Skip to main content
Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences logoLink to Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences
. 2006 Feb 28;273(1592):1355–1360. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2005.3445

Correlated preferences for facial masculinity and ideal or actual partner's masculinity

Lisa M DeBruine 1,*, Benedict C Jones 2, Anthony C Little 3, Lynda G Boothroyd 4, David I Perrett 1, Ian S Penton-Voak 5, Philip A Cooper 6, Lars Penke 7,8, David R Feinberg 9, Bernard P Tiddeman 10
PMCID: PMC1560296  PMID: 16777723

Abstract

Studies of women's preferences for male faces have variously reported preferences for masculine faces, preferences for feminine faces and no effect of masculinity–femininity on male facial attractiveness. It has been suggested that these apparently inconsistent findings are, at least partly, due to differences in the methods used to manipulate the masculinity of face images or individual differences in attraction to facial cues associated with youth. Here, however, we show that women's preferences for masculinity manipulated in male faces using techniques similar to the three most widely used methods are positively inter-related. We also show that women's preferences for masculine male faces are positively related to ratings of the masculinity of their actual partner and their ideal partner. Correlations with partner masculinity were independent of real and ideal partner age, which were not associated with facial masculinity preference. Collectively, these findings suggest that variability among studies in their findings for women's masculinity preferences reflects individual differences in attraction to masculinity rather than differences in the methods used to manufacture stimuli, and are important for the interpretation of previous and future studies of facial masculinity.

Keywords: masculinity, femininity, sexual dimorphism, faces, computer graphics

1. Introduction

Previous studies of females have reported both general preferences for male faces with masculinized proportions (Johnston et al. 2001; Penton-Voak et al. 2001) and general preferences for male faces with feminized proportions (Perrett et al. 1998; Penton-Voak et al. 1999; Little et al. 2001, 2002; Rhodes et al. 2003a). Other studies have found no overall preference for masculinity (Swaddle & Riersen 2002; Cornwell et al. 2004). Methodological differences have been put forward as an explanation (Johnston et al. 2001; Swaddle & Riersen 2002; Fink et al. 2005; Rhodes 2006), although diverse methodologies produce similar results for female face preferences. Alternatively, individual differences (e.g. attractiveness, partnership status) affect masculinity preferences (Little et al. 2001, 2002; Penton-Voak et al. 2003), and differences in the average characteristics of study populations may explain differences among studies.

Facial masculinity is thought to signal heritable immunity to infectious disease, because only men with strong immune systems are able to withstand the immunosuppressant effects of high levels of circulating testosterone necessary to develop masculine features (see Thornhill & Gangestad (1999) for a review). Although this proposal remains controversial, recent studies have demonstrated that masculinity of male facial appearance is positively associated with circulating levels of testosterone (assessed from saliva samples; Penton-Voak & Chen 2004) and negatively associated with incidence of past health problems (assessed from medical records; Rhodes et al. 2003a). While many theories of attraction propose that men with masculine features might be attractive to women due to hypothesized preferences for men displaying cues to good genes for immunocompetence (Thornhill & Gangestad 1999), women's preferences for masculine male faces appear to be highly variable (see Penton-Voak & Perrett (2000) for a review). Aversions to masculinity may reflect perceived ‘costs’ associated with preferring masculine partners, such as unwillingness to invest care and resources in partners or offspring (Mazur & Booth 1998; Perrett et al. 1998; Gangestad & Simpson 2000). Indeed, Gray et al. (2002) reported that men with high circulating levels of testosterone spend less time with their partners and offspring than men with relatively low levels of testosterone do.

