Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2007 Jan 15.
Published in final edited form as: Physiol Behav. 2005 Sep 15;86(1-2):5–8. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2005.06.021

Too much of a good thing: Neurobiology of non-homeostatic eating and drug abuse

Rebecca L Corwin a,*, Andras Hajnal b
PMCID: PMC1769469  NIHMSID: NIHMS15068  PMID: 16081112

Abstract

In this paper, a framework involving four aspects to be considered when establishing an operational definition of non-homeostatic appetitive behavior is presented. The four aspects are (1) the quantity of the commodity consumed, (2) the quality or type of commodity consumed, (3) the context in which the behavior occurs, and (4) the specific kind of behavior that is directed toward obtaining and consuming the commodity of interest. This framework permits comparisons among a variety of non-homeostatic behaviors and accommodates different theoretical approaches reflected in the use of mechanistic, systems, behavioral, nutritional, and clinical experimental strategies. The speakers of this symposium were selected to emphasize the four aspects of non-homeostatic behavior, to represent several different approaches, and to facilitate discussion regarding neural similarities and differences between non-homeostatic eating and drug abuse. The various talks illustrated that boundaries need not exist among research fields, and that communication among the various areas enhances the research effort.

Keywords: Consumatory behavior, Dysfunctional, Eating disorder, Food intake, Maladaptive, Nervous system, Obesity, Substance abuse

1. Definition of non-homeostatic appetitive behavior

The first requirement of any informed discussion of non-homeostatic behavior is to clearly define what that behavior is. We have identified four aspects that we believe are important to consider in an operational definition of non-homeostatic appetitive behavior. These are: 1) the quantity of the commodity consumed, 2) the quality or type of commodity consumed, 3) the context in which the behavior occurs, and 4) the specific kind of behavior that is directed toward obtaining and consuming the commodity of interest.

2. The factor of quantity

Non-homeostatic behaviors usually involve engaging in a certain behavior to an excessive amount, for instance gambling compulsively or drinking excessive amounts of alcohol. Non-homeostatic eating, on the other hand, involves consuming too much or too little food relative to what is biologically required (i.e. defined by homeostatic needs). It is quite normal, of course, to overeat on some occasions and to undereat on others. It is regarded “normal” not only because of the consensus of our society [1], but also because homeostatic regulatory mechanisms for the short-term, and adaptive regulation for the long-term, compensate for deficits and excesses within a certain regulatory range [2]. We have elected to call such events “compensated” non-homeostatic eating. However, when overeating or undereating occurs repetitively or excessively, mental and/or physical health can become compromised, i.e. the behavior ceases to be “normal” and becomes “maladaptive” or “dysfunctional.” We have elected to use the term “maladaptive” because it refers to the failure to adapt appropriately. Adaptability is an essential feature of consumatory behavior that increases the chance of survival of the organism/individual. Excessive intake – in either direction –becomes maladaptive when it shortens life expectancy or increases the risk of disease. “Maladaptive” overeating promotes the development of obesity, while “maladaptive” undereating characterizes the eating disorder anorexia nervosa. Recent evidence suggests that neural mechanisms associated with obesity [3] and anorexia nervosa among human subjects [4] may overlap with mechanisms associated with substance abuse.

3. Quality also matters

The aspect of quality refers to the type of commodity toward which behavior is directed. In the case of drug abuse, quality would refer to the type of drug consumed, e.g. alcohol, cocaine, heroin, etc. Non-homeostatic eating involves consuming specific foods or nutrients in quantities that do not promote health (i.e., disproportional to need). Much research has focused on understanding the neurobiology involved in the overconsumption of fat and sugar [59]. Conversely, consuming insufficient quantities of certain foods and/or nutrients can certainly be non-homeostatic, as the many nutritional deficiency diseases demonstrate. However, even without inducing a nutrient deficiency, underconsumption of certain foods and/or nutrients may still be considered non-homeostatic. For instance, recent studies suggest that diets containing lower amounts of calcium and/ or low-fat dairy products can contribute to increased body weight [10].

