Abstract
Our prospective cohort study of nonsmoking African-American and Dominican mothers and children in New York City is evaluating the role of prenatal exposure to urban pollutants, including polycyclic aromatic hydrocarbons (PAHs), environmental tobacco smoke (ETS), and pesticides, in the pathogenesis of neurobehavioral disorders. We used the Bayley Scales of Infant Development to evaluate the effects on child mental and psychomotor development of prenatal exposure to airborne PAHs monitored during pregnancy by personal air sampling. Behavioral development was assessed by the Child Behavior Checklist. We adjusted for potential confounders including sociodemographic factors and prenatal exposure to ETS and chlorpyrifos. Prenatal exposure to PAHs was not associated with psychomotor development index or behavioral problems. However, high prenatal exposure to PAHs (upper quartile) was associated with lower mental development index at age 3 [β= –5.69; 95% confidence interval (CI), –9.05 to –2.33; p < 0.01]. The odds of cognitive developmental delay were also significantly greater for children with high prenatal exposure (odds ratio = 2.89; 95% CI, 1.33 to 6.25; p = 0.01). General estimated equation analysis showed a significant age × PAH effect on mental development (p = 0.01), confirming the age-specific regression findings. Further adjustment for lead did not alter the relationships. There were no differences in effect sizes by ethnicity. The results require confirmation but suggest that environmental PAHs at levels recently encountered in New York City air may adversely affect children’s cognitive development at 3 years of age, with implications for school performance.
Keywords: air pollution, neurodevelopment, polycyclic aromatic hydrocarbons, prenatal
The impact of environmental toxicants on children’s health is increasingly recognized as significant (Faustman 2000; Greater Boston Physicians for Social Responsibility 2000; Landrigan et al. 1999; Perera et al. 2002). Human and experimental studies indicate that the fetus and infant are more sensitive than adults to diverse environmental toxicants, including lead, mercury, environmental tobacco smoke (ETS), polycyclic aromatic hydrocarbons (PAHs), and pesticides (National Research Council 1993; Neri et al. 2006; Perera et al. 2005b; Whyatt and Perera 1995; World Health Organization 1986). Urban minority populations represent high-risk groups for adverse health and developmental outcomes (Claudio et al. 1999; Federico and Liu 2003; New York City Department of Health 1998; Perera et al. 2002). Although urban air pollution crosses geographic and socioeconomic boundaries, these same populations are likely to be more heavily exposed to indoor and outdoor air pollution and pesticides (Breysse et al. 2005; Olden and Poje 1995; Perera et al. 2002). As reported previously, the present study cohort has had substantial although variable exposure to multiple contaminants during pregnancy, with 100% of subjects having exposure to PAHs and pesticides in the air during pregnancy and 40% reporting ETS exposure (Perera et al. 2003; Rauh et al. 2004; Whyatt et al. 2002). PAH exposure in this urban cohort of nonsmokers is largely due to traffic sources and ETS (which was controlled for in analyses).
To our knowledge, there have been no prior human studies of the effect of prenatal exposure to airborne PAHs on child development. However, prenatal exposure to ETS has been associated with reduced fetal growth and cognitive functioning (Martinez et al. 1994; Rauh et al. 2004; Schuster and Ludwig 1994; Sexton et al. 1990; Windham et al. 1999; Yolton et al. 2005). Associations have been observed between prenatal exposure to the pesticide chlorpyrifos (CPF) and neurodevelopmental outcomes in experimental systems (Aldridge et al. 2005). Lead and mercury are known developmental toxicants affecting fetal development (Agency for Toxic Substances and Disease Registry 1999; Canfield et al. 2003; Grandjean et al. 1997; Lanphear et al. 2000).
In addition to being genotoxic and carcinogenic, PAHs such as benzo[a]pyrene (BaP) are endocrine disruptors (Bostrom et al. 2002; Bui et al. 1986; Kazeto et al. 2004). Prior laboratory and human studies in Central Europe and in our New York City cohort indicate that transplacental exposure to PAHs is associated with adverse birth outcomes (Barbieri et al. 1986; Bui et al. 1986; Dejmek et al. 2000; Legraverend et al. 1984; Perera et al. 1998, 2005a). In the present analysis, we evaluated the effects of prenatal exposure to airborne PAHs, estimated by personal air sampling of the mother during pregnancy, on mental and psychomotor development of children through 36 months of age, controlling for physical, biologic, and psychosocial determinants of these outcomes.
Materials and Methods
Study subjects
The present cohort study is being conducted by the Columbia Center for Children’s Environmental Health (CCCEH) (Perera et al. 2003). The study was approved by the Institutional Review Board (IRB) of Columbia University. Dominican and African-American women (ethnicity classified by self-report) residing in Washington Heights, Central Harlem, and the South Bronx, New York, who registered at the obstetrics/gynecology clinics at New York Presbyterian Medical Center and Harlem Hospital by the 20th week of pregnancy were approached in the clinics for consent. At that time, the women agreeing to participate in the prospective cohort study signed the IRB-approved consent form. Eligible women were nonsmokers during the current pregnancy; were free of diabetes, hypertension, and known HIV; had no documented or reported drug abuse; and had resided in the area for at least 1 year. At the time of this report, of 648 consenting and eligible mother–infant pairs, 536 were still participating in the cohort study; 271 children had reached 3 years of age. The retention rate for the full cohort was 83% at the 3-year follow-up. There were no significant differences between women retained in the study versus those who were lost to follow-up, on maternal age, ethnicity, marital status, education, income, gestational age, or birth weight of the newborn.