Although women's preferences for masculine male faces appear highly variable, variation is predicted by evolutionary theory. Attraction to masculine male faces is stronger during the fertile phase of the menstrual cycle (i.e. the late follicular phase) than in other phases (Penton-Voak et al. 1999, 2001; Johnston et al. 2001). It is also stronger when women judge the attractiveness of male faces for short-term relationships than for long-term relationships (Little et al. 2002; Penton-Voak et al. 2003). This latter effect is particularly pronounced for judgements made by women with a partner (Little et al. 2002) and also by relatively unattractive women, as attractive women may be better placed to offset the costs associated with preferring a masculine primary partner (Little et al. 2001; Penton-Voak et al. 2003). Oral contraceptive use disrupts many of these correlations (Penton-Voak et al. 1999; Little et al. 2002). In addition to these sources of individual differences in face preferences, many researchers have proposed that differences in the methods used to manufacture stimuli for the assessment of attraction to faces also contribute to variation among studies in women's preferences for sexual dimorphism in male faces (Johnston et al. 2001; Swaddle & Riersen 2002; Valenzano et al. 2004; Fink et al. 2005; Rhodes 2006).

(a) Methods for manipulating masculinity

Facial masculinity preferences have been assessed using three main methods of defining a masculinity–femininity dimension: sexual dimorphism, perceived masculinity and pubertal development.

Perrett et al. (1998) and others (Penton-Voak et al. 2001; Cornwell et al. 2004) have used the differences in shape between prototype male and female faces to manufacture face stimuli varying in sexual dimorphism. Using this method, adding a positive multiple of the female to male differences increases the masculinity of a subject's facial image, while adding a negative amount increases their femininity. Many researchers have criticized this method, stating that manipulating the appearance of face images using a continuum defined by male and female prototypes does not necessarily reflect changes to facial appearance that are due to the influence of testosterone (Meyer & Quong 1999; Swaddle & Riersen 2002).

Johnston et al. (2001) have suggested that the technique developed by Perrett et al. (1998), which assumes that an extreme masculine face shape can be generated by a linear extrapolation of the differences between male and female average faces, may not be valid because between-sex differences in facial shape are the result of bone growth caused by complex interactions between growth hormone, androgens and oestrogen (Tanner 1978; Grumbach 2000). Johnston et al. (2001) transformed the shape and colour along a three-part continuum defined by female and male student faces and male model faces perceived as particularly masculine, finding that women generally preferred masculine male faces. Perceived masculinity has been found to correlate positively with salivary testosterone levels (Penton-Voak & Chen 2004).

To address similar criticisms of Perrett et al.'s (1998) methods for manipulating masculinity in male faces, Swaddle & Riersen (2002) manipulated the shape of male faces using data from studies of face proportion change during puberty (e.g. Enlow 1990). While this influenced the perceived dominance of the faces, the manipulations did not influence face preferences.

Because no study has previously compared women's preferences for face stimuli manufactured in these different ways, it remains unclear whether differences in preferences among studies using the methods of Perrett et al. (1998), Swaddle & Riersen (2002) or Johnston et al. (2001) reflect individual differences in women's face preferences or are a consequence of differences in the methods used to manipulate the appearance of face images.

In an effort to establish if variability in face preferences among studies that used different methods to manipulate the masculinity of male face images is due to differences in the methods used or due to individual differences in women's face preferences, here we tested the inter-relationships among women's preferences for male face images that had been transformed in appearance using the differences in shape between male and female prototypes (sensu Perrett et al. 1998), the differences in shape between prototypes manufactured from pre- and post-pubertal male faces (a method derived from Swaddle & Riersen 2002) and the differences between prototypes manufactured from male faces that were perceived (by independent raters) as particularly masculine or feminine in appearance (a method derived from Johnston et al. (2001)). If variation among studies in their findings for women's preferences for masculine male faces reflects individual differences in face preferences, then attraction to masculine faces manufactured from these three methods will be positively inter-related. If this is not the case, it would suggest that variation among studies in their findings for women's preferences for masculine male faces reflects differences in the methods used to manipulate face images.

(b) Validating preferences for masculinity

Variation in preferences for masculinity may not be peculiar to judgements of face images, but may be indicative of preferences for a masculine partner. Preferences for masculinity in the separate domains of face (Penton-Voak et al. 1999), voice (Feinberg et al. 2006; Puts 2005) and behaviour (Gangestad et al. 2004) are influenced in the same way by menstrual cycle phase. Additionally, Cornwell et al. (2004) found that attraction to masculine male faces was positively associated with attraction to a putative male sex pheromone. Although many theories of attraction suggest that face preferences reflect mate preferences more generally, we know of no studies that have explicitly tested for a relationship between face preferences and actual partner characteristics.

