Skip to main content
Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry logoLink to Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry
. 1999 Apr;66(4):502–509. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.66.4.502

Impairment of willed actions and use of advance information for movement preparation in schizophrenia

R Fuller 1, M Jahanshahi 1
PMCID: PMC1736300  PMID: 10201424

Abstract

OBJECTIVES—To assess willed actions in patients with schizophrenia using reaction time (RT) tasks that differ in the degree to which they involve volitionally controlled versus stimulus driven responses.
METHODS—Ten patients diagnosed with schizophrenia and 13 normal controls of comparable age were tested. Subjects performed a visual simple RT (SRT), an uncued four choice reaction time (CRT), and a fully cued four choice RT task. A stimulus 1(S1)−stimulus 2(S2) paradigm was used. The warning signal/precue (S1) preceded the imperative stimulus (S2) by either 0 (no warning signal or precue) 200, 800, 1600, or 3200ms.
RESULTS—The patients with schizophrenia had significantly slower RTs and movement times than normal subjects across all RT tasks. The unwarned SRT trials were significantly faster than the uncued CRT trials for both groups. For both groups, fully cued CRTs were significantly faster than the uncued CRTs. However, the S1−S2 interval had a differential effect on CRTs in the two groups. For the normal subjects fully cued CRTs and SRTs were equivalent when S1-S2 intervals were 800 ms or longer. A similar pattern of effects was not seen in the patients with schizophrenia, for whom the fully cued CRT were unexpectedly equivalent to SRT for the 200 ms interval and expectedly for the 1600 ms S1-S2 interval, but not the 3200 or 800 ms intervals.
CONCLUSIONS—Patients with schizophrenia were able to use advance information inherent in SRT or provided by the precue in fully cued CRT to speed up RT relative to uncued CRT. However, in the latter task, in which the volitional demands of preprogramming are higher since a different response has to be prepared on each trial, patients showed some unusual and inconsistent interval effects suggesting instability of attentional set. It is possible that future studies using RT tasks with higher volitional demands in patients with predominance of negative signs may disclose greater deficits in willed action in schizophrenia.



Full Text

The Full Text of this article is available as a PDF (140.0 KB).

Selected References

These references are in PubMed. This may not be the complete list of references from this article.

  1. Carnahan H., Chua R., Elliott D., Velamoor V. R., Carnahan C. J. Effects of schizophrenia and prefrontal leukotomy on movement preparation and generation. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 1994 Apr;16(2):253–260. doi: 10.1080/01688639408402636. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Crow T. J. Molecular pathology of schizophrenia: more than one disease process? Br Med J. 1980 Jan 12;280(6207):66–68. doi: 10.1136/bmj.280.6207.66. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Elkins I. J., Cromwell R. L. Priming effects in schizophrenia: associative interference and facilitation as a function of visual context. J Abnorm Psychol. 1994 Nov;103(4):791–800. doi: 10.1037//0021-843x.103.4.791. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Folstein M. F., Folstein S. E., McHugh P. R. "Mini-mental state". A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J Psychiatr Res. 1975 Nov;12(3):189–198. doi: 10.1016/0022-3956(75)90026-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Frith C. D., Done D. J. Routes to action in reaction time tasks. Psychol Res. 1986;48(3):169–177. doi: 10.1007/BF00309165. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. HEILIZER F. The effects of chlorpromazine upon psychomotor and psychiatric behavior of chronic schizophrenic patients. J Nerv Ment Dis. 1959 Apr;128(4):358–364. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Held J. M., Cromwell R. L., Frank E. T., Jr, Fann W. E. Effect of phenothiazines on reaction time in schizophrenics. J Psychiatr Res. 1970 Feb;7(3):209–213. doi: 10.1016/0022-3956(70)90008-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Hemsley D. R. Attention and information processing in schizophrenia. Br J Soc Clin Psychol. 1976 Jun;15(2):199–209. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8260.1976.tb00025.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Jahanshahi M., Brown R. G., Marsden C. D. Simple and choice reaction time and the use of advance information for motor preparation in Parkinson's disease. Brain. 1992 Apr;115(Pt 2):539–564. doi: 10.1093/brain/115.2.539. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Krawiecka M., Goldberg D., Vaughan M. A standardized psychiatric assessment scale for rating chronic psychotic patients. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 1977 Apr;55(4):299–308. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.1977.tb00174.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Mannuzza S., Kietzman M. L., Berenhaus I. J., Ramsey P. H., Zubin J., Sutton S. The modality shift effect in schizophrenia: fact or artifact? Biol Psychiatry. 1984 Sep;19(9):1317–1331. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Marshall W. L. Cognitive functioning in schizophrenia. I. Stimulus analysing and response selection processes. Br J Psychiatry. 1973 Oct;123(575):413–423. doi: 10.1192/bjp.123.4.413. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Nestor P. G., Faux S. F., McCarley R. W., Penhune V., Shenton M. E., Pollak S., Sands S. F. Attentional cues in chronic schizophrenia: abnormal disengagement of attention. J Abnorm Psychol. 1992 Nov;101(4):682–689. doi: 10.1037//0021-843x.101.4.682. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Nuechterlein K. H. Reaction time and attention in schizophrenia: a critical evaluation of the data and theories. Schizophr Bull. 1977;3(3):373–428. doi: 10.1093/schbul/3.3.373. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. PEARL D. Phenothiazine effects in chronic schizophrenia. J Clin Psychol. 1962 Jan;18:86–89. doi: 10.1002/1097-4679(196201)18:1<86::aid-jclp2270180128>3.0.co;2-h. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Pfefferbaum A., Ford J. M., White P. M., Roth W. T. P3 in schizophrenia is affected by stimulus modality, response requirements, medication status, and negative symptoms. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1989 Nov;46(11):1035–1044. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1989.01810110077011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Posner M. I., Early T. S., Reiman E., Pardo P. J., Dhawan M. Asymmetries in hemispheric control of attention in schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1988 Sep;45(9):814–821. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1988.01800330038004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Pugh L. A. Response time and electrodermal measures in chronic schizophrenia: the effects of chlorpromazine. J Nerv Ment Dis. 1968 Jan;146(1):62–70. doi: 10.1097/00005053-196801000-00007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Rosenbaum D. A. Human movement initiation: specification of arm, direction, and extent. J Exp Psychol Gen. 1980 Dec;109(4):444–474. doi: 10.1037//0096-3445.109.4.444. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. SHAKOW D. Segmental set. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1962 Jan;6:1–17. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1962.01710190003001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Schwartz F., Carr A. C., Munich R. L., Glauber S., Lesser B., Murray J. Reaction time impairment in schizophrenia and affective illness: the role of attention. Biol Psychiatry. 1989 Mar 1;25(5):540–548. doi: 10.1016/0006-3223(89)90214-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Slade P. D. Rate of information processing in a schizophrenic and a control group: the effect of increasing task complexity. Br J Soc Clin Psychol. 1971 Jun;10(2):152–159. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8260.1971.tb00728.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. VENABLES P. H. Stimulus complexity as a determinant of the reaction time of schizophrenics. Can J Psychol. 1958 Sep;12(3):187–190. doi: 10.1037/h0083755. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Vrtunski P. B., Simpson D. M., Meltzer H. Y. Voluntary movement dysfunction in schizophrenics. Biol Psychiatry. 1989 Mar 1;25(5):529–539. doi: 10.1016/0006-3223(89)90213-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Weinberger D. R., Berman K. F., Zec R. F. Physiologic dysfunction of dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in schizophrenia. I. Regional cerebral blood flow evidence. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1986 Feb;43(2):114–124. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1986.01800020020004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. White C., Farley J., Charles P. Chronic schizophrenic disorder. II. Reaction time, social performance and arousal. Br J Psychiatry. 1987 Mar;150:374–379. doi: 10.1192/bjp.150.3.374. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Williams R. M., Hemsley D. R. Choice reaction time performance in hospitalized schizophrenic patients and depressed patients. Eur Arch Psychiatry Neurol Sci. 1986;236(3):169–173. doi: 10.1007/BF00380945. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Yates A. J., Korboot P. Speed of perceptual functioning in chronic nonparanoid schizophrenics. J Abnorm Psychol. 1970 Dec;76(3):453–461. doi: 10.1037/h0030281. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry are provided here courtesy of BMJ Publishing Group

RESOURCES