Abstract
The ability of food to establish and maintain response habits and conditioned preferences depends largely on the function of brain dopamine systems. While dopaminergic transmission in the nucleus accumbens appears sufficient for some forms of reward, the role of dopamine in food reward does not appear to be restricted to this region. Dopamine plays an important role in both the ability to energize feeding and to reinforce food-seeking behaviour; the role in energizing feeding is secondary to the prerequisite role in reinforcement. Dopaminergic activation is triggered by the auditory and visual as well as the tactile, olfactory, and gustatory stimuli of foods. While dopamine plays a central role in the feeding and food-seeking of normal animals, some food rewarded learning can be seen in genetically engineered dopamine-deficient mice.
Keywords: food, reward, reinforcement, dopamine
1. Introduction
While the cognitive and behavioural disturbance we call ‘hunger’ is innate, the appetites for specific foods are learned. Undifferiented hunger is controlled largely by fluctuations of peripheral and hypothalamic peptides (Saper et al. 2002; Horvath & Diano 2004) and thirst is controlled by fluctuation in vagal input (Kraly et al. 1975) triggered by hypovolemia (Fitzsimons 1961) and by dehydration of cells in the lateral preoptic area of the hypothalamus (Blass & Epstein 1971; Peck & Novin 1971), However, neither hunger nor thirst results in unconditioned goal-directed behaviour (Changizi et al. 2002). Chance encounters with the sweet (Pfaffmann 1960) or salty (Denton 1982) taste of preferred foods or with the oral cooling by ingested fluids (Mendelson & Chillag 1970; Freed & Mendelson 1974) are required before goal-directed behaviour results from the interaction of internal need states with the salience of environmental cues (Bindra 1972). The infant recognizes (Steiner et al. 2001) and can learn to seek out (Johanson & Hall 1979) sweet tastants, but the appetite for a specific food is controlled by the interaction of the hunger-associated peptide levels with the brain circuitry that codes the animal's reinforcement history with that food. Until it has received reinforcing feedback from various foods, the infant indiscriminately mouths both food and non-food objects. The monkey's appetite for a yellow banana depends on the prior learning of the association of the sight of the yellow banana skin with the sweet taste of the white banana meat (Wise 2004b) and with the post-ingestive consequences of the ingested fruit (Le Magnen 1959). Similarly, the vitamin-deficient rat does not innately know what foods contain the deficient vitamin. Rather, the vitamin-deficient rat progressively loses its food neophobia until, by sampling new foods randomly, it chances on and ingests a food with the missing vitamin (Rodgers & Rozin 1966; Rozin & Rodgers 1967; Rozin 1969). The specific preference for a particular substance is only established when the post-ingestional consequences of the repleting food stamp in or ‘reinforce’ the tendency to approach that food (Rozin & Kalat 1971). Similarly, food- or water-seeking behaviours develop only after the animal has had the paired experience of hunger with eating or thirst with drinking, respectively (Changizi et al. 2002).
Our understanding of the brain circuitry through which various rewards control behaviour began with the findings that rats would learn to work for the direct electrical stimulation of the brain (Olds & Milner 1954) or for the pharmacological stimulation of the brain by psychomotor stimulant drugs (Pickens & Harris 1968). The finding that the rewarding effects of brain stimulation (Liebman & Butcher 1974; Fouriezos & Wise 1976) and of psychomotor stimulants (Yokel & Wise 1975; de Wit & Wise 1977) was blocked or attenuated by dopamine antagonists first implicated brain dopamine in reward function. Similar attenuation of food reward by dopamine antagonists (Wise et al. 1978a,b) first implicated brain dopamine in the control of behaviour by natural rewards.
2. Importance of dopamine for non-food reinforcement
Dopamine antagonists impair learning (Wise & Schwartz 1981) and, by extinguishing them, previously learned (Wise et al. 1978a,b) instrumental responding for food. Several lines of study confirm that they do so by blunting reward function itself (Wise 1982, 2004a; Beninger 1983; Smith 1995) rather than, as has been suggested (Mason et al. 1980; Koob 1982; Tombaugh et al. 1982; Salamone 1986), by simply impairing performance capacity.
The earliest evidence that dopamine plays an important role in motivational function was that brain stimulation and psychomotor stimulants were simply ineffective as reinforcers in animals treated with response-sparing doses of dopamine antagonists. Intravenous amphetamine and cocaine failed to maintain responding when tested under the influence of dopamine antagonists, despite evidence of adequate response capacity. Indeed, in this case animals respond at higher than normal rates before ceasing to respond following pretreatment with dopamine antagonists (Yokel & Wise 1975, 1976; de Wit & Wise 1977; Ettenberg et al. 1982).
In the case of brain stimulation reward, responding is generally lower when animals are treated with dopamine antagonists; however, several conditions reveal that the low response rates are due to ineffectiveness of the reinforcer and not incapacitation of the animal. First, responding decreases progressively, both within sessions and across sessions, in animals pretreated with dopamine antagonists (Fouriezos & Wise 1976; Fouriezos et al. 1978; Franklin 1978; Franklin & McCoy 1979). When animals pretreated with moderate doses of dopamine antagonists are required to traverse an alleyway for access to the response lever that delivers the stimulation, performance is initially normal and deteriorates after several trials. Moreover, lever-pressing in the goal box deteriorates before running speed or latency to leave the start box; thus brain stimulation loses its ability to maintain responding in the goal box before the animals stop running to obtain it (Fouriezos et al. 1978). Second, after animals have stopped responding for brain stimulation reward under conditions of dopamine blockade, a reward-predicting environmental stimulus can, temporarily, reinstate normal performance; thus it appears that the decrement in responding results from the progressive loss of the expectancy of reward rather than from the immediate loss of response capacity (Fouriezos & Wise 1976; Franklin & McCoy 1979; Gallistel et al. 1982). Finally, when animals are tested in a ‘rate–frequency’ paradigm (an analogue of a dose–response paradigm), it is the stimulation frequency required to motivate the animal, rather than the maximum response rate that can be obtained from the animal, that is altered by dopamine blockade; animals can respond normally, but they require a higher stimulation ‘payoff’ if they are to keep doing so (Franklin 1978; Gallistel & Karras 1984). Finally, if dopamine-blocked rats are trained to earn rewarding brain stimulation in two ways (traverse a runway or press a lever) and are then tested in the two tasks sequentially, the animals initiate responding normally in the second task despite having ceased to respond normally in the first task; thus response capacity in the second task is unimpaired despite cessation of responding in the first task (Gallistel et al. 1982). Thus, while performance may be partially impaired by treatment with dopamine antagonists, data from several paradigms confirm that these drugs attenuate the ability of the stimulation to sustain normal performance before they interfere with the animal's capacity to generate such performance.
3. Importance of dopamine for food reinforcement
The concept of reinforcement is, at its core, a concept of how stimulus (Pavlov 1928) and response (Thorndike 1933) associations are formed and how they serve as the basis of habit acquisition (Skinner 1938). Food does not serve as a normal reinforcer in animals pretreated with dopamine antagonists; such treatment causes, for example, a dose-dependent decrease in how quickly animals learn to lever-press for food (Wise & Schwartz 1981). Under pretreatment with low doses of the dopamine antagonist animals eventually reach the normal performance asymptote; however, they require more trials to do so. With higher pretreatment doses learning is slower and may not reach the same performance asymptote. With yet higher doses there is no evidence of learning.
While the concept of reinforcement is most frequently used to explain response learning (Thorndike 1933; Skinner 1935; Hull 1937), it was first used in relation to stimulus learning (Pavlov 1928). Stimulus learning is now known to contribute significantly to response learning (Rescorla & Solomon 1967; Bindra 1972) and dopamine is thought to play a role in both (Wise 1989). Most studies of the reinforcing efficacy of food reward deal with the ability of the reward to maintain rather than to establish instrumental behaviour; without reinforcement both stimulus associations (Pavlov 1928) and response associations (Skinner 1933) extinguish.