To examine whether masculinity preferences can relate to real world partner choice, we tested for a relationship between preferences for masculine faces as assessed by an experiment and ideal partner masculinity as reported in a questionnaire. Additionally, we tested for a relationship between preferences for masculine faces and actual partner masculinity (as reported in a questionnaire) in partnered women. As preferences for masculinity and age in faces are positively related (Cunningham et al. 1990; Boothroyd et al. 2005) and masculine faces appear older than feminine faces (Perrett et al. 1998; Boothroyd et al. 2005), we also tested for relationships between masculinity preference and ideal or actual partner age.

2. Material and methods

(a) Stimulus manufacture

Six individual male faces (ages 17–19) were masculinized and feminized using prototype-based computer graphic transformations (Tiddeman et al. 2001). Faces were transformed in shape (sexual dimorphism and pubertal development methods) or shape, colour and texture (perceived masculinity method) relative to the differences between two composite prototype faces, one ‘masculine’ and one ‘feminine’. The computer algorithm changed either the shape alone or the shape, colour and texture of an individual face by 50% of the shape, or the shape, colour and texture difference between the two prototype faces, respectively. For example, if the masculine prototype face has thinner lips compared to the feminine prototype face, then masculinizing an individual face will cause the lips to become thinner. Note that this is not the same as morphing an individual face towards or away from a male or female face. A male face that is more masculine than average will be even more masculine than it originally was after transforming the face relative to male and female prototypes. In contrast, a hyper-masculine male face would be more feminine than it originally was after morphing towards an average male face. Further details of the methods and computer algorithms used to transform faces in shape and colour can be found in Rowland & Perrett (1995); details of texture transformation are given in Tiddeman et al. (2001).

Three different pairs of prototype faces were used to masculinize and feminize the individual male faces by altering their (i) sexual dimorphism, (ii) perceived masculinity or (iii) pubertal development. All prototypes were made symmetric by averaging the composite face with its miror-image, preventing the transformations from altering symmetry.

The sexual dimorphism method (figure 1a) used a composite of 20 male faces (mean age=19.5, s.d.=2.3) and a composite of 20 female faces (mean age=18.4, s.d.=0.7). The perceived masculinity method (figure 1b) used a composite of 20 male faces rated as particularly masculine (mean age=19.5, s.d.=3.1) and a composite of 20 male faces rated as particularly feminine (mean age=19.4, s.d.=3.6). These sets were also matched for age. Masculinity ratings were done by 20 independent raters (10 males). The pubertal development method (figure 1c) used a composite of 13 post-pubescent male faces (mean age=20.0, s.d.=2.6) and a composite of 13 pre-pubescent male faces (mean age=8.46, s.d.=0.52). All face images were taken in the same room under the same lighting.

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Pairs of faces transformed to be 50% more feminine (top row) or masculine (bottom row) using three different transformation endpoints: (a) adult female and male composites, (b) composites of male faces rated as particularly feminine or masculine and (c) boys and young men. See electronic supplementary material, appendix A for a high resolution colour version of this image.

(b) Stimulus calibration

Seventy-six women (mean age=26.4, s.d.=9.3) validated the faces’ perceived masculinity and 49 women (mean age=20.8, s.d.=5.4) showed that, as in previous research (Perrett et al. 1998), the faces differed in perceived dominance. Calibration was done using a two-alternative forced choice paradigm, where participants chose the more masculine or dominant from each pair of the masculinized and feminized versions of each identity. Since each of the six original face identities was manipulated in the same way for each method, participants were treated as the unit of analysis. For all three methods, the masculinized face was perceived as more masculine (one-sample t-tests: all t75>65.4, p<0.001) and more dominant (all t48>14.6, p<0.001) than the feminized face (see table 1). Three hundred twenty-four women (mean age=24.0, s.d.=7.3) were asked to choose the more attractive face from each pair. For all three methods, the masculinized face was perceived as the more attractive (all t323>10.8, p<0.001).

Table 1.