4. Why is context so important?

The context in which behavior occurs can contribute to the maintenance of that behavior through classical and/or operant conditioning. That is, once a stimulus gains behavioral relevance, it becomes a cue or stimulus that, in turn, can promote the repeated occurrence of the behavior. For this reason, the context in which behavior occurs is important to consider when defining any behavior as non-homeostatic. Occasionally eating during the night, for instance, is normal; repeatedly eating at night becomes maladaptive [11]. Animal research relevant to context has shown that access conditions can differentially affect cocaine’s reinforcing efficacy as well as the amount of food consumed [12,13]. Furthermore, contextual cues associated with addictive drugs and palatable foods cause similar patterns of early gene expression in the prefrontal cortex [14]. Other recent work used a protocol in which brief 20-min daily access to sucrose was provided in a context of time cues as well as following chow availability in restricted-fed rats. Under these conditions, dopaminergic alterations occurred after as few as six days that were not apparent in control rats that had equal access to sucrose but received it outside of the context or without food restriction [1517]. Such studies may be useful for studying snacking behavior or the neurochemical plasticity in restricting bulimics.

These reports indicate that something more than simply the preference for certain foods (i.e. systems involved in “liking”) is required for development of neural plasticity that is similar to the effect of drugs of abuse (i.e. systems involved in learning and “wanting”) [18]. Many have examined neural similarities (see for example Refs. [69,19]) and differences between the responses to food and drugs of abuse (see for example [2022]). Whether discussing similarities or differences, it is clear that conditioned incentives attached to both drug- and food-related cues are important in modulating behavior directed toward obtaining and consuming both commodities. Such conditioning serves as a model for the contribution of environmental context to the maintenance of non-homeostatic behavior, and is thought to be critical to relapse [23,24].

5. Disordered behavioral patterns are characteristic of non-homeostatic behavior

Finally, the specific expression of the behavior itself is important to consider. In non-homeostatic eating, meal-size and appetite need to be studied separately from the overall amount consumed. Even if overall energy intake is in equilibrium with expenditure, consuming that amount of food rapidly in a brief period of time (bingeing) would certainly challenge physiological equilibrium. Everyone binges occasionally, which is normal. However, when bingeing occurs repeatedly over extended periods of time it becomes maladaptive, since neither the capacity nor the clearance and feedback mechanisms of our alimentary and central regulatory systems were designed for such an overload [25,26].

Disordered eating patterns can involve undereating or overeating, as well as combinations of both. For instance, inappropriate self restriction is a compensatory behavior used by many bulimics, whereas bingeing is characteristic of the binge/purge type of anorexia nervosa. Whether these combinations reflect homeostatic compensatory attempts or rather are part of the pathophysiology of the disorders (e.g. opponent-processes theory, [27]) is not known. Nonetheless, the fact that maladaptive patterns are maintained in spite of the negative consequences associated with them characterizes many of the behavioral disorders, including substance abuse and disordered eating.

6. Potential approaches to the study of maladaptive non-homeostatic eating

In summary, we are characterizing non-homeostatic appetitive behavior based upon the quantity and quality of the commodity consumed, the context in which the behavior occurs, and the manner in which the commodity is obtained and consumed. This framework permits comparisons among a variety of non-homeostatic behaviors. Drug intake, for instance, can be characterized using these four aspects, since the transition from casual use to addiction and relapse involves the quantity of drug consumed, the type of drug consumed (heroin, cocaine, ethanol, etc.), the environmental context in which the behavior occurs, and the manner in which the drug is self-administered (intravenous, nasal, oral, as well as binge/abstinence cycles).

It has been proposed that non-homeostatic behaviors directed toward food and drugs result in allostasis, with its associated costs and system specific consequences. That is, the shift from “normal” to maladaptive behavior has been proposed to involve a spiral of dysregulation that alters brain reward circuitry [28]. Behavioral models that induce non-homeostatic behaviors are needed in order to understand this process. To this end, comparisons between non-homeostatic behaviors directed toward obtaining and consuming food and drugs of abuse are fruitful. Overlap certainly exists in the neural mechanisms involved in behavior directed toward both commodities. However, it is also important to keep in mind critical differences that may account for differential counter-regulatory responses to food stimuli and drugs of abuse [21,22]. When studying non-homeostatic food intake, it is important to determine if regulatory controls involving the oral cavity, gastrointestinal tract, enteric nervous system, vagus nerve, hindbrain, etc., as well as other forebrain motivational systems are involved.

Despite differences in how drugs and foods impinge on the neural system in the development of maladaptive patterns, one possible common mechanism may be that adaptation/ dysadaptation occurs within a critical neural domain that can be a neural circuit or a specific chemical system. An alteration in one part of the system with a consequence of an altered gating in signal processing may, in turn, alter the functioning of the whole system (“domino effect”). This scenario traditionally has been considered as a primary mechanism for effects of drugs that has a well-defined site of action within the CNS [29]. However, we also know that elements of food and related metabolic, hormonal signals can alter neural functions. These effects can be exerted via a specific but widely distributed central sensor system as was proposed for glucose [30] or via a more specific molecular interaction as has been proposed for insulin and leptin [31,32]. An alternative mechanism could be that an otherwise neutral stimulus becomes part of a different context (Pavlovian to instrumental transfer) or gains different importance; if the behavior occurs repeatedly, the system could conceivably respond with a plasticity that promotes or sustains changes in psychophysical processes (e.g. anhedonia [33,34]; incentive sensitization [35]).

It is worth noting, however, that these possible scenarios may simply represent different approaches, i.e. a bottom-up or top-down approach, or different aspects of the system that are responsible for the organization of the behavior. That is, neural substrates that sense rewarding (sensory) characteristics of the stimuli and facilitate the acquisition of behaviors are partially different from those substrates that mediate the expression of responses established by Pavlovian or instrumental learning [36,37]. Likewise, some research pursues the input side of the behavior (the sensory and reward systems), other approaches focus preferentially on the learning/cognitive aspects and output organization of the behavior.

Different theoretical approaches are also reflected in the strategies that are used to study non-homeostatic behaviors. The most common is a mechanistic, neurobiological strategy that assesses the role of a variety of neuro-modulators in various aspects of the behavior. A systems strategy investigates motivational, sensory, or cognitive functions, whereas behavioral strategies identify critical variables in animal models (for example: binge-models, stress studies, and learning models). Nutritional strategies attempt to identify neural substrates that modulate behavior directed toward obtaining and consuming specific nutrients (e.g. sugars, fats, proteins). Clinical strategies are based on symptomatology and focus on etiology and mechanisms. All of these strategies are relevant and are strengthened by intercommunication. For instance, recent work describes behavioral strategies that can be used to distinguish “casual” from “addictive” drug taking behavior in rats. Such strategies could also be applied to the study of non-homeostatic eating [13,38,39]. The current focus on promoting translational research between the basic and clinical sciences will also enhance our understanding of these complicated behaviors.

7. What did this symposium accomplish?

The speakers of this symposium were selected to represent several different approaches, to emphasize the different aspects of non-homeostatic behavior described above, and to facilitate discussion regarding neural similarities and differences between non-homeostatic eating and drug abuse. The various talks illustrated that boundaries need not exist among research fields and that communication among the various areas enhances the productivity of the research effort and the validity of the results.

References

  • 1.Germov J, Williams L. Introducing the social appetite: towards sociology of food and nutrition. In: Germov J, Williams L, editors. A sociology of food and nutrition: the social appetite. 2nd edition. Oxford/New York: Oxford University Press; 2004. pp. 3–27. [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Speakman JR. Obesity: the integrated role of environment and genetics. J Nutr. 2004;134:2090S–105S. doi: 10.1093/jn/134.8.2090S. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Wang GJ, Volkow ND, Thanos PK, Fowler JS. Similarity between obesity and drug addiction as assessed by neurofunctional imaging: a concept review. J Addict Dis. 2004;23:39 –53. doi: 10.1300/J069v23n03_04. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Kaye WH, Bailer UF, Frank GK, Wagner A, Henry SE. Brain imaging of serotonin after recovery from anorexia and bulimia nervosa. Physiol Behav. 2005;86:15 –7. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2005.06.019. [this issue] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Levine AS, Kotz CM, Gosnell BA. Sugars and fats: the neurobiology of preference. J Nutr. 2003;133:831S–4S. doi: 10.1093/jn/133.3.831S. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Avena NM, Hoebel BG. Amphetamine-sensitized rats show sugar-induced hyperactivity (cross-sensitization) and sugar hyperphagia. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 2003;74:635–9. doi: 10.1016/s0091-3057(02)01050-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Avena NM, Hoebel BG. A diet promoting sugar dependency causes behavioral cross-sensitization to a low dose of amphetamine. Neuroscience. 2003;122:17–20. doi: 10.1016/s0306-4522(03)00502-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Colantuoni C, Rada P, McCarthy J, Patten C, Avena NM, Chadeayne A, et al. Evidence that intermittent, excessive sugar intake causes endogenous opioid dependence. Obes Res. 2002;10:478–88. doi: 10.1038/oby.2002.66. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Spangler R, Wittkowski KM, Goddard NL, Avena NM, Hoebel BG, Leibowitz SF. Opiate-like effects of sugar on gene expression in reward areas of the rat brain. Brain Res Mol Brain Res. 2004;124:134–42. doi: 10.1016/j.molbrainres.2004.02.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Zemel MB, Miller SL. Dietary calcium and dairy modulation of adiposity and obesity risk. Nutr Rev. 2004;62:125–31. doi: 10.1111/j.1753-4887.2004.tb00034.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Stunkard AJ, Allison KC. Two forms of disordered eating in obesity: binge eating and night eating. Int J Obes Relat Metab Disord. 2003;27:1–12. doi: 10.1038/sj.ijo.0802186. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Corwin RL, Buda-Levin A. Behavioral models of binge-type eating. Physiol Behav. 2004;82:123–30. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2004.04.036. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Robert DCS. Preclinical evidence for GABAB agonists as a pharmacotherapy for cocaine addiction. Physiol Behav. 2005;86:19–21. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2005.06.017. [this issue] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Schroeder BE, Binzak JM, Kelley AE. A common profile of prefrontal cortical activation following exposure to nicotine- or chocolate-associated contextual cues. Neuroscience. 2001;105:535–45. doi: 10.1016/s0306-4522(01)00221-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Bello NT, Lucas LR, Hajnal A. Repeated sucrose access influences dopamine D2 receptor density in the striatum. Neuroreport. 2002;13:1575–8. doi: 10.1097/00001756-200208270-00017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Bello NT, Sweigart KL, Lakoski JM, Norgren R, Hajnal A. Restricted feeding with scheduled sucrose access results in an upregulation of the rat dopamine transporter. Am J Physiol Regul Integr Comp Physiol. 2003;284:R1260–8. doi: 10.1152/ajpregu.00716.2002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Hajnal A, Norgren R. Repeated access to sucrose augments dopamine turnover in the nucleus accumbens. Neuroreport. 2002;13:2213–6. doi: 10.1097/00001756-200212030-00010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Berridge KC, Robinson TE. Parsing reward. Trends Neurosci. 2003;26:507–13. doi: 10.1016/S0166-2236(03)00233-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Grigson PS. Like drugs for chocolate: separate rewards modulated by common mechanisms? Physiol Behav. 2002;76:389–95. doi: 10.1016/s0031-9384(02)00758-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Carelli RM, Ijames SG, Crumling AJ. Evidence that separate neural circuits in the nucleus accumbens encode cocaine versus “natural” (water and food) reward. J Neurosci. 2000;20:4255–66. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.20-11-04255.2000. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Di Chiara G, Bassareo V, Fenu S, De Luca MA, Spina L, Cadoni C, et al. Dopamine and drug addiction: the nucleus accumbens shell connection. Neuropharmacology. 2004;47(Suppl 1):227–41. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2004.06.032. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Di Chiara G. Dopamine in disturbances of food and drug motivated behavior: a case of homology? Physiol Behav. 2005;86:9–10. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2005.06.020. [this issue] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Kelley AE. Memory and addiction; shared neural circuitry and molecular mechanisms. Neuron. 2004;44:161–79. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2004.09.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Shaham Y, Shalev U, Lu L, De Wit H, Stewart J. The reinstatement model of drug relapse: history, methodology and major findings. Psychopharmacology. 2003;168:3–20. doi: 10.1007/s00213-002-1224-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Bjorntorp P. Thrifty genes and human obesity. Are we chasing ghosts? Lancet. 2001;358:1006 –8. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(01)06110-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Schwartz MW, Woods SC, Seeley RJ, Barsh GS, Baskin DG, Leibel RL. Is the energy homeostasis system inherently biased toward weight gain? Diabetes. 2003;52:232–8. doi: 10.2337/diabetes.52.2.232. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Solomon RL, Corbit JD. An opponent-process theory of motivation. I. Temporal dynamics of affect. Psychol Rev. 1974;81:119–45. doi: 10.1037/h0036128. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Koob GF, Le Moal M. Drug addiction, dysregulation of reward, and allostasis. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2001;24:97 –129. doi: 10.1016/S0893-133X(00)00195-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Koob GF, Ahmed SH, Boutrel B, Chen SA, Kenny PJ, Markou A, et al. Neurobiological mechanisms in the transition from drug use to drug dependence. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2004;27:739–49. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2003.11.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Levin BE. Glucosensing neurons do more than just sense glucose. Int J Obes Relat Metab Disord. 2001;25(Suppl 5):S68–72. doi: 10.1038/sj.ijo.0801916. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Figlewicz DP, Evans SB, Murphy J, Hoen M, Baskin DG. Expression of receptors for insulin and leptin in the ventral tegmental area/ substantia nigra (Vta/Sn) of the rat. Brain Res. 2003;964:107–15. doi: 10.1016/s0006-8993(02)04087-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Niswender KD, Schwartz MW. Insulin and leptin revisited: adiposity signals with overlapping physiological and intracellular signaling capabilities. Front Neuroendocrinol. 2003;24:1–10. doi: 10.1016/s0091-3022(02)00105-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Wise RA. Neuroleptics and operant behavior: the anhedonia hypothesis. Behav Brain Sci. 1982;5:39–87. [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Roberts AJ, Koob GF. The neurobiology of addiction. Alcohol Health Res World. 1997;21:101–6. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Robinson TE, Berridge KC. Animal models in craving research. The psychology and neurobiology of addiction: an incentive-sensitization view. Addiction. 2000;95(Suppl 2):S91–117. doi: 10.1080/09652140050111681. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Cardinal RN, Everitt BJ. Neural and psychological mechanisms underlying appetitive learning: links to drug addiction. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2004;14:156–62. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2004.03.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Kelley AE. Ventral striatal control of appetitive motivation: role in ingestive behavior and reward-related learning. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2004;27:765–76. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2003.11.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Deroche-Gamonet V, Belin D, Piazza PV. Evidence for addiction-like behavior in the rat. Science. 2004;305:1014–7. doi: 10.1126/science.1099020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Vanderschuren LJ, Everitt BJ. Drug seeking becomes compulsive after prolonged cocaine self-administration. Science. 2004;305:1017 –9. doi: 10.1126/science.1098975. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES