disorders. International University Press; New York: 1976. [Google Scholar]
  5. Berscheid E, Walster E, Bohrnstedt G. The happy American body: A survey report. Psychology Today. 1973;7:119–131. [Google Scholar]
  6. Bryk AS, Raudenbush SW, Cheong YF, Congdon RT. HLM 5 Hierarchical linear and nonlinear modeling. Scientific Software Int.; Lincolnwood, IL: 2000. [Google Scholar]
  7. Buss AH, Plomin R. Temperament: Early developing personality traits. Lawrence Erlbaum; Hillsdale, NJ: 1984. [Google Scholar]
  8. Byely L, Archibald AB, Graber J, Brooks-Gunn J. A prospective study of familial and social influences on girls' body image and dieting. International Journal of Eating Disorders. 2000;28:155–164. doi: 10.1002/1098-108x(200009)28:2<155::aid-eat4>3.0.co;2-k. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Cohen J. Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. 2nd Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; Hillsdale, NJ: 1988. [Google Scholar]
  10. Fairburn CG, Cooper Z. The eating disorder examination (12th edition) In: Fairburn C, Wilson G, editors. Binge eating: Nature, assessment, and treatment. Guilford; NY: 1993. pp. 317–360. [Google Scholar]
  11. Field AE, Carmargo CA, Taylor CB, Berkey CS, Roberts SB, Colditz GA. Peer, parent, and media influences on the development of weight concerns and frequent dieting among preadolescent and adolescent girls and boys. Pediatrics. 2001;107:54–60. doi: 10.1542/peds.107.1.54. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Fox KR, Page A, Peters DM, Armstrong N, Kirby B. Dietary restraint and fatness in early adolescent girls and boys. Journal of Adolescence. 1994;17:149–161. [Google Scholar]
  13. Furman W, Buhrmester D. Children's perceptions of the personal relations in their social networks. Developmental Psychology. 1985;21:1016. [Google Scholar]
  14. Furman W. The measurement of friendship perceptions: Conceptual and methodological issues. In: Bukowski WM, Newcomb AF, Hartup WW, editors. The company we keep. Cambridge University; New York: 1996. pp. 41–65. [Google Scholar]
  15. Gardner RM, Friedman BN, Jackson N. A Body size estimations, body dissatisfaction, and ideal size preferences in children six through thirteen. Journal of Youth & Adolescence. 1999;28:603–618. [Google Scholar]
  16. Hargreaves D, Tiggemann M. The role of appearance schematicity in the development of adolescent body dissatisfaction. Cognitive Therapy and Research. 2002;26:691–700. [Google Scholar]
  17. Hayward C, Killen JD, Kraemer HC, Taylor CB. Linking childhood behavioral inhibition to adolescent social phobia: A prospective study. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 1998;37:1. doi: 10.1097/00004583-199812000-00015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Hill JP, Lynch ME. The intensification of gender-related role expectations during early adolescence. In: Brooks-Gunn J, Petersen AC, editors. Girls at Puberty: Biological and psychosocial perspectives. Plenum; New York: 1983. pp. 201–228. [Google Scholar]
  19. Hoffman JM, Brownell KD. Sex differences in the relationship of body fat distribution with psychosocial variables. International Journal of Eating Disorders. 1997;22:139–145. doi: 10.1002/(sici)1098-108x(199709)22:2<139::aid-eat4>3.0.co;2-g. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Hohlstein LA, Smith GT, Atlas JG. An application of expectancy theory to eating disorders: Development and validation of measures of eating and dieting expectancies. Psychological Assessment. 1998;10:49–58. [Google Scholar]
  21. Jacobi L, Cash TF. In pursuit of the perfect appearance: Discrepancies among selfdeal percepts of multiple physical attributes. Journal of Applied Social Psychology. 1994;24:379–396. [Google Scholar]
  22. Johnson F, Wardle J. Dietary restraint, body dissatisfaction, and psychological distress: A prospective analysis. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2005;115:119–125. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.114.1.119. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Jones DC. Body image among adolescent girls and boys: A longitudinal study. Developmental Psychology. 2004;40:823–835. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.40.5.823. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Keel PK, Klump KL, Leon GR, Fulkerson JA. Disordered eating in adolescent males from a school-based sample. International Journal of Eating Disorders. 1998;23:125–132. doi: 10.1002/(sici)1098-108x(199803)23:2<125::aid-eat2>3.0.co;2-m. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Keel PK, Fulkerson JA, Leon GR. Disordered eating precursors in pre- and early adolescent girls and boys. Journal of Youth and Adolescence. 1997;26:203–216. [Google Scholar]