In this report we focus on the 183 children 3 years of age who had valid prenatal PAH monitoring data, all three annual developmental assessments, prenatal questionnaire data on ETS, measurements of cotinine in maternal and cord blood samples ≥25 ng/mL (to exclude the possibility that the mother was an active smoker), and CPF level in cord blood. This group did not differ in any of the maternal or infant characteristics or prenatal exposures in Table 1 from the 80 children 3 years of age excluded from the analysis because of missing data. Of these, 64 children were excluded because of missing developmental testing data.
Table 1.
Prenatal PAH exposure level |
||
---|---|---|
Characteristic | High exposureb (n = 42) | Low exposureb (n = 141) |
Maternal characteristics | ||
Ethnicity (%) | ||
African American | 40.5 | 47.5 |
Latino | 59.5 | 52.5 |
Age (years) | 25.03 ± 4.79 | 24.80 ± 5.53 |
Married (%) | 11.9 | 15.1 |
No high school degree (%) | 47.6 | 31.9 |
Maternal intelligence quotient | 84.37 ± 10.27 | 86.27 ± 13.45 |
Caretaking home environment | 37.55 ± 5.58* | 39.82 ± 5.82* |
Infant characteristics | ||
Birth weight (g) | 3,357.44 ± 529.69 | 3,413.16 ± 462.47 |
Birth length (cm) | 50.73 ± 2.72 | 50.79 ± 3.85 |
Birth head circumference (cm) | 34.01 ± 1.69 | 34.34 ± 1.95 |
Gestational age (week) | 39.17 ± 1.25 | 39.40 ± 1.38 |
Percent male | 47.6 | 45.4 |
Prenatal exposure | ||
ETS [at least one smoker in house (%)] | 38.1 | 39.7 |
CPF (pg/g) | 5.94 ± 11.44 | 3.56 ± 4.45 |
Cord lead (μg/dL)c | 1.01 ± 0.69 | 1.08 ± 0.79 |
Values are mean ± SD or percent.
Includes subjects with MDI and/or PDI (n = 183).
High exposure was defined as the fourth quartile of PAH; low exposure was defined as all others (quartiles 1, 2, 3). There was no difference between the two exposure groups with respect to any of the characteristics, except for the home environment.
Cord lead was available in a subset of 135.
p < 0.05, by Wilcoxon rank-sum test.
Personal interview
A 45-min questionnaire was administered by a trained bilingual interviewer during the last trimester of pregnancy (Perera et al. 2003). The questionnaire elicited demographic information, residential, health, and environmental history, including active and passive smoking [household members who smoke and estimated cigarettes smoked per day by smoker(s)], and socioeconomic information related to income and education. Postnatal interviews were administered at 6 months, annually, and every 3–6 months in between to determine any changes in residence, exposure to ETS, and other health or environmental conditions.
Prenatal personal PAH assessment
During the third trimester of pregnancy, personal monitoring was carried out as previously described (Perera et al. 2003). Vapors and particles ≥2.5 μg in diameter were collected on a precleaned quartz microfiber filter and a pre-cleaned polyurethane foam cartridge backup. The samples were analyzed at Southwest Research Institute (San Antonio, TX) for benz[a]anthracene, chrysene, benzo[b]fluroanthene, benzo[k]fluroanthene, BaP, indeno-[1,2,3-cd]pyrene, disbenz[a,h]anthracene, and benzo[g,h,i]perylene as described by Tonne et al. (2004). For quality control, each personal monitoring result was assessed as to accuracy in flow rate, time, and completeness of documentation. All of the 183 subjects had samples of acceptable quality.
Biologic sample collection and analysis
A sample of umbilical cord blood (30–60 mL) was collected at delivery by syringing blood into a heparinized syringe to avoid clotting. A sample of maternal blood (30–35 mL) was collected within 2 days postpartum into heparinized Vacutainer tubes (BD Medical, Franklin Lakes, NJ) by hospital staff. Samples were processed at the CCCEH laboratory, and portions were sent to the Environmental Health Laboratory at the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC; Atlanta, GA) for analysis of cotinine, heavy metals, and pesticides. Plasma cotinine was analyzed using high-performance liquid chromatography atmospheric-pressure ionization tandem mass spectrometry as described by Bernert et al. (1997, 2000). Plasma levels of CPF were analyzed using isotope-dilution gas chromatography–high-resolution mass spectrometry as described by Barr et al. (2002). In a subset (n = 135) of subjects, lead was analyzed by inductively coupled plasma mass spectrometry (CDC 2003).
Information on pregnancy outcomes
Information was abstracted by the research workers from mothers’ and infants’ medical records after delivery, including gestational age at birth, infant sex, birth weight, length, head circumference, infant malformations, and pregnancy complications. Gestational age was based on medical records for almost all subjects. Where those data were missing, gestational age was calculated from the last menstrual period.
Measures of child behavior and neurodevelopment
We used the Bayley Scales of Infant Development–Revised (BSID-II) to assess cognitive and psychomotor development at 12, 24, and 36 months of age (Bayley 1993). The BSID-II is the most widely used norm-referenced developmental test for young children, can be used to diagnose developmental delay, and is known to be sensitive to the developmental effects of toxic exposures such as low-level intrauterine lead. The stability of cognitive assessments during the first few years of life is limited, but the predictive power increases after 2 years. When administered at 3 years of age, the BSID-II has moderate predictive power for subsequent intelligence and school performance and is clinically useful for the identification of children performing in the subnormal range (Bayley 1993; Burchinal et al. 2000; Sternberg et al. 2001). Each test yields a developmental quotient (raw score/chronologic age), which generates a mental development index (MDI) and a corresponding psychomotor development index (PDI). In addition, children are classified as normal (> 85), moderately delayed (> 70 and ≥85), or severely delayed (≥70) based on standardized cut-points. Each child was tested under controlled conditions at the CCCEH by a bilingual research assistant, trained and checked for reliability. In the present study, the interrater reliability for the 24-month MDI was r = 0.92, based on double scoring of a random 5% of the sample (Rauh et al. 2004). One hundred eighty-one children had complete MDI at 1, 2, and 3 years of age; 181 had complete data on PDI, and 183 had either complete MDI or PDI.
