Skip to main content
Health and Quality of Life Outcomes logoLink to Health and Quality of Life Outcomes
. 2007 Jan 19;5:4. doi: 10.1186/1477-7525-5-4

Factor structure of the Singapore English version of the KINDL® children quality of life questionnaire

Hwee-Lin Wee 1, Ulrike Ravens-Sieberer 2, Michael Erhart 2, Shu-Chuen Li 3,
PMCID: PMC1796858  PMID: 17239229

Abstract

Background

Quality of life (QoL) outcomes are useful in the assessment of physical, mental and social well-being and for informed healthcare decision making. However, few studies have evaluated QoL issues among Asian children due largely to the lack of culturally valid and reliable QoL questionnaires. Hence, we aimed to report the psychometric properties, in particular factor structure, of KINDL (Singapore) questionnaires among school-going children.

Methods

Students aged 8–16 years from participating schools were selected by convenience sampling. Subjects self-completed KINDL-Kid (Singapore) and KINDL-Kiddo (Singapore) questionnaires, which were cross-culturally adapted from KINDL (Germany English) for use in Singapore. We evaluated floor and ceiling effects, internal consistency and performed factor analysis.

Results

A total of 328 respondents (mean (SD) age: 9.6 (1.31) years; 67% female; 75% Chinese, 16% Malays, 9% Indians and others) completed KINDL-Kid while 1,026 respondents (mean (SD) age: 14.0 (1.00) years; 82% female; 82% Chinese, 12% Malays; 6% Indians and others) completed KINDL-Kiddo. Mean (SD) TOTAL QoL score was 65.5 (12.76) and 56.6 (11.92) for KINDL-Kid and KINDL-Kiddo, respectively. Floor and ceiling effects were important in five of six KINDL-Kid and two of six KINDL-Kiddo subscales. Reliability coefficients ranged from 0.40 to 0.71 (KINDL-Kid) and 0.44 to 0.84 (KINDL-Kiddo). Factor analysis generated eight and seven factors in KINDL-Kid and KINDL-Kiddo, respectively.

Conclusion

KINDL-Kiddo exhibited good psychometric properties and may be used to assess QoL in this multi-ethnic English-speaking Asian population. However, psychometric properties of KINDL-Kid may need to be improved either by developing new items or modifying existing items.

Background

Worldwide, there is an increasing interest in the measurement of quality of life (QoL) outcomes, which are useful in the assessment of physical, mental and social well-being and are potentially useful for informing healthcare decision making [1,2]. However, QoL assessments within Asian contexts have been limited due to the lack of questionnaires with demonstrated validity and reliability among Asians and the problem is particularly acute in the Asian pediatric population.

Although several children QoL questionnaires have been developed [3], very few were culturally adapted and validated for use in Asia. At the same of this study (2003), we identified only three questionnaires (in Arabic, Korean, Hebrew and Turkish) that were validated in Asian pediatric populations – Child Health Questionnaire, Childhood Health Assessment Questionnaire, and Quality of Life in Childhood Cancer [4-8]. However, only one of these questionnaires is a generic (Child Health Questionnaire) QoL questionnaire while the others are disease-specific questionnaires which have limited applications for the general pediatric population.

Furthermore, these language versions were not used in most countries in Asia, in particular Southeast Asia. Hence, we undertook a study to evaluate the psychometric properties of KINDL® – a generic children's quality of life questionnaire among English-speaking Singaporeans [9]. English was chosen because it is a universal language with potentially wide application in several Asian countries, including Hong Kong, the Philippines and Malaysia.

It was encouraging that since our first study was published [1], several more QoL questionnaires became available for use in the Asian pediatric population [10-13]. A few other QoL validation studies among Asians [14-17] were also published at around the same time as our study. However, only two of these studies evaluated factor validity of the questionnaire used [13,18]. In addition, language availability remains a problem. In the case of Singapore, as many as 11% (44,756 of 412,796) of Singaporeans aged 15–24 understood English only [19]. Hence, an English version of a generic QoL questionnaire is needed if such studies are to be representative of the general pediatric population.