  26. Klesges RC, Isbell TR, Klesges LM. Relationship between dietary restraint, energy intake, physical activity, and body weight: A prospective analysis. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 1992;101:668–674. doi: 10.1037//0021-843x.101.4.668. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Kostanski M, Fisher A, Gullone E. Current conceptualization of body image dissatisfaction: Have we got it wrong? Journal of Child Psycology & Psychiatry. 2004;45:1317–1325. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.00315.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Lewinsohn PM, Roberts RE, Seeley JR, Rohde P, Gotlib IH, Hops H. Adolescent psychopathology: II. Psychosocial risk factors for depression. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 1994;103:302–315. doi: 10.1037//0021-843x.103.2.302. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Lowe MR, Levine AS. Eating motives and the controversy over dieting: Eating less than needed versus less than wanted. Obesity Research. 2005;13:797–806. doi: 10.1038/oby.2005.90. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. McCarthy M. The thin ideal, depression, and eating disorders in women. Behavioral Research and Therapy. 1990;28:205–218. doi: 10.1016/0005-7967(90)90003-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. McCabe MP, Riccciardelli LA. Parent, peer, and media influences on body image and strategies to both increase and decrease body size among adolescent boys and girls. Adolescence. 2001;36:225–240. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. McCabe MP, Ricciardelli LA, Banfield S. Body image, strategies to change muscles and weight, and puberty: Do they impact on positive and negative affect among adolescent boys and girls? Eating Behaviors. 2001;2:129–149. doi: 10.1016/s1471-0153(01)00025-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. McCabe MP, Ricciardelli LA. A longitudinal study of pubertal timing and extreme body change behaviors among adolescent boys and girls. Adolescence. 2004;39:145–166. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. McCreary DR, Sasse DK. An exploration of the drive for muscularity in adolescent boys and girls. Journal of American College Health. 2000;48:297–304. doi: 10.1080/07448480009596271. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Moore DC. Body image and eating behavior in adolescent boys. American Journal of Diseases of the Child. 1990;144:475. doi: 10.1001/archpedi.1990.02150280097020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Muth JL, Cash TF. Body image attitudes: What difference does gender make? Journal of Applied Social Psychology. 1997;27:1438–1452. [Google Scholar]
  37. Neumark-Sztainer D, Story M, Falkner NH, Beuhring T, Resnick MD. Sociodemographic and personal characteristics of adolescents engaged in weight loss and weight/muscle gain behaviors: Who is doing what? Preventive Medicine. 1999;28:40–50. doi: 10.1006/pmed.1998.0373. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Nichter M, Nichter M. Hype and weight. Medical Anthropology. 1991;13:249–84. doi: 10.1080/01459740.1991.9966051. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Pietrobelli A, Faith M, Allison D, Gallagher D, Chiumello G, Heymsfield S. Body mass index as a measure of adiposity among children and adolescents: A validation study. Journal of Pediatrics. 1998;132:204–210. doi: 10.1016/s0022-3476(98)70433-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Presnell K, Bearman SK, Stice E. Risk Factors for Body Dissatisfaction in Adolescent Boys and Girls: A Prospective Study. International Journal of Eating Disorders. 2004;36:389–401. doi: 10.1002/eat.20045. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Presnell K, Stice E. An experimental test of the effect of weight-loss dieting on bulimic pathology: Tipping the scales in a different direction. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2003;112:166–170. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Raudenbush SW, Byrk AS. Hierarchical Linear Models: Applications and Data Analysis Methods. Second Sage Publications; Thousand Oaks: 2002. [Google Scholar]
  43. Ricciardelli LA, McCabe MP. Sociocultural influences on body image and body changes among adolescent boys and girls. Journal of Social Psychology. 2003;143:5–26. doi: 10.1080/00224540309598428. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Ricciardelli LA, McCabe MP. Dietary restraint and negative affect as mediators of body dissatisfaction and bulimic behavior in adolescent girls and boys. Behaviour Research and Therapy. 2001;39:1317–1328. doi: 10.1016/s0005-7967(00)00097-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Richards MH, Boxer AM, Petersen AC, Albrecht R. Relation of weight to body image in pubertal girls and boys from two communities. Developmental Psychology. 1990;26:313–321. [Google Scholar]