Behavior problems were measured by maternal report on the 99-item Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL) for children 1.5–5 years of age, which collects information on child behaviors occurring in the past 2 months (Achenbach and Rescorla 2000). The CBCL is well validated, easy to administer, and useful as a screen for behavior problems. The Total Problems (T) score is the sum of the scores on the specific problem items plus the highest score on any additional problems entered by the respondent for the open-ended item 100, and is computed by summing the scores for the problems. T scores > 63 (> 90th percentile) represent the clinical range, and T scores between 60 and 63 (83rd to 90th percentile) represent the borderline range. The CBCL also yields scales derived from the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders 2000 that are intended to approximate clinical diagnoses, including affective, anxiety, pervasive developmental, attention deficit/hyperactivity, and oppositional defiant problems. All sub-scales are scored continuously and also categorically using a borderline or clinical cut-point corresponding to the 98th percentile for each domain. One hundred sixty-eight children of the children with Bayley scores also had CBCL data.
Maternal nonverbal intelligence was measured by the Test of Non-Verbal Intelligence, second edition (Brown et al. 1990), a 15-min, language-free measure of general intelligence, relatively stable and free of cultural bias. The test was administered when the child was 3 years of age. The quality of the proximal caretaking environment was measured by the Home Observation for Measurement of the Environment (Caldwell and Bradley 1979), administered at 3 years of age. The instrument assesses physical and interactive home characteristics (Bradley et al. 1989), is predictive of developmental scores in early childhood, and has been widely used in studies of neurotoxicity (e.g., Bellinger et al. 1988).
Statistical analysis
As in prior analysis (Perera et al. 2003), a composite PAH variable was computed from the eight inter-correlated PAH air concentration measures (r values ranging from 0.34 to 0.94; all p-values < 0.001 by Spearman’s rank). This variable was dichotomized at the fourth quartile (4.16 ng/m3) to obtain a measure of high/low exposure that is more robust than the continuous variable. The CPF variable was also dichotomized at the fourth quartile as previously described (Whyatt et al. 2004). The concentration of lead in cord blood was treated as a continuous variable.
We estimated the associations between pre-natal PAH exposure (high/low) and developmental scores (cognitive and psychomotor) for 12, 24, and 36 months of age using multiple linear regression for continuous outcomes (MDI and PDI) and logistic regression for categorical outcomes (likelihood of being classified as developmentally delayed). We estimated associations between prenatal PAH exposure (high/low) and behavior problems in the clinical range at 36 months of age using logistic regression. We used general estimated equation (GEE) (Liang and Zeger 1986) to estimate the size of the PAH effect over time (through 36 months) and at specific time points. To evaluate trends over time, the model compares the two exposure groups in terms of the difference in MDI scores obtained at 1 year of age (baseline) versus 2 years and 1 year versus 3 years, respectively. GEE has the advantage of requiring fewer assumptions than other methods; thus, the results of GEE are more robust. All effect estimates, 95% confidence intervals (CIs), and p-values (α = 0.05) were generated using SPSS (version 11.5; SPSS Inc., Chicago, IL) and SAS (version 9.0; SAS Institute Inc., Cary, NC). Covariates were retained in the models as potential confounders if they exhibited a relationship (p ≥0.1) with motor or mental development, regardless of their association with PAH exposure. The final models included an indicator for PAH exposure, the child’s exact age at test administration, child’s sex, ethnicity, gestational age at birth, quality of the home (caretaking) environment, and prenatal exposure to ETS and CPF measured as described above. In addition, the possible confounding of prenatal exposure by lead was tested in the subset of 135 children with available data. Interactions of PAH exposure with other independent variables were tested as appropriate.
We assessed potential mediation of the association between PAH exposure and development over time by including those fetal growth parameters previously shown to be affected by prenatal PAH exposure (birth weight and head circumference) in the models. If the estimate of the PAH effect on neurodevelopment was attenuated in the presence of a fetal growth parameter, mediation was considered to be present.
Results
Table 1 describes the characteristics of the sample stratified by level of PAH exposure. There were no significant differences between high- and low-exposure groups except for the home environment, which was less favorable in the high-exposure group. Prenatal PAH exposures averaged 3.49 ng/m3, with a range of 0.65–36.47 ng/m3; 39.3% of children had prenatal ETS exposure. Table 2 shows the mean ± SD or proportion for the developmental outcomes. These include the indices of performance on the BSID-II, including MDI, PDI, proportion moderately delayed on the mental index, and proportion moderately delayed on the motor index for each of the exposure groups at 12, 24, and 36 months of age. Table 2 also shows the mean ± SD for the CBCL score for total behavior problems.
Table 2.
