Skip to main content
Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences logoLink to Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences
. 1998 Mar 7;265(1394):427–431. doi: 10.1098/rspb.1998.0312

Competitive altruism: from reciprocity to the handicap principle

G Roberts
PMCID: PMC1688900

Abstract

Current work on cooperation is focused on the theory of reciprocal altruism. However, reciprocity is just one way of getting a return on an investment in altruism and is difficult to apply to many examples. Reciprocity theory addresses how animals respond dynamically to others so as to cooperate without being exploited. I discuss how introducing differences in individual generosity together with partner choice into models of reciprocity can lead to an escalation in altruistic behaviour. Individuals may compete for the most altruistic partners and non-altruists may become ostracized. I refer to this phenomenon as competitive altruism and propose that it can represent a move away from the dynamic responsiveness of reciprocity. Altruism may be rewarded in kind, but rewards may be indirectly accrued or may not involve the return of altruism at all, for example if altruists tend to be chosen as mates. This variety makes the idea of competitive altruism relevant to behaviours which cannot be explained by reciprocity. I consider whether altruism might act as a signal of quality, as proposed by the handicap principle. I suggest that altruistic acts could make particularly effective signals because of the inherent benefits to receivers. I consider how reciprocity and competitive altruism are related and how they may be distinguished.

Full Text

The Full Text of this article is available as a PDF (124.2 KB).

Selected References

These references are in PubMed. This may not be the complete list of references from this article.

  1. Axelrod R., Hamilton W. D. The evolution of cooperation. Science. 1981 Mar 27;211(4489):1390–1396. doi: 10.1126/science.7466396. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Boyd R., Richerson P. J. The evolution of reciprocity in sizable groups. J Theor Biol. 1988 Jun 7;132(3):337–356. doi: 10.1016/s0022-5193(88)80219-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Bull J. J., Rice W. R. Distinguishing mechanisms for the evolution of co-operation. J Theor Biol. 1991 Mar 7;149(1):63–74. doi: 10.1016/s0022-5193(05)80072-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Frean M. R. The prisoner's dilemma without synchrony. Proc Biol Sci. 1994 Jul 22;257(1348):75–79. doi: 10.1098/rspb.1994.0096. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Grafen A. Biological signals as handicaps. J Theor Biol. 1990 Jun 21;144(4):517–546. doi: 10.1016/s0022-5193(05)80088-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Heinsohn R., Packer C. Complex cooperative strategies in group-territorial African lions. Science. 1995 Sep 1;269(5228):1260–1262. doi: 10.1126/science.7652573. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Mesterton-gibbons M, Dugatkin LA. Cooperation and the Prisoner's Dilemma: towards testable models of mutualism versus reciprocity. Anim Behav. 1997 Sep;54(3):551–557. doi: 10.1006/anbe.1996.0455. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Milinski M. TIT FOR TAT in sticklebacks and the evolution of cooperation. 1987 Jan 29-Feb 4Nature. 325(6103):433–435. doi: 10.1038/325433a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Peck J. R. Friendship and the evolution of co-operation. J Theor Biol. 1993 May 21;162(2):195–228. doi: 10.1006/jtbi.1993.1083. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Zahavi A. Mate selection-a selection for a handicap. J Theor Biol. 1975 Sep;53(1):205–214. doi: 10.1016/0022-5193(75)90111-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences are provided here courtesy of The Royal Society

RESOURCES