In cross-cultural adaptation and translation of QoL questionnaires, it is important to ensure that psychometric properties of the original questionnaire are preserved in the new cultural setting. In our earlier study among children aged 8–16 years [9], internal consistencies of KINDL-Kid was 0.75 and that of KINDL-Kiddo was 0.84. The general accepted Cronbach's alpha for group level comparison was >= 0.5 [20,21] and that for person level comparison was >= 0.8 [21]. Hence, our results showed that the KINDL questionnaires were satisfactory for group level comparison and less satisfactory for person level comparison. This might suggest the translated KINDL questionnaire might be psychometrically different from the source English version. We aimed to confirm this hypothesis using a large school-based sample (who are likely to be representative of the general pediatric population). Thus, the aim of this paper is to evaluate the psychometric properties, in particular, factor structure of the Singapore English version of KINDL questionnaires among school-going children.

Methods

Study design and subject recruitment

This study was conducted over several days in March 2002. Permission to conduct the study in school children was obtained from the Ministry of Education, Singapore. Invitations for participation were sent via either facsimile or e-mail to primary and secondary schools (equivalent to the junior and high schools in the United States). A follow-up facsimile or e-mail was sent a week after the first mailer stating the study aims and design. Further details were provided to those principals who agreed to participate in the study.

Students aged 8–16 years old were selected by convenience sampling by the teacher in-charge of the survey. The aim was to minimize administrative burden so as to encourage participation. As English is the primary medium of teaching in Singapore, subjects completed the Singapore English versions of KINDL questionnaires and a simple demographic information sheet by self-completion without any assistance.

Instrument

The Singapore English versions of KINDL questionnaires were cross-culturally adapted [9] from the source Germany English versions [22] (available with permission from the developers), using standard guidelines, including independent forward and back translation and cognitive debriefing in focus groups. KINDL questionnaire is available in three age versions: KINDL-Kiddy (4–7 years), KINDL-Kid (8–12 years) and KINDL-Kiddo (13–16 years) with both parents and self-reports. In this study, we used the self-report versions of KINDL-Kid and KINDL-Kiddo. Each KINDL questionnaire comprised 24 items (with five answer categories) yielding a general QoL score (TOTAL) and six subscales: physical health (PH), general health (GH), family functioning (FAM), self-esteem (PER), social functioning (FREN) and school functioning (SCH) scores (Table 1). Reverse scoring was applied to some items so that higher item scores represent better QoL. Item scores were summed up to give subscale scores, and subscale scores summed up to give a TOTAL score. The raw score was transformed to a scale of 0–100 to facilitate interpretation.

Table 1.

Items in KINDL-Kid and KINDL-Kiddo

KINDL-Kid KINDL-Kiddo
Physical Health Scale Physical Health Scale
PH1. ... felt ill PH1. ... felt ill
PH2. ... headache or tummy-ache PH2. ... in pain
PH3. ... tired and sleepy PH3. ... tired and sleepy
PH4. ... strong and full of energy PH4. ... felt strong and full of energy
General Health Scale General Health Scale
GEN1. ... had fun and laughed a lot GEN1. ... had fun and laughed a lot
GEN2. ... bored GEN2. ... was bored
GEN3. ... felt alone GEN3. ... felt alone
GEN4. ... scared GEN4. ... was scared or unsure of myself
Self-esteem Scale Self-esteem Scale
PER1. ... proud of myself PER1. ... proud of myself
PER2. ... felt on top of the world PER2. ... felt on top of the world
PER3. ... felt pleased with myself PER3. ... felt pleased with myself
PER4. ... had lots of good ideas PER4. ... had lots of good ideas
Family Functioning Scale Family Functioning Scale
FAM1. ... got on well with my parents FAM1. ... got on well with my parents
FAM2. ... felt fine at home FAM2. ... felt fine at home
FAM3. ... quarrelled at home FAM3. ... quarrelled at home
FAM4. ... stopped from doing certain things FAM4. ... felt restricted by my parents
Social Functioning Scale Social Functioning Scale
FREN1. ... played with friends FREN1. ... did things together with my friends
FREN2. ... other kids liked me FREN2. ... was a "success" with my friends
FREN3. ... got along well with my friends FREN3. ... got along well with my friends
FREN4. ... felt different from other children FREN4. ... felt different from other people
School Functioning Scale School Functioning scale
SCH1. ... doing my school work was easy SCH1. ... doing my school work was easy
SCH2. ... enjoyed my lessons SCH2. ... found school interesting
SCH3. ... looked forward to the weeks ahead SCH3. ... worried about my future
SCH4. ... was afraid of bad marks or grades SCH4. ... was worried about getting bad marks or grades