Age at assessment |
|||
---|---|---|---|
Domain | 12 months | 24 months | 36 months |
MDIa | 94.25 ± 9.45 | 85.01 ± 12.59 | 89.66 ± 11.21 |
PDIb | 95.85 ± 12.17 | 97.40 ± 11.59 | 100.80 ± 13.21 |
Total behavior problemsc | NA | NA | 50.20 ± 10.47 |
Moderate mental development delay (%)a,d | 14.9 | 48.1 | 33.1 |
Severe mental development delay (%)a,e | 0.6 | 9.4 | 2.8 |
Moderate psychomotor development delay (%)b,f | 14.4 | 13.3 | 10.5 |
Severe psychomotor development delay (%)b,g | 1.7 | 1.7 | 1.7 |
NA, not applicable.
n = 181 with all 3 years of MDI data.
n = 181 with all 3 years of PDI data.
n = 168 with all 3 years of MDI and/or PDI and behavioral data (T score).
MDI < 85.
MDI < 70.
PDI < 85.
PDI < 70.
In univariate analysis of children with all three developmental measures, prenatal exposure to PAHs was significantly associated with MDI at age 3 (β= –4.68; 95% CI, –8.13 to –1.24; p = 0.01; n = 263) but not MDI at 1 or 2 years of age nor with PDI. Table 3 shows the age-specific parameter estimates for prenatal PAH exposure effects on mental and motor development, by multiple regression adjusting for the covariates as described in statistical analysis (n = 181). There was a significant effect of prenatal PAH exposure on MDI at age 3 (β= –5.69; 95% CI, –9.05 to –2.33; p < 0.01) but not 1 or 2 years of age. PDI was not associated with PAH exposure at any age. Altogether, the exposures and covariates accounted for approximately 31.2% of the variance in MDI scores at 36 months. None of the interaction terms of PAHs and the sociodemographic or exposure variables was significant. There was no significant interaction between PAH exposure level and home environment, suggesting that the magnitude of the 36-month prenatal PAH effect is not affected by the quality of the caretaking environment.
Table 3.
Model 1: 12 months |
Model 2: 24 months |
Model 3: 36 months |
||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
β | p-Value | β | p-Value | β | p-Value | |
MDI | ||||||
Constant | 74.52 | < 0.01 | 37.73 | 0.16 | 52.79 | 0.02 |
PAHs | 0.48 | 0.78 | –1.73 | 0.41 | –5.69 | < 0.01 |
Ethnicity (1 = African American; 0 = others) | 0.24 | 0.88 | 6.66 | < 0.01 | 6.34 | < 0.01 |
Sex (1 = male) | –2.09 | 0.14 | –4.51 | 0.01 | –2.20 | 0.13 |
Gestational age | 0.38 | 0.49 | 0.95 | 0.16 | 0.41 | 0.44 |
Home environment | 0.13 | 0.30 | 0.28 | 0.08 | 0.54 | < 0.01 |
PDI | ||||||
Constant | 95.34 | < 0.01 | 109.77 | < 0.01 | 54.00 | 0.06 |
PAHs | 1.32 | 0.55 | –2.08 | 0.32 | –0.97 | 0.68 |
Ethnicity (1 = African American; 0 = others) | –2.79 | 0.16 | 1.87 | 0.32 | 4.45 | 0.03 |
Sex (1 = male) | 1.12 | 0.54 | 0.52 | 0.76 | –1.24 | 0.52 |
Gestational age | 0.15 | 0.82 | –0.52 | 0.43 | 0.95 | 0.19 |
Home environment | –0.10 | 0.56 | 0.16 | 0.30 | 0.25 | 0.15 |
Models were also adjusted for prenatal ETS and CPF. Further inclusion of maternal IQ and maternal education as covariates did not alter the results.
In univariate logistic regression analysis, the likelihood of a child experiencing moderate mental developmental delay at 3 years of age was significantly increased as a function of prenatal PAH exposure (odds ratio = 2.05; 95% CI, 1.15–3.63; p = 0.01), but again, the relationship was not seen at 1 or 2 years of age nor with psychomotor developmental delay. Table 4 shows the results of the logistic regression analysis adjusting for the relevant covariates as described in statistical analyses. The odds ratio for delayed mental development at 36 months of age was 2.89 (95% CI, 1.33–6.25; n = 181). PAH exposure was not a significant predictor of psychomotor development. There were no significant interactions between PAHs and the other covariates in logistic regression.
Table 4.
Model 1: 12 months |
Model 2: 24 months |
Model 3: 36 months |
|||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
β | p-Value | Exp(β) | β | p-Value | Exp(β) | β | p-Value | Exp(β) | |
Dependent variable: moderate delay (MDI < 85) | |||||||||
Constant | 2.65 | 0.67 | 14.12 | 3.04 | 0.53 | 20.92 | 6.68 | 0.24 | 798.33 |
PAHs | −0.19 | 0.71 | 0.82 | −0.16 | 0.68 | 0.86 | 1.06 | 0.01 | 2.89 |
Ethnicity (1 = African American; 0 = others) | 0.32 | 0.50 | 1.37 | −0.90 | 0.01 | 0.41 | −0.77 | 0.06 | 0.46 |
Sex (1 = male) | 0.66 | 0.12 | 1.94 | 0.84 | 0.01 | 2.31 | 0.50 | 0.16 | 1.65 |
Gestational age | −0.07 | 0.67 | 0.93 | −0.02 | 0.89 | 0.98 | −0.10 | 0.47 | 0.90 |
Home environment | −0.05 | 0.19 | 0.95 | −0.06 | 0.03 | 0.94 | −0.10 | < 0.01 | 0.90 |
Dependent variable: moderate delay (PDI < 85) | |||||||||
Constant | −1.50 | 0.82 | 0.22 | −1.11 | 0.87 | 0.33 | −2.30 | 0.79 | 0.10 |
PAHs | −0.92 | 0.16 | 0.40 | 0.41 | 0.41 | 1.51 | −0.22 | 0.72 | 0.80 |
Ethnicity (1 = African American; 0 = others) | 0.39 | 0.41 | 1.48 | −0.21 | 0.67 | 0.81 | −0.65 | 0.28 | 0.52 |
Sex (1 = male) | −0.18 | 0.68 | 0.83 | −0.42 | 0.35 | 0.66 | 0.01 | 0.98 | 1.01 |
Gestational age | −0.01 | 0.97 | 0.99 | −0.02 | 0.89 | 0.98 | 0.04 | 0.85 | 1.04 |
Home environment | −0.01 | 0.88 | 0.99 | 0.01 | 0.86 | 1.01 | −0.05 | 0.25 | 0.95 |
Models were also adjusted for prenatal ETS and CPF. Further inclusion of maternal IQ and maternal education as covariates did not alter the results.
