Skip to main content
American Journal of Public Health logoLink to American Journal of Public Health
. 2007 Mar;97(3):526–530. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2005.080663

Associations of Abdominal Fat With Perceived Racism and Passive Emotional Responses to Racism in African American Women

Anissa I Vines 1, Donna Day Baird 1, June Stevens 1, Irva Hertz-Picciotto 1, Kathleen C Light 1, Maya McNeilly 1
PMCID: PMC1805011  PMID: 17267721

Abstract

Objectives. An excess in abdominal fat may predispose African American women to chronic health conditions such as diabetes and cardiovascular disease. Because stress may increase body fat in the center-body region, we used the waist-to-hip ratio (WHR) to examine associations between excess abdominal fat and perceived racism (a chronic stressor) and daily stress. Passive emotional responses to perceived racism, hypothesized to have particularly adverse effects, were also examined.

Methods. We controlled for body mass index in multiple logistic regression models among 447 African American women who completed a telephone interview on perceived racism.

Results. Passive emotional responses were not related to WHR (odds ratio [OR]=1.4; 95% confidence interval [CI]=0.8, 2.4). High perceived racism was associated with a low WHR in this population (OR=0.4; 95% CI=0.3, 0.8). However, high daily stress was related to a high WHR (OR=2.7; 95% CI=1.1, 6.7).

Conclusions. Findings support an association between daily stress and WHR but do not support our hypothesis that passive emotional responses to perceived racism increase abdominal fat. Further study of the stress physiology of perceived racism in African American women is warranted.


Compared with White women, African American women have high rates of morbidity and mortality from many chronic diseases, such as heart disease and diabetes.13 Abdominal body fat, the “apple-shaped” body type, has been shown to be a risk factor for these conditions, independent of total body fat.4 Factors that have been associated with higher waist-to-hip ratios (WHRs) in African Americans are low socioeconomic status,5 smoking, older age, lower education, and less physical activity.68

Psychological stress may be another risk factor for the accumulation of body fat in the abdominal region, but this has not been explored widely among African Americans.912 Bjorntorp hypothesized that the distribution of body fat in the visceral cavity may be attributable to activation of the hypothalamus as a result of chronic stress and the body’s inability to adjust to insurmountable life circumstances.9,10 In a study of socially subordinate cynomolgus monkeys, chronic stress induced by an inability to overcome adverse situations caused an increase in weight and the centralization of adipose tissue.13 In the few studies that have examined the relation between chronic stress and increased central adiposity in humans, there has been variation in the measures used to assess stress.1425 Despite the increasing disparities between African American and White women in obesity and obesity-related diseases, the number of African Americans in these studies has been small. In addition, coping in response to the stressor has not been studied in relation to increased central adiposity.

Focusing only on traditional stress measures is not adequate when studying African Americans because these measures do not capture the full range of stress they experience.26,27 Racism can be a chronic stressor because of its continued influence in the lives of many African Americans.28 Likewise, how the racist event is perceived can cause stress and induce both emotional and behavioral responses. In a survey of 200 African American adults who resided in a metropolitan area, 33% reported experiences of racism within the past 6 months.27 The results of the survey indicated that the experiences of racism, like other stressful events, produced feelings of distress. Moreover, studies have shown that emotions and coping responses are important determinants of physical and psychological health.2930

It is plausible that chronic stress caused by perceived racism could influence the distribution of body fat among African American women, especially among those who respond to perceived racism with passive emotional coping responses. These are responses that do not lead to action and that involve the suppression of emotions. In a person who lacks adequate resources to circumvent its effects, the stress of perceived racism could disrupt the body’s homeostasis and produce central abdominal obesity, similar to the effects seen in stressed cynomolgus monkeys.13 Our study examined the associations of perceptions of racism and passive emotional responses to racism with central abdominal obesity in a sample of African American women. We hypothesized that higher levels of perceived racism and higher levels of passive emotional responses (e.g., hopelessness) to racism would be associated with increased central obesity.

METHODS

Participants

The study population comprised 476 non-Hispanic African American women who participated in the National Institute of Environmental Health Sciences Uterine Fibroid Study31 for an investigation of perceived racism.32 Participants in the parent study were aged 35 to 49 years and were randomly selected from the membership rolls of a prepaid health plan. Eighty percent of eligible women agreed to participate in the parent study. Those eligible for the assessment of perceived racism were premenopausal women who self-reported as either Black or African American and had completed a clinic visit, a telephone interview, and a mailed questionnaire during the initial phase of the study (77% of all premenopausal African American participants). We use the term African American to represent these women. Of those eligible for the study on perceived racism (N = 529), 476 completed an interview (response rate 90%).

In the parent study, participants were screened for fibroids using ultrasound. Because large uterine fibroids can increase the size of the uterus and affect waist circumference, we excluded 20 women with uterine size equivalent to a fetus of 12 weeks gestation or larger.33 An additional 6 women had missing uterine size data but were excluded on the basis of their self-report of a previous diagnosis of having a fibroid of 5 cm or larger in diameter. Thus, a total sample of 450 women remained for this analysis.

Anthropometric Variables

Body mass and waist and hip circumferences were measured in the clinic by a trained field supervisor. Participants were asked to wear lightweight clothing to the appointment, and they removed their shoes before measurements were taken. Weight was measured to the nearest half pound using a nondigital scale. Waist and hip circumferences were measured to the nearest quarter inch with a standard tape measure around the smallest part of the waist, where the umbilicus was the reference point, and around the largest part of the hips. Although computerized tomography and magnetic resonance imaging can provide accurate measures of central adiposity, these procedures require clinic time and are quite expensive to use in a large epidemiological study.34 Therefore, in this study, the WHR was used to assess abdominal body fat. A high WHR, indicating high risk, was defined as greater than 0.80.35,36 Body mass index (BMI) was calculated as weight in kilograms divided by height in meters squared.

Measures of Perceived Racism and Passive Emotional Responses to Racism

The Telephone-Administered Perceived Racism Scale,32 a multidimensional instrument that consists of 3 scales and 5 response sub-scales, was administered during our study to assess perceptions of and responses to racism. Cronbach alpha reliabilities for all scales and subscales were 0.75 or above except for the passive behavioral subscale, with a Cronbach alpha of 0.68. Test–retest reliability varied from 0.61 to 0.82. Details about the perceived racism instrument’s construction and psychometric properties can be found elsewhere.32

We used 2 measures of the perceived racism instrument: the perceived racism scale (mean [SD] = 63 [13.4]; range = 29–95) and the passive emotional response subscale (mean [SD] = 8.9 [3.8]; range = 4–16). Each was standardized to range from 0 (lower passive emotional responses to racism and lower perceptions of racism) to 1 to facilitate the interpretation of findings. These data were analyzed as continuous variables and in tertiles. The perceived racism scale includes questions about personal experiences of racism and the respondent’s perceptions of racism experienced by African Americans as a group. The passive emotional response subscale was used to measure passive emotional coping, responses that do not lead to action or may suppress emotions (represented in the scale by feelings of hopelessness or powerlessness).

Demographic, behavioral, and psychosocial variables were obtained from the mailed and telephone questionnaires. Educational attainment was categorized as completed high school or lower, some college or technical school, or a bachelor’s degree or higher. Income was categorized as less than $40 000 (10% had an annual household income of < $20 000), $40 000 to $60 000, or greater than $60 000. Current and former smokers were grouped together as ever-smokers. Alcohol consumption (yes or no) for each type of alcoholic beverage (beer, wine, liquor) was obtained from a self-administered food-frequency questionnaire. Parity, the number of live births, was also included in the analyses. Physical activity data were collected by asking about time spent in vigorous (e.g., running, jogging, soccer, and basketball), moderate (e.g., dancing, bicycling short distances, golfing, and gardening), and walking forms of exercise in hours per week. Daily stress (i.e., none, mild, moderate, severe) was assessed by asking, “How stressful is your day-to-day life?” in the self-administered questionnaire.

The relations between WHR and each of the 2 main predictor variables of interest, perceived racism and passive emotional responses to racism, were examined with logistic regression models adjusted for BMI. Potential confounders were tested 1 at a time in separate models with perceived racism and with passive emotional responses. In all models we controlled for BMI. Educational attainment, annual household income, childhood social status (categorized into high or middle income vs low income), parity, alcohol use, and physical activity were not included in the multivariate model because the estimates for the stress-related variables changed by less than 10% when these potential confounders were included in the models. Smoking status, age, and daily stress were included in the full logistic regression model adjusted for BMI. Analyses were limited to 447 women with data for BMI.

RESULTS

The mean scores on the perceived racism scale and the passive emotional response subscale were 0.54 and 0.41, respectively (Table 1). The mean WHR was 0.80, and the average BMI was 30. There was a significant correlation of 0.50 between these 2 anthropometric indices. Participants represented a range of educational attainment and income levels (Table 1). Half of the women reported being raised as a child in a middle-income or well-off household.

TABLE 1—

Sample Characteristics: African American Women (n = 450), Uterine Fibroid Study, Washington, DC, 1996–1998

Characteristic Value
Passive emotional response to racism,a mean (SD) 0.41 (0.32)
Perceived racism,a mean (SD) 0.54 (0.18)
WHR, mean (SD) 0.80 (0.08)
BMI,b mean (SD) 30 (7.5)
Perceived racism scale, no. (%)
    Low 132 (29)
    Medium 126 (28)
    High 192 (43)
Passive emotional response subscale, no. (%)
    Low 147 (33)
    Medium 140 (31)
    High 163 (36)
Levels of WHR, no. (%)
    ≤ 0.80 241 (54)
    > 0.80 209 (46)
BMI,b no. (%)
    Normal (< 25) 116 (26)
    Overweight (25–29) 141 (31)
    Obese (≥ 30) 190 (42)
Educational attainment, no. (%)
    Completed high school or lower 86 (19)
    Some college or technical school 210 (47)
    Bachelor’s degree or higher 154 (34)
Annual household income, no. (%)
    < $40,000 187 (42)
    $40,000–$60,000 115 (26)
    > $60,000 148 (33)
Age at interview, no. (%), y
    < 40 96 (21)
    40–44 158 (35)
    ≥ 45 196 (44)
Childhood socioeconomic status, no. (%)
    Well-off or middle-income 242 (54)
    Quite poor or low-income 208 (46)

Source. Uterine Fibroid Study’s ancillary study on perceived racism, 1998.31,32

Note. BMI = body mass index; WHR = waist-to-hip ratio.

aStandardized to a scale and subscale ranging from 0 (i.e., lower passive emotional responses to racism and lower perceptions of racism) to 1 (i.e., higher passive emotional responses to racism and higher perceptions of racism)

bn = 447 because of missing data.

Table 2 shows results of the analyses that examined the associations of perceived racism and daily stress with WHR. Higher perceived racism was significantly associated with lower WHR (odds ratio [OR] = 0.5; 95% confidence interval [CI] = 0.3, 0.9). This association was seen in crude and adjusted models, including the model adjusting for daily stress. High daily stress was related to a high WHR (OR = 2.7; 95% CI = 1.1, 6.7) after we controlled for age, smoking, and perceived racism. The association was slightly weaker when perceived racism was not included in the model (OR = 2.2; 95% CI = 0.9, 5.2).

TABLE 2—

Associations of Perceived Racism and Daily Stress With Waist-to-Hip Ratio (WHR > 0.80), Adjusted for BMI, in African American Women (n = 447): Uterine Fibroid Study, Washington, DC, 1996–1998

Model 1 Model 2 Model 3 Model 4 Model 5
OR (95% CI) OR (95% CI) OR (95% CI) OR (95% CI) OR (95% CI)
Perceptions of racism
    Low (referent) 1.0 1.0 . . . . . . 1.0
    Medium 1.0 (0.6, 1.7) 1.0 (0.6, 1.7) . . . . . . 1.0 (0.6, 1.6)
    High 0.6 (0.3, 1.0) 0.5 (0.3, 0.9)* . . . . . . 0.4 (0.3, 0.8)*
Daily stress
    None (referent) . . . . . . 1.0 1.0 1.0
    Mild stress . . . . . . 1.3 (0.7, 2.6) 1.4 (0.7, 2.8) 1.4 (0.7, 2.9)
    Moderate stress . . . . . . 1.7 (0.8, 3.3) 1.6 (0.8, 3.4) 1.8 (0.9, 3.8)
    Severe stress . . . . . . 2.4 (1.0, 5.7) 2.2 (0.9, 5.2) 2.7 (1.1, 6.7)*
Age, y
    < 40 (referent) . . . 1.0 . . . 1.0 1.0
    40–44 . . . 1.4 (0.8, 2.6) . . . 1.4 (0.8, 2.5) 1.4 (0.8, 2.5)
    ≥ 45 . . . 2.4 (1.3, 4.3)* . . . 2.1 (1.2, 3.8)* 2.3 (1.3, 4.1)*
Smoking status
    Never-smoker (referent) . . . 1.0 . . . 1.0 1.0
    Ever-smoker . . . 1.7 (1.1, 2.7)* . . . 1.6 (1.1, 2.5)* 1.7 (1.1, 2.6)*

Note. BMI = body mass index; OR = odds ratio; CI = confidence interval. Omitted variables (. . .) not included by model design. Model 1 and Model 3 show perceptions of racism and daily stress adjusted only for BMI. Model 2 and Model 4 show perceptions of racism and daily stress adjusted for BMI, smoking, and age. Model 5 shows the association of WHR with daily stress and perceived racism.

* P < .05.

There was no significant association between tertiles of the passive emotional response sub-scale and high WHR (Table 3). The strongest effect of the passive emotional response sub-scale appeared in the upper tertile when perceived racism was included in the model, but the effect was weak, with broad confidence intervals (OR=1.4; 95% CI=0.8, 2.4).

TABLE 3—

Association Between Passive Emotional Responses and Waist-to-Hip Ratio (WHR > 0.80), Adjusted for BMI, in African American Women (n = 447): Uterine Fibroid Study, Washington, DC, 1996–1998

Crude Model Model 1 Model 2a Model 3a
OR (95% CI) OR (95% CI) OR (95% CI) OR (95% CI)
Passive emotional response
    Low (referent) 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0
    Medium 0.7 (0.4, 1.2) 0.7 (0.4, 1.2) 0.8 (0.5, 1.4) 0.6 (0.4, 1.1)
    High 1.1 (0.7, 1.8) 1.1 (0.6, 1.8) 1.4 (0.8, 2.4) 0.9 (0.6, 1.6)
Perceptions of racism
    Low (referent) . . . . . . 1.0 . . .
    Medium . . . . . . 1.0 (0.6, 1.7) . . .
    High . . . . . . 0.5 (0.3, 0.8)* . . .
Daily stress
    None (referent) . . . . . . . . . 1.0
    Mild stress . . . . . . . . . 1.5 (0.7, 3.0)
    Moderate stress . . . . . . . . . 1.8 (0.8, 3.7)
    Severe stress . . . . . . . . . 2.3 (1.0, 5.6)*
Age, y
    < 40 (referent) . . . 1.0 1.0 1.0
    40–44 . . . 1.4 (0.7, 2.5) 1.4 (0.8, 2.5) 1.3 (0.7, 2.4)
    ≥ 45 . . . 2.1 (1.2, 3.8)* 2.3 (1.3, 4.2)* 2.1 (1.2, 3.7)*
Smoking status
    Never-smoker (referent) . . . 1.0 1.0 1.0
    Ever-smoker . . . 1.7 (1.1, 2.6)* 1.8 (1.2, 2.8)* 1.7 (1.1, 2.6)*

Source. Uterine Fibroid Study’s ancillary study on perceived racism, 1998.31,32

Note. BMI = body mass index; OR = odds ratio; CI = confidence interval. Omitted variables (. . .) not included by model design. Three respondents were excluded because of missing BMI data.

aAdditional adjustment for perceptions of racism (Model 2) and daily stress (Model 3).

* P < .05.

DISCUSSION

This was the first study to examine the associations of abdominal body fat distribution with both perceptions of racism as a chronic stressor and the passive emotional coping responses to racism in a general population of African American women. Bjorntorp hypothesized9,10 that chronic stress-induced defeat (i.e., responses that do not lead to action) coping responses are associated with central body fat; we therefore explored whether higher levels of passive emotional response are associated with an increased risk of a high WHR. Passive emotional responses were not related to WHR, and surprisingly, higher perceptions of racism were related to a low rather than high WHR in this population. However, higher levels of daily stress were associated with increased WHR.

We evaluated several variables that could potentially confound a relation between perceived racism and WHR, including cigarette smoking, alcohol consumption, parity, educational attainment, and annual household income. Consistent with other studies, our research found that cigarette smoking and an age of 45 years and older were associated with higher WHRs after adjusting for BMI.5,7,8 Alcohol consumption among this group of women was low, and this may have prevented our finding an association with WHR.

We identified 13 previous studies that examined a relation between psychosocial factors and central adiposity.1425,38 Two of the studies explored this relation in diabetic populations,17,18 and 3 examined only men.14,15,23 Twin studies in Sweden and Finland found a significant and positive association between depression-related factors and increased central adiposity,22,38 as did studies of middle-aged women in Allegheny County, Pa,16 and 2 groups of Swedish women.21,24 Hispanic Health and Nutrition Examination Survey data were used to detect an association between depression and abdominal body fat in Cuban Americans but not in other Hispanic groups.25

Only 2 studies of abdominal body fat and psychosocial factors included substantial numbers of African American women,19,20 and neither reported results for depressive-related symptoms or passive responses to racism. Kaye et al. examined several psychosocial factors among 1464 African American women, 18 to 30 years old, and found significant associations between increased hostility scores and WHR.19 Tull et al. focused on internalized racism as a psychosocial stressor among US-born women living in the Virgin Islands and African Caribbean immigrants to the Virgin Islands.20 After adjusting for age, education, and BMI, Tull and colleagues found that the odds of having a high WHR ( > 0.80) were 2.4 (95% CI = 1.1, 5.3) for women with high internalized racism (the extent to which African Americans agree with racist stereotypes about African Americans).39

Our findings of an inverse association between perceived racism and WHR may be consistent with the findings of Tull et al.20 Although we did not measure internalized racism, it has been hypothesized that women who perceive little or no racism (low scores on the perceived racism scale) have higher internalized racism.40 If that is the case, low perceptions of racism might increase risk, and, as we observed, high perceptions would be associated with low WHR. A self-report of high perceived racism may reflect more successful coping with this stressor. Alternatively, it is possible that higher perceived racism is associated with some unknown variable that is linked to healthier lifestyle, such as social integration or greater access to health information. In our study population, perceived racism was associated with higher education.41 When we evaluated adjustment for education to address unmeasured confounding, however, there was not a significant change in the association between perceived racism and WHR.

In our sample, higher perceived racism was only modestly correlated with higher levels of daily stress (ρ = 0.20), which was associated with higher WHR. This supports the growing evidence in diverse populations for psychosocial influences on WHR and the concomitant adverse health effects.

Limitations

Our study had several limitations. The main limitation was its cross-sectional design, which limited inferences about causality. This study examined perceived racism rather than actual experiences of racism. However, it may be the perceptions of racism that determine stress levels and, ultimately, health status. We used WHR to assess central body fat instead of the more accurate computed tomography scans. However, studies have shown WHR and computed tomography scans to be correlated.35 Variation in the waist and hip measures was minimized because only 1 person conducted the anthropometric assessments. This study also used logistic regression to calculate the odds ratio for estimating risk. Although this is not an optimal modeling strategy when the outcome of interest is common, the odds ratio is still a valid measure of association. Furthermore, this is the first study to examine perceived racism and passive emotional responses to racism in relation to increased abdominal fat.

Conclusions

Our study adds to the evidence that stress has adverse affects on health. Given the surprising inverse association between perceived racism and WHR, it is clear that more research is needed to understand how African American women experience and cope with racism. Studies with more extensive psychometric measures are needed, as are longitudinal studies, perhaps beginning during the formative years. Our measure of perceived racism relied on a willingness of the respondent to acknowledge and report experiences of racism. Being able to acknowledge and report racism may reflect psychological responses integral to a healthy physiological response.

Acknowledgments

The final preparation of this article was supported in part by the University of North Carolina Program on Ethnicity, Culture and Health Outcomes. Data collection and analyses for this project were supported by the Intramural Research Training Program at the National Institute of Environmental Health Sciences and by the National Institute of Mental Health (grant 1-RO3-MH61057–01 to A. I. Vines).

Thanks to the entire Uterine Fibroid Study staff at CODA, Inc, Research Triangle Park, NC, for their hard work and commitment to this project, and to the women who shared their experiences of racism during the telephone interviews.

Human Participant Protection …This study was approved by the University of North Carolina’s institutional review board.

Peer Reviewed

Contributors…A. I. Vines originated the study, conducted the analyses, interpreted the data, and prepared the article. D. D. Baird provided access to the data. J. Stevens provided obesity expertise. I. Hertz-Picciotto contributed to all aspects of the study. K. C. Light provided the stress expertise. M. D. McNeilly provided expertise in perceived racism as a chronic stressor and coping mechanisms. All authors contributed to the origination of the study, interpretation of findings, and preparation of the article.

References

  • 1.Williams DR, Lavizzo-Mourey R, Warren RC. The concept of race and health status in America. Public Health Rep. 1994;109:26–41. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Tull ES, Thurland A, LaPorte RE, Chambers EC. Acculturation and psychosocial stress show differential relationships to insulin resistance (HOMA) and body fat distribution in two groups of Blacks living in the US Virgin Islands. J Natl Med Assoc. 2003;95:560–569. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Kumanyika SK. Obesity in African Americans: biobehavioral consequences of culture. Ethn Dis. 1998; 8:93–96. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Harris M, Stevens J. Fat distribution. In: Sandler BC, ed. Fat Patterning. Encyclopedia of Human Nutrition. London: Academic Press; 1998, 1440–1446.
  • 5.Keenan NL, Strogatz DS, James SA, Ammerman AS, Rice BL. Distribution and correlates of waist-to-hip ratio in Black adults: the Pitt County Study. Am J Epidemiol. 1992;135:678–684. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Stevens J, Plankey MW, Keil JE, Rust PF, Tyroler HA, Davis CE. Black women have smaller abdominal girths than White women of the same relative weight. J Clin Epidemiol. 1994;47:495–499. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Duncan BB, Chambless LE, Schmidt MI, et al. Correlates of body fat distribution. Variation across categories of race, sex, and body mass in the atherosclerosis risk in communities study. The Atherosclerosis Risk in Communities (ARIC) Study Investigators. Ann Epidemiol. 1995;5:192–200. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Croft JB, Freedman DS, Keenan NL, Sheridan DP, Macera CA, Wheeler FC. Education, health behaviors, and the Black–White difference in waist-to-hip ratio. Obes Res. 1996;4:505–512. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Bjorntorp P. The associations between obesity, adipose tissue distribution and disease. Acta Med Scand Suppl. 1988;723:121–134. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Bjorntorp P. Visceral fat accumulation: the missing link between psychosocial factors and cardiovascular disease? J Intern Med. 1991;230:195–201. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Bjorntorp P. Obesity and adipose tissue distribution as risk factors for the development of disease. A review. Infusionstherapie. 1990;17:24–27. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Peeke PM, Chrousos GP. Hypercortisolism and obesity. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 1995;771:665–676. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Shively CA, Clarkson TB. Social status and coronary artery atherosclerosis in female monkeys. Arterioscler Thromb. 1994;14:721–726. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Rosmond R, Lapidus L, Marin P, Bjorntorp P. Mental distress, obesity and body fat distribution in middle-aged men. Obes Res. 1996;4:245–252. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Raikkonen K, Keltikangas-Jarvinen L, Adlercreutz H, Hautanen A. Psychosocial stress and the insulin resistance syndrome. Metabolism. 1996;45:1533–1538. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Wing RR, Matthews KA, Kuller LH, Meilahn EN, Plantinga P. Waist to hip ratio in middle-aged women. Associations with behavioral and psychosocial factors and with changes in cardiovascular risk factors. Arterioscler Thromb. 1991;11:1250–1257. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Lloyd CE, Wing RR, Orchard TJ. Waist to hip ratio and psychosocial factors in adults with insulin-dependent diabetes mellitus: the Pittsburgh Epidemiology of Diabetes Complications study. Metabolism. 1996;45:268–272. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Bell RA, Summerson JH, Spangler JG, Konen JC. Body fat, fat distribution, and psychosocial factors among patients with type 2 diabetes mellitus. Behav Med. 1998;24:138–143. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Kaye SA, Folsom AR, Jacobs DR Jr, Hughes GH, Flack JM. Psychosocial correlates of body fat distribution in Black and White young adults. Int J Obes Relat Metab Disord. 1993;17:271–277. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Tull SE, Wickramasuriya T, Taylor J, et al. Relationship of internalized racism to abdominal obesity and blood pressure in Afro-Caribbean women. J Natl Med Assoc. 1999;91:447–452. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Lapidus L, Bengtsson C, Hallstrom T, Bjorntorp P. Obesity, adipose tissue distribution and health in women–results from a population study in Gothenburg, Sweden. Appetite. 1989;13:25–35. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Nelson TL, Palmer RF, Pedersen NL, Miles TP. Psychological and behavioral predictors of body fat distribution: age and gender effects. Obes Res. 1999;7: 199–207. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Raikkonen K, Hautanen A, Keltikangas-Jarvinen L. Association of stress and depression with regional fat distribution in healthy middle-aged men. J Behav Med. 1994;17:605–616. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Raikkonen K, Matthews KA, Kuller LH. Anthropometric and psychosocial determinants of visceral obesity in healthy postmenopausal women. Int J Obes Relat Metab Disord. 1999;23:775–782. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Georges E, Mueller WH, Wear ML. Body fat distribution in men and women of the Hispanic health and nutrition examination survey of the United States: associations with behavioural variables. Ann Hum Biol. 1993;20:275–291. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Williams DR. Racism and health: a research agenda. Ethn Dis. 1996;6:1–8. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Thompson VL. Perceived experiences of racism as stressful life events. Community Ment Health J. 1996; 32:223–233. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Clark R, Anderson NB, Clark VR, Williams DR. Racism as a stressor for African Americans: a biopsychosocial model. Am Psychol. 1999;54:805–816. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Lazarus RS. Stress, Appraisal, and Coping. New York: Springer; 1984.
  • 30.Baum A, Singer JE, eds. Stress. Hillsdale, NJ: L. Erlbaum Associates; 1987. Handbook of Psychology and Health; vol 5.
  • 31.Day Baird D, Dunson DB, Hill MC, Cousins D, Schectman JM. High cumulative incidence of uterine leiomyoma in Black and White women: ultrasound evidence. Am J Obstet Gynecol. 2003;188:100–107. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Vines AI, Baird DD, McNeilly M, Hertz-Picciotto I, Light KC, Stevens J. Development and reliability of a Telephone-Administered Perceived Racism Scale (TPRS): a tool for epidemiological use. Ethn Dis. 2001;11:251–262. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Cantuaria GH, Angioli R, Frost L, Duncan R, Penalver MA. Comparison of bimanual examination with ultrasound examination before hysterectomy for uterine leiomyoma. Obstet Gynecol. 1998;92:109–112. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Ferland M, Despres JP, Tremblay A, et al. Assessment of adipose tissue distribution by computed axial tomography in obese women: association with body density and anthropometric measurements. Br J Nutr. 1989;61:139–148. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Stevens J. Obesity, fat patterning and cardiovascular. In: Longenecker JB, Kritchevsky D, Drezner MK, eds. Nutrition and Biotechnology in Heart Disease and Cancer. New York: Plenum Press; 1995, chap 3.
  • 36.Bray G. What is the ideal body weight? J Nutr Biochem. 1998;9:489–492. [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Marniemi J, Kronholm E, Aunola S, et al. Visceral fat and psychosocial stress in identical twins discordant for obesity. J Intern Med. 2002;251:35–43. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Tull ES, Sheu YT, Butler C, Cornelious K. Relationships between perceived stress, coping behavior and cortisol secretion in women with high and low levels of internalized racism. J Natl Med Assoc. 2005;97: 206–212. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Krieger N, Sidney S. Racial discrimination and blood pressure: the CARDIA study of young Black and White adults. Am J Public Health. 1996;86:1370–1378. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Vines AI, Baird DD, McNeilly M, et al. Social correlates of the chronic stress of perceived racism among Black women. Ethn Dis. 2006;16:101–107. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from American Journal of Public Health are provided here courtesy of American Public Health Association

RESOURCES