Mean frequency (per cent) with which the masculinized face was chosen as the more masculine, dominant or attractive face for three methods of manipulating masculinity.

method masculinity (n=76) dominance (n=49) attractiveness (n=324)
mean (%) s.d. (%) mean (%) s.d. (%) mean (%) s.d. (%)
sexual dimorphism 98.2 6.43 89.5 18.9 66.0 26.7
perceived masculinity 99.8 1.92 87.0 15.7 73.0 25.7
pubertal development 99.2 4.63 91.2 19.3 66.8 21.7

(c) Participants

Participants in the preference test were 124 women (a subset of the 324 women who completed the masculinity preference test reported previously) between the ages of 16 and 30 (mean age=21.9, s.d.=3.51), who reported a heterosexual preference and completed a questionnaire about partner preferences.

(d) Procedure

Facial masculinity preference was assessed using a two-alternative forced choice paradigm, where the masculinized and feminized versions of one face identity were presented on a computer screen at the same time and participants were asked to indicate which was the more attractive by clicking on the face. The six identities were shown in each of the three transformation methods, totaling 18 trials presented in randomized order. Participants also completed a questionnaire where they were asked to indicate their sex, age, their ideal partner's sex, age and masculinity. Participants were also asked whether or not they had a partner and, if so, their partner's sex, age and masculinity. Ideal partner and actual partner's masculinity were assessed using a seven-point Likert scale ranging from ‘much less masculine than average’ to ‘much more masculine than average’. The definition of masculinity was left open and could include masculinity in multiple domains (e.g. face, body, voice and behaviour). The participant was the unit of analysis for all analyses.

3. Results

One-sample t-tests (compared to 50%) demonstrated that women preferred the masculinized face more often than the feminized face when the faces were transformed using the sexual dimorphism method (mean=67%, t123=6.85, p<0.001), the perceived masculinity method (mean=74%,t123=10.03,p<0.001) and the pubertal development method (mean=67%,t123=9.16,p<0.001).

Facial masculinity preferences for all three methods were significantly correlated (all r>0.53,n=124,p<0.001), so we entered the data into a principal components analysis which revealed a single component (explaining 74.5% of variance). This factor was used in further analyses as an overall measure of facial masculinity preference (See table 2).

Table 2.

Correlations (Pearson's r) among masculinity preferences using the three methods of masculinity transformation: sexual dimorphism (SD), perceived masculinity (PM), pubertal development (PD) and their first principal component (PC). (Correlations are also shown between facial masculinity preferences assessed by the different transformation methods and ideal or partner's actual masculinity. *p<0.001, **p<0.005, ***p<0.03.)

method perceived masculinity pubertal development principal component ideal masculinity partner masculinity
SD 0.677* 0.634* 0.898* 0.361* 0.360**
PM 0.538* 0.856* 0.258** 0.140
PD 0.834* 0.201*** 0.273***
PC 0.319* 0.302***

(a) Ideal masculinity preference

Linear regression was used to assess the relationship between ideal masculinity preference (as assessed on a seven-point scale) and measured facial masculinity preference. The effects of participant age and their stated ideal partner's age were also assessed. The model was significant (F3,120=5.18,p=0.002) and the only significant predictor of facial masculinity preference was ideal masculinity preference (B=0.296, t=3.54, p=0.001). Participant's age (B=0.04,t=0.78,p=0.44) and ideal partner's age (B=0.01,t=0.20,p=0.84) did not significantly predict facial masculinity preference.

(b) Actual partner's masculinity

Linear regression was also used to assess the relationship between partner's perceived masculinity (as assessed on a seven-point scale) and measured facial masculinity preference for the 69 women with a male partner. The effects of participant's age and partner's age were also assessed. The model was not quite significant (F3,65=2.46,p=0.070) and, again, the only significant predictor of facial masculinity preference was partner's perceived masculinity (B=0.224,t=2.42,p=0.018). Participant's age (B=0.01,t=0.29,p=0.77) and partner's age (B=0.01,t=0.26,p=0.80) did not significantly predict facial masculinity preference.

4. Discussion

Preferences for facial masculinity were assessed using three different techniques for manipulating masculinity in face images and all three techniques were inter-related and produced consistent preferences. Techniques manipulating face shape (sexual dimorphism and pubertal development) produced the same results as the technique manipulating both face shape and colour (perceived masculinity). This suggests that the differences in general preference for masculinity in male faces found in various studies are more likely to result from individual differences among the participants than from differences in the techniques used to manipulate facial masculinity.

Although the construction of our stimuli was not identical to previous work on preferences for facial masculinity, we do show that stimuli made from masculinity continua based on male/female differences (sensu Perrett et al. 1998; Penton-Voak et al. 1999), pre-pubertal male/post-pubertal male differences (sensu Swaddle & Riersen 2002) and perceived masculine male/perceived feminine male differences (sensu Johnston et al. 2001) produce the same preferences in the same population.

Here we also demonstrated that preferences for facial masculinity are predicted by stated preference for masculinity in an ideal partner and also by an actual partner's rated masculinity. These relationships were not explained or qualified by the participant's own age or partner's age, providing evidence against the idea that masculinity preferences solely reflect preferences for maturity versus youth (e.g. Cunningham et al. 1990), at least gauged from stated preference for ideal partner age. The results of this study cannot be explained as the product of a general response bias (whereby a general tendency to use extreme or moderate points on a rating scale causes a false correlation), because facial masculinity preference was assessed using a forced-choice design.

We did not examine the sources of individual variation in preferences for masculinity, but we did examine whether or not preferences for facial masculinity are predictive of ratings of ideal and actual partner's masculinity. The causal direction of the relationship we found is not known. Women might have masculine partners because they prefer masculine men for any of various reasons such as own attractiveness (Little et al. 2001), oral contraceptive use (Penton-Voak et al. 1999; Little et al. 2002), or other sources of individual differences in face preferences not yet determined to influence masculinity preferences. Alternatively, women who demonstrate strong preferences for facial masculinity might do so because they have more experience with masculine men. Visual experience with faces of a certain type has been shown to increase normality perceptions of and preferences for similar faces (Rhodes et al. 2003b; Webster et al. 2004; Little et al. 2005).

Although it has been suggested that variability among studies in the general face preferences they report may partly be due to the use of different methods to manipulate masculinity in male face images (Johnston et al. 2001; Swaddle & Riersen 2002; Fink et al. 2005; Rhodes 2006), our findings do not support this proposal. Indeed, the directions of preferences for masculinity were the same for all three methods considered here. These preferences were positively inter-related and loaded onto a single factor when analysed using principal components analysis. Furthermore, that stated masculinity preference and actual partner masculinity were positively related to facial masculinity preferences (tested by each of the methods) supports the proposal that variability among women in their preferences for masculine male faces reflects attraction to an underlying quality that is potentially expressed in different ways (e.g. a masculine face shape, or a masculine face colour and texture). Although the pubertal development method necessarily also transformed facial age, the lack of a relationship between ideal/actual partner's age and facial masculinity preference or ideal/actual partner's masculinity suggests that variability among women in their preferences for masculine males does not primarily reflect individual differences in their preference for youth. Overall, our findings are important for the interpretation of findings from previous studies.

Supplementary Material

Example stimuli transformed in masculinity

Pairs of faces transformed to be 50% more feminine (top row) or masculine (bottom row) using three different transformation endpoints: (a) adult female and male composites, (b) composites of male faces rated as particularly feminine or masculine and (c) boys and young men.

rspb20053445s02.pdf (224.3KB, pdf)