Table 5 and Figure 1 show the results of the GEE analysis of PAH effects on cognitive development over the 3-year follow-up period. The significant age × PAH effect on mental development (p = 0.01) confirms the age-specific regression findings showing that an adverse impact of prenatal PAH exposure on this developmental domain was seen only over time. For motor development, there was no significant relationship (Table 5). At 3 years of age, the decrease in MDI from baseline was significantly greater for high-exposed compared to low-exposed children (p = 0.01), whereas the difference was not significant at 2 years of age. The results of analyses using the continuous measure of PAHs were generally similar but less significant. Inclusion of fetal growth parameters (birth weight or head circumference) did not alter the effect of PAHs on 36 month MDI. Inclusion of cord lead as a covariate did not materially alter the association between PAHs and development. The results of regression of total CBCL behavior problems on prenatal PAH exposure were not significant, nor were any of the subscales significantly related to PAH exposure.
Table 5.
MDI |
PDI |
|||
---|---|---|---|---|
β | p-Value | β | p-Value | |
Intercept | 60.46 | < 0.01 | 84.76 | < 0.01 |
24 months | −9.87 | < 0.01 | −2.12 | 0.20 |
36 months | −4.29 | 0.01 | 3.53 | 0.08 |
PAHs | 0.69 | 0.66 | 1.89 | 0.35 |
24 months × PAHs | −1.12 | 0.62 | −3.14 | 0.16 |
36 months × PAHs | −5.57 | 0.01 | −4.65 | 0.08 |
Ethnicity (1 = African American; 0 = others) | −0.23 | 0.88 | −3.38 | 0.07 |
24 months × ethnicity | 6.80 | < 0.01 | 5.71 | 0.01 |
36 months × ethnicity | 7.14 | < 0.01 | 7.91 | < 0.01 |
Sex (1 = male) | −2.80 | 0.01 | 0.08 | 0.95 |
Gestational age | 0.56 | 0.16 | 0.23 | 0.62 |
Home environment | 0.31 | < 0.01 | 0.11 | 0.39 |
Models were also adjusted for prenatal ETS and CPF.
Discussion
Previous results from this cohort have indicated that exposure to PAH air pollutants during pregnancy has produced DNA damage and impaired fetal growth (Perera et al. 2003, 2005a, 2005b). The present analysis suggests a further impact of prenatal PAH exposure on cognitive development. The infants who had been exposed prenatally to the highest PAH levels scored significantly lower on MDI at 3 years of age than did those with lower levels of PAH exposure. Although the adjusted mean MDI scores of the high- and low-exposed PAH groups differed by only 5.69 points, among the highly exposed children the odds of having MDI scores < 85 at 3 years of age (indicating moderate delay) were 2.89 times greater than the odds among unexposed children. This suggests that more exposed children are potentially at risk for performance deficits (language, reading, and math) in the early school years. In fact, developmentally delayed children are eligible for early intervention services designed for children who are at possible risk for early school failure. The observed magnitude of the PAH effect on early development in this study is comparable to that reported for low-level lead exposure (Schwartz 1994). In this study, there was no effect of PAHs on cognitive development at 1 and 2 years of age, nor were psychomotor development and behavioral problems associated with PAHs. The impact of PAHs on mental development at 3 years of age does not appear to be mediated by birth weight or head circumference, fetal growth parameters previously shown to be associated with prenatal PAH exposure in this cohort (Perera et al. 2003). The children are being followed to 7–8 years of age, so subsequent testing will provide a picture of the developmental trajectory of this group.
To our knowledge, there have been no prior studies of the role of prenatal exposure to airborne PAHs in child neurodevelopment. However, a study in the Czech Republic reported that schoolchildren in the district of Teplice, which had higher levels of PAHs and other air pollutants from coal burning than did the district of Prachatrice, had a significantly higher teacher referral rate for clinical assessment compared to Prachatrice, although most objective performance measures did not differ (Šrám et al. 1996).
The mechanisms by which PAHs might affect the developing brain are not known. However, fetal toxicity may be caused by antiestrogenic effects (Bui et al. 1986) binding to the human aryl hydrocarbon receptor to induce P450 enzymes (Manchester et al. 1987), DNA damage resulting in activation of apoptotic pathways (Meyn 1995; Nicol et al. 1995; Wood and Youle 1995), or binding to receptors for placental growth factors resulting in decreased exchange of oxygen and nutrients (Dejmek et al. 2000).
In the present cohort, the mean developmental scores are below average for the normed population, reflecting the low-income nature of the catchment area in this study (e.g., Bradley and Corwyn 2002; Burchinal et al. 1997; Luster and McAdoo 1996). In addition, children from homes with low levels of stimulation and mother–child interaction had significantly lower scores on the Bayley cognitive scales, independent of PAH exposure.