When well-trained animals are tested under the influence of dopamine antagonists, food loses the ability to maintain normal responding. Whereas normal responding is initiated, responding slows progressively both within sessions and across sessions (Wise et al. 1978b; Dickinson et al. 2000). Similar progressive loss, both within and across trials, can be seen in the ability of food to maintain free feeding (Wise & Raptis 1986). The response slowing resembles what is seen in extinction conditions (when the normal reward is withheld), and is generally interpreted as a reflection of the impoverishment or ‘devaluation’ of food reward in the dopamine-impaired animal (Wise et al. 1978a,b; Xenakis & Sclafani 1981, 1982; Geary & Smith 1985). (See Salamone et al. (2005) for a dissenting opinion and Wise (2004a) for rebuttal.)
Few alternative hypotheses have been offered to explain the progressive response deficits seen when animals are tested under conditions of dopamine blockade. There is the suggestion that the progressive deficit might reflect a susceptibility to fatigue (or some other progressive within-trial performance impairment) caused by dopamine antagonists. This hypothesis can be ruled out from a variety of findings. First, the deficits are not only progressive within-trials; responding decreases progressively across repeated tests that are spaced days apart, with normal levels of responding between the days when the dopamine antagonist is given (Fouriezos et al. 1978; Wise et al. 1978b; Wise & Raptis 1986). Second, animals trained under intermittent dopamine blockade, like animals under intermittent reinforcement, respond more, not less, when tested for habit strength during extinction trials (Ettenberg & Camp 1986). There is no suggestion of any fatigue-like effect in this paradigm. Finally, when animals are trained in single daily trials to traverse a runway for food, dopamine blockade does not interfere with latency or running speed on the trial when the dopamine antagonist is given; rather, performance is impaired only on the following day, when the animals are free of the antagonist (McFarland & Ettenberg 1998). Fatigue due to dopamine blockade can explain neither the normal performance on the treatment day nor the slow performance on the day after treatment; here performance is impaired by the animal's memory of the previous day's experience and not by the pharmacological treatment itself.
Another suggested alternative for the progressive within-trial slowing of feeding and responding for food was that the slowing reflected the effects of enhanced satiety rather than the effects of blunted reward. This suggestion has been falsified in three ways. First, the same within-session progressive deficits are seen when dopamine-blocked animals are offered non-nutritive saccharin as when they are offered nutritive food reward; no such deficits are seen in control animals that do not receive the dopamine antagonist (Wise et al. 1978a). Second, within-session progressive deficits are seen when ingested sucrose is not absorbed but is, rather, drained through an open gastric fistula (Geary & Smith 1985). Third, the satiety hypothesis (like the fatigue hypothesis) cannot explain the fact that performance decreases across successive tests as a function of how much experience the animal has previously had with food in the dopamine-blocked condition (Wise et al. 1978b; Wise & Raptis 1986) and not as a function of experience with dopamine blockade in the absence of food (Wise et al. 1978b). Thus, it appears to be the memory of the food experience in the dopamine-blocked condition, not the experience of dopamine blockade itself, that determines the decline in responding between trials in the dopamine-blocked animal.
When animals are tested with a number of sucrose concentrations, dopamine-blocked animals respond to a given concentration as if it were weaker than normal (Xenakis & Sclafani 1981; Geary & Smith 1985; Bailey et al. 1986; Schneider et al. 1986, 1990). Thus, normal sucrose lick rates are shown in dopamine-blocked animals if the concentration of the sucrose solution is increased to 10% from the normal 5%. The devaluation of sucrose reward—the treatment of high concentrations as if they were lower—is seen either in animals pretreated with either D1- or D2-type dopamine receptor blockade (Schneider et al. 1986). Thus, like many dopamine-mediated behaviours (Clark & White 1987), performance for sweet reward appears to require co-activation of D1- and D2-type receptors.
The hypothesis that dopamine transmission is important for food reward implies that food reward elevates dopamine levels, as do, for example, some drug rewards (Hurd et al. 1989; Pettit & Justice 1989; Wise et al. 1995a,b; Ranaldi et al. 1999). Indeed, food reward (Hernandez & Hoebel 1988) and food reward-associated stimuli (Bassareo & Di Chiara 1999) do elevate dopamine levels in the nucleus accumbens. Indeed, just as μ and δ opiate agonists are rewarding in proportion to their ability to elevate dopamine levels (Devine et al. 1993; Devine & Wise 1994), so are different sucrose concentrations rewarding in proportion to their ability to elevate dopamine levels in the nucleus accumbens (Hajnal et al. 2004).
4. Recent issues
An important role for dopamine in reward function has been well established for many years, but several fine points continue to be discussed in the literature. Is dopamine absolutely necessary for reward? Is dopamine more important for the expectancy of reward before it is delivered or for the impact of reward after it is delivered? Is the dopamine in nucleus accumbens more important for reward than the dopamine in other brain regions? Some of these recent issues are best resolved by consideration of the early literature.
First, studies involving pharmacological blockade of dopamine receptors have suggested a necessary role for dopamine in the reward function (Wise & Rompré 1989; Wise 2004a). Recent studies with genetically engineered mice challenge this strong position. First, deletion of the tyrosine hydroxylase (TH) gene with rescue of noradrenergic function results in mice that are born superficially normal and eat and gain weight for 10–15 days, at which time their eyes normally open and they begin nibbling and foraging for solid food. Unless treated with L-DOPA, they then lose weight, usually dying by 4 weeks of age (Zhou & Palmiter 1995). If treated with L-DOPA, however, they are alert and active for about 8 h after their daily treatment, eating enough during this period to maintain themselves (Szczypka et al. 1999). Restoration of TH expression in the caudate nucleus but not in the nucleus accumbens is sufficient to restore normal feeding in the TH knockout animals (Szczypka et al. 2001).
If these dopamine-deficient mice are maintained by daily L-DOPA treatment but tested when showing Parkinsonian akinesia, 18 or 28 h after the previous L-DOPA maintenance injection, they show normal sucrose and saccharin preferences over water, and drink more of these solutions than water in single bottle tests even after any trace levels of residual dopamine are purged by treatment with the dopamine-releaser amphetamine (Cannon & Palmiter 2003). Such animals show between-session (but not within-session) learning in a water escape task (Denenberg et al. 2004). If aroused with caffeine, such animals can learn a side-preference (but not reverse it) for food reward in a T-maze (Robinson et al. 2005). These findings establish that, whatever its importance in normal animals, normal dopamine function is not an absolutely necessary condition for rudimentary instrumental learning.
Another recent issue is whether dopamine is important for the motivation to seek anticipated food or rather for the reinforcing effects of food once it has been earned and received (Berridge & Robinson 1998; Salamone & Correa 2002). Food rewards have both kinds of effect (Wise 1989, 2004b). The primary effect would appear to be the ability to reinforce learning, as evidenced by: (i) the fact that animals do not learn food-seeking responses when their dopamine systems are blocked (Wise & Schwartz 1981); (ii) the fact that the effects of finding a piece of food when the dopamine system is blocked are more evident on the day after the dopamine blockade than on the day of the dopamine blockade (McFarland & Ettenberg 1998); and (iii) the fact that food-seeking habits extinguish when animals are tested under dopamine blockade (Wise et al. 1978a,b).