Statistical analyses

The number of responses at minimum (floor) and maximum (ceiling) scores were reported. Internal consistencies of individual subscales were evaluated using Cronbach's alpha. Floor and ceiling effects exceeding 15% have been considered high [23]. In our study, however, we applied a more stringent criteria with floor and ceiling effects exceeding 5% considered as high. Factor structure of KINDL-Kid (Singapore) and KINDL-Kiddo (Singapore) were evaluated using principal component analysis with varimax rotation. The criterion chosen to determine that an extracted factor accounted for a reasonably large proportion of the total variance was based on an eigenvalue greater than 1. There is no consensus on the minimum sample size for factor analysis, with recommendations ranging from 100 to 300 [24,25]. We hypothesised that KINDL (Singapore) will retain the same factor structure (Table 1) as the source KINDL (Germany). All statistical analyses were performed using STATA [26].

Results

Subjects

A total of 181 e-mail and faxes was sent with follow-up e-mail or faxes. In response, three primary (one girl's school, two co-ed) and five secondary schools (one boy's school, one girl's, three co-ed) agreed to participate. The most common reason that principals gave to decline the invitation was that their students were already involved in other studies. A total of 328 respondents (mean (standard deviation, SD) age: 9.6 (1.31) years; 67% female; 75% Chinese, 16% Malays, 9% Indians and others) completed KINDL-Kid (Singapore) while 1,026 respondents (mean (SD) age: 14.0 (1.00) years; 82% female; 82% Chinese, 12% Malays, 6% Indians and others) completed KINDL-Kiddo (Singapore). There were no missing data.

Data analyses

Mean, Standard deviation (SD), Floor and Ceiling Effects (Table 2)

Table 2.

Score distributions, floor and ceiling responses and internal consistency of KINDL-Kid (Singapore) and KINDL-Kiddo (Singapore) subscales

KINDL-Kid Subscale Mean (SD) Range Floor Responses (N, %) Ceiling Responses (N, %) Internal Consistency
TOTAL 65.5 (12.76) 31.3 to 100 0 2 (0.6) 0.79
PH 75.1 (17.08) 12.5 to 100 0 53 (16.2) 0.49
GEN 76.7 (17.47) 12.5 to 100 0 39 (11.9) 0.50
PER 44.0 (25.10) 0 to 100 24 (7.3) 12 (3.7) 0.71
FAM 71.9 (18.56) 25 to 100 0 36 (11.0) 0.46
FREN 66.2 (20.79) 12.5 to 100 0 22 (6.7) 0.52
SCH 59.0 (20.32) 0 to 100 3 (0.9) 18 (5.4) 0.40

KINDL-Kiddo Subscale Mean (SD) Range Floor Responses (N, %) Ceiling Responses (N, %) Internal Consistency

TOTAL 56.6 (11.92) 3.1 to 95.8 0 0 0.83
PH 61.4 (16.66) 0 to 100 3 (0.3) 9 (0.9) 0.62
GEN 66.8 (17.36) 0 to 100 2 (0.2) 28 (2.7) 0.63
PER 39.7 (22.69) 0 to 100 57 (5.6) 35 (3.4) 0.84
FAM 68.3 (20.93) 0 to 100 3 (0.3) 76 (7.4) 0.76
FREN 62.2 (17.53) 0 to 100 4 (0.4) 21 (2.1) 0.61
SCH 41.4 (16.57) 0 to 100 18 (1.8) 3 (0.3) 0.44

Abbreviations: TOTAL = Total QoL Scores; PH = Physical health; GEN = General health; PER = Self-esteem; FAM = Family functioning; FREN = Social functioning; SCH = School functioning.

Internal consistency was measured using Cronbach's alpha.

Mean (SD) of the TOTAL QoL score was 65.5 (12.76) and 56.6 (11.92) for KINDL-Kid (Singapore) and KINDL-Kiddo (Singapore), respectively. For KINDL-Kid (Singapore), floor effect was negligible for all subscales except PER, where number of response at minimum scores was 7.3%. Ceiling effect was more pronounced with all subscales except PER having ceiling responses exceeding 5%. For KINDL-Kiddo (Singapore), similar to KINDL-Kid (Singapore), floor effect was negligible for all subscales except PER, where floor response was 5.6%. However, ceiling effect was less pronounced than in KINDL-Kid (Singapore), with only one (FAM) of six subscales having ceiling responses that exceeded 5%.

Internal consistency (Table 2)

KINDL-Kid (Singapore): Internal consistency as measured by Cronbach's alpha was 0.79 for KINDL TOTAL and ranged from 0.40 (SCH) to 0.71 (FAM) for the subscales. None of the scales had internal consistencies that were considered acceptable for person level comparisons. Overall these internal consistencies were lower than the alphas reported for the original German version (alpha = 0.63 to 0.84), which however were calculated across both KINDL-Kid and KINDL-Kiddo [27].

KINDL-Kiddo (Singapore): The overall internal consistency of KINDL-Kiddo (Singapore) was higher than KINDL-Kid (Singapore). Cronbach's alpha was 0.83 for KINDL TOTAL with Cronbach's alpha ranging from 0.44 (SCH) to 0.84 (PER) for the subscales. Internal consistencies of the TOTAL and the subscales PER and FAM were considered acceptable for person level comparisons. Some internal consistencies were lower than the alphas reported for the original German version (alpha = 0.63 to 0.84) [27], while others were higher.

Factor analysis (Table 3)

Table 3.

Factor structure of KINDL-Kid (Singapore) and KINDL-Kiddo (Singapore) questionnaires

KINDL-Kid subscale Factor loading* KINDL-Kiddo subscale Factor loading*
1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

PH1 -0.80 PH1 0.85
PH2 -0.78 PH2 0.70
PH3 -0.47 PH3 0.44
PH4 0.47 0.47 PH4 0.41
GEN1 0.48 GEN1 0.60
GEN2 -0.86 GEN2 -0.70
GEN3 -0.80 GEN3 -0.72
GEN4 0.54 GEN4 -0.62
PER1 0.70 PER1 0.85
PER2 0.71 PER2 0.83
PER3 0.71 PER3 0.82
PER4 0.54 PER4 0.69
FAM1 0.75 FAM1 0.80
FAM2 0.68 FAM2 0.74
FAM3 0.72 FAM3 0.70
FAM4 0.57 FAM4 0.68
FREN1 0.81 FREN1 0.84
FREN2 0.63 FREN2 0.75
FREN3 0.69 FREN3 0.76
FREN4 0.71 FREN4 -0.47
SCH1 0.68 SCH1 0.73
SCH2 0.63 SCH2 0.74
SCH3 0.44 0.50 SCH3 0.83
SCH4 0.72 SCH4 0.86

* Only factor loadings above 0.40 are shown.

KINDL-Kid (Singapore): KINDL-Kid (Singapore) items did not load onto the six factors originally hypothesised. Instead, eight factors were generated. The cumulative percentage of total variance explained by the eight-factor solution was 60.4%. A six-factor solution would have explained 51.3% of the total variance. One of the new factors comprised of GEN4, FAM4 and SCH4, items that were originally from three different subscales (Factor 2, Table 4). In addition, some items were reorganized and loaded onto subscales different from those originally hypothesised. For example, GEN1 loaded onto FREN instead of GEN (Factor 5, Table 4).

Table 4.

Distribution of items based on factor structure of KINDL-Kid (Singapore) and KINDL-Kiddo (Singapore) questionnaires

KINDL-Kid KINDL-Kiddo
Factor 1 Factor 1
PH1. ... felt ill PH1. ... felt ill
PH2. ... headache or tummy-ache PH2. ... in pain
PH3. ... tired and sleepy PH3. ... tired and sleepy
PH4. ... felt strong and full of energy
Factor 2 Factor 2
GEN4. ... scared GEN2. ... was bored
FAM4. ... stopped from doing certain things GEN3. ... felt alone
SCH4. ... was afraid of bad marks or grades GEN4. ... was scared or unsure of myself
FREN4. ... felt different from other people
Factor 3 Factor 3
PER1. ... proud of myself PER1. ... proud of myself
PER2. ... felt on top of the world PER2. ... felt on top of the world
PER3. ... felt pleased with myself PER3. ... felt pleased with myself
PER4. ... had lots of good ideas PER4. ... had lots of good ideas
PH4. ... strong and full of energy
Factor 4 Factor 4
FAM1. ... got on well with my parents FAM1. ... got on well with my parents
FAM2. ... felt fine at home FAM2. ... felt fine at home
PH4. ... strong and full of energy FAM3. ... quarrelled at home
FAM4. ... felt restricted by my parents
Factor 5 Factor 5
FREN1. ... played with friends FREN1. ... did things together with my friends
FREN2. ... other kids liked me FREN2. ... was a "success" with my friends
FREN3. ... got along well with my friends FREN3. ... got along well with my friends
GEN1. ... had fun and laughed a lot GEN1. ... had fun and laughed a lot
Factor 6 Factor 6
SCH1. ... doing my school work was easy SCH1. ... doing my school work was easy
SCH2. ... enjoyed my lessons SCH2. ... found school interesting
SCH3. ... looked forward to the weeks ahead
Factor 7 Factor 7
GEN2. ... bored SCH3. ... worried about my future
GEN3. ... felt alone SCH4. ... was worried about getting bad marks or grades
Factor 8
FAM3. ... quarrelled at home
FREN4. ... felt different from other children

KINDL-Kiddo (Singapore): Results of factor analysis was more reassuring for KINDL-Kiddo (Singapore) compared with KINDL-Kid (Singapore). Seven factors were identified (eigenvalue > 1.0). The cumulative percentage of total variance explained by the seven-factor solution was 62.2%. A six-factor solution would have explained 57.8% of the total variance. Factors 1, 3 and 4 (Table 4) of KINDL-Kid (Singapore), corresponded exactly to PH, PER and FAM subscales of KINDL-Kiddo (Germany). Factors 2 and 5 of KINDL-Kiddo (Singapore) were almost identical to GEN and FREN subscales of KINDL-Kiddo (Germany) except for an exchange of two items GEN4 and FREN4 between the two subscales. Interestingly, Factors 6 and 7 actually comprised of items (two per factor) that were from the SCH subscale of KINDL-Kid (Germany).

Discussion

In this study, we evaluated the psychometric properties of KINDL (Singapore) questionnaires among English-speaking Singaporean children and adolescents recruited from schools. The psychometric properties of KINDL-Kiddo (Singapore) in this Asian population were reassuring albeit with a few areas needing improvement. First, internal consistency was good for the TOTAL instrument score. Two subscales displayed problems with regards to floor and ceiling effects. Internal consistency was sometimes insufficient in the subscales, but apart from this was good for group level comparisons. However, we should aim to achieve internal consistency good enough for person level comparisons. Yet, this is technically difficult as respondents were relatively young, and their responses are likely to be less reliable than adults. Arguably the homogeneity of the sample – comprising of healthy children and adolescents only with presumably fewer variation in their QoL trait parameter values – had led to slightly reduced Cronbach alpha values. Second, factor structure of KINDL-Kiddo (Singapore) was almost identical to KINDL-Kiddo (Germany), except for the partitioning of SCH subscale into two factors in KINDL-Kiddo (Singapore). This could be addressed by modifying one or more items in SCH subscale.

The psychometric properties of KINDL-Kid (Singapore) were less reassuring and needs to be improved in several aspects. Otherwise, cross-cultural comparisons of findings made using KINDL-Kid questionnaires could be misleading because the equivalence of the construct measured is questionable. First, ceiling effects needs to be reduced, possibly by including items of greater difficulty. Second, internal consistency needs to be improved by either eliminating items that do not contribute to a subscale or by developing new items with better psychometric properties and/or modifying existing items. Third, we would suggest to further examine the items in KINDL-Kid (Singapore) either by modifying existing items or adding new items such that the factor structure reflects that of the KINDL-Kid (Germany). This is because in a focus group study among Singaporean children and adolescents [27], we found that Singaporean and Western children/adolescents share a remarkably similar notion of general and health-related QoL. Hence, it is theoretically possible for both KINDL-Kid (Singapore) and KINDL-Kid (Germany) to achieve the same factor structure.

We recognized there are limitations in this study. First, our sample is unlikely to be representative of the general pediatric population given the very low participation rate (4.5%) from the school community. Second, at the school level, we opted for convenience sampling in order to improve participation rate. As a result, there was an over-representation of female subjects (general population: 50.5% female [28]) and slight under-representation of the ethnic minorities (general population: 13.9% Malays and 9.3% Indians and others [28]). Nevertheless, the results were encouraging and provided empirical support for further studies to evaluate KINDL (Singapore) questionnaires. Third, Singapore is a multi-ethnic, multi-lingual society but we have used only the English language questionnaires. However, as English is the main language (besides mother tongue) used in all educational institutions, this is unlikely to pose a problem. It should be noted that there were no subjects who had to be excluded from the study because they could not understand English.

Conclusion

KINDL-Kiddo (Singapore) exhibited good psychometric properties and may be used to assess quality of life in this multi-ethnic English-speaking Asian population. However, the psychometric properties of KINDL-Kid (Singapore) need to be improved. Furthermore, to cater to the multi-lingual sociocultural environment in Singapore and other Asian countries, various language versions will be needed. For the moment, it is strongly recommended to focus on KINDL TOTAL score when interpreting KINDL data.

List of abbreviations

FAM Family functioning

FREN Social functioning

GEN General health

PER Self-esteem

PH Physical

QoL quality of life

SCH School functioning

SD Standard deviation

TOTAL General QoL score

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Authors' contributions

HLW conceived the study, participated in the design and coordination of the study and performed the statistical analysis. SCL and URS conceived the study, participated and provided oversight for its design and coordination. ME performed the statistical analysis.

Acknowledgments

Acknowledgements

We would like to express our appreciation to the Ministry of Education, Republic of Singapore, for granting us the permission to conduct the study. Special thanks go to Miss Ivy Peck-Yong Chua, Head, Data Administration Centre, Ministry of Education, for her assistance in the approval process. We would also like to thank the principals and teachers of participating schools for their invaluable support.

Contributor Information

Hwee-Lin Wee, Email: hweelin.wee@gmail.com.

Ulrike Ravens-Sieberer, Email: Ravens-SiebererU@rki.de.

Michael Erhart, Email: ErhartM@rki.de.

Shu-Chuen Li, Email: Shuchuen.li@newcastle.edu.au.

References

  1. Neumann PJ, Goldie SJ, Weinstein MC. Preference-based measures in economic evaluation in health care. Annu Rev Public Health. 2000;21:587–611. doi: 10.1146/annurev.publhealth.21.1.587. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Hyland ME. Quality-of-life measures as providers of information on value-for-money of health interventions. Comparison and recommendations for practice. Pharmacoeconomics. 1997;11:19–31. doi: 10.2165/00019053-199711010-00004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Eiser C, Morse R. Quality-of-life measures in chronic diseases of childhood. Health Technol Assess. 2001;5:1–157. doi: 10.3310/hta5040. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. el Miedany YM, Youssef SS, el Gaafary M. Cross cultural adaptation and validation of the Arabic version of the Childhood Health Assessment Questionnaire for measuring functional status in children with juvenile idiopathic arthritis. Clin Exp Rheumatol. 2003;21:387–393. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Bae SC, Ruperto N, Lee JH, Uhm WS, Park YW, Kim SY, Paediatric Rheumatology International Trials Organisation The Korean version of the Childhood Health Assessment Questionnaire (CHAQ) and the Child Health Questionnaire (CHQ) Clin Exp Rheumatol. 2001;19:S96–100. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Hashkes P, Uziel Y, Press J, Brik R, Navon-Elkan P, Mukamel M, Libman E, Tauber T, Ruperto N, Barash J, Paediatric Rheumatology International Trials Organisation The Hebrew version of the Childhood Health Assessment Questionnaire (CHAQ) and the Child Health Questionnaire (CHQ) Clin Exp Rheumatol. 2001;19:S86–90. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Ozdogan H, Ruperto N, Kasapcopur O, Bakkaloglu A, Arisoy N, Ozen S, Ugurlu U, Unsal E, Melikoglu M, Paediatric Rheumatology International Trials Organisation The Turkish version of the Childhood Health Assessment Questionnaire (CHAQ) and the Child Health Questionnaire (CHQ) Clin Exp Rheumatol. 2001;19:S158–162. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Yeh CH, Hung LC. Construct validity of newly developed quality of life assessment instrument for child and adolescent cancer patients in Taiwan. Psychooncology. 2003;12:345–356. doi: 10.1002/pon.647. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Wee HL, Lee WW, Ravens-Sieberer U, Erhart M, Li SC. Validation of the English version of the KINDL generic children's health-related quality of life instrument for an Asian population–results from a pilot test. Qual Life Res. 2005;14:1193–1200. doi: 10.1007/s11136-004-2957-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Chuh AA. Validation of a Cantonese version of the Children's Dermatology Life Quality Index. Pediatr Dermatol. 2003;20:479–481. doi: 10.1111/j.1525-1470.2003.20604.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Madi SM, Al-Mayouf SM, Grainger CG, Bahabri SA. The Arabic version of childhood health assessment questionnaire modified for Arabic children. Saudi Med J. 2004;25:83–87. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Hanh VT, Guillemin F, Cong DD, Parkerson GR, Jr, Thu PB, Quynh PT, Briancon S. Health related quality of life of adolescents in Vietnam: cross-cultural adaptation and validation of the Adolescent Duke Health Profile. J Adolesc. 2005;28:127–146. doi: 10.1016/j.adolescence.2003.11.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Lee CF, Chien LY, Ko YL, Chou YH, Tai CJ, Liou YM. Development and psychometric properties of the Chinese language version of the TAPQOL: a health-related quality of life instrument for preschool children. Int J Nurs Stud. 2005;42:457–465. doi: 10.1016/j.ijnurstu.2004.08.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Ng JY, Landgraf JM, Chiu CS, Cheng NL, Cheung YF. Preliminary evidence on the measurement properties of the Chinese version of the Child Health Questionnaire, parent form (CHQ-pF50) and child form (CHQ-CF87) Qual Life Res. 2005;14:1775–1781. doi: 10.1007/s11136-005-1005-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Ng TP, Lim LC, Jin A, Shinfuku N. Ethnic differences in quality of life in adolescents among Chinese, Malay and Indians in Singapore. Qual Life Res. 2005;14:1755–1768. doi: 10.1007/s11136-005-1741-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Yam WK, Chow SM, Ronen GM. Chinese version of the parent-proxy health-related quality of life measure for children with epilepsy: translation, cross-cultural adaptation, and reliability studies. Epilepsy Behav. 2005;7:697–707. doi: 10.1016/j.yebeh.2005.05.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Chan LF, Chow SM, Lo SK. Preliminary validation of the Chinese version of the Pediatric Quality of Life Inventory. Int J Rehabil Res. 2005;28:219–227. doi: 10.1097/00004356-200509000-00004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Yeh CH, Chao KY, Hung LC. The quality of life for cancer children (QOLCC) in Taiwan (part I): reliability and construct validity by confirmatory factor analysis. Psychooncology. 2004;13:161–170. doi: 10.1002/pon.728. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Singapore Department of Statistics. Literacy and language http://www.singstat.gov.sg/keystats/c2000/literacy.pdf
  20. Helmstadter GC. Principals of Psychological Measurement. NY: Appleton-Century-Crofts; 1964. [Google Scholar]
  21. Nunnally JC, Bernstein IR. Psychometric theory. 3. NY: McCraw-Hill; 1994. [Google Scholar]
  22. Ravens-Sieberer U, Bullinger M. Assessing health-related quality of life in chronically ill children with the German KINDL: first psychometric and content analytical results. Qual Life Res. 1998;7:399–407. doi: 10.1023/A:1008853819715. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. O'Mahony PG, Rodgers H, Thomson RG, Dobson R, James OF. Is the SF-36 suitable for assessing health status of older stroke patients? Age Ageing. 1998;27:19–22. doi: 10.1093/ageing/27.1.19. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Gorsuch RL. Factor Analysis. 2. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum; 1983. [Google Scholar]
  25. Comrey AL, Lee HB. A first course in factor analysis. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum; 1992. [Google Scholar]
  26. Stata Statistical Software: Release 8. College Station, TX: StataCorp LP; 2003. StataCorp. [Google Scholar]
  27. Ravens-Sieberer U, Bullinger M. KINDLR Questionnaire for Measuring Health-Related Quality of Life in Children and Adolescents Revised Version – Manual. 2000 http://www.kindl.org/daten/pdf/ManEnglish.pdf [Google Scholar]
  28. Wee HL, Chua HX, Li SC. Meaning of health-related quality of life among children and adolescents in an Asian country: a focus group approach. Qual Life Res. 2006;15:821–31. doi: 10.1007/s11136-005-5092-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Singapore Department of Statistics. Key indicators of the Resident Population http://www.singstat.gov.sg/keystats/c2000/r1/indicators.pdf

Articles from Health and Quality of Life Outcomes are provided here courtesy of BMC

RESOURCES