This study has the advantage of being based on individual prenatal exposure data from personal monitoring and biomarker analyses, as well as extensive medical record and questionnaire data. However, it is limited by the modest sample of subjects for whom data from all relevant domains are currently available. Moreover, relationships observed in low-income minority women might be different in women of other races or ethnic, cultural, or socioeconomic backgrounds. Further, it is possible that high levels of PAHs may be associated with living near an exposure source such as a bus route or garage leading to some uncontrolled confounding by socioeconomic status even within our low-income population.
Another limitation is that we lacked air monitoring data for all three trimesters and were therefore not able to compare exposures across these three periods. We also lacked post-natal personal air monitoring data for PAHs and were unable to control directly for postnatal PAH exposure. However, when we controlled for change in residence as a proxy for variation in PAH exposure between the pre-and postnatal periods, the effects of prenatal PAHs remained. We also lacked postnatal environmental lead exposure data, which may have been a critical period of exposure to this known developmental toxicant. Additional studies are needed to tease apart the effects of prenatal and postnatal exposure to PAHs and to confirm the present findings.
Conclusion
This study provides evidence that environmental PAHs at levels recently encountered in the air of New York City may adversely affect cognitive development of children. The results require confirmation but are of potential concern because compromised mental performance in the preschool years is an important precursor of subsequent educational performance deficits. PAHs are widespread in urban environments worldwide largely as a result of fossil fuel combustion. Fortunately, airborne PAH concentrations can be reduced through currently available pollution controls, greater energy efficiency, and the use of alternative energy sources (Wong et al. 2004).
Footnotes
We acknowledge H. Andrews, R. Garfinkel, Y.H. Jin, L. Qu, J. Zhou, A. Reyes, and M. Borjas at the Columbia Center for Children’s Environmental Health, and L. Needham, T. Bernert, R. Jones, and K. Caldwell at the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention.
This study was supported by the National Institute of Enviromental Health Sciences (5P01ES09600, 5RO1ES08977, RO1ES111158, RO1ES012468, ES09089), the U.S. Environmental Protection Agency (R827027, 8260901, RR00645), Educational Foundation of America, Horace W. Goldsmith Foundation, the Irving A. Hansen Memorial Foundation, Gladys & Roland Harriman Foundation, the New York Community Trust, the Bauman Family Foundation, the Beldon Fund, and the John Merck Fund.
References
- Achenbach T, Rescorla L. 2000. Child Behavior Checklist for Ages 1 1/2–5. 7-28-0 ed. Burlington, VT:Achenbach System of Empirically Based Assessment (ASEBA).
- Agency for Toxic Substances and Disease Registry 1999. Toxicological Profile for Mercury. Atlanta, GA:Agency for Toxic Substances and Disease Registry.
- Aldridge JE, Meyer A, Seidler FJ, Slotkin TA. Alterations in central nervous system serotonergic and dopaminergic synaptic activity in adulthood after prenatal or neonatal chlorpyrifos exposure. Environ Health Perspect. 2005;113:1027–1031. doi: 10.1289/ehp.7968. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barbieri O, Ognio E, Rossi O, Astigiano S, Rossi L. Embryotoxicity of benzo(a)pyrene and some of its synthetic derivatives in Swiss mice. Cancer Res. 1986;46:94–98. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barr DB, Barr JR, Maggio VL, Whitehead RD, Sadowski MA, Whyatt RM, et al. A multi-analytic method for the quantification of contemporary pesticides in human serum and plasma using high resolution mass spectrometry. J Chromatogr B Analyt Technol Biomed Life Sci. 2002;778:99–111. doi: 10.1016/s0378-4347(01)00444-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bayley N. 1993. Bayley Scales of Infant Development. 2nd ed. San Antonio, TX:Psychological Corp.
- Bellinger D, Leviton A, Waternaux C, Needleman H, Rabinowitz M. Low-level lead exposure, social class, and infant development. Neurotoxicol Teratol. 1988;10:497–503. doi: 10.1016/0892-0362(88)90084-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bernert JT, McGuffey JE, Morrison MA, Pirkle JL. Comparison of serum and salivary cotinine measurements by a sensitive high-performance liquid chromatography-tandem mass spectrometry method as an indicator of exposure to tobacco smoke among smokers and non-smokers. J Anal Toxicol. 2000;24:333–339. doi: 10.1093/jat/24.5.333. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bernert JT, Turner WE, Pirkle JL, Sosnoff CS, Akins JR, Waldrep MK, et al. Development and validation of sensitive method for determination of serum cotinine in smokers and nonsmokers by liquid chromatography/ atmospheric pressure ionization tandem mass spectrometry. Clin Chem. 1997;43:2281–2291. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bostrom CE, Gerde P, Hanberg A, Jernstrom B, Johansson C, Kyrklund T, et al. Cancer risk assessment, indicators, and guidelines for polycyclic aromatic hydrocarbons in the ambient air. Environ Health Perspect. 2002;110:451–488. doi: 10.1289/ehp.110-1241197. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bradley RH, Caldwell B, Rock S, Ramey C, Barnard K, Gray C, et al. Home environment and cognitive development in the first 3 years of life: a collaborative study involving six sites and three ethnic groups in North America. Dev Psychol. 1989;25:217–235. [Google Scholar]
- Bradley RH, Corwyn RF. Moderating effect of perceived amount of family conflict on the relation between home environmental processes and the well-being of adolescents. J Fam Psychol. 2000;14:349–364. doi: 10.1037//0893-3200.14.3.349. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Breysse PN, Buckley TJ, Williams D, Beck CM, Jo SJ, Merriman B, et al. Indoor exposures to air pollutants and allergens in the homes of asthmatic children in inner-city Baltimore. Environ Res. 2005;98:167–176. doi: 10.1016/j.envres.2004.07.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brown L, Sherbenou RJ, Johnson SK. 1990. Test of Non-Verbal Intelligence: A Language-Free Measure of Cognitive Ability. 2nd ed. Austin, TX:PRO-ED Inc.