However, secondary to its role in the reinforcement history of the animal, dopamine clearly does have a role in the motivation of reward-seeking behaviours. While food-seeking is thought to be initiated by hunger, it is food-predictive or ‘incentive–motivational’ environmental cues in the environment that release and guide the behaviour. The incentive–motivational salience of these stimuli depends upon the prior dopamine-dependent reinforcement of their association with the reward. Again, the McFarland & Ettenberg (1998) study clearly illustrates the point. Their trained animals left the start box promptly and ran the runway quickly except on the day after they obtained food in the goal box while under the influence of the dopamine antagonist haloperidol. Thus incentive–motivation—the process by which reward-predictive cues activate and motivate an animal—depends on a dopamine-dependent history of association (reinforcement) between the cues and the reward they predict.
In addition, a ‘priming’ presentation of a reward sample (rather than of a conditioned predictor) can arouse an animal and motivate it to seek more of the priming stimulus. Salted peanuts and potato chips are good examples of rewards that are particularly good at priming further reward-seeking. A taste of such rewards is particularly effective at renewing reward-seeking behaviours after they have been given-up because they are no longer rewarded (Skinner 1938). Administering dopamine agonists is among the most effective ways to reinstate extinguished reward-seeking (de Wit & Stewart 1981, 1983; Wise et al. 1990).
Questions about the role of dopamine in reward function have also arisen from evidence that dopamine seems unimportant for the facial responses to oral presentation of sweet tastants (Berridge et al. 1989; Pecina et al. 1997). If one was to assume correspondence between the facial expression of ‘liking’ food and the ability of that food to serve as a reinforcer, this finding would pose a challenge to the view that dopamine is important for the ability of food to stamp in stimulus and response associations. The assumption of correspondence between the facial expression of liking and the hedonic response to reinforcement is, however, open to serious question.
Initial studies of the orofacial responses of humans to various tastants identified unique reactions to sweet, sour, and bitter stimuli (Steiner 1973). In rodents, the reactions to sweet and bitter stimuli are clearly distinguishable and have been classified as ‘ingestive’ and ‘aversive’ fixed action patterns (a misnomer, as it is the stimulus, not the action pattern, that is aversive) (Berridge & Grill 1984). The rodent orofacial responses to sweet and bitter tastants are, essentially, the licking of lips associated with the acceptance of a fluid and the gagging and chin-rubbing associated with fluid rejection, respectively. Following Schneirla's (1959) argument that approach and withdrawal responses are the only objective terms applicable to all motivated behaviour in all animals, Berridge & Robinson (2003) have suggested that these oral fixed action patterns reflect the hedonic assessments—liking or disliking—of various tastants. What is not clear, however, is how well the fixed action patterns of ingestion and rejection, expressed in decorticate rats (Grill & Norgren 1978; Hall & Bryan 1981) and anencephalic children (Steiner 1973), correlate with the higher-level subjective hedonic responses to and objective reinforcement induced by various foods.
The recent arguments of Berridge & Robinson (1998) that dopamine is important for the wanting of rewards but not the liking of rewards is based on several assumptions about the relation of the taste reactivity test to generalized emotional states. First, there is the assumption that the brainstem reactions to taste stimuli determine the hedonic response to those stimuli (Berridge 2000). This assumption begs the question of how humans learn to like bitter tastants like coffee and broccoli. Second, there is the assertion that the liking of a tastant need not be conscious (Berridge & Robinson 1998; Berridge & Winkielman 2003); in this view, whether the subject likes or dislikes a tastant is more directly evident to an outside observer than to the subject itself. This may well be true, but the possibility raises the question of why subjective lay terms like ‘wanting’ and ‘liking’ should be substituted for the more traditional motivational labels ‘drive,’ ‘incentive–motivation’ and ‘reinforcement.’ Third, there is the implicit assumption that all rewards are pleasant; this assumption is falsified by the fact that animals can be trained to work for aversive footshock (Kelleher & Morse 1968) and that initial injections of heroin, while extremely habit-forming, are often reported to be aversive (Haertzen 1966). Finally, there is the assumption that the mechanism of wanting and liking of tastants can be generalized to other rewards such as sexual interactions and addictive drugs (Robinson & Berridge 2003).
In order to see the relevance of subjective wanting and liking for the behavioural control by the motivating and reinforcing effects of food reward, it is important to distinguish between the wanting and liking of an abstract concept such as ‘sweets’ and the wanting and liking of a specific food morsel that is currently available to the peripheral senses. While Berridge & Robinson (2003) hold that you can both want and like a given tastant (like chocolate fudge) at the same time, you cannot experience at the same time the wanting and liking of a given specific morsel of food. If you do not yet have the morsel you can want it without knowing for certain that you will like it; if you have it in your mouth you can like it but it is no longer available to want. Inasmuch as it is a real morsel and not an imagined category that controls behaviour at a given time, we can, for behavioural analysis, identify wanting with the state of mind of an animal prior to earning a given food morsel and identify liking as the state of mind once the reward has been earned and is being sensed. From this perspective, the animals in the McFarland & Ettenberg (1998) study discussed above want the food pellet before they have tasted it on the haloperidol treatment day. If there is a deficit in the wanting of food in these animals, it is a deficit in wanting the next pellet, the one offered on the day after the pellet eaten during haloperidol treatment. In as much as the animals ran normally on the haloperidol treatment day and failed to do so on the day following the haloperidol treatment, it seems an inescapable conclusion that they wanted the food despite dopaminergic dysfunction. This separation of food-seeking into discrete trials allows us to see that haloperidol treatment disrupts food-seeking only after the animal has had the opportunity to taste the food under the influence of the dopamine blocker. As discussed above, it would appear that the importance of dopamine for the wanting of food on a given day exposure results from the role dopamine played in the prior liking of food on earlier exposures (whether it be days, trials, morsels, or bites). It is the prior liking of (or reinforcement by) a foodstuff—a dopamine-dependent function—that establishes a subsequent craving for that foodstuff (Rozin & Kalat 1971).
Another challenge of the view that dopamine plays an important role in food reinforcement stems from the unwarranted assumption that all the reward-relevant dopamine functions occur in nucleus accumbens (Salamone et al. 2001). While a good deal of work implicates nucleus accumbens in the rewarding effects of psychomotor stimulants (Roberts et al. 1977; Ikemoto et al. 1997; Roberts et al. 1980), and while it is nucleus accumbens dopamine fluctuations, for the most part, that have been correlated with drug reward and food reward (Di Chiara & Imperato 1988; Hernandez & Hoebel 1988; Hurd et al. 1989; Pettit & Justice 1989; Wise et al. 1995a,b; Ranaldi et al. 1999; Bassareo & Di Chiara 1999), and while protein synthesis in nucleus accumbens impairs instrumental learning for food reward (Baldwin et al. 2002), it nonetheless remains the case that lesions of the dopamine projection to nucleus accumbens do not cause feeding deficits (Ungerstedt 1971; Ervin et al. 1977), while lesions of the dopamine projections to the dorsal striatum do (Ungerstedt 1971). Moreover, it is genetic restoration of dopamine function in the dorsal striatum, not in nucleus accumbens, that rescues feeding in dopamine-deficient knockout mice (Szczypka et al. 2001). While lesions of nucleus accumbens disrupt cocaine self-administration more than they disrupt heroin self-administration (Pettit et al. 1984), ventral tegmental lesions that damage nigrostriatal as well as mesolimbic fibres disrupt both behaviours (Bozarth & Wise 1986). While nucleus accumbens injections of opiates (Olds 1982; Goeders et al. 1984) or psychomotor stimulants (Hoebel et al. 1983; Carlezon et al. 1995; Carlezon & Wise 1996; Ikemoto et al. 1997) are rewarding, injections of cocaine into the medial prefrontal cortex (Goeders & Smith 1983, 1986) or olfactory tubercle (Ikemoto 2003), or injections of opiates into the ventral tegmental area (Bozarth & Wise 1981; Devine & Wise 1994; Zangen et al. 2002), are also rewarding. Thus, nucleus accumbens is not the exclusive seat of dopamine-dependent reward function, and nucleus accumbens lesions should not be expected to disrupt all rewards.
Another recent issue is whether activation of dopamine neurons represents reward, prediction of reward, or an error signal reflecting the difference between earned and expected reward. The issue arises from electrophysiological studies of Schultz and collaborators, suggesting that dopaminergic neurons respond to rewards as long as they are not fully predictable, but transfer to conditioned stimuli predicting reward once the predictive significance has been learned (Schultz 1986; Romo & Schultz 1990; Ljungberg et al. 1991, 1992; Schultz et al. 1993). It is naive, of course, to expect that dopamine would play a specialized role in only one of these functions.
Moreover, Schultz's (Schultz 2002) distinction between ‘reward-predicting’ and ‘rewarding’ stimuli in these studies merits closer analysis. First, the rewarding event is not consistently defined in the various studies of Schultz's monkeys. In these studies, the reward was sometimes identified with presentation of the rewarding object rather than with the oral contact with the reward or with the post-ingestional consequences of that reward that truly constitute the rewarding event. In some studies, the primary rewarding stimulus was a piece of apple presented in a cup at arm's reach; in others it was a drop of fruit juice presented in a spout near the animal's mouth. In the case of the juice, the rewarding event was assumed to be the delivery of the juice rather than the taste of the juice; presumably, in this case, latencies were short enough that the presentation of the reward and the tasting of the reward were almost concurrent. In the case of pieces of apple, however, dopamine responses were noted when the monkey touched the food and again when the monkey tasted the food. Here, the tactile rather than the taste contact was taken as the rewarding event. Whether receipt of reward was defined by the touch of the apple pieces or the delivery of the juice, dopamine neurons responded to what was designated as the rewarding event in early stages of training but to what was designated as the reward-predicting event (sound and sight of latch opening in the case of the apple reward and sight of illumination of a light cue in the case of the juice reward) and not, after a great deal of training (thousands of training trials) to the ‘reward’ itself.
Clarification of the influence of training in these studies comes from subsequent studies in which the effectiveness of the reward-predictive cues was varied. When visual or auditory cues, or both, predicted reward with 100% certainty, the responses of dopamine neurons shifted from the reward itself (as defined above) to the reward-predictive environmental stimuli (Schultz 1986; Romo & Schultz 1990; Ljungberg et al. 1991, 1992; Schultz et al. 1993; Fiorillo et al. 2003). In the case where presentation of the reward was predicted by both auditory and visual stimuli, the responses of dopamine neurons to the reward-predictor (latch-opening) was weakened when the visual stimulus (sight of the door) was occluded (Schultz 1986). When a visual stimulus predicted reward with 75, 50, or 25% probability, responsiveness to the visual stimulus decreased and responsiveness to the tactile or taste stimulus increased accordingly (Fiorillo et al. 2003). This finding suggests the need for a closer examination of the distinction between reward and reward-prediction (Wise 2004b). While food might be considered a ‘primary’ reward (Schultz 1986), food is identified by each of the five senses and it is only in the taste of sweet foods (and perhaps the taste of salt in the case of sodium deficiency; Quartermain et al. 1967) that a strong argument can be made that the sensory experience of food is innately rewarding (Steiner 1974; Hall & Bryan 1981) in the absence of learned association with its post-ingestive consequences (Le Magnen 1959; Rozin & Kalat 1971; Messier & White 1984; Sclafani 2004). To the degree that it is the post-ingestional effects of a food that is reinforcing, however, as is the case with the rewarding effects of minerals or vitamins for deficient animals, the sensory experience of a given foodstuff becomes a reinforcer in its own right: a conditioned reinforcer (Robbins 1978). Certainly, the touch of a piece of apple is a learned reinforcer for Schultz's very well trained monkeys (Schultz 1986), as, it would appear, are the click or sight of the opening door behind which food is to be found (Schultz 1986). Even the sweet taste of saccharin appears to be a learned reward (Messier & White 1984). Thus, Schultz's distinction between reward-predictors and primary reward is fuzzy distinction; the visual or auditory awareness of the availability of food, if it is a 100% predictor, is certainly as primary as the tactile awareness of that availability, and it is arguably as primary as the olfactory or gustatory awareness of foods that satisfy a mineral deficiency. The fact that dopamine neurons no longer fire in response to the taste of apple when that taste has been predicted by the feel of the apple or the click that predicts the feel is completely consistent with the fact that food-predicting stimuli can become conditioned reinforcers in their own right so long as dopamine function is not impaired during the association of the conditioned stimulus with the food (Beninger & Phillips 1980; Taylor & Robbins 1986).
This does not negate the fact that the phasic activation of dopamine neurons occurs in proportion to the discrepancy between the expected reward and the observed reward, or that such information participates in the learning associated with reinforcement. It should be noted, however, that the short-latency activation of dopamine neurons by visual stimuli occurs before the eye moves to fixate a peripheral visual stimulus. Thus dopamine neurons are likely to be only reporters of the discrimination made by the inferior colliculus between reward-predictive or otherwise salient stimuli and various stimulus events that bear no relation to rewarding events (Dommett et al. 2005).
5. Conclusions and perspectives
Brain dopamine plays several roles in the ability of food to serve as a reward. It is important—but apparently not completely necessary—for the reinforcement function of rewards, their ability to stamp in stimulus and response associations. Current evidence suggests that dopamine in the caudate nucleus may play a more important role than dopamine in the nucleus accumbens in the reinforcement of response habits (White & McDonald 2002; Wise 2004a). Past dopamine-dependent reinforcement of stimulus–reward associations is, in turn, important for the incentive–motivational energizing effect of reward-predictive cues in the environment. And, of course, some degree of basal dopamine is important for rudimentary behaviour of any kind (Hornykiewicz 1979; Stricker & Zigmond 1985). This does not mean that dopamine plays a specialized or exclusive role in reward function. Other neurotransmitter systems are certainly involved and it is not clear what subsets of dopamine neurons contribute to reward function. Reward function—and food reward in particular—is only one of the many functions in which dopamine plays an important contributing role.
Acknowledgements
Preparation of this review was supported by the Intramural Research Program of the NIH, National Institute on Drug Abuse.
Footnotes
One contribution of 16 to a Theme Issue ‘Appetite’.
References
- Bailey C.S, Hsiao S, King J.E. Hedonic reactivity to sucrose in rats: modification by pimozide. Physiol. Behav. 1986;38:447–452. doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(86)90409-9. doi:10.1016/0031-9384(86)90409-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Baldwin A.E, Sadeghian K, Holahan M.R, Kelley A.E. Appetitive instrumental learning is impaired by inhibition of cAMP-dependent protein kinase within the nucleus accumbens. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 2002;77:44–62. doi: 10.1006/nlme.2000.4002. doi:10.1006/nlme.2000.4002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bassareo V, Di Chiara G. Differential responsiveness of dopamine transmission to food-stimuli in nucleus accumbens shell/core compartments. Neuroscience. 1999;89:637–641. doi: 10.1016/s0306-4522(98)00583-1. doi:10.1016/S0306-4522(98)00583-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Beninger R.J. The role of dopamine in locomotor activity and learning. Brain Res. Rev. 1983;6:173–196. doi: 10.1016/0165-0173(83)90038-3. doi:10.1016/0165-0173(83)90038-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Beninger R.J, Phillips A.G. The effect of pimozide on the establishment of conditioned reinforcement. Psychopharmacology. 1980;68:147–153. doi: 10.1007/BF00432132. doi:10.1007/BF00432132 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Berridge K.C. Measuring hedonic impact in animals and infants: microstructure of affective taste reactivity patterns. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2000;24:173–198. doi: 10.1016/s0149-7634(99)00072-x. doi:10.1016/S0149-7634(99)00072-X [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Berridge K.C, Grill H.J. Isohedonic tastes support a two-dimensional hypothesis of palatability. Appetite. 1984;5:221–231. doi: 10.1016/s0195-6663(84)80017-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Berridge K.C, Robinson T.E. What is the role of dopamine in reward: hedonic impact, reward learning, or incentive salience? Brain Res. Rev. 1998;28:309–369. doi: 10.1016/s0165-0173(98)00019-8. doi:10.1016/S0165-0173(98)00019-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Berridge K.C, Robinson T.E. Parsing reward. Trends Neurosci. 2003;26:507–513. doi: 10.1016/S0166-2236(03)00233-9. doi:10.1016/S0166-2236(03)00233-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Berridge K.C, Winkielman P. What is an unconscious emotion? (The case for unconscious ‘liking’) Cognition Emotion. 2003;17:181–211. doi: 10.1080/02699930302289. doi:10.1080/02699930302289 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Berridge K.C, Venier I.L, Robinson T.E. Taste reactivity analysis of 6-hydroxydopamine-induced aphagia: implications for arousal and anhedonia hypotheses of dopamine function. Behav. Neurosci. 1989;103:36–45. doi: 10.1037//0735-7044.103.1.36. doi:10.1037/0735-7044.103.1.36 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bindra D. A unified account of classical conditioning and operant training. In: Black A.H, Prokasy WF W.F, editors. Classical conditioning II: current theory and research. Appleton-Century-Crofts; New York, NY: 1972. pp. 453–481. [Google Scholar]
- Blass E.M, Epstein A.N. A lateral preoptic osmosensitive zone for thirst in the rat. J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol. 1971;76:378–394. doi: 10.1037/h0031371. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bozarth M.A, Wise R.A. Intracranial self-administration of morphine into the ventral tegmental area in rats. Life Sci. 1981;28:551–555. doi: 10.1016/0024-3205(81)90148-x. doi:10.1016/0024-3205(81)90148-X [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bozarth M.A, Wise R.A. Involvement of the ventral tegmental dopamine system in opioid and psychomotor stimulant reinforcement. In: Harris L.S, editor. Problems of drug dependence 1985. U.S. Government Printing Office; Washington, D.C.: 1986. pp. 190–196. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cannon C.M, Palmiter R.D. Reward without dopamine. J. Neurosci. 2003;23:10 827–10 831. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-34-10827.2003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carlezon W.A, Jr, Devine D.P, Wise R.A. Habit-forming actions of nomifensine in nucleus accumbens. Psychopharmacology. 1995;122:194–197. doi: 10.1007/BF02246095. doi:10.1007/BF02246095 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carlezon W.A, Jr, Wise R.A. Rewarding actions of phencyclidine and related drugs in nucleus accumbens shell and frontal cortex. J. Neurosci. 1996;16:3112–3122. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.16-09-03112.1996. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Changizi M.A, McGehee R.M, Hall W.G. Evidence that appetitive responses for dehydration and food-deprivation are learned. Physiol. Behav. 2002;75:295–304. doi: 10.1016/s0031-9384(01)00660-6. doi:10.1016/S0031-9384(01)00660-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Clark D, White F.J. Review: D1 dopamine receptor—the search for a function: a critical evaluation of the D1/D2 dopamine receptor classification and its function implications. Synapse. 1987;1:347–388. doi: 10.1002/syn.890010408. doi:10.1002/syn.890010408 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- de Wit H, Wise R.A. Blockade of cocaine reinforcement in rats with the dopamine receptor blocker pimozide, but not with the noradrenergic blockers phentolamine or phenoxybenzamine. Can. J. Psychol. 1977;31:195–203. doi: 10.1037/h0081662. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- de Wit H, Stewart J. Reinstatement of cocaine-reinforced responding in the rat. Psychopharmacology. 1981;75:134–143. doi: 10.1007/BF00432175. doi:10.1007/BF00432175 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- de Wit H, Stewart J. Drug reinstatement of heroin-reinforced responding in the rat. Psychopharmacology. 1983;79:29–31. doi: 10.1007/BF00433012. doi:10.1007/BF00433012 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Denenberg V.H, Kim D.S, Palmiter R.D. The role of dopamine in learning, memory, and performance of a water escape task. Behav. Brain Res. 2004;148:73–78. doi: 10.1016/s0166-4328(03)00183-9. doi:10.1016/S0166-4328(03)00183-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Denton D. The hunger for salt. Springer-Verlag; Berlin, Germany: 1982. [Google Scholar]
- Devine D.P, Wise R.A. Self-administration of morphine, DAMGO, and DPDPE into the ventral tegmental area of rats. J. Neurosci. 1994;14:1978–1984. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.14-04-01978.1994. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Devine D.P, Leone P, Pocock D, Wise R.A. Differential involvement of ventral tegmental mu, delta and kappa opioid receptors in modulation of basal mesolimbic dopamine release: in vivo microdialysis studies. J. Pharmacol. Exp. Ther. 1993;266:1236–1246. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Di Chiara G, Imperato A. Drugs abused by humans preferentially increase synaptic dopamine concentrations in the mesolimbic system of freely moving rats. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci., USA. 1988;85:5274–5278. doi: 10.1073/pnas.85.14.5274. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dickinson A, Smith J, Mirenowicz J. Dissociation of Pavlovian and instrumental incentive learning under dopamine antagonists. Behav. Neurosci. 2003;114:468–483. doi: 10.1037//0735-7044.114.3.468. doi:10.1037/0735-7044.114.3.468 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dommett E, Coizet V, Blaha C.D, Martindale J, Lefebvre V, Walton N, Mayhew J.E, Overton P.G, Redgrave P. How visual stimuli activate dopaminergic neurons at short latency. Science. 2005;307:1476–1479. doi: 10.1126/science.1107026. doi:10.1126/science.1107026 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ervin G.N, Fink J.S, Young R.C, Smith G.P. Different behavioral responses to L-DOPA after anterolateral or posterolateral hypothalamic injections of 6-hydroxydopamine. Brain Res. 1977;132:507–520. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(77)90198-6. doi:10.1016/0006-8993(77)90198-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ettenberg A, Camp C.H. Haloperidol induces a partial reinforcement extinction effect in rats: implications for a dopamine involvement in food reward. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 1986;25:813–821. doi: 10.1016/0091-3057(86)90392-8. doi:10.1016/0091-3057(86)90392-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ettenberg A, Pettit H.O, Bloom F.E, Koob G.F. Heroin and cocaine intravenous self-administration in rats: mediation by separate neural systems. Psychopharmacology. 1982;78:204–209. doi: 10.1007/BF00428151. doi:10.1007/BF00428151 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fiorillo C.D, Tobler P.N, Schultz W. Discrete coding of reward probability and uncertainty by dopamine neurons. Science. 