References

  1. Boothroyd L, Burt D.M, Cornwell R.E, Jones B.C, Little A.C, Tiddeman B.P, Perret D.I. Facial masculinity is related to perceived age but not perceived health. Evol. Hum. Behav. 2005;26:417–431. 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2005.01.001 [Google Scholar]
  2. Cornwell R.E, Boothroyd L, Burt D.M, Feinberg D.R, Jones B.C, Little A.C, Pitman R, Whiten S, Perrett D.I. Concordant preferences for opposite-sex signals? Human pheromones and facial characteristics. Proc. R. Soc. B. 2004;271:635–640. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2003.2649. 10.1098/rspb.2003.2649 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Cunningham M.R, Barbee A.P, Pike C.L. What do women want? Facialmetric assessment of multiple motives in the perception of male physical attractiveness. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 1990;59:61–72. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.59.1.61. 10.1037/0022-3514.59.1.61 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Enlow D.H. Harcourt Brace; Philadelphia, PA: 1990. Facial growth. [Google Scholar]
  5. Feinberg D.R, Jones B.C, Law Smith M.J, Moore F.R, DeBruine L.M, Cornwell R.E, Hillier S.G, Perrett D.I. Menstrual cycle, trait estrogen level, and masculinity preferences in the human voice. Horm. Behav. 2006;49:215–222. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2005.07.004. 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2005.07.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Fink B, Grammer K, Mittroecker P, Gunz P, Schaefer K, Bookstein F.L, Manning J.T. Second to fourth digit ratio and face shape. Proc. R. Soc. B. 2005;272:1995–2001. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2005.3179. 10.1098/rspb.2005.3179 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Gangestad S.W, Simpson J.A. The evolution of human mating: trade-offs and strategic pluralism. Behav. Brain Sci. 2000;23:573–644. doi: 10.1017/s0140525x0000337x. 10.1017/S0140525X0000337X [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Gangestad S.W, Simpson J.A, Cousins A.J, Garver-Apgar C.E, Christensen P.N. Women's preferences for male behavioral displays change across the menstrual cycle. Psychol. Sci. 2004;15:203–207. doi: 10.1111/j.0956-7976.2004.01503010.x. 10.1111/j.0956-7976.2004.01503010.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Gray P.B, Kahlenberg S.M, Barrett E.S, Lipson S.F, Ellison P.T. Marriage and fatherhood are associated with lower testosterone in males. Evol. Hum. Behav. 2002;23:193–201. 10.1016/S1090-5138(01)00101-5 [Google Scholar]
  10. Grumbach M.M. Estrogen, bone, growth and sex: a sea change in conventional wisdom. J. Pediatr. Endocrinol. Metab. 2000;13:1439–1455. doi: 10.1515/jpem-2000-s619. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Johnston V.S, Hagel R, Franklin M, Fink B, Grammer K. Male facial attractiveness: evidence for a hormone-mediated adaptive design. Evol. Hum. Behav. 2001;22:251–267. 10.1016/S1090-5138(01)00066-6 [Google Scholar]
  12. Little A.C, Burt D.M, Penton-Voak I.S, Perrett D.I. Self-perceived attractiveness influences human female preferences for sexual dimorphism and symmetry in male faces. Proc. R. Soc. B. 2001;268:39–44. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2000.1327. 10.1098/rspb.2000.1327 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Little A.C, Jones B.C, Penton-Voak I.S, Burt D.M, Perrett D.I. Partnership status and the temporal context of relationships influence human female preferences for sexual dimorphism in male face shape. Proc. R. Soc. B. 2002;269:1095–1103. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2002.1984. 10.1098/rspb.2002.1984 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Little A.C, DeBruine L.M, Jones B.C. Sex-contingent face aftereffects suggest distinct neural populations code male and female faces. Proc. R. Soc. B. 2005;272:2283–2287. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2005.3220. 10.1098/rspb.2005.3220 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Mazur A, Booth A. Testosterone and dominance in men. Behav. Brain Sci. 1998;21:353–363. 10.1017/S0140525X98001228 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Meyer D.A, Quong M.W. The bio-logic of facial geometry. Nature. 1999;397:661–662. doi: 10.1038/17720. 10.1038/17720 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Penton-Voak I.S, Chen J.Y. High salivary testosterone is linked to masculine male facial appearance in humans. Evol. Hum. Behav. 2004;25:229–241. 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2004.04.003 [Google Scholar]