- Bui QQ, Tran MB, West WL. A comparative study of the reproductive effects of methadone and benzo(a)pyrene in the pregnant and pseudopregnant rat. Toxicology. 1986;42:195–204. doi: 10.1016/0300-483x(86)90009-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Burchinal MR, Campbell FA, Bryant DM, Wasik BH, Ramey CT. Early intervention and mediating processes in cognitive performance of children of low-income African American families. Child Dev. 1997;68:935–954. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.1997.tb01972.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Burchinal MR, Roberts JE, Kooper S, Zeisel SA. Cumulative risk and early cognitive development: a comparison of statistical risk models. Dev Psychol. 2000;36:793–807. doi: 10.1037//0012-1649.36.6.793. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Caldwell BM, Bradley RH. 1979. Home Observation for Measurement of the Environment. Little Rock, AK:University of Arkansas Press.
- Canfield RL, Henderson CR, Cory-Slechta DA, Cox C, Jusko TA, Lanphear BP. Intellectual impairment in children with blood lead concentrations below 10 μg per deciliter. N Engl J Med. 2003;348:1517–1521. doi: 10.1056/NEJMoa022848. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- CDC 2003. Whole Blood Lead, Cadmium and Mercury Determined Using Inductively Coupled Plasma Mass Spectrometry. DLS method code 2003-01/OD. CLIA Methods. Atlanta, GA:Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, Division of Laboratory Science.
- Claudio L, Tulton L, Doucette J, Landrigan PJ. Socioeconomic factors and asthma hospitalization rates in New York City. J Asthma. 1999;36:343–350. doi: 10.3109/02770909909068227. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dejmek J, Solansky I, Beneš I, Leníc ek J, Šrám RJ. The impact of polycyclic aromatic hydrocarbons and fine particles on pregnancy outcome. Environ Health Perspect. 2000;108:1159–1164. doi: 10.1289/ehp.001081159. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders 2000. 4th ed. Arlington, VA:American Psychiatric Publishing.
- Faustman EM. Mechanisms underlying children’s susceptibility to environmental toxicants. Environ Health Perspect. 2000;108:13–21. doi: 10.1289/ehp.00108s113. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Federico MJ, Liu AH. Overcoming childhood asthma disparities of the inner-city poor. Pediatr Clin North Am. 2003;50:655–675. doi: 10.1016/s0031-3955(03)00045-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grandjean P, Weihe P, White R, Debes F, Arak S, Yokoyama K, et al. Cognitive deficit in 7-year-old children with prenatal exposure to methylmercury. Neurotoxicol Teratol. 1997;20:1–12. doi: 10.1016/s0892-0362(97)00097-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Greater Boston Physicians for Social Responsibility 2000. In Harm’s Way: Toxic Threats to Child Development. Cambridge, MA:Greater Boston Physicians for Social Responsibility.
- Kazeto Y, Place AR, Trant JM. Effects of endocrine disrupting chemicals on the expression of CYP19 genes in zebrafish (Danio rerio) juveniles. Aquat Toxicol. 2004;69:25–34. doi: 10.1016/j.aquatox.2004.04.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Landrigan PJ, Claudio L, Markowitz SB, Berkowitz GS, Brenner BL, Romero H, et al. Pesticides and inner-city children: exposures, risks, and prevention. Environ Health Perspect. 1999;107:431–437. doi: 10.1289/ehp.99107s3431. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lanphear BP, Dietrich K, Auinger P, Cox C. Cognitive deficits associated with blood lead concentrations < 10 microg/dL in US children and adolescents. Public Health Rep. 2000;115:521–529. doi: 10.1093/phr/115.6.521. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Legraverend C, Guenthner TM, Nebert DW. Importance of the route of administration for genetic differences in benzo(a)pyrene-induced in utero toxicity and teratogenicity. Teratology. 1984;29:35–47. doi: 10.1002/tera.1420290106. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Liang KY, Zeger LL. Longitudinal data analysis using generalized linear models. Biometrika. 1986;73:13–22. [Google Scholar]
- Luster T, McAdoo H. Family and child influences on educational attainment: a secondary analysis of the High/ Scope Perry preschool data. Dev Psychol. 1996;32:23–39. [Google Scholar]
- Manchester DK, Gordon SK, Golas CL, Roberts EA, Okey AB. Ah receptor in human placenta: stabilization by molybdate and characterization of binding of 2,3,7,8-tetra-chlorodibenzo-p-dioxin, 3-methylcholanthrene, and benzo(a)pyrene. Cancer Res. 1987;47:4861–4868. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Martinez FD, Wright AL, Taussig LM. The effect of paternal smoking on the birthweight of newborns whose mothers did not smoke. Am J Public Health. 1994;84:1489–1491. doi: 10.2105/ajph.84.9.1489. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Meyn MS. Ataxia-telangiectasia and cellular responses to DNA damage. Cancer Res. 1995;55:5991–6001. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- National Research Council 1993. Pesticides in the Diets of Infants and Children. Washington, DC:National Academy Press. [PubMed]
- Neri M, Ugolini D, Bonassi S, Fucic A, Holland N, Knudsen LE, et al. Children’s exposure to environmental pollutants and biomarkers of genetic damage. II. Results of a comprehensive literature search and meta-analysis. Mutat Res. 2006;612:14–39. doi: 10.1016/j.mrrev.2005.04.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- New York City Department of Health 1998–1999. Vital Statistics. New York:New York City Department of Health.