  46. Rosenblum GD, Lewis M. The relations among body image, physical attractiveness, and body mass in adolescence. Child Development. 1999;70:50–64. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. S-PLUS 6 Guide to Statistics. Insightful Corporation; Seattle WA: 1998. [Google Scholar]
  48. Schwartz DJ, Phares V, Tantleff-Dunn S, Thompson JK. Body image, psychological functioning, and parental feedback regarding physical appearance. International Journal of Eating Disorders. 1999;25:339–343. doi: 10.1002/(sici)1098-108x(199904)25:3<339::aid-eat13>3.0.co;2-v. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Seigel JM. Body image change and adolescent depressive symptoms. Journal of Adolescent Research. 2002;17:27–41. [Google Scholar]
  50. Smolak L, Levine M, Thompson JK. The use of the Sociocultural Attitudes Towards Appearance Questionnaire with middle school boys and girls. International Journal of Eating Disorders. 2001;29:216–223. doi: 10.1002/1098-108x(200103)29:2<216::aid-eat1011>3.0.co;2-v. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Stice E. A prospective test of the dual pathway model of bulimic pathology: Mediating effects of dieting and negative affect. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2001;110:124. doi: 10.1037//0021-843x.110.1.124. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Stice E, Bearman SK. Body image and eating disturbances prospectively predict increases in depressive symptoms in adolescent girls: A growth curve analysis. Developmental Psychology. 2001;37:597–607. doi: 10.1037//0012-1649.37.5.597. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Stice E, Cameron RP, Killen JD, Taylor CB. Naturalistic weight-reduction efforts prospectively predict growth in relative weight and onset of obesity among female adolescents. Journal of Consulting & Clinical Psychology. 1999;67:967–974. doi: 10.1037//0022-006x.67.6.967. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Stice E, Fisher M, Martinez E. Eating disorder diagnostic scale: Additional evidence of reliability and validity. Psychological Assessment. 2004;16:60–71. doi: 10.1037/1040-3590.16.1.60. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Stice E, Hayward C, Cameron R, Killen JD, Taylor CB. Body image and eating related factors predict onset of depression in female adolescents: A longitudinal study. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2000;109:438–444. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Stice E, Presnell K, Shaw H, Rohde P. Psychological and Behavioral Risk Factors for Obesity Onset in Adolescent Girls: A Prospective Study. Journal of Consulting & Clinical Psychology. 2005;73:195–202. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.73.2.195. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Stice E, Presnell K, Spangler D. Risk factors for binge eating onset: A prospective investigation. Health Psychology. 2002;21:131–138. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Stice E, Whitenton K. Risk factors for body dissatisfaction in adolescent girls: A longitudinal investigation. Developmental Psychology. 2002;38:669–678. doi: 10.1037//0012-1649.38.5.669. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Taylor MJ, Cooper PJ. An experimental study of the effects of mood on body size perception. Behaviour Research and Therapy. 1992;30:53–58. doi: 10.1016/0005-7967(92)90096-y. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Thompson JK, Heinberg LJ, Altabe M, Tantleff-Dunn S. Exacting Beauty: Theory, assessment, and treatment of body image disturbance. American Psychological Association; Washington, DC: 1999. [Google Scholar]
  61. Tiggemann M, Pennington B. The development of gender differences in body-size dissatisfaction. Australian Psychologist. 1990;25:306–313. [Google Scholar]
  62. van Strien T, Fritjters JER, Bergers GPA, Dafares PB. The Dutch Eating Behaviour Questionnaire (DEBQ) for assessment of restrained, emotional, and external eating behavor. International Journal of Eating Disorders. 1986a;5:295–315. [Google Scholar]
  63. van Strien T, Frijters JE, van Staveren WA, Defares PB, Deurenberg P. The predictive validity of the Dutch Restrained Eating Scale. International Journal of Eating Disorders. 1986b;5:747–755. [Google Scholar]
  64. Wichstrom L. The emergence of gender difference in depressed mood during adolescence: the role of intensified gender socialization. Developmental Psychology. 1999;35:232–245. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Wood KC, Becker JA, Thompson JK. Body image dissatisfaction in preadolescent children. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology. 1996;17:85–100. [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES