Abstract
Regulators of G protein signaling (RGS) proteins accelerate the GTPase activity of Gα protein subunits in vitro, negatively regulating G protein-coupled receptor signaling. The physiological role of mammalian RGS proteins is largely unknown. The RGS family member rgs2 was cloned as an immediate early response gene up-regulated in T lymphocytes after activation. To investigate the role of RGS2 in vivo, we generated rgs2-deficient mice. We show that targeted mutation of rgs2 in mice leads to reduced T cell proliferation and IL-2 production, which translates in an impaired antiviral immunity in vivo. Interestingly, rgs2−/− mice also display increased anxiety responses and decreased male aggression in the absence of cognitive or motor deficits. RGS2 also controls synaptic development and basal electrical activity in hippocampal CA1 neurons. Thus, RGS2 plays an important role in T cell activation, synapse development in the hippocampus, and emotive behaviors.
G protein-coupled receptors (GPCR) control fundamental cellular processes ranging from embryogenesis and neurotransmitter release to cell survival, proliferation, and differentiation. Heterotrimeric G proteins are composed of α, β, and γ subunits that relay GPCR stimulation to downstream signaling pathways (1). On ligand binding, GPCR undergoes conformation changes of its intracellular loops that promote the exchange of GDP for GTP on Gα and the subsequent dissociation of Gα and Gβγ. Both GTP-bound Gα and free Gβγ propagate the signal through interactions with downstream effectors. Deactivation of GPCR signaling depends on the rate of Gα GTP hydrolysis.
Studies of GPCR desensitization have led to the discovery of a large and evolutionarily conserved family of GTP-activating (GAP) factors for Gα termed “regulators of G protein signaling” (RGS). RGS proteins share an RGS domain (≈120 amino acids) capable of accelerating Gα-dependent GTP hydrolysis and the conversion of active GTP-bound Gα to its inactive GDP-bound form (2–7). The first RGS identified was the yeast protein Sst2, isolated in a screen for mutants that failed to down-regulate the GPCR-mediated response to mating pheromone (8–9). Over 20 mammalian RGS proteins have been identified (7, 10). Recently it has been shown that RGS9–1 controls the photosensitization response in the mouse eye (11). Axin, which contains a RGS domain, negatively regulates the Wnt-signaling pathway, and axin-mutant mice display defects in embryonic axis formation (12). However, Axin has no detectable GAP activity for Gα (10). The physiological roles of other mammalian RGS proteins are yet to be established.
The RGS family member rgs2 was cloned as an early response gene up-regulated in activated T cells (13–14). RGS2 is also up-regulated in central nervous system (CNS) neurons after stimuli that evoke long-term neuronal plasticity (15–16). In vitro, RGS2 interacts with Gαq/11 and stimulates its GTPase activity (17–18), and RGS2 overexpression in cells inhibits Gαi- (15) and Gαs-dependent (19) signaling. To investigate the role of RGS2 in vivo, we generated rgs2-deficient mice. rgs2 deficiency in mice leads to impaired T cell activation in vitro and in vivo. Interestingly, mice lacking RGS2 also exhibit reduced male aggressive behavior and increased anxiety, whereas morphological and electrophysiological analyses indicate a role for RGS2 in hippocampal CA1 neuron function.
Materials and Methods
rgs2 Mutant Mice.
RGS2 exons 4 and 5 encoding the RGS domain were replaced with PGK-NeoR in antisense orientation. G418-resistant E14K embryonic stem (ES) cells were screened by PCR and Southern blot for homologous recombination. Targeted ES cell clones were injected into C57BL/6 blastocysts and heterozygous rgs-2+/− mice obtained by mating chimeras with C57BL/6J mice. PCR primers: wild-type allele (5′-CCGAGTTCTGTGAAGAAAACATTG3′; 5′GGGACTCCTGGTCTCATGTAGCAT3′) and mutant allele (5′-GCTAAAGCGCATGCTCCAGAC-3′; 5′-GGCCCACATTTACACGAACC-3′). All experiments were carried out by using littermate mice on a C57BL/6J background (F5 generation), and mice were maintained in accordance with institutional guidelines.
Immunocytometry.