- Nicol CJ, Harrison ML, Laposa RR, Gimelshtein IL, Wells PG. A teratologic suppressor role for p53 in benzo[a]pyrene-treated transgenic p53-deficient mice. Nat Genet. 1995;10:181–187. doi: 10.1038/ng0695-181. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Olden K, Poje J. Environmental justice and environmental health. Bull Soc Occup Environ Health. 1995;4:3–4. [Google Scholar]
- Perera FP, Illman SM, Kinney PL, Whyatt RM, Kelvin EA, Shepard P, et al. The challenge of preventing environmentally related disease in young children: community-based research in New York City. Environ Health Perspect. 2002;110:197–204. doi: 10.1289/ehp.02110197. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Perera FP, Rauh V, Tsai WY, Kinney P, Camann D, Barr D, et al. Effects of transplacental exposure to environmental pollutants on birth outcomes in a multi-ethnic population. Environ Health Perspect. 2003;111:201–205. doi: 10.1289/ehp.5742. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Perera FP, Rauh V, Whyatt RM, Tang D, Tsai WY, Bernert JT, et al. A summary of recent findings on birth outcomes and developmental effects of prenatal ETS, PAH, and pesticide exposures. Neurotoxicology. 2005a;26:573–587. doi: 10.1016/j.neuro.2004.07.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Perera FP, Tang D, Whyatt R, Lederman S, Jedrychowski W. DNA damage from polycyclic aromatic hydrocarbons (PAHs) measured by benzo[a]pyrene DNA-adducts in mothers and newborns from Northern Manhattan, the World Trade Center area, Poland, and China. Cancer Epidemiol Biomarker Prev. 2005b;14:709–714. doi: 10.1158/1055-9965.EPI-04-0457. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Perera FP, Whyatt RM, Jedrychowski W, Rauh V, Manchester D, Santella RM, et al. Recent developments in molecular epidemiology: a study of the effects of environmental polycylic aromatic hydrocarbons on birth outcomes in Poland. Am J Epidemiol. 1998;147:309–314. doi: 10.1093/oxfordjournals.aje.a009451. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rauh VA, Whyatt RM, Garfinkel R, Andrews H, Hoepner L, Reyes A, et al. Developmental effects of exposure to environmental tobacco smoke and material hardship among inner-city children. J Neurotoxicol Teratol. 2004;26:373–385. doi: 10.1016/j.ntt.2004.01.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schuster KJ, Ludwig H. Smoking and the risk of cancer. Wien Med Wochenschr. 1994;144:540–544. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schwartz J. Low-level lead exposure and children’s IQ: a meta-analysis and search for a threshold. Environ Res. 1994;65:42–55. doi: 10.1006/enrs.1994.1020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sexton M, Fox NL, Hebel JR. Prenatal exposure to tobacco. II. Effects on cognitive functioning at age three. Int J Epidemiol. 1990;19:72–77. doi: 10.1093/ije/19.1.72. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Šrám RJ, Beneš I, Binkova B, Dejmek J, Horstman D, Kotesovec F, et al. Teplice Program—the impact of air pollution on human health. Environ Health Perspect. 1996;104:699–714. doi: 10.1289/ehp.104-1469669. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sternberg RJ, Grigorenko EL, Bandy DA. The predictive value of IQ. Merrill-Palmer Q. 2001;47:1–41. [Google Scholar]
- Tonne CC, Whyatt RM, Camann DE, Perera FP, Kinney PL. Predictors of personal polycyclic aromatic hydrocarbon exposures among pregnant minority women in New York City. Environ Health Perspect. 2004;112:754–759. doi: 10.1289/ehp.5955. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Whyatt RM, Camann DE, Kinney PL, Reyes A, Ramirez J, Dietrich J, et al. Residential pesticide use during pregnancy among a cohort of urban minority women. Environ Health Perspect. 2002;110:507–514. doi: 10.1289/ehp.02110507. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Whyatt RM, Perera FP. Application of biologic markers to studies of environmental risks in children and the developing fetus. Environ Health Perspect. 1995;103:105–110. doi: 10.1289/ehp.95103s6105. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Whyatt RW, Rauh V, Barr DB, Camann DE, Andrews HF, Garfinkel R, et al. Prenatal insecticide exposures and birth weight and length among an urban minority cohort. Environ Health Persepect. 2004;112:1125–1132. doi: 10.1289/ehp.6641. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Windham GC, Eaton A, Hopkins B. Evidence for an association between environmental tobacco smoke exposure and birth weight: a meta-analysis and new data. Paediatr Perinat Epidemiol. 1999;13:35–57. doi: 10.1046/j.1365-3016.1999.00150.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wong EY, Gohlke J, Griffith WC, Farrow S, Faustman EM. Assessing the health benefits of air pollution reduction for children. Environ Health Perspect. 2004;112:226–232. doi: 10.1289/ehp.6299. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wood KA, Youle RJ. The role of free radicals and p53 in neuron apoptosis in vivo. J Neurosci. 1995;15:5851–5857. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.15-08-05851.1995. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- World Health Organization 1986. Principles for Evaluating Health Risks from Chemicals during Infancy and Early Childhood: The Need for a Special Approach. Environmental Health Criteria 59. Geneva:World Health Organization.
- Yolton K, Dietrich K, Auinger P, Lanphear BP, Hornung R. Exposure to environmental tobacco smoke and cognitive abilities among U.S. children and adolescents. Environ Health Perspect. 2005;113:98–103. doi: 10.1289/ehp.7210. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]