  18. Penton-Voak I.S, Perrett D.I. Consistency and individual differences in facial attractiveness judgements—an evolutionary perspective. Soc. Res. 2000;67:219–245. [Google Scholar]
  19. Penton-Voak I.S, Perrett D.I, Castles D.L, Kobayashi T, Burt D.M, Murray L.K, Minamisawa R. Menstrual cycle alters face preference. Nature. 1999;399:741–742. doi: 10.1038/21557. 10.1038/21557 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Penton-Voak I.S, Jones B.C, Little A.C, Baker S, Tiddeman B, Burt D.M, Perrett D.I. Symmetry, sexual dimorphism in facial proportions and male facial attractiveness. Proc. R. Soc. B. 2001;268:1617–1623. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2001.1703. 10.1098/rspb.2001.1703 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Penton-Voak I.S, Little A.C, Jones B.C, Burt D.M, Tiddeman B.P, Perrett D.I. Female condition influences preferences for sexual dimorphism in faces of male humans (Homo sapiens) J. Comp. Psychol. 2003;117:264–271. doi: 10.1037/0735-7036.117.3.264. 10.1037/0735-7036.117.3.264 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Perrett D.I, Lee K.J, Penton-Voak I, Rowland D, Yoshikawa S, Burt D.M, Henzi S.P, Castles D.L, Akamatsu S. Effects of sexual dimorphism on facial attractiveness. Nature. 1998;394:884–887. doi: 10.1038/29772. 10.1038/29772 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Puts D.A. Mating context and menstrual phase affect women's preferences for male voice pitch. Evol. Hum. Behav. 2005;26:388–397. 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2005.03.001 [Google Scholar]
  24. Rhodes G. The evolutionary psychology of facial beauty. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2006;57:199–226. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.57.102904.190208. 10.1146/annurev.psych.57.102904.190208 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Rhodes G, Chan J, Zebrowitz L.A, Simmons L.W. Does sexual dimorphism in human faces signal health? Proc. R. Soc. B. 2003a;270(Suppl. 1):S93–S95. doi: 10.1098/rsbl.2003.0023. 10.1098/rsbl.2003.0023 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Rhodes G, Jeffery L, Watson T, Nakayama C. Fitting the mind to the world: face adaptation and attractiveness aftereffects. Psychol. Sci. 2003;14:558–566. doi: 10.1046/j.0956-7976.2003.psci_1465.x. 10.1046/j.0956-7976.2003.psci_1465.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Rowland D, Perrett D.I. Manipulating facial appearance through shape and color. IEEE Comput. Graphics Appl. 1995;15:70–76. 10.1109/38.403830 [Google Scholar]
  28. Swaddle J.P, Riersen G.W. Testosterone increases perceived dominance but not attractiveness in human males. Proc. R. Soc. B. 2002;269:2285–2289. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2002.2165. 10.1098/rspb.2002.2165 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Tanner J.M. Open Books; London, UK: 1978. Foetus into man: physical growth from conception to maturity. [Google Scholar]
  30. Thornhill R, Gangestad S.W. Facial attractiveness. Trends Cogn. Sci. 1999;3:452–460. doi: 10.1016/s1364-6613(99)01403-5. 10.1016/S1364-6613(99)01403-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Tiddeman B.P, Perrett D.I, Burt D.M. Prototyping and transforming facial textures for perception research. IEEE Comput. Graphics Appl. Res. 2001;21:42–50. 10.1109/38.946630 [Google Scholar]
  32. Valenzano, D. R., Tartarelli, G., Mennucci, A. & Cellerino, A. 2004 Evidence for the existence of two alternative perceptions of male facial attractiveness. Paper presented at the Human Behavior and Evolution Society Meeting, Free University of Berlin, Germany, 21–25 July 2004.
  33. Webster M.A, Kaping D, Mizokami Y, Duhamel P. Adaptation to natural facial categories. Nature. 2004;428:557–561. doi: 10.1038/nature02420. 10.1038/nature02420 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Example stimuli transformed in masculinity

Pairs of faces transformed to be 50% more feminine (top row) or masculine (bottom row) using three different transformation endpoints: (a) adult female and male composites, (b) composites of male faces rated as particularly feminine or masculine and (c) boys and young men.

rspb20053445s02.pdf (224.3KB, pdf)

Articles from Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences are provided here courtesy of The Royal Society

RESOURCES