2003;299:1898–1902. doi: 10.1126/science.1077349. doi:10.1126/science.1077349 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fitzsimons J.T. Drinking by rats depleted of body fluid without increase in osmotic pressure. J. Physiol. 1961;159:297–309. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1961.sp006809. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fouriezos G, Wise R.A. Pimozide-induced extinction of intracranial self-stimulation: response patterns rule out motor or performance deficits. Brain Res. 1976;103:377–380. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(76)90809-x. doi:10.1016/0006-8993(76)90809-X [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fouriezos G, Hansson P, Wise R.A. Neuroleptic-induced attenuation of brain stimulation reward in rats. J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol. 1978;92:661–671. doi: 10.1037/h0077500. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Franklin K.B.J. Catecholamines and self-stimulation: reward and performance effects dissociated. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 1978;9:813–820. doi: 10.1016/0091-3057(78)90361-1. doi:10.1016/0091-3057(78)90361-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Franklin K.B.J, McCoy S.N. Pimozide-induced extinction in rats: stimulus control of responding rules out motor deficit. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 1979;11:71–75. doi: 10.1016/0091-3057(79)90299-5. doi:10.1016/0091-3057(79)90299-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Freed W.J, Mendelson J. Airlicking: thirsty rats prefer a warm dry airstream to a warm humid airstream. Physiol. Behav. 1974;12:557–561. doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(74)90203-0. doi:10.1016/0031-9384(74)90203-0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gallistel C.R, Karras D. Pimozide and amphetamine have opposing effects on the reward summation function. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 1984;2:73–77. doi: 10.1016/0091-3057(84)90104-7. doi:10.1016/0091-3057(84)90104-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gallistel C.R, Boytim M, Gomita Y, Klebanoff L. Does pimozide block the reinforcing effect of brain stimulation? Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 1982;17:769–781. doi: 10.1016/0091-3057(82)90360-4. doi:10.1016/0091-3057(82)90360-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Geary N, Smith G. Pimozide decreases the positive reinforcing effect of sham fed sucrose in the rat. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 1985;22:787–790. doi: 10.1016/0091-3057(85)90528-3. doi:10.1016/0091-3057(85)90528-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goeders N.E, Smith J.E. Cortical dopaminergic involvement in cocaine reinforcement. Science. 1983;221:773–775. doi: 10.1126/science.6879176. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goeders N.E, Smith J.E. Reinforcing properties of cocaine in the medial prefrontal cortex: primary action on presynaptic dopaminergic terminals. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 1986;25:191–199. doi: 10.1016/0091-3057(86)90252-2. doi:10.1016/0091-3057(86)90252-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goeders N.E, Lane J.D, Smith J.E. Self-administration of methionine enkephalin into the nucleus accumbens. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 1984;20:451–455. doi: 10.1016/0091-3057(84)90284-3. doi:10.1016/0091-3057(84)90284-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grill H.J, Norgren R. The taste reactivity test. II. Mimetic responses to gustatory stimuli in chronic thalamic and chronic decerebrate rats. Brain Res. 1978;143:281–297. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(78)90569-3. doi:10.1016/0006-8993(78)90569-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Haertzen C.A. Development of scales based on patterns of drug effects, using the Addiction Research Center Inventory. Psychol. Rep. 1966;18:163–194. doi: 10.2466/pr0.1966.18.1.163. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hajnal A, Smith G.P, Norgren R. Oral sucrose stimulation increases accumbens dopamine in the rat. Am. J. Physiol. Regul. Integr. Comp. Physiol. 2004;286:R31–R37. doi: 10.1152/ajpregu.00282.2003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hall W.G, Bryan T.E. The ontogeny of feeding in rats: IV. Taste development as measured by intake and behavioral responses to oral infusions of sucrose and quinine. J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol. 1981;95:240–251. doi: 10.1037/h0077771. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hernandez L, Hoebel B.G. Food reward and cocaine increase extracellular dopamine in the nucleus accumbens as measured by microdialysis. Life Sci. 1988;42:1705–1712. doi: 10.1016/0024-3205(88)90036-7. doi:10.1016/0024-3205(88)90036-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hoebel B.G, Monaco A.P, Hernandez L, Aulisi E.F, Stanley B.G, Lenard L. Self-injection of amphetamine directly into the brain. Psychopharmacology. 1983;81:158–163. doi: 10.1007/BF00429012. doi:10.1007/BF00429012 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hornykiewicz O. Brain dopamine in Parkinson's disease and other neurological disturbances. In: Horn A.S, Korf J, Westerink B.H.C, editors. The neurobiology of dopamine. Academic Press; New York, NY: 1979. pp. 633–653. [Google Scholar]
- Horvath T.L, Diano S. The floating blueprint of hypothalamic feeding circuits. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2004;5:662–667. doi: 10.1038/nrn1479. doi:10.1038/nrn1479 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hull C.L. Mind, mechanism, and adaptive behavior. Psychol. Rev. 1937;44:1–32. [Google Scholar]
- Hurd J.L, Weiss F, Koob G.F, Anden N.-E, Ungerstedt U. Cocaine reinforcement and extracellular dopamine overflow in rat nucleus accumbens: an in vivo microdialysis study. Brain Res. 1989;498:199–203. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(89)90422-8. doi:10.1016/0006-8993(89)90422-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ikemoto S. Involvement of the olfactory tubercle in cocaine reward: intracranial self-administration studies. J. Neurosci. 2003;23:9305–9511. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-28-09305.2003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ikemoto S, Glazier B.S, Murphy J.M, McBride W.J. Role of dopamine D1 and D2 receptors in the nucleus accumbens in mediating reward. J. Neurosci. 1997;17:8580–8587. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.17-21-08580.1997. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Johanson I.B, Hall W.G. Appetitive learning in 1-day-old rat pups. Science. 1979;205:419–421. doi: 10.1126/science.451612. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kelleher R.T, Morse W.H. Schedules using noxious stimuli. 3. Responding maintained with response produced electric shocks. J. Exp. Anal. Behav. 1968;11:819–838. doi: 10.1901/jeab.1968.11-819. doi:10.1901/jeab.1968.11-819 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Koob G.F. The dopamine anhedonia hypothesis: a pharmacological phrenology. Behav. Brain Sci. 1982;5:63–64. [Google Scholar]
- Kraly F.S, Gibbs J, Smith G.P. Disordered drinking after abdominal vagotomy in rats. Nat. Neurosci. 1975;258:226–228. doi: 10.1038/258226a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Le Magnen J. Effects des administrations post-prandiales de glucose sur l'établissement des appétits. C.R. Soc. Biol. 1959;153:212–215. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Liebman J.M, Butcher L.L. Comparative involvement of dopamine and noradrenaline in rate-free self-stimulation in substantia nigra, lateral hypothalamus, and mesencephalic central gray. N-S. Arch. Pharmacol. 1974;284:167–194. doi: 10.1007/BF00501121. doi:10.1007/BF00501121 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ljungberg T, Apicella P, Schultz W. Responses of monkey midbrain dopamine neurons during delayed alternation performance. Brain Res. 1991;567:337–341. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(91)90816-e. doi:10.1016/0006-8993(91)90816-E [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ljungberg T, Apicella P, Schultz W. Responses of monkey dopamine neurons during learning of behavioral reactions. J. Neurophysiol. 1992;67:145–163. doi: 10.1152/jn.1992.67.1.145. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mason S.T, Beninger R.J, Fibiger H.C, Phillips A.G. Pimozide-induced suppression of responding: evidence against a block of food reward. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 1980;12:917–923. doi: 10.1016/0091-3057(80)90453-0. doi:10.1016/0091-3057(80)90453-0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McFarland K, Ettenberg A. Haloperidol does not affect motivational processes in an operant runway model of food-seeking behavior. Behav. Neurosci. 1998;112:630–635. doi: 10.1037//0735-7044.112.3.630. doi:10.1037/0735-7044.112.3.630 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mendelson J, Chillag D. Tongue cooling: a new reward for thirsty rodents. Science. 1970;170:1418–1419. doi: 10.1126/science.170.3965.1418. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Messier C, White N.M. Contingent and non-contingent actions of sucrose and saccharin reinforcers: effects on taste preference and memory. Physiol. Behav. 1984;32:195–203. doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(84)90129-x. doi:10.1016/0031-9384(84)90129-X [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Olds M.E. Reinforcing effects of morphine in the nucleus accumbens. Brain Res. 1982;237:429–440. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(82)90454-1. doi:10.1016/0006-8993(82)90454-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Olds J, Milner P.M. Positive reinforcement produced by electrical stimulation of septal area and other regions of rat brain. J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol. 1954;47:419–427. doi: 10.1037/h0058775. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pavlov I.P. Lectures on conditioned reflexes. International Publishers; New York, NY: 1928. [Google Scholar]
- Pecina S, Berridge K.C, Parker L.A. Pimozide does not shift palatability: separation of anhedonia from sensorimotor suppression by taste reactivity. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 1997;58:801–811. doi: 10.1016/s0091-3057(97)00044-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Peck J.W, Novin D. Evidence that osmoreceptors mediating drinking in rabbits are in the lateral preoptic area. J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol. 1971;74:134–147. doi: 10.1037/h0030310. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pettit H.O, Justice J.B. Dopamine in the nucleus accumbens during cocaine self-administration as studied by in vivo microdialysis. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 1989;34:899–904. doi: 10.1016/0091-3057(89)90291-8. doi:10.1016/0091-3057(89)90291-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pettit H.O, Ettenberg A, Bloom F.E, Koob G.F. Destruction of dopamine in the nucleus accumbens selectively attenuates cocaine but not heroin self-administration in rats. Psychopharmacology. 1984;84:167–173. doi: 10.1007/BF00427441. doi:10.1007/BF00427441 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pfaffmann C. The pleasures of sensation. Psychol. Rev. 1960;67:253–268. doi: 10.1037/h0045838. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pickens R, Harris W.C. Self-administration of d-amphetamine by rats. Psychopharmacologia. 1968;12:158–163. doi: 10.1007/BF00401545. doi:10.1007/BF00401545 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Quartermain D, Miller N.E, Wolf G. Role of experience in relationship between sodium deficiency and rate of bar pressing for salt. J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol. 1967;63:417–420. doi: 10.1037/h0024611. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ranaldi R, Pocock D, Zereik R, Wise R.A. Dopamine fluctuations in the nucleus accumbens during maintenance, extinction, and reinstatement of intravenous D-amphetamine self- administration. J. Neurosci. 1999;19:4102–4109. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.19-10-04102.1999. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rescorla R.A, Solomon R.L. Two-process learning theory: relationships between Pavlovian conditioning and instrumental learning. Psychol. Rev. 1967;74:151–182. doi: 10.1037/h0024475. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Robbins T.W. The acquisition of responding with conditioned reinforcement: effects of pipradrol, methylphenidate, D-amphetamine and nomifensine. Psychopharmacology. 1978;58:78–87. doi: 10.1007/BF00426794. doi:10.1007/BF00426794 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Roberts D.C.S, Corcoran M.E, Fibiger H.C. On the role of ascending catecholaminergic systems in intravenous self-administration of cocaine. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 1977;6:615–620. doi: 10.1016/0091-3057(77)90084-3. doi:10.1016/0091-3057(77)90084-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Roberts D.C.S, Koob G.F, Klonoff P, Fibiger H.C. Extinction and recovery of cocaine self-administration following 6-OHDA lesions of the nucleus accumbens. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 1980;12:781–787. doi: 10.1016/0091-3057(80)90166-5. doi:10.1016/0091-3057(80)90166-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Robinson T.E, Berridge K.C. Addiction. Ann. Rev. Psychol. 2003;54:25–53. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.54.101601.145237. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.54.101601.145237 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Robinson S, Sandstrom S.M, Denenberg V.H, Palmiter R.D. Distinguishing whether dopamine regulates liking, wanting, and/or learning about rewards. Behav. Neurosci. 2005;119:5–15. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.119.1.5. doi:10.1037/0735-7044.119.1.5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rodgers W, Rozin P. Novel food preferences in thiamine-deficient rats. J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol. 1966;61:1–4. doi: 10.1037/h0022864. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Romo R, Schultz W. Dopamine neurons of the monkey midbrain: contingencies of responses to active touch during self-initiated arm movements. J. Neurophysiol. 1990;63:592–606. doi: 10.1152/jn.1990.63.3.592. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rozin P. Adaptive food sampling patterns in vitamin deficient rats. J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol. 1969;69:126–132. doi: 10.1037/h0027940. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rozin P, Kalat J.W. Specific hungers and poison avoidance as adaptive specializations of learning. Psychol. Rev. 1971;78:459–486. doi: 10.1037/h0031878. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rozin P, Rodgers W. Novel-diet preferences in vitamin-deficient rats and rats recovered from vitamin deficiency. J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol. 1967;63:421–428. doi: 10.1037/h0024614. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Salamone J.D. Different effects of haloperidol and extinction on instrumental behaviours. Psychopharmacology. 1986;88:18–23. doi: 10.1007/BF00310507. doi:10.1007/BF00310507 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Salamone J.D, Correa M. Motivational views of reinforcement: implications for understanding the behavioral functions of nucleus accumbens dopamine. Behav. Brain Res. 2002;137:3–25. doi: 10.1016/s0166-4328(02)00282-6. doi:10.1016/S0166-4328(02)00282-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Salamone J.D, Wisniecki A, Carlson B.B, Correa M. Nucleus accumbens dopamine depletions make animals highly sensitive to high fixed ratio requirements but do not impair primary food reinforcement. Neuroscience. 2001;105:863–870. doi: 10.1016/s0306-4522(01)00249-4. doi:10.1016/S0306-4522(01)00249-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Salamone J.D, Correa M, Mingote S.M, Weber S.M. Beyond the reward hypothesis: alternative functions of nucleus accumbens dopamine. Curr. Opin. Pharmacol. 2005;5:34–41. doi: 10.1016/j.coph.2004.09.004. doi:10.1016/j.coph.2004.09.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Saper C.B, Chou T.C, Elmquist J.K. The need to feed: homeostatic and hedonic control of eating. Neuron. 2002;36:199–211. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(02)00969-8. doi:10.1016/S0896-6273(02)00969-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schneider L.H, Gibbs J, Smith G.P. Selective D-1 or D-2 receptor antagonists inhibit sucrose sham feeding in rats. Appetite. 1986;7:294–295. doi: 10.1016/0361-9230(86)90231-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schneider L.H, Davis J.D, Watson C.A, Smith G.P. Similar effect of raclopride and reduced sucrose concentration on the microstructure of sucrose sham feeding. Eur. J. Pharmacol. 1990;186:61–70. doi: 10.1016/0014-2999(90)94060-b. doi:10.1016/0014-2999(90)94060-B [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schneirla T.C. An evolutionary and developmental theory of biphasic processes underlying approach and withdrawal. In: Jones M.R, editor. Nebraska Symposium on Motivation. University of Nebraska Press; Lincoln, NE: 1959. pp. 1–42. [Google Scholar]
- Schultz W. Responses of midbrain dopamine neurons to behavioral trigger stimuli in the monkey. J. Neurophysiol. 1986;56:1439–1461. doi: 10.1152/jn.1986.56.5.1439. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schultz W. Getting formal with dopamine and reward. Neuron. 2002;36:241–263. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(02)00967-4. doi:10.1016/S0896-6273(02)00967-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schultz W, Apicella P, Ljungberg T. Responses of monkey dopamine neurons to reward and conditioned stimuli during successive steps of learning a delayed response task. J. Neurosci. 1993;13:900–913. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.13-03-00900.1993. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sclafani A. Oral and postoral determinants of food reward. Physiol. Behav. 2004;81:773–779. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2004.04.031. doi:10.1016/j.physbeh.2004.04.031 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Skinner B.F. On the rate of extinction of a conditioned reflex. J. Gen. Psychol. 1933;8:114–129. [Google Scholar]
- Skinner B.F. Two types of conditioned reflex and a pseudotype. J. Gen. Psychol. 1935;12:66–77. [Google Scholar]
- Skinner B.F. The behavior of organisms. Appleton-Century-Crofts; New York, NY: 1938. [Google Scholar]
- Smith G.P. Dopamine and food reward. In: Morrison A, Fluharty S, editors. Progress in psychobiology and physiological psychology. Academic Press; New York, NY: 1995. pp. 83–144. [Google Scholar]
- Steiner J.E. The gustofacial response: observation on normal and anencephalic newborn infants. Symp. Oral Sens. Percept. 1973;4:254–278. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Steiner J.E. Innate, discriminative human facial expressions to taste and smell stimulation. Ann. NY Acad. Sci. 1974;237:229–233. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1974.tb49858.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Steiner J.E, Glaser D, Hawilo M.E, Berridge K.C. Comparative expression of hedonic impact: affective reactions to taste by human infants and other primates. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2001;25:53–74. doi: 10.1016/s0149-7634(00)00051-8. doi:10.1016/S0149-7634(00)00051-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stricker E.M, Zigmond M.J. Brain catecholamine and motivated behavior: specific or nonspecific contributions. In: Usdin E, Carlsson A, Dahlstrom A, Engel J, editors. Catecholamines: neuropharmacology and central nervous system theoretical aspects. Liss; New York, NY: 1985. pp. 259–269. [Google Scholar]
- Szczypka M.S, Rainey M.A, Kim D.S, Alaynick W.A, Marck B.T, Matsumoto A.M, Palmiter R.D. Feeding behavior in dopamine-deficient mice. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA. 1999;96:12 138–12 143. doi: 10.1073/pnas.96.21.12138. doi:10.1073/pnas.96.21.12138 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Szczypka M.S, Kwok K, Brot M.D, Marck B.T, Matsumoto A.M, Donahue B.A, Palmiter R.D. Dopamine production in the caudate putamen restores feeding in dopamine-deficient mice. Neuron. 2001;30:819–828. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(01)00319-1. doi:10.1016/S0896-6273(01)00319-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Taylor J.R, Robbins T.W. 6-hydroxydopamine lesions of the nucleus accumbens, but not of the caudate nucleus, attenuate enhanced responding with conditioned reinforcement produced by intra-accumbens amphetamine. Psychopharmacology. 1986;90:310–317. doi: 10.1007/BF00179197. doi:10.1007/BF00179197 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thorndike E.L. A theory of the action of the after-effects of a connection upon it. Psychol. Rev. 1933;40:434–439. [Google Scholar]
- Tombaugh T.N, Szostak C, Voorneveld P, Tombaugh J.W. Failure to obtain functional equivalence between dopamine receptor blockade and extinction: evidence supporting a sensory–motor conditioning hypothesis. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 1982;16:67–72. doi: 10.1016/0091-3057(82)90015-6. doi:10.1016/0091-3057(82)90015-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ungerstedt U. Adipsia and aphagia after 6-hydroxydopamine induced degeneration of the nigro-striatal dopamine system. Acta Physiol. Scand. Suppl. 1971;367:95–122. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-201x.1971.tb11001.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- White N.M, McDonald R.J. Multiple parallel memory systems in the brain of the rat. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 2002;774:125–184. doi: 10.1006/nlme.2001.4008. doi:10.1006/nlme.2001.4008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wise R.A. Neuroleptics and operant behavior: the anhedonia hypothesis. Behav. Brain Sci. 1982;5:39–87. [Google Scholar]
- Wise R.A. The brain and reward. In: Liebman J.M, Cooper S.J, editors. The neuropharmacological basis of reward. Oxford University Press; Oxford, UK: 1989. pp. 377–424. [Google Scholar]
- Wise R.A. Dopamine, learning and motivation. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2004a;5:483–494. doi: 10.1038/nrn1406. doi:10.1038/nrn1406 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wise R.A. Drive, incentive, and reinforcement: the antecedents and consequences of motivation. Nebr. Symp. Motiv. 2004b;50:159–195. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wise R.A, Raptis L. Effects of naloxone and pimozide on initiation and maintenance measures of free feeding. Brain Res. 1986;368:62–68. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(86)91042-5. doi:10.1016/0006-8993(86)91042-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wise R.A, Rompré P.-P. Brain dopamine and reward. Ann. Rev. Psychol. 1989;40:191–225. doi: 10.1146/annurev.ps.40.020189.001203. doi:10.1146/annurev.ps.40.020189.001203 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wise R.A, Schwartz H.V. Pimozide attenuates acquisition of lever pressing for food in rats. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 1981;15:655–656. doi: 10.1016/0091-3057(81)90225-2. doi:10.1016/0091-3057(81)90225-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wise R.A, Spindler J, Legault L. Major attenuation of food reward with performance sparing doses of pimozide in the rat. Can. J. Psychol. 1978a;32:77–85. doi: 10.1037/h0081678. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wise R.A, Spindler J, deWit H, Gerber G.J. Neuroleptic-induced ‘anhedonia’ in rats: pimozide blocks reward quality of food. Science. 1978b;201:262–264. doi: 10.1126/science.566469. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wise R.A, Murray A, Bozarth M.A. Bromocriptine self-administration and bromocriptine-reinstatement of cocaine-trained and heroin-trained lever pressing in rats. Psychopharmacology. 1990;100:355–360. doi: 10.1007/BF02244606. doi:10.1007/BF02244606 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wise R.A, Leone P, Rivest R, Leeb K. Elevations of nucleus accumbens dopamine and DOPAC levels during intravenous heroin self-administration. Synapse. 1995a;21:140–148. doi: 10.1002/syn.890210207. doi:10.1002/syn.890210207 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wise R.A, Newton P, Leeb K, Burnette B, Pocock P, Justice J.B. Fluctuations in nucleus accumbens dopamine concentration during intravenous cocaine self-administration in rats. Psychopharmacology. 1995;120:10–20. doi: 10.1007/BF02246140. doi:10.1007/BF02246140 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Xenakis S, Sclafani A. The effects of pimozide on the consumption of a palatable saccharin–glucose solution in the rat. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 1981;15:435–442. doi: 10.1016/0091-3057(81)90274-4. doi:10.1016/0091-3057(81)90274-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Xenakis S, Sclafani A. The dopaminergic mediation of a sweet reward in normal and VMH hyperphagic rats. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 1982;16:293–302. doi: 10.1016/0091-3057(82)90163-0. doi:10.1016/0091-3057(82)90163-0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yokel R.A, Wise R.A. Increased lever pressing for amphetamine after pimozide in rats: implications for a dopamine theory of reward. Science. 1975;187:547–549. doi: 10.1126/science.1114313. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yokel R.A, Wise R.A. Attenuation of intravenous amphetamine reinforcement by central dopamine blockade in rats. Psychopharmacology. 1976;48:311–318. doi: 10.1007/BF00496868. doi:10.1007/BF00496868 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zangen A, Ikemo S, Zadina J.E, Wise R.A. Rewarding and psychomotor stimulant effects of endomorphin-1: anterior–posterior differences within the ventral tegmental area and lack of effect in nucleus accumbens. J. Neurosci. 2002;22:7225–7233. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.22-16-07225.2002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zhou Q.-Y, Palmiter R.D. Dopamine-deficient mice are severely hypoactive, adipsic, and aphagic. Cell. 1995;83:1197–1209. doi: 10.1016/0092-8674(95)90145-0. doi:10.1016/0092-8674(95)90145-0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]