Single-cell suspensions of thymi, lymph nodes, and spleens were stained with FITC-, phycoerythrin-, or biotin-conjugated Abs reactive to CD3ɛ, TCRαβ, CD4, CD8, CD25, CD44, CD28, CD69, CD11b (Mac-1), CD11c, B220, CD43, sIgM, sIgD, or Gr-1. Biotinylated antibodies were visualized by using streptavidin-RED670. Samples were analyzed by flow cytometry by using a FACScan (Becton Dickinson).
T and B Cell Assays.
For proliferation assays, freshly isolated T cells were activated with PMA (10 ng/ml, phorbol myristate acetate) plus Ca+2 ionophore (100 ng/ml, ionomycin), soluble anti-CD3ɛ (0.1–1 μg/ml, clone 145–2C11), anti-CD28 (0.02–0.2 μg/ml, clone 37.51), or recombinant mouse IL-2 (50 units/ml) in triplicate for 12, 24, 48, 72, or 120 h, followed by a 12-h pulse with [3H]thymidine (1 μ Ci/ml). Cytokine production was determined by ELISA. For B cell analysis, splenic B lymphocytes were purified (90–95% B220+ cells) by negative magnetic sorting and activated with various stimuli. Basal serum Ig levels (IgM, IgG1, IgG2a, IgG2b, IgG3, and IgA) were determined by ELISA (20). For lymphocytic choriomeningitis virus (LCMV) infections, mice were infected with LCMV (700 plaque-forming units, s.c.) into the hind footpad. Cellular infiltration was measured daily as described (21).
Behavior Studies.
Four- to five-month-old sex-matched littermate mice were examined for neuromotor functions (22): vibrissae, eyes, rearing/support/standing, muscle tone, tail hanging-induced turns and limbs/digits extension, ear reflex, eye blink and pupillary response to light, sweet/bitter tasting acceptance/rejection, and orientation toward sound. Circadian and open-field activities (i.e., cage crosses, total distance, vertical movements) were measured in cages fitted with infrared sensors. Exploration test was performed by putting the mice in the center of a white disk (183-cm diameter), with or without objects distributed in four quadrants. The mice were video tracked (seven trials). Fecal pellets and rears were counted. Motor coordination was analyzed by a rotating rod task. For a balance task, a mouse was held by the tail and allowed to grasp a suspended horizontal wooden rod (3 mm Ø) with its hind limbs, with recording of the time for the mouse to pull itself into balance and the number of falls.
Mice were studied in a spatial water maze for 7 days with a hidden platform in the southwest quadrant (22). On day 7, a final probe trial (60 s in the pool without platform) was applied. From day 8 to 16, tests were reassumed with the platform moved to the northeast quadrant (reversal, 2 trials per day). Time to find the platform and total distance were measured. In the T-maze test, on day 1 the mice were placed in an empty maze for 10 min with access only to its central and right arms. Several objects were placed as cues around the maze. On day 2, each subject was again placed in the T-maze and given access to the left arm of the maze. The mice were observed for 5 min, with recording of time spent in the new area and first choice. Step-down avoidance test was also carried out, in which the mice were required to remember that prior stepping off a safe platform resulted in a foot shock (1 s, 0.12 mA). Latency on the platform was recorded in each trial. Aggression was tested by using displacement from a plastic tube (30 cm long and 3.05 cm Ø). A rgs2−/− and littermate control mouse were released into either end of the tube, with the first mouse to exit being deemed the “loser,” that is, this mouse was deemed to be the less aggressive of the two. For light/dark preference tests, cages were divided into dark and light halves and the time spent by each mouse in each half measured.
Electrophysiology.
Electophysiology studies were carried out as described (23). Single hippocampal slices (400 μm) were superfused with artificial cerebrospinal fluid (2 ml/min; saturated with 95% O2 and 5% CO2 at 30°C). Field potentials were sampled at 10 kHz and analyzed with pclamp6 software (Axon Instruments, Foster City, CA). Field excitatory postsynaptic potentials (EPSP) were recorded with a micropipette (2–4 MΩ) placed in the stratum radiatum of CA1 area. Stimulation was delivered at Schaffer collateral. Test stimulus frequency was 0.1 Hz. The tetanic stimulation consisted of four trains of 100-Hz stimulation lasting 500 ms at 10-s intervals. EPSP slope was calculated from linear regression of the rising phase 10–65% of the peak response. The baseline value of EPSP slope was that from the first 5-min period and was defined as 100%. Input/output relationship was constructed by plotting the amplitude of presynaptic volley (input) vs. EPSP slope (output).
Results and Discussion
rgs2 Gene Targeting.
The rgs2 locus was inactivated in ES cells by replacing exons 4 and 5 corresponding to the RGS domain (Fig. 1A). Chimeric mice were bred to obtain germ-line transmission of the mutated rgs2 allele (Fig. 1 B and C). The rgs2 mutation proved to be null, as shown by Northern blot analyses of brain tissue (Fig. 1D). rgs2−/− mice were born at the expected Mendelian frequency, were fertile, and displayed normal growth. No anatomical or histological abnormalities were observed. Differentiation of all hematopoietic lineages, including neutrophils, macrophages, erythrocytes, and platelets, was normal in rgs2−/− mice.
Defective Proliferation and IL-2 Production by rgs2−/− T Cells.
Because RGS2 expression is induced in activated T cells after TCR engagement or stimulation by mitogens such as ConA or PMA plus Ca2+ ionophore (PMA + Iono) (ref. 14 and data not shown), we determined whether RGS2 plays a role in T cell activation. rgs2−/− mice displayed normal numbers and subsets of CD4+ and CD8+ T cells and B cells in lymphoid organs (Fig. 2A). The surface levels of TCRαβ, CD3, CD4, CD8, CD28, CD45, CD5, H2Kb, CD44, LFA1, CD25, and CD69 on both splenic and lymph node peripheral CD4+ and CD8+ T cells were comparable in rgs2+/− and rgs2−/− mice (not shown). To examine peripheral T cell function, T cells from the lymph nodes of rgs2+/− and rgs2−/− littermates were stimulated with anti-CD3ɛ mAb, anti-CD3ɛ plus anti-CD28 mAbs, or PMA plus Ca2+-ionophore. The proliferation in vitro of rgs2−/− T cells was significantly reduced in response to TCR engagement, a deficit that could not be overcome by CD28 costimulation (Fig. 2B). Consistent with the identification of RGS2 as an early response gene in activated T cells (13–14), the proliferative defect was most obvious at 24 h of stimulation. Induction and kinetics of apoptosis were comparable among rgs2+/− and rgs2−/− T cells (not shown). Thus, RGS2 acts as an immediate early gene downstream of TCR activation in T cells.
In addition to their proliferative defect, rgs2−/− T cells produced significantly lower levels of the T cell growth factor IL-2 compared with controls (Fig. 2C). IL-2 insufficiency per se does not explain the proliferative deficit because IL-2 supplementation did not restore proliferation to normal (Fig. 2B). The high-affinity IL-2 receptor α chain (CD25) and the activation markers CD69, CD44, ICAM1, and CTLA4 were up-regulated to a normal extent and with normal kinetics in rgs2−/− T cells in response to TCR engagement or mitogen treatment (not shown). The functional defect in T cell proliferation (Fig. 2B) and cytokine production (Fig. 2C) can also be observed in rgs2−/− T cells treated with PMA/Ca2+ ionophore, a stimulus that bypasses the proximal TCR signal. Consistent with this finding, the kinetics and extent of early TCR-mediated signaling events, including Ca2+ mobilization, overall tyrosyl phosphorylation, and activation of MAPK and SAPK/JNKs, were normal in rgs2−/− lymph node T cells (not shown), suggesting that their proximal TCR signaling pathways are intact.
With regards to B cells, rgs2−/− mice displayed normal numbers and differentiation (not shown). In vitro proliferation of rgs2−/− B cells in response to treatment with anti-IgM Ab, the F(ab′)2 fragment of the anti-IgM Ab, anti-CD40, or LPS was similar to that of rgs2+/− B cells (Fig. 2D and data not shown). Furthermore, baseline serum Ig levels were equivalent in rgs2−/− and rgs2+/− mice. Thus, RGS2 is not required for B cell activation. Our results show that antigen receptor-triggered RGS2 expression in T cells is required for proliferation and IL-2 production.
Chemokines are critical regulators of T cell function, signaling via GPCRs expressed on the surface of resting and activated T cells (24). It has also been shown that overexpression of RGS-family members in cell lines can modulate chemokine receptor signaling (18). However, no differences were observed between both resting and preactivated (6-h activation with anti-CD3ɛ before chemokine treatment) rgs2+/− and rgs2−/− CD4+ or CD8+ T cells in either proliferation or cell migration in response to the chemokines SDF-1α, RANTES, or MIP-1α (not shown). These results do not preclude regulation by RGS2 of T cell responsiveness to signals mediated by other chemokine receptors. Furthermore, rgs2+/− and rgs2−/− T lymphocytes showed comparable responses to the GPCR agonists lysophosphatidic acid, histamine, and adenosine (not shown). The GPCRs whose functions are under RGS2 control in T cells remain to be identified.
Impaired Immunity in rgs2−/− Mice.
To determine whether RGS2 plays a role in T cell responses in vivo, rgs2+/− and rgs2−/− mice were injected in the footpad with LCMV (21). rgs2+/− mice developed an effective LCMV-induced footpad swelling reaction starting from day 6 postinfection (Fig. 3). In contrast, the footpad swelling reaction was significantly decreased and delayed in rgs2−/− mice, indicating that RGS2 expression is required for an efficient antiviral response in vivo. Thus, the in vitro deficit in proliferation and IL-2 production by rgs2−/− T cells translates into an impaired anti-virus response in vivo, indicating that RGS2 expression is physiologically relevant for normal T cell function. These results provide the first genetic evidence that RGS proteins play a role in the activation of lymphocyte.
RGS2 Controls Male Aggression and Anxiety.
During our evaluation of the mutant mice, we noted unusual fighting between male rgs2+/− and rgs2−/− littermates that sometimes resulted in severe injuries. Stimuli triggering long-term plasticity in the CNS induce the up-regulation of RGS2 in the hippocampus, cortex, caudate putamen, striatum, and amygdala (15–16). Because these regions regulate a variety of behavioral pathways, we investigated whether RGS2 plays a role in CNS function. We first determined whether RGS2 deficiency in mice leads to alterations in motor functions. Both rgs2−/− and rgs2+/− littermate mice showed normal motor responses (tests listed in Materials and Methods). Circadian activity was normal in the mutant mice (Fig. 4A), as was exploratory behavior during two independent open field and exploration tests (not shown). rgs2−/− mice also exhibited appropriate performances in rotating rod (Fig. 4B) and balance tests (not shown), confirming the integrity of the neurological pathways responsible for motor coordination and balance. Spatial and conditional learning, as determined by the hidden platform, water maze, and passive avoidance tests, were comparable between rgs2−/− and rgs2+/− littermates. Long-term potentiation in the Schaffer collateral CA1 pathway of individual hippocampus slices was also equivalent in rgs2−/− and rgs2+/− mice (Fig. 4C). These data show that RGS2 deficiency has no apparent effect on motor responses, circadian activity, exploratory behavior, motor coordination, or spatial learning and memory.
Social dominance and territorial aggression are well-characterized behaviors in male mice (25). Intriguingly, rgs2−/− male mice exhibited significantly reduced aggressive behavior compared with their rgs2+/− littermates, whereas the aggressive behavior of rgs2−/− female mice was normal (Fig. 5A). Because aggression correlates with anxiety (25), we subjected rgs2−/− male mice to the light/dark preference test. rgs2−/− mice showed a greater preference for the dark compared with rgs2+/− control littermates (Fig. 5B), indicating increased anxiety. Consistent with this behavioral phenotype, rgs2−/− mice were found to drop significantly more fecal pellets than rgs2+/− littermates during the light/dark preference test (2.3 ± 0.6 vs. 0.8 ± 0.3 pellets) and exploration test (3.5 ± 0.6 vs. 1.3 ± 0.6 pellets). In addition, rgs2−/− mice exhibited an increased response to acoustic startling. These data show that RGS2 controls neuronal circuits for aggression and anxiety.
RGS2 Role in Synapse Development and Basal Electrical Activity of Hippocampal CA1 Neurons.
Fear and anxiety usually involve the amygdala, and certain components of fear can be affected by hippocampal inputs into the amygdala (26). Neurons from the CA1 region of the hippocampus project to the amygdala and removal of the hippocampus affects fear-based behavior (27). Moreover, it has also been proposed that RGS2 can regulate the activity of the G protein-coupled metabotropic glutamate receptor 1, a receptor implicated in long-term synaptic plasticity in the hippocampus (28). Detailed examination of rgs2−/− mice revealed that the number of neurons in the hippocampus (Fig. 6 A–D), in other brain nuclei, the brainstem, cerebellum, and cortex (not shown) were not affected by RGS2 deficiency. Interestingly, whereas dendritic morphology and branching were comparable among cortical neurons of rgs2+/− and rgs2−/− mice, hippocampal CA1 neurons of rgs2−/− mice showed a significant decrease in the density of apical and basilar spines compared with those of rgs2+/− control littermates (Fig. 6E). The number of spines correlates with synapse numbers and neuronal plasticity (29). Importantly, the morphological deficit in spine numbers of rgs2−/− CA1 neurons correlated with significantly reduced electrical input/output relationship in these cells (Fig. 7 A and B). These data show that RGS2 has a role in synaptic development and basal electrical activity of hippocampal CA1 neurons.
Alterations in aggressive behavior and fear responses are important manifestations in highly prevalent psychiatric disorders such as depression or addictive behaviors. Because these disorders are responsible for a large social-economic burden, it is critical to identify gene products potentially involved in the initiation and/or progression of these behavioral “traits” into clinically manifest “disease.” Multiple G protein-coupled receptors can regulate these behaviors, and pharmacological drugs that modulate these receptors have been developed to mitigate anxiety disorders and aggression. Our study provides genetic evidence that loss of a molecular regulators of G-protein signaling, RGS2, leads to impaired synaptic development in the hippocampus, reduced male aggressive behavior, and increased fear. Thus, RGS-family member such as RGS2 could be involved in the genetic predisposition to human neuropsychiatric disorders and could be potential targets for the generation of novel neuropharmacological drugs. rgs2−/− mice also provide a useful animal model to elucidate molecular mechanisms of fear and male aggression.
G protein-coupled receptors regulate fundamental processes of development, cell activation, and behavior. The present study shows that RGS2, a member of the RGS family, has important roles in T cell proliferation and IL-2 production. The T cell activation deficit observed in vitro translates into an impaired anti-virus response in vivo, indicating that RGS2 is physiologically relevant for normal T cell function and the establishment of an effective antiviral immune response. In addition, mice lacking RGS2 exhibit significantly reduced aggressive behavior and increased anxiety in the absence of any measured cognitive and motor deficits. Furthermore, morphological and electrophysiological analyses indicate that RGS2 expression is required for the generation of synaptic plasticity in hippocampal CA1 neurons. Thus, RGS2 is a critical regulator of T cell activation, neuronal function, and emotive fear, and aggression in vivo.
Acknowledgments
We are most grateful to K. Bachmaier, Y.-Y. Kong, and I. Kozieradzki for help and to M. Saunders for editing of the manuscript. J.M.P. and J.R. are supported by grants from the Medical Research Council of Canada. D.P.S. is supported by National Institutes of Health Grant AA11605.
Abbreviations
- GPCR
G protein-coupled receptors
- GAP
GTP-activating
- RGS
regulators of G protein signaling
- CNS
central nervous system
- ES
embryonic stem
- LCMV
lymphocytic choriomeningitis virus
- EPSP
excitatory postsynaptic potentials
Footnotes
Article published online before print: Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA, 10.1073/pnas.220414397.
Article and publication date are at www.pnas.org/cgi/doi/10.1073/pnas.220414397
References
- 1.Gilman A G. Annu Rev Biochem. 1987;56:615–649. doi: 10.1146/annurev.bi.56.070187.003151. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Berman D M, Wilkie T M, Gilman A G. Cell. 1996;86:445–452. doi: 10.1016/s0092-8674(00)80117-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Watson N, Linder M E, Druey K M, Kehrl J H, Blumer K J. Nature (London) 1996;383:172–175. doi: 10.1038/383172a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Hunt T W, Fields T A, Casey P J, Peralta E G. Nature (London) 1996;383:175–177. doi: 10.1038/383175a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Snow B E, Hall R A, Krumins A M, Brothers G M, Bouchard D, Brothers C A, Chung S, Mangion J, Gilman A G, et al. J Biol Chem. 1998;273:17749–17755. doi: 10.1074/jbc.273.28.17749. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Koelle M R, Horvitz H R. Cell. 1996;84:115–125. doi: 10.1016/s0092-8674(00)80998-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Berman D M, Gilman A G. J Biol Chem. 1998;273:1269–1272. doi: 10.1074/jbc.273.3.1269. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Chan R K, Otte C A. Mol Cell Biol. 1982;2:11–20. doi: 10.1128/mcb.2.1.11. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Dohlman H G, Song J, Ma D, Courchesne W E, Thorner J. Mol Cell Biol. 1996;16:5194–5209. doi: 10.1128/mcb.16.9.5194. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Siderovski D P, Strockbine B, Behe C I. Crit Rev Biochem Mol Biol. 1999;34:215–251. doi: 10.1080/10409239991209273. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Chen C K, Burns M E, He W, Wensel T G, Baylor D A, Simon M I. Nature (London) 2000;403:557–560. doi: 10.1038/35000601. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Zeng L, Fagotto F, Zhang T, Hsu W, Vasicek T J, Perry W L, 3rd, Lee J J, Gumbiner B M, Costantini F. Cell. 1997;90:181–192. doi: 10.1016/s0092-8674(00)80324-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Siderovski D P, Heximer S P, Forsdyke D R. DNA Cell Biol. 1994;13:125–147. doi: 10.1089/dna.1994.13.125. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Heximer S P, Cristillo A D, Forsdyke D R. DNA Cell Biol. 1997;16:589–598. doi: 10.1089/dna.1997.16.589. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Ingi T, Krumins A M, Chidiac P, Brothers G M, Chung S, Snow B E, Barnes C A, Lanahan A A, Siderovski D P, Ross E M, et al. J Neurosci. 1998;18:7178–7188. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.18-18-07178.1998. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Burchett S A, Volk M L, Bannon M J, Granneman J G. J Neurochem. 1998;70:2216–2219. doi: 10.1046/j.1471-4159.1998.70052216.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Heximer S P, Watson N, Linder M E, Blumer K J, Hepler J R. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 1997;94:14389–14393. doi: 10.1073/pnas.94.26.14389. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Beadling C, Druey K, Richter G, Kehrl J, Smith K A. J Immunol. 1999;162:2677–2682. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Tseng C-C, Zhang X Y. Endocrinology. 1998;139:4470–4475. doi: 10.1210/endo.139.11.6282. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Liu Q, Oliveira-Dos-Santos A J, Mariathasan S, Bouchard D, Jones J, Sarao R, Kozieradzki I, Ohashi P, Penninger J M, Dumont D. J Exp Med. 1998;188:1333–1342. doi: 10.1084/jem.188.7.1333. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Penninger J M, Fischer K D, Sasaki T, Kozieradzki I, Le J, Tedford K, Bachmaier K, Ohashi P S, Bachmann M F. Eur J Immunol. 1999;29:1709–1718. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1521-4141(199905)29:05<1709::AID-IMMU1709>3.0.CO;2-O. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Whishaw I Q, Haun F, Kolb B. In: Modern Techniques in Neuroscience. Windhorst U, Johansson H, editors. New York: Springer; 1999. [Google Scholar]
- 23.Lu Y M, Roder J C, Davidow J, Salter M W. Science. 1998;279:1363–1367. doi: 10.1126/science.279.5355.1363. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Kehrl J H. Immunity. 1998;8:1–10. doi: 10.1016/s1074-7613(00)80453-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Parmigiani S, Ferrari P F, Palanza P. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 1998;23:143–153. doi: 10.1016/s0149-7634(98)00016-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Rogan M T, LeDoux J E. Cell. 1996;85:469–475. doi: 10.1016/s0092-8674(00)81247-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.White N M, McDonald R J. Behav Brain Res. 1993;55:269–281. doi: 10.1016/0166-4328(93)90122-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Kammermeier P J, Ikeda S R. Neuron. 1999;22:819–829. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(00)80740-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Moser M B. Cell Mol Life Sci. 1999;55:593–600. doi: 10.1007/s